Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Pharmacol., 13 July 2021
Sec. Neuropharmacology
This article is part of the Research Topic Psychedelic Sociality: Pharmacological and Extrapharmacological Perspectives View all 22 articles

Role of the 5-HT2A Receptor in Acute Effects of LSD on Empathy and Circulating Oxytocin

  • 1Clinical Pharmacology and Toxicology, Department of Biomedicine and Department of Clinical Research, University Hospital Basel and University of Basel, Basel, Switzerland
  • 2Department of Pharmaceutical Sciences, University of Basel, Basel, Switzerland
  • 3Psychiatric University Hospital, University of Basel, Basel, Switzerland
  • 4Transfaculty Research Platform Molecular and Cognitive Neuroscience, University of Basel, Basel, Switzerland

The psychedelic lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD) has experienced a revival in research, including clinical trials that evaluate LSD-assisted psychotherapy. LSD induces perceptual alterations and influences emotion processing in ways that may support psychotherapy. Here, we investigated the effects of LSD on emotional empathy and mediating role of the serotonin 5-hydroxytryptamine-2A (5-HT2A) receptor by administering 25, 50, 100, and 200 µg LSD alone and 200 µg LSD combined with pretreatment with the 5-HT2A receptor antagonist ketanserin (40 mg) using a placebo-controlled, double-blind, random-order, crossover design in 16 healthy subjects. The Multifaceted Empathy Test (MET) was used to assess the effects of LSD on emotional empathy. Plasma oxytocin levels were also measured. LSD dose-dependently increased implicit and explicit emotional empathy, with the highest 200 µg LSD dose having a significant effect compared with placebo. The 200 µg dose of LSD also moderately increased plasma oxytocin levels compared with placebo. Ketanserin reduced the LSD-induced elevations of oxytocin but not the LSD-induced increases in emotional empathy. These findings confirm that LSD enhances empathy, and this effect may be partially independent of its primary action on 5-HT2A receptors to induce subjective psychedelic effects. In contrast, LSD-induced oxytocin release may depend on 5-HT2A receptor stimulation, which is consistent with the psychedelic effect of LSD. Further studies are needed to investigate whether LSD may also enhance empathy and potentially produce therapeutic effects in patients who have deficits in empathy and impairments in social functioning.

Introduction

The prototypical psychedelic lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD) is experiencing a revival in psychiatric research. Possible medical benefits of the substance are currently investigated in Phase 2 trials in patients with anxiety clinicaltrials.gov no. NCT03153579; (Gasser et al., 2014), depression (clinicaltrials.gov no. NCT03866252), and cluster headache clinicaltrials.gov no. NCT03781128; (Gasser et al., 2014; Carhart-Harris et al., 2015; Dolder et al., 2015; Schmid et al., 2015; Carhart-Harris et al., 2016a; Strajhar et al., 2016; Holze et al., 2020; Holze et al., 2021). The mechanism by which LSD produce potential therapeutic effects in different disorders is not well understood. The primary target for LSD to produce its acute psychedelic effects in humans appears to be the serotonin 5-hydroxytryptamine-2A (5-HT2A) receptor because the typical acute subjective effects of LSD can be nearly completely blocked by pretreatment with the 5-HT2A receptor antagonist ketanserin (Preller et al., 2017; Holze et al., 2021). Ketanserin alone possesses no psychoactive properties but may result in dry mouth, nasal congestion, tiredness, and reduced blood pressure (Persson et al., 1991). However, LSD also binds with high affinity to 5-HT1A and dopamine receptors (Rickli et al., 2016). Many downstream mechanisms have been proposed to be involved in the potential therapeutic effects of psychedelics, including LSD, such as an increase in neuroplasticity, reflected by increases in brain-derived neurotrophic factor (Ly et al., 2018; Hutten et al., 2020), alterations of brain network connectivity (Carhart-Harris et al., 2016b; Mueller et al., 2018; Mueller et al., 2017b), socioemotional effects (Schmid et al., 2015), and related changes in emotion processing (Dolder et al., 2016; Mueller et al., 2017a). LSD has been shown to produce empathogenic and prosocial effects (Dolder et al., 2016; Schmid et al., 2015). Specifically, LSD acutely increased feelings of subjective well-being, happiness, closeness to others, openness, and trust (Dolder et al., 2016; Schmid et al., 2015), impaired the recognition of sad and fearful faces in the Face Emotion Recognition Task, enhanced emotional empathy in the Multifaceted Empathy Test (MET), and increased prosocial behavior in the Social Value Orientation Test. Such empathogenic and prosocial effects were otherwise typically reported for the prototypical empathogen 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA; (Hysek et al., 2014; Bershad et al., 2016; Dolder et al., 2018), which mainly releases serotonin and oxytocin (Dumont et al., 2009; Hysek et al., 2012; Hysek et al., 2014; Francis et al., 2016) and is also investigated in substance-assisted psychotherapy. The findings indicate that LSD and MDMA may have overlapping socioemotional effect properties. Oxytocin is thought to play a critical role in emotion processing. Intranasal oxytocin administration increased emotional empathy, trust, and positive communication and enhanced emotion recognition (Kosfeld et al., 2005; Di Simplicio et al., 2009; Ditzen et al., 2009; Hurlemann et al., 2010). Interestingly, LSD also increased plasma oxytocin levels, although only at a high dose (Schmid et al., 2015; Holze et al., 2020) and to a lesser extent than MDMA (Holze et al., 2020). The effects of different low to high doses of LSD on empathy have not yet been investigated. Unclear is whether the empathy-enhancing and oxytocin-releasing effects of LSD are mediated by 5-HT2A receptors.

Therefore, the present study investigated the effects of LSD (0, 25, 50, 100, and 200 μg) on the MET. Additionally, the 5-HT2A receptor antagonist ketanserin or placebo was administered prior to the highest 200 μg LSD dose to examine whether the effects of LSD on emotional empathy and circulating plasma oxytocin levels are mediated by 5-HT2A receptors. The subjective and autonomic effects of LSD and pharmacokinetics of LSD that were investigated in the present study were reported elsewhere (Holze et al., 2021). However, subjective effects (“any drug effect”) and plasma LSD concentrations are reported herein as peak responses and at 6 h after LSD administration when the MET was performed.

Materials and Methods

Study Design

The study used a double-blind, placebo-controlled, crossover design with six experimental test sessions to investigate the responses to (i) placebo, (ii) 25 µg LSD, (iii) 50 µg LSD, (iv) 100 µg LSD, (v) 200 µg LSD, and (vi) 200 µg LSD 1 h after ketanserin administration (40 mg) as reported in detail elsewhere (Holze et al., 2021). Block randomization was used to counterbalance the different dosing conditions. The washout periods between sessions were at least 10 days. The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and International Conference on Harmonization Guidelines in Good Clinical Practice and approved by the Ethics Committee of Northwest Switzerland (EKNZ) and Swiss Federal Office for Public Health. All of the subjects provided written consent before participating in the study, and they were paid for their participation. The study was registered at ClinicalTrials.gov (NCT03321136). Other effects of LSD that were assessed in this study were reported previously (Holze et al., 2021).

Participants

Sixteen healthy subjects (eight men and eight women; mean age ± SD: 29 ± 6.4 years; range: 25–52 years) were recruited by word of mouth or an advertisement that was posted on the web market platform of the University of Basel. Mean body weight was 69 kg. Exclusion criteria were age <25 years or >65 years, pregnancy (urine pregnancy test at screening and before each test session), personal or family (first-degree relative) history of major psychiatric disorders (assessed by the Semi-structured Clinical Interview for Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edition, Axis I disorders by a trained psychiatrist), the use of medications that may interfere with the study medications (e.g., antidepressants, antipsychotics, and sedatives), chronic or acute physical illness (e.g., abnormal physical exam, electrocardiogram, or hematological and chemical blood analyses), tobacco smoking (> 10 cigarettes/day), lifetime prevalence of illicit drug use > 10 times (except for Δ9-tetrahydrocannabinol), illicit drug use within the last 2 months, and illicit drug use during the study period (determined by urine drug tests). The participants were asked to consume no more than 10 standard alcoholic drinks/week and have no more than one drink on the day before the test sessions. Additionally, the participants were not allowed to drink xanthine-containing liquids after midnight on the study day. The drug use history of the participants was reported elsewhere (Holze et al., 2021). Drug of abuse tests that were performed for each subject once during screening and once during the study were negative.

Study Procedures

The study included a screening visit, six 25-h test sessions (each separated by at least 10 days), and an end-of-study visit. The sessions were conducted in a calm standard hospital room equipped with a standard hospital bed for the participant and a desk and a chair for the investigator. The room had an adjoining balcony, which participants were allowed to access after peak effects had subsided. Only one research subject and one investigator were present during each test session. Participants were allowed to bring their own music and to bring occupation for the time after effects had subsided or for placebo days (e.g. book, laptop, games etc.). Blindfolds were provided upon request. The test sessions began at 7:45 AM. A urine sample was taken to verify abstinence from drugs of abuse, and a urine pregnancy test was performed in women. The subjects then underwent baseline measurements. Ketanserin (40 mg) or placebo was administered at 8:00 AM. LSD or placebo was administered at 9:00 AM. Standardized lunches and dinners were served at 1:30 PM and 6:00 PM, respectively. The subjects were never alone during the first 16 h after drug administration, and the investigator was in a room next to the subject for up to 24 h. The subjects were sent home the next day at 9:15 AM.

Study Drug

LSD base (> 99% purity; Lipomed AG, Arlesheim, Switzerland) was administered as an oral solution that was produced according to good manufacturing practice in units that contained 100 or 25 µg LSD in 1 ml of 96% ethanol (Holze et al., 2019; Holze et al., 2021). Ketanserin was obtained as the marketed drug Ketensin (20 mg, Janssen-Cilag, Leiden, Netherlands) and encapsulated with opaque capsules to ensure blinding. Placebo consisted of identical opaque capsules that were filled with mannitol. A double-dummy method was used. The subjects received two capsules and two solutions in each session: (i) two placebo capsules and placebo/placebo solutions, (ii) two placebo capsules and 25 µg LSD/placebo solutions, (iii) two placebo capsules and 25 µg LSD/25 µg LSD solutions, (iv) two placebo capsules and 100 µg LSD/placebo solutions, (v) two placebo capsules and 100 µg LSD/100 µg LSD solutions, and (vi) two ketanserin capsules and 100 µg LSD/100 µg LSD solutions.

Measures

Multifaceted Empathy Test. The MET is a reliable and valid task that assesses cognitive and emotional aspects of empathy (Dziobek et al., 2008). The MET has been shown to be sensitive to the effects of oxytocin (Hurlemann et al., 2010), MDMA (Hysek et al., 2014; Kuypers et al., 2014; Schmid et al., 2014), psilocybin (Preller et al., 2015), and LSD (Dolder et al., 2016). The computer-assisted test consisted of 40 photographs that showed people in emotionally charged situations. To measure emotional empathy, the subjects were asked to rate how much they were feeling for an individual in each scene (i.e., explicit emotional empathy) and how much they were aroused by each scene (i.e., implicit emotional empathy) on a 1–9 point scale. The latter rating provides an inherent additional assessment of emotional empathy, which is considered to reduce the likelihood of socially desirable answers. To assess cognitive empathy, the participants were required to infer the mental state of the subject in each scene and indicate the correct mental state from a list of four responses. Cognitive empathy was defined as the percentage of correct responses relative to total responses. The three aspects of empathy were each tested with 20 stimuli with positive valence and 20 stimuli with negative valence, resulting in a total of 120 trials. The MET was performed 6 h after LSD administration.

Subjective mood. The visual analog scale (VAS) for “any drug effect” was assessed repeatedly over 24 h. The VAS was presented as a 100-mm horizontal line (0 ± 100%), marked from “not at all” on the left to “extremely” on the right. The questionnaire was also administered 6 h after drug administration, immediately before the MET was performed.

Oxytocin concentrations. Oxytocin concentrations in blood plasma were measured using an oxytocin enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay kit (ENZO Life Sciences, Ann Arbor, MI, United States) according the manufacturer’s protocol (Holt-Lunstad et al., 2008). Plasma was diluted in a 2:1 ratio in 1.5% TFA-H2O and centrifuged at 17,100 × g for 15 min at 4°C. The supernatant was collected and then loaded onto an Oasis PRiME HLB 96-well plate with 30 mg sorbent (Waters, Milford, MA, United States) and washed with 0.1% TFA-H2O. The sample was then eluted with 95% acetonitrile + 5% of a 0.1% TFA-H2O solution. The elute was collected and dried in an Eppendorf centrifugal vacuum concentrator (model no. 5301, Eppendorf AG, Hamburg, Germany). For the measurements, the residue was reconstituted in assay buffer. The reconstituted samples, standards, and controls were plated on a goat anti-rabbit immunoglobulin G microtiter plate and incubated with oxytocin conjugate at 4°C overnight. The plate was then washed, and the substrate was added. The reaction was stopped after 1 h, and the plate was read at 405 nm. Oxytocin concentrations were determined from a standard curve that was calculated from a four-parameter logistics curve fit. Blood samples for oxytocin concentration determination were taken 1, 3, and 8 h after LSD or placebo administration.

LSD concentrations. Blood samples for the analysis of plasma LSD levels were collected repeatedly in lithium heparin tubes. Plasma concentrations of LSD were determined by ultra-high-performance liquid chromatography tandem mass spectrometry with a lower limit of quantification of 5 pg/ml as reported elsewhere (Holze et al., 2019; Holze et al., 2021).

Statistical Analyses

Dose-response effects of LSD (25, 50, 100, and 200 µg) on emotional empathy measures were assessed using repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA), with drug as the within-subjects factor, followed by the Tukey post hoc test based on significant main effects. Separate ANOVAs were applied to assess the effect of ketanserin on the LSD response, with drug (LSD alone, LSD + ketanserin, and placebo) as the within-subjects factor, followed by the Tukey post hoc test based on significant main effects. The data were analyzed using Statistica 12 software (StatSoft, Tulsa, OK, United States). The criterion for significance was p < 0.05.

Results

Empathy

The effects of LSD on explicit and implicit emotional empathy are shown in Figure 1. There were significant main effects of LSD on explicit and implicit emotional empathy ratings (F4,44 = 4.64, p < 0.01, and F4,44 = 4.82, p < 0.01, respectively), indicating that LSD dose-dependently and significantly increased both aspects of emotional empathy. Only the highest 200 µg dose of LSD significantly affected explicit (p < 0.05) and implicit (p < 0.01) empathy scores compared with placebo. LSD did not alter cognitive empathy (data not shown).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. Acute effects of LSD on emotional empathy on the Multifaceted Empathy Test. LSD dose-dependently increased explicit (A) and implicit (B) emotional empathy compared with placebo, with the 200 µg LSD dose producing significant effects. The increases in explicit (C) and implicit (D) empathy that were induced by the 200 µg LSD dose were only slightly reduced by ketanserin pretreatment compared with placebo pretreatment and remained elevated compared with placebo. The data are expressed as the mean ± SEM in 16 subjects (*)p < 0.1, *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.01.

We then examined the effect of pretreatment with ketanserin on the LSD-induced increase in empathy. There were significant main effects of drug on explicit and implicit emotional empathy ratings (F2,28 = 4.53, p < 0.05, and F2,28 = 8.56, p < 0.01, respectively). The post hoc tests showed that both LSD and LSD + ketanserin increased explicit (p < 0.05 and p = 0.09, respectively) and implicit (p < 0.001 and p < 0.01, respectively) empathy compared with placebo. Ketanserin did not significantly reduce the LSD-induced increase in empathy (Figure 1).

Subjective Drug Effects

The subjective effects of LSD on “any drug effect,” assessed by the VAS, are shown in Figure 2. LSD dose-dependently produced subjective effects that reached half-maximal levels 6 h after LSD administration when the MET was administered.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. Acute subjective effects of LSD. LSD dose-dependently induced subjective effects, measured as “any drug effects,” on the VAS. The data show the subjective effects as maximal response and at 6 h after drug administration when the empathy test was performed. The data are expressed as the mean ± SEM in 16 subjects.

Oxytocin Concentrations

There were significant main effects on oxytocin release 1 and 3 h after drug intake (F2,28 = 7.00, p < 0.01, and F2,28 = 4.21, p < 0.05, respectively). The post hoc tests showed that only LSD alone increased oxytocin levels 1 and 3 h after drug intake (p < 0.01 and p = 0.06, respectively) compared with placebo and at 1 and 3 h after drug intake (p < 0.01 and p < 0.05, respectively) compared with the LSD + ketanserin (Figure 3). Thus, ketanserin effectively prevented the LSD-induced increase in circulating oxytocin.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 3. Effects of LSD on circulating oxytocin. LSD alone significantly increased plasma oxytocin concentrations 60 min after administration compared with placebo. LSD did not increase oxytocin levels after ketanserin pretreatment compared with placebo pretreatment. The data are expressed as the mean ± SEM in 16 subjects (*)p < 0.1, **p < 0.01, compared with placebo; +p < 0.05, compared with LSD + ketanserin.

Plasma Drug Levels

Plasma LSD concentrations were 2.2 ± 1.0 ng/ml (mean ± SD) 6 h after administration of the 200 µg dose and 2.5 ± 0.9 ng/ml 6 h after the administration of 200 µg LSD + ketanserin, which was at the time when the empathy task began. For comparison, Emax concentrations after 200 µg LSD and 200 µg LSD + ketanserin were 3.9 ± 0.8 and 4.4 ± 0.8 ng/ml, respectively. The full pharmacokinetic data on LSD from this study were published previously in detail (Holze et al., 2021).

Discussion

LSD enhanced explicit and implicit emotional empathy and increased plasma oxytocin concentrations at a dose of 200 µg. The results confirmed previous findings (Schmid et al., 2015; Dolder et al., 2016). Consistent with past study findings, both explicit and implicit emotional empathy were enhanced by LSD and only by the highest 200 µg dose and not by doses of 100 µg or lower (Dolder et al., 2016). Similarly, the psychedelic psilocybin and empathogen MDMA have been shown to increase emotional empathy on the MET (Hysek et al., 2014; Kuypers et al., 2014; Preller et al., 2015), indicating that all of these psychoactive substances that are currently being investigated to assist psychotherapy similarly influence emotion processing. Additionally, the present study investigated the mediating role of 5-HT2A receptor stimulation on psychedelic-induced alterations of emotional empathy. Ketanserin has been previously shown to very effectively diminish all acute subjective responses to LSD (Preller et al., 2017; Holze et al., 2021), while not influencing plasma LSD concentrations (Holze et al., 2021). In the present study, ketanserin only weakly attenuated the LSD-induced increases in explicit and implicit empathy, and ratings remained elevated compared with placebo. This finding indicates that the empathogenic effects of LSD may not critically depend on 5-HT2A receptor activation, unlike its subjective psychedelic effects, but could potentially be mediated by other receptors, including serotonin 5-HT1 receptors. A previous study that tested the 5-HT1A agonist buspirone demonstrated the involvement of 5-HT1A receptors in emotion processing (Bernasconi et al., 2015). LSD shows strong binding to 5-HT1A receptors (Rickli et al., 2016), and this receptor is not blocked by ketanserin and could thus play a role in the LSD-induced enhancement of emotional empathy. Oxytocin has been shown to contribute to emotional processes, including empathy (Kosfeld et al., 2005; Di Simplicio et al., 2009; Ditzen et al., 2009; Hurlemann et al., 2010). Therefore, we also tested whether LSD increases circulating levels of oxytocin and whether this effect depends on 5-HT2A receptor activation. LSD increased plasma oxytocin concentrations at the 200 μg dose (not measured for lower doses), consistent with a previous study (Schmid et al., 2015). In contrast, a lower dose of 100 μg LSD did not increase plasma oxytocin levels in another study (Holze et al., 2020). Furthermore, the LSD-induced increase in oxytocin in the present and previous studies by a high dose (Schmid et al., 2015) was only 1.25 to 3-fold higher compared with placebo, whereas MDMA increased plasma oxytocin levels 3 to 11-fold compared with placebo (Dolder et al., 2018; Holze et al., 2020). Thus, LSD may less effectively induce the release of oxytocin compared with MDMA. With regard to the mechanism of action, preclinical data indicate that MDMA induces its marked effects on oxytocin release via serotonin release and consecutive 5-HT1 receptor stimulation (Thompson et al., 2007; Hunt et al., 2011). In contrast, in the present study, ketanserin prevented the moderate LSD-induced increase in plasma oxytocin, indicating that the effects of LSD on the oxytocin system involve 5-HT2A receptors, which is consistent with most other effects of LSD (Kraehenmann et al., 2017a; Kraehenmann et al., 2017b; Preller et al., 2017; Barrett et al., 2018; Preller et al., 2018; Holze et al., 2021). Thus, the mechanisms that result in oxytocin release may be distinct for MDMA and LSD and need further investigation. For example, the effects of LSD on oxytocin may be more indirect. LSD acutely induces marked alterations of perception and also moderately increases anxiety at 200 μg (Holze et al., 2021) while additionally elevating autonomic stimulation and plasma markers of serotonergic activity and stress, including circulating prolactin (Schmid et al., 2015) and glucocorticoids (Strajhar et al., 2016). Oxytocin may moderately increase in response to LSD-induced psychological stress. For example, oxytocin has been considered an anti-stress hormone (Rae et al., 2021). Thus, the oxytocin response might be triggered by subjective LSD-induced effects rather than by a direct pharmacological effect of LSD. Supporting this possibility, oxytocin levels did not increase when the subjective mind-altering effects of LSD were blocked with ketanserin. Instead, oxytocin levels only increased when subjective effects were also highly elevated. Taken together, the present findings indicate that LSD-induced empathogenic effects are neither mediated via oxytocin release nor via direct 5-HT2A receptor activation and that the involvement of other factors and receptors in emotion processing is likely.

The present study has several strengths. The study used a pharmaceutically well-defined investigational drug product (Holze et al., 2019; Holze et al., 2021) and a highly valid double-blind, multi-dose, random-order design that included both inactive and “active” placebo in the form of low-dose LSD. Additionally, we assessed emotional empathy not only for a single dose but also for a range of doses and included an evaluation of the mediating role of a key target receptor of LSD within the same study and participants.

The present study also has limitations. We only included a small sample size of 16 participants. However, because we used a within-subjects design, we included data from 96 study sessions. Oxytocin was only measured for the highest 200 μg dose of LSD because the 100 μg dose showed no effect in previous studies. Because of the strong acute effects of LSD on cognitive performance and concentration (Schmid et al., 2015; Holze et al., 2020; Holze et al., 2021), the MET could not meaningfully be performed during the peak response to LSD. Instead, it was conducted 6 h after LSD administration when plasma LSD concentrations and subjective effects declined to approximately half-maximal levels. One possibility or likelihood was that the lower dose of LSD of 100 μg similarly enhanced empathy during the peak response at 3 h as the 200 μg at 6 h after LSD administration, but this was not tested. Thus, we cannot exclude the possibility that lower doses of LSD also significantly increase empathy. Furthermore, the present study included healthy subjects in a highly controlled laboratory setting, and the effects of LSD may be different in situations of uncontrolled recreational use or in therapeutic settings in patients (de Wit et al., 2021a; Schmid et al., 2021).

In conclusion, the present findings indicate that LSD enhances empathy, which is likely also beneficial for therapeutic application in patients. Psychedelics and MDMA have been shown to potentially trigger therapeutic processes and marked changes in personal attitudes and well-being after single-dose administration and with often lasting effects even in healthy subjects (Griffiths et al., 2008; Schmid and Liechti, 2018; de Wit et al., 2021b). These sometimes long-lasting effects are at least partially associated with alterations of mind and mystical-type experiences (Griffiths et al., 2008; Schmid and Liechti, 2018) and may also be linked to the acute experience of feeling strong emotional concern for others (i.e., emotional empathy) and a connection with other people (de Wit et al., 2021b). Remaining to be investigated is whether LSD also enhances empathy in patients with deficits in empathy and impairments in social functioning and whether such effects contribute to potential therapeutic effects of LSD.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusion of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Ethikkommission Nordwest- und Zentralschweiz (EKNZ). The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

FH, IA, NV, and AE analyzed the data. FH, IA, and ML wrote the manuscript. ML conceived the study and obtained funding.

Funding

This work was supported by the Swiss National Science Foundation (grant no. 32003B_185111 to ML) and University of Basel. Knowhow and data associated with this work and owned by the University Hospital Basel were licensed by Mind Medicine, Inc., after study completion. Mind Medicine, Inc., had no role in financing, planning, or conducting the present study or the present publication.

Conflict of Interest

ML is a consultant for Mind Medicine, Inc.

The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors acknowledge the assistance of M. Arends with text editing.

References

Barrett, F. S., Preller, K. H., Herdener, M., Janata, P., and Vollenweider, F. X. (2018). Serotonin 2A Receptor Signaling Underlies LSD-Induced Alteration of the Neural Response to Dynamic Changes in Music. Cereb. Cortex 28 (11), 3939–3950. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhx257

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bernasconi, F., Kometer, M., Pokorny, T., Seifritz, E., and Vollenweider, F. X. (2015). The Electrophysiological Effects of the Serotonin 1A Receptor Agonist Buspirone in Emotional Face Processing. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 25 (4), 474–482. doi:10.1016/j.euroneuro.2015.01.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bershad, A. K., Miller, M. A., Baggott, M. J., and de Wit, H. (2016). The Effects of MDMA on Socio-Emotional Processing: Does MDMA Differ from Other Stimulants? J. Psychopharmacol. 30, 1248–1258. doi:10.1177/0269881116663120

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carhart-Harris, R. L., Kaelen, M., Bolstridge, M., Williams, T. M., Williams, L. T., Underwood, R., et al. (2016a). The Paradoxical Psychological Effects of Lysergic Acid Diethylamide (LSD). Psychol. Med. 46, 1379–1390. doi:10.1017/S0033291715002901

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carhart-Harris, R. L., Kaelen, M., Whalley, M. G., Bolstridge, M., Feilding, A., and Nutt, D. J. (2015). LSD Enhances Suggestibility in Healthy Volunteers. Psychopharmacology 232 (4), 785–794. doi:10.1007/s00213-014-3714-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carhart-Harris, R. L., Muthukumaraswamy, S., Roseman, L., Kaelen, M., Droog, W., Murphy, K., et al. (2016b). Neural Correlates of the LSD Experience Revealed by Multimodal Neuroimaging. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 113, 4853–4858. doi:10.1073/pnas.1518377113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Wit, H., Bershad, A. K., and Grob, C. (2021a). Challenges in Translational Research: MDMA in the Laboratory versus Therapeutic Settings. J. Psychopharmacol., 026988112110152. doi:10.1177/02698811211015221

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Wit, H., Bershad, A. K., Hutchison, W., and Bremmer, M. (2021b). Can MDMA Change Sociopolitical Values? Insights from a Research Participant. Biol. Psychiatry 89, e61–e62. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2021.01.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Di Simplicio, M., Massey-Chase, R., Cowen, P., and Harmer, C. (2009). Oxytocin Enhances Processing of Positive versus Negative Emotional Information in Healthy Male Volunteers. J. Psychopharmacol. 23 (3), 241–248. doi:10.1177/0269881108095705

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ditzen, B., Schaer, M., Gabriel, B., Bodenmann, G., Ehlert, U., and Heinrichs, M. (2009). Intranasal Oxytocin Increases Positive Communication and Reduces Cortisol Levels during Couple Conflict. Biol. Psychiatry 65 (9), 728–731. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.10.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dolder, P. C., Müller, F., Schmid, Y., Borgwardt, S. J., and Liechti, M. E. (2018). Direct Comparison of the Acute Subjective, Emotional, Autonomic, and Endocrine Effects of MDMA, Methylphenidate, and Modafinil in Healthy Subjects. Psychopharmacology 235 (2), 467–479. doi:10.1007/s00213-017-4650-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dolder, P. C., Schmid, Y., Haschke, M., Rentsch, K. M., and Liechti, M. E. (2015). Pharmacokinetics and Concentration-Effect Relationship of Oral LSD in Humans. Ijnppy 19, pyv072. doi:10.1093/ijnp/pyv072

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dolder, P. C., Schmid, Y., Müller, F., Borgwardt, S., and Liechti, M. E. (2016). LSD Acutely Impairs Fear Recognition and Enhances Emotional Empathy and Sociality. Neuropsychopharmacol 41, 2638–2646. doi:10.1038/npp.2016.82

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dumont, G. J. H., Sweep, F. C. G. J., van der Steen, R., Hermsen, R., Donders, A. R. T., Touw, D. J., et al. (2009). Increased Oxytocin Concentrations and Prosocial Feelings in Humans after Ecstasy (3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine) Administration. Soc. Neurosci. 4 (4), 359–366. doi:10.1080/17470910802649470

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dziobek, I., Rogers, K., Fleck, S., Bahnemann, M., Heekeren, H. R., Wolf, O. T., et al. (2008). Dissociation of Cognitive and Emotional Empathy in Adults with Asperger Syndrome Using the Multifaceted Empathy Test (MET). J. Autism Dev. Disord. 38 (3), 464–473. doi:10.1007/s10803-007-0486-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Francis, S. M., Kirkpatrick, M. G., de Wit, H., and Jacob, S. (2016). Urinary and Plasma Oxytocin Changes in Response to MDMA or Intranasal Oxytocin Administration. Psychoneuroendocrinology 74, 92–100. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.08.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gasser, P., Holstein, D., Michel, Y., Doblin, R., Yazar-Klosinski, B., Passie, T., et al. (2014). Safety and Efficacy of Lysergic Acid Diethylamide-Assisted Psychotherapy for Anxiety Associated with Life-Threatening Diseases. J. Nerv Ment. Dis. 202 (7), 513–520. doi:10.1097/NMD.0000000000000113

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Griffiths, R., Richards, W., Johnson, M., McCann, U., and Jesse, R. (2008). Mystical-type Experiences Occasioned by Psilocybin Mediate the Attribution of Personal Meaning and Spiritual Significance 14 Months Later. J. Psychopharmacol. 22 (6), 621–632. doi:10.1177/0269881108094300

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holt-Lunstad, J., Birmingham, W. A., and Light, K. C. (2008). Influence of a "warm Touch" Support Enhancement Intervention Among Married Couples on Ambulatory Blood Pressure, Oxytocin, Alpha Amylase, and Cortisol. Psychosom Med. 70 (9), 976–985. doi:10.1097/PSY.0b013e318187aef7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holze, F., Duthaler, U., Vizeli, P., Müller, F., Borgwardt, S., and Liechti, M. E. (2019). Pharmacokinetics and Subjective Effects of a Novel Oral LSD Formulation in Healthy Subjects. Br. J. Clin. Pharmacol. 85, 1474–1483. doi:10.1111/bcp.13918

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holze, F., Vizeli, P., Ley, L., Müller, F., Dolder, P., Stocker, M., et al. (2021). Acute Dose-dependent Effects of Lysergic Acid Diethylamide in a Double-Blind Placebo-Controlled Study in Healthy Subjects. Neuropsychopharmacol. 46 (3), 537–544. doi:10.1038/s41386-020-00883-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holze, F., Vizeli, P., Müller, F., Ley, L., Duerig, R., Varghese, N., et al. (2020). Distinct Acute Effects of LSD, MDMA, and D-Amphetamine in Healthy Subjects. Neuropsychopharmacol. 45 (3), 462–471. doi:10.1038/s41386-019-0569-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hunt, G. E., McGregor, I. S., Cornish, J. L., and Callaghan, P. D. (2011). MDMA-induced C-Fos Expression in Oxytocin-Containing Neurons Is Blocked by Pretreatment with the 5-HT-1A Receptor Antagonist WAY 100635. Brain Res. Bull. 86 (1-2), 65–73. doi:10.1016/j.brainresbull.2011.06.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hurlemann, R., Patin, A., Onur, O. A., Cohen, M. X., Baumgartner, T., Metzler, S., et al. (2010). Oxytocin Enhances Amygdala-dependent, Socially Reinforced Learning and Emotional Empathy in Humans. J. Neurosci. 30 (14), 4999–5007. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5538-09.2010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hutten, N. R. P. W., Mason, N. L., Dolder, P. C., Theunissen, E. L., Holze, F., Liechti, M. E., et al. (2020). Low Doses of LSD Acutely Increase BDNF Blood Plasma Levels in Healthy Volunteers. ACS Pharmacol. Transl. Sci. 4 (2), 461–466. doi:10.1021/acsptsci.0c00099

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hysek, C. M., Schmid, Y., Simmler, L. D., Domes, G., Heinrichs, M., Eisenegger, C., et al. (2014). MDMA Enhances Emotional Empathy and Prosocial Behavior. Soc. Cogn. Affect Neurosci. 9, 1645–1652. doi:10.1093/scan/nst161

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hysek, C. M., Simmler, L. D., Nicola, V. G., Vischer, N., Donzelli, M., Krähenbühl, S., et al. (2012). Duloxetine Inhibits Effects of MDMA ("ecstasy") In Vitro and in Humans in a Randomized Placebo-Controlled Laboratory Study. PLoS One 7 (5), e36476. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0036476

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kosfeld, M., Heinrichs, M., Zak, P. J., Fischbacher, U., and Fehr, E. (2005). Oxytocin Increases Trust in Humans. Nature 435 (7042), 673–676. doi:10.1038/nature03701

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kraehenmann, R., Pokorny, D., Aicher, H., Preller, K. H., Pokorny, T., Bosch, O. G., et al. (2017a). LSD Increases Primary Process Thinking via Serotonin 2A Receptor Activation. Front. Pharmacol. 8, 814. doi:10.3389/fphar.2017.00814

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kraehenmann, R., Pokorny, D., Vollenweider, L., Preller, K. H., Pokorny, T., Seifritz, E., et al. (2017b). Dreamlike Effects of LSD on Waking Imagery in Humans Depend on Serotonin 2A Receptor Activation. Psychopharmacology 234 (13), 2031–2046. doi:10.1007/s00213-017-4610-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuypers, K. P. C., de la Torre, R., Farre, M., Yubero-Lahoz, S., Dziobek, I., Van den Bos, W., et al. (2014). No Evidence that MDMA-Induced Enhancement of Emotional Empathy Is Related to Peripheral Oxytocin Levels or 5-HT1a Receptor Activation. PLoS One 9 (6), e100719. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0100719

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ly, C., Greb, A. C., Cameron, L. P., Wong, J. M., Barragan, E. V., Wilson, P. C., et al. (2018). Psychedelics Promote Structural and Functional Neural Plasticity. Cell Rep. 23 (11), 3170–3182. doi:10.1016/j.celrep.2018.05.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mueller, F., Lenz, C., Dolder, P. C., Harder, S., Schmid, Y., Lang, U. E., et al. (2017a). Acute Effects of LSD on Amygdala Activity during Processing of Fearful Stimuli in Healthy Subjects. Transl Psychiatry 7, e1084. doi:10.1038/tp.2017.54

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Müller, F., Dolder, P. C., Schmidt, A., Liechti, M. E., and Borgwardt, S. (2018). Altered Network Hub Connectivity after Acute LSD Administration. Neuroimage Clin. 18, 694–701. doi:10.1016/j.nicl.2018.03.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Müller, F., Lenz, C., Dolder, P., Lang, U., Schmidt, A., Liechti, M., et al. (2017b). Increased Thalamic Resting-State Connectivity as a Core Driver of LSD-Induced Hallucinations. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 136, 648–657. doi:10.1111/acps.12818

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Persson, B., Heykants, J., and Hedner, T. (1991). Clinical Pharmacokinetics of Ketanserin. Clin. Pharmacokinet. 20 (4), 263–279. doi:10.2165/00003088-199120040-00002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Preller, K. H., Burt, J. B., Ji, J. L., Schleifer, C. H., Adkinson, B. D., Stämpfli, P., et al. (2018). Changes in Global and Thalamic Brain Connectivity in LSD-Induced Altered States of Consciousness Are Attributable to the 5-HT2A Receptor. Elife 7, e35082. doi:10.7554/eLife.35082

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Preller, K. H., Herdener, M., Pokorny, T., Planzer, A., Kraehenmann, R., Stämpfli, P., et al. (2017). The Fabric of Meaning and Subjective Effects in LSD-Induced States Depend on Serotonin 2A Receptor Activation. Curr. Biol. 27, 451–457. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2016.12.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Preller, K. H., Pokorny, T., Krähenmann, R., Dziobek, I., Stämpfli, P., and Vollenweider, F. X. (2015). The Effect of 5-HT2A/1a Agonist Treatment on Social Cognition, Empathy, and Social Decision-Making. Eur. Psychiatry 30 (Suppl. 1), 22. doi:10.1016/S0924-9338(15)30017-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rae, M., Lemos Duarte, M., Gomes, I., Camarini, R., and Devi, L. A. (2021). Oxytocin and Vasopressin: Signalling, Behavioural Modulation and Potential Therapeutic Effects. Br. J. Pharmacol. doi:10.1111/bph.15481

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rickli, A., Moning, O. D., Hoener, M. C., and Liechti, M. E. (2016). Receptor Interaction Profiles of Novel Psychoactive Tryptamines Compared with Classic Hallucinogens. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 26, 1327–1337. doi:10.1016/j.euroneuro.2016.05.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmid, Y., Enzler, F., Gasser, P., Grouzmann, E., Preller, K. H., Vollenweider, F. X., et al. (2015). Acute Effects of Lysergic Acid Diethylamide in Healthy Subjects. Biol. Psychiatry 78 (8), 544–553. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.11.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmid, Y., Gasser, P., Oehen, P., and Liechti, M. E. (2021). Acute Subjective Effects in LSD- and MDMA-Assisted Psychotherapy. J. Psychopharmacol. 35, 362–374. doi:10.1177/0269881120959604

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmid, Y., Hysek, C. M., Simmler, L. D., Crockett, M. J., Quednow, B. B., and Liechti, M. E. (2014). Differential Effects of MDMA and Methylphenidate on Social Cognition. J. Psychopharmacol. 28, 847–856. doi:10.1177/0269881114542454

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmid, Y., and Liechti, M. E. (2018). Long-lasting Subjective Effects of LSD in normal Subjects. Psychopharmacology 235 (2), 535–545. doi:10.1007/s00213-017-4733-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strajhar, P., Schmid, Y., Liakoni, E., Dolder, P. C., Rentsch, K. M., Kratschmar, D. V., et al. (2016). Acute Effects of Lysergic Acid Diethylamide on Circulating Steroid Levels in Healthy Subjects. J. Neuroendocrinol 28, 12374. doi:10.1111/jne.12374

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson, M. R., Callaghan, P. D., Hunt, G. E., Cornish, J. L., and McGregor, I. S. (2007). A Role for Oxytocin and 5-HT1A Receptors in the Prosocial Effects of 3,4 Methylenedioxymethamphetamine ("ecstasy"). Neuroscience 146 (2), 509–514. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2007.02.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: LSD, empathy, oxytocin, emotion processing, ketanserin, 5-HT2A receptor, psychedelic

Citation: Holze F, Avedisian I, Varghese N, Eckert A and Liechti ME (2021) Role of the 5-HT2A Receptor in Acute Effects of LSD on Empathy and Circulating Oxytocin. Front. Pharmacol. 12:711255. doi: 10.3389/fphar.2021.711255

Received: 18 May 2021; Accepted: 30 June 2021;
Published: 13 July 2021.

Edited by:

Katrin H. Preller, University of Zurich, Switzerland

Reviewed by:

Charles D. Nichols, Louisiana State University, United States
Danilo De Gregorio, McGill University, Canada

Copyright © 2021 Holze, Avedisian, Varghese, Eckert and Liechti. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Matthias E. Liechti, bWF0dGhpYXMubGllY2h0aUB1c2IuY2g=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.