Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Pharmacol., 12 June 2018
Sec. Neuropharmacology

Prevalence Estimates for Pharmacological Neuroenhancement in Austrian University Students: Its Relation to Health-Related Risk Attitude and the Framing Effect of Caffeine Tablets

\r\nPavel Dietz,*Pavel Dietz1,2*Benedikt IberlBenedikt Iberl3Emanuel SchuettEmanuel Schuett3Mireille van Poppel,Mireille van Poppel1,2Rolf UlrichRolf Ulrich3Matteo Christian Sattler,Matteo Christian Sattler1,2
  • 1Research Group of Physical Activity and Public Health, Institute of Sports Science, University of Graz, Graz, Austria
  • 2Working Group Social and Health Sciences of Sport, Institute for Sports and Sports Science, Karlsruhe Institute of Technology, Karlsruhe, Germany
  • 3Research Group of Cognition and Perception, Institute of Psychology, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany

Background: Pharmacological neuroenhancement (PN) is defined as the use of illicit or prescription drugs by healthy individuals for cognitive-enhancing purposes. The present study aimed (i) to investigate whether including caffeine tablets in the definition of PN within a questionnaire increases the PN prevalence estimate (framing effect), (ii) to investigate whether the health-related risk attitude is increased in students who use PN.

Materials and methods: Two versions of a paper-and-pencil questionnaire (first version included caffeine tablets in the definition of PN, the second excluded caffeine tablets) were distributed among university students at the University of Graz, Austria. The unrelated question model (UQM) was used to estimate the 12-month PN prevalence and the German version of the 30-item Domain-Specific Risk-Taking (DOSPERT) scale to assess the health-related risk attitude. Moreover, large-sample z-tests (α = 0.05) were performed for comparing the PN prevalence estimates of two groups.

Results: Two thousand four hundred and eighty-nine questionnaires were distributed and 2,284 (91.8%) questionnaires were included in analysis. The overall PN prevalence estimate for all students was 11.9%. One-tailed large-sample z-tests revealed that the PN estimate for students with higher health-related risk attitude was significantly higher compared to students with lower health-related risk attitude (15.6 vs. 8.5%; z = 2.65, p = 0.004). Furthermore, when caffeine tablets were included into the example of PN, the prevalence estimate of PN was significantly higher compared to the version without caffeine tablets (14.9 vs. 9.0%; z = 2.20, p = 0.014).

Discussion: This study revealed that the PN prevalence estimate increases when caffeine tablets are included in the definition of PN. Therefore, future studies investigating the prevalence of, and predictors for, PN should be performed and interpreted with respect to potential framing effects. This study further revealed that the PN prevalence estimate is increased in students with a higher health-related risk attitude compared to students with a lower one. Therefore, future education and prevention programs addressing PN in the collective of students should not only inform about potential side effects of its use but also address the limited effects on cognition and potential alternatives of PN.

Introduction

The term “pharmacological neuroenhancement (PN)” – also called “pharmacological cognitive enhancement” – is generally defined as the use of illicit (e.g., illicit stimulants, cocaine, ecstasy) or prescription drugs (e.g., stimulants such as methylphenidate and amphetamines, modafinil as well as antidementives and antidepressants) by healthy individuals for cognitive-enhancing purposes such as improving vigilance, attention, concentration, or mood (Franke and Lieb, 2010; Dietz et al., 2013b; Sattler, 2016). In the last decade, a considerable number of epidemiological studies using different survey methods investigated the prevalence of PN in various populations. For example, based on a survey by a large German health insurance company, a lifetime prevalence of 5% for PN was reported for the general working population in Germany (DAK, 2009), and lifetime prevalences of about 20% for scientists (Maher, 2008), surgeons (Franke et al., 2013), and economists (Dietz et al., 2016b). In addition, a systematic review by Wilens et al. (2008) estimated a lifetime prevalence for the use of stimulants including prescription and illicit drugs of 5–9% in graduate and high school students, and 5–35% among college students in the United States (Wilens et al., 2008). Similar results were obtained for student collectives in Western Europe. For example, lifetime prevalences for the use of prescription drugs of 8% and 9% were reported for pupils in Germany (Wolff and Brand, 2013) and Switzerland (Liakoni et al., 2015), respectively. Moreover, for university students the lifetime prevalence was about 5% for the use of prescription drugs for cognitive-enhancing purposes among German students (Sattler and Wiegel, 2013), 7.6% among Swiss students (Maier et al., 2013), around 2% among Dutch students (Schelle et al., 2015), 16% for the use of drugs among Italian students (Castaldi et al., 2012), and about 4% for the use of methylphenidate, 6% for modafinil, and 2% for Adderall for cognitive-enhancing purposes among students in Ireland/United Kingdom (Singh et al., 2014). Using an indirect survey technique, Dietz et al. (2013a) estimated a 12-month prevalence of 20% for PN for German university students including prescription drugs, illicit drugs, and caffeine tablets.

The heterogeneity of the prevalence estimates across studies can be attributed to various factors. For example, some studies employed direct and others indirect questioning methods (Lensvelt-Mulders et al., 2005b; Simon et al., 2006; Franke et al., 2013) when assessing such sensitive issues (Sieber and Stanley, 1988; Lee and Renzetti, 1990). In addition, some studies assessed the prevalence for a single substance (e.g., methylphenidate only) while other studies for a whole group of substances. Finally, studies also differed with respect to the definition and description of PN (Sattler, 2016; Schleim and Quednow, 2017). The latter issue can produce framing effects that biases people’s responses (Rothman et al., 1993; Stocké, 2002; Plous, 2007). In order to omit such bias effects like social desirability, Dietz et al. (2013a,b) conducted two surveys to estimate PN prevalence in university students and triathletes using an indirect survey technique. In these latter studies, the terms “illicit and prescription drugs” were explicitly defined in the survey questionnaire as “…substances which can only be prescribed by a doctor, are available in a pharmacy, or can be bought on the black market (e.g., caffeine tablets, stimulants, cocaine, methylphenidate, modafinil, beta-blockers) and are used to enhance your cognitive performance.” Although caffeine tablets are not illicit and hence a prescription is not needed to receive them, the authors included caffeine tablets in their example of PN because in Germany (in contrast to the United States), caffeine tablets can only be bought in pharmacies and not in supermarkets or drug-stores. In addition, the consumption of caffeine tablets differs markedly from the consumption of a cup of coffee, because coffee may also be consumed for appetite whereas the only reason for consuming caffeine tablets would be to reduce fatigue (Franke et al., 2011b, 2015). Other authors have argued that prevalence estimates for PN of 20% in university students and 15.1% in triathletes are particularly a consequence of this inclusion of caffeine tablets in the definition of PN (Maier et al., 2013; Liakoni et al., 2015; Maier and Schaub, 2015; Schleim and Quednow, 2017), even though other studies revealed quite comparable results without including caffeine tablets (Maher, 2008; Franke et al., 2013; Dietz et al., 2016b). Nonetheless, although postulated by scientists to be needed (Schleim and Quednow, 2017), no systematic research has been conducted whether the inclusion of caffeine tablets in the definition of PN increases PN prevalence estimates. Given that 11% of students (Franke et al., 2011b) and 13% of surgeons (Franke et al., 2015) reported to have used caffeine tablets for cognitive-enhancing purposes once in their life, we hypothesized that including caffeine tablets in the definition of PN would increase PN prevalence estimates compared to excluding caffeine tablets in the definition.

Furthermore, PN consumption is, depending on dosage, associated with diverse adverse acute and chronic effects on physical and mental health, has been assumed to lead to addiction and produces a gateway to other drugs (Kumar, 2008; Dietz et al., 2013b; Wolff and Brand, 2013; LaBotz and Griesemer, 2016). For example, stimulant use is associated with the risk of cardiovascular events, hypertonia, tachycardia, and even sudden cardiac death (Kumar, 2008). In addition, long-term use of methylphenidate has been associated with neuronal changes comparable to those of cocaine use (Steiner and van Waes, 2013; Noble et al., 2015). A qualitative study among 19 university students in Australia revealed that students were aware of potential side effects of PN (Partridge et al., 2013). Fortunately, knowing potential side effects of PN reduces the willingness to consume PN (Sattler et al., 2014). Consequently, it has been assumed that the prospect of expected side effects from drug use acts as a protective factor against PN (Sattler and Wiegel, 2013). However, knowing about health risks does not necessarily prevent everybody from using PN, and several studies indicated that increased risk-taking behavior is associated with drug use (Khan et al., 2012; Noble et al., 2015; Nicholls et al., 2017). Furthermore, a study performed by Maier et al. (2015) comparing personality traits/attitudes of users of PN and non-users indicated that risk-taking-related personality traits/attitudes such as novelty seeking, self-reported impulsivity as well as antisocial personality were increased in users of PN compared to controls (Maier et al., 2015). Therefore, we hypothesized that people who consume PN have an increased health-related risk attitude compared to people who do not consume PN.

The present study aimed to address the following: (i) to investigate whether including caffeine tablets in the definition of PN within a questionnaire increases the PN prevalence estimate, (ii) to investigate whether the health-related risk attitude is increased in students who use PN, by providing first evidence on the prevalence of PN in Austrian university students. Investigating these knowledge gaps is of public health concern in order to develop more individually tailored education and prevention concepts.

Materials and Methods

Survey Procedure

A paper-and-pencil survey was conducted among university students at the University of Graz, Austria. The study administration online platform of the university was used to identify all major classes from different disciplines during the summer term of 2017. Two weeks before the survey was distributed, all teachers/lecturers of the identified classes were informed about the study and the procedure by email. The email also requested consent to distribute the survey in their classes. When consent was obtained, a trained team of assistants visited the classes one-time and distributed the questionnaires (two versions, one per participant; for more details see the section on “questionnaire” below). In a short verbal introduction, the assistants stressed the anonymity of the study and told the students to fill in the questionnaire immediately and to drop it into black boxes that were set up in the lecture hall. They further emphasized that all questionnaires had to be returned regardless if they were completed or not in order enable an accurate calculation of the response rate. The whole procedure lasted 10–15 min. Ethical approval to conduct this study was obtained by the Ethics Committee of the University of Graz, Austria (GZ. 39/40/63 ex 2016/17).

Questionnaire

The anonymous paper-and-pencil questionnaire was two pages long and, after a short introduction explaining the content of the study and that participation would be anonymous and voluntary, was comprised of three parts. The first part addressed the 12-month prevalence of PN using the unrelated question model (UQM; Greenberg et al., 1969), a randomized response technique developed specifically to obtain more valid prevalence estimates when sensitive topics are studied by guaranteeing anonymity (Warner, 1965, 1971). The methodological background of the UQM as used for the present study has been explicitly described in several previous articles (Dietz et al., 2013a,b, 2016a; Franke et al., 2013, 2017; Schröter et al., 2016) and will therefore not be repeated in detail. In short, participants had to consider a certain birthday (of their mother). If this birthday was in the first third of the month (1st to 10th day), they had to answer a neutral question and if not, they had to answer the sensitive question regarding the prevalence of PN. Thus, the probability for receiving the neutral question was 120/365.25 and the probability for receiving the sensitive question was 245.25/365.25. The neutral question asked whether the birthday they considered is in the first half of the year (before 1st of July). Therefore, the probability for answering the neutral question with ‘yes’ (denoted as πn) was 181.25/365.25. The proportion of ‘yes’ responses with respect to the sensitive question (i.e., the prevalence estimate www.frontiersin.orgs) can then be derived from the proportion of total ‘yes’ responses in the sample (denoted as a). A specific arithmetic example is given in the publication of Franke et al. (2013).

The translated wording of the sensitive question regarding PN was “for the purpose of enhancing your cognitive performance during studying, have you used substances which are only available in a pharmacy, can be prescribed by a doctor or can be bought on the black marked during the last 12 month (examples).” We used two different questionnaire versions containing different versions of the example for substances. The first version included caffeine tablets in the example “(e.g., caffeine tablets, stimulants, cocaine, methylphenidate, beta-blockers, modafinil)” and the second did not “(e.g., stimulants, cocaine, methylphenidate, beta-blockers, modafinil).” This procedure enabled us to calculate two different PN prevalence estimates, one for including caffeine tablets and one for excluding caffeine tablets. The order for distributing the two questionnaire versions was determined by simple randomization (Altman and Bland, 1999; Schulz and Grimes, 2007) using Windows Excel (questionnaires with an uneven number included caffeine tablets, questionnaires with an even numbers did not). Within the second part of the questionnaire, we assessed the health-related risk attitude using the German version of the 30-item Domain-Specific Risk-Taking (DOSPERT) scale for adults (Blais and Weber, 2006). The DOSPERT scale contains 30 items in five distinct domains of risk taking (six items per domain): ethical, financial, health/safety, recreational, and social. The six items for health/safety were adapted to our questionnaire in order to obtain the domain specific score. On a seven-point Likert scale (1 = very unlikely, 2 = unlikely, 3 = somewhat unlikely, 4 = unsure, 5 = somewhat likely, 6 = likely, 7 = very likely) the following items for the health/safety domain had to be rated: how likely is it for you (i) drinking five or more glasses of alcohol at one evening, (ii) engaging in unprotected sex, (iii) driving a car without wearing a seat belt, (iv) riding a motorcycle without a helmet, (v) sunbathing without sunscreen, and (vi) walking home alone at night in an unsafe area of town. For each student, the average rating to these items was calculated. This score was used to classify the participants into groups of higher or lower health-related risk attitude and enabled us to calculate separate PN prevalence estimates for these two subgroups. Finally, the third part of the questionnaire contained four items concerning the participant’s characteristics: semester (continuous), field of study (nominal), age (continuous), and gender (male/female). The complete questionnaire is provided in the Supplementary Material to this article.

Statistics

Descriptive data are presented as mean ± SD values for continuous scaled variables and as numbers and percentages for non-continuous scaled variables using SPSS software, version 22. Prevalence estimates (www.frontiersin.orgs) for PN are presented as percentages with 95% confidence intervals (CI) and standard error (SE) using Matlab version R2015a. Moreover, R software version 3.2.3 was used to perform large-sample z-tests for comparing the prevalence estimates of PN between two groups (for example between participants with lower and higher health-related risk attitude). The central hypotheses of the study regarding the influence of caffeine tablets and health-related risk attitude on PN prevalence estimate were directed hypotheses and analyzed one-tailed (right-tailed, α = 0.05). Non-directed variables (gender, age, and semester) were analyzed two-tailed (α = 0.05). Therefore, the continuous variables ‘age’ and ‘health related risk attitude’ were dichotomized by median and in a second analysis the variable ‘semester’ manually by visual splitting.

Results

A total number of 2,489 questionnaires were distributed among students from the University of Graz and 2,464 (99%) were returned. One hundred and eighty students stated that they had already participated in the survey in a previous class/lecture and hence these students were excluded from data analysis resulting in a total number of 2,284 (91.8%) questionnaires for the analysis. Mean age was 22.4 years (SD = 5.2 years), mean semester was 3.7 (SD = 2.5), 1,343 (59.1%) participants were female, and 1,138 (49.8%) obtained the questionnaire version that included caffeine tablets in the example of PN. The 16 different fields of study stated by the respondents were summarized into five groups taking the local faculty affiliation of the different fields as well as previous classification schemes (Dietz et al., 2013a) into account. The mean health-related risk attitude assessed by the DOSPERT scale was 22.3 (SD = 5.9). The respondent’s characteristics are presented in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Characteristics of the respondents.

The overall PN prevalence estimate for all students was 11.9% (Table 2). One-tailed large-sample z-tests revealed that PN estimate for students with higher health-related risk attitude was significantly higher (15.6%) compared to the group of students with a lower (8.5%) health-related risk attitude (z = 2.65, p = 0.004). Furthermore, when caffeine tablets were included into the example of PN, the prevalence estimate of PN was significantly higher (14.9%) compared to the version without caffeine tablets (9.0%, z = 2.20, p = 0.014). Two-tailed large-sample z-tests revealed that prevalence estimates were not significantly different between female (10.2%) and male (14.9%) participants (z = 1.70, p = 0.088), participants aged younger than or equal to 21 years (12.3%) and older (11.7%) participants (z = 0.22, p = 0.826) as well as between students studying in the first (12.5%) or higher than first (11.5%) year (z = 0.37, p = 0.712). Figure 1 presents the prevalence estimates together with their 95% confidence intervals for the five different fields of study indicating that the prevalence estimate is the lowest in technical studies and informatics (5.4%) and the highest in medicine, pharmacy, and psychology (14.9%). The prevalence estimates for the remaining three groups were 11.5%, 11.5%, and 12.5%, respectively.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Estimated 12-month prevalence for pharmacological neuroenhancement.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. Estimated 12-month prevalence and 95% confidence intervals for pharmacological neuroenhancement separated for field of study. USW, Umweltsystemwissenschaften (environmental systems sciences).

Discussion

The present study addressed two questions. First, whether the PN prevalence estimate increases when caffeine tablets are included in the definition of PN. Second, whether the PN prevalence estimate increases with increasing health-related risk attitude. Therefore, a randomized-response survey was conducted among university students at the University of Graz. Regarding the first question, the present results support the assumption (Maier et al., 2013; Liakoni et al., 2015; Maier and Schaub, 2015; Schleim and Quednow, 2017) that the questionnaire version including caffeine tablets in the PN definition increased the PN prevalence estimate (14.9% without vs. 9.0% with caffeine tablets). This difference of about 6% is consistent with the last-year prevalence for the use of caffeine tablets for cognitive-enhancing purposes, which was also reported to be about 6% (Franke et al., 2015). The results also demonstrate the relevance of caffeine tablets from a public health point of view, since the consumption of high dosages of caffeine is discussed to be associated with comparable adverse health effects as described for prescription and illicit psychoactive drugs (Jackson et al., 2013; Temple et al., 2017). In addition, according to several studies, high concentrations of caffeine (of 80 mg/L) are considered lethal (Banerjee et al., 2014; Silva et al., 2014). However, from a methodological point of view we have to stress that participants who responded to version 2 of the questionnaire (the version without caffeine tablets) may still have considered caffeine tablets as PN. Consequently, we cannot directly assess the prevalence of caffeine tablets but only calculate the difference of prevalence estimation of PN between the two questionnaire versions (with and without caffeine tablets).

Regarding the health-related risk attitude, the present results provide evidence that the PN prevalence is higher in participants with a higher health-related risk attitude (15.6%) compared to participants with a lower health-related risk attitude (8.5%). Consequently, our study supports the results of a previous study performed among university students in Switzerland (Maier et al., 2015). In their study, risk-taking-related personality traits/attitudes such as novelty seeking, self-reported impulsivity as well as antisocial personality were increased in users of PN compared to controls. Likewise, previous studies reported comparable associations between risk attitude and the use of other substances such as alcohol, tobacco, illicit drugs, and doping substances (Khan et al., 2012; Noble et al., 2015; Nicholls et al., 2017). This association might be of particular interest for the student population, since the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention of the United States and others have shown that students engage in various health risk behaviors and ignore preventive safety habits, which may have long-term implications for their health (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 1997; Steptoe and Wardle, 2001; Steptoe et al., 2002; Von Ah et al., 2004; Kann et al., 2013). Therefore, we conclude that prevention and education programs addressing PN in the collective of students likely fail if they only inform about potential side effects of its use (approach of deterrence). In fact, such programs have to further share knowledge about the limited efficacy of PN in healthy individuals (Repantis et al., 2010; Ullrich et al., 2015) and that using these substances is not associated with better school marks (Franke et al., 2011a) nor with better academic performance (McCabe et al., 2005). In addition, students have to be informed about potential alternatives of PN which are discussed to have cognitive-enhancing effects, such as sports and exercise (Tomporowski, 2003; Lambourne and Tomporowski, 2010; Dietz, 2013), nutrition (Chung et al., 2012), sleep (Diekelmann and Born, 2010), or meditation (Lutz et al., 2008; Sedlmeier et al., 2012).

The overall 12-month PN prevalence estimate of 11.9% for the collective of university students in Graz, Austria is quite in the middle of the heterogeneous prevalences reported for university students from other countries in Europe (e.g., prevalence of 2% since the start of their university studies for Dutch (Schelle et al., 2015) vs. 12-month prevalence of 20% for German (Dietz et al., 2013a) university students. As stated above, the differences across studies may be attributed to various methodological factors. Additionally, the Dutch study did not include caffeine tablets whereas the German study did. Since the present overall prevalence estimate is a mix of both versions (with caffeine tablets and without) it is quite plausible that this PN prevalence estimate is quite in the middle of the Dutch and the German prevalence. Finally, although the present study was the first large survey assessing the prevalence of PN in Austrian University students using RRT, the present results are not representative for Austria because the survey was restricted to the University of Graz. Nonetheless, we were able to demonstrate that the used study design and survey techniques are highly appropriate to detect cross-sectional associations between potential predictor variables and PN.

The variables gender and semester did not significantly influence PN prevalence estimates. For gender, our results support the outcomes of other studies indicating that there is no difference in the PN usage between female and male students (Teter et al., 2006; Franke et al., 2011a; Mache et al., 2012). Yet some studies reported gender differences, with males having a higher risk for PN compared to females (DeSantis et al., 2008; Arria et al., 2011). In a recent meta-analysis by Benson et al. (2015), of 19 studies on gender differences in misuse of stimulant medication among students, 13 studies reported a higher prevalence for males compared to females, whereas six studies observed no difference. This heterogeneous result pattern may reflect differences in methodology, for example, by using cross-sectional and longitudinal study designs, different survey techniques as well as assessing prevalences for different time periods such as lifetime versus last-year prevalence (Benson et al., 2015). In contrast to the present study, a survey among university students in Germany reported that students of the first semester were of higher risk for PN compared to higher semester students. In the present survey, only 105 participants were first semester students. Therefore, we could not calculate a reliable PN prevalence estimate for the first semester students (Ulrich et al., 2012). To examine the influence of time spent at the university on the prevalence of PN, future studies should collect longitudinal data, which may enable researchers to assess changes in PN consumption over time.

One limitation of the present study might be that we used a self-designed question to assess the overall prevalence of PN. This has been done for the following reason. It is known from previous studies (e.g., doping in athletes) that non-professional people are not aware about which substances are illicit or need to be prescribed by a doctor. Therefore, we provided a simple explanation by what we mean with “for the purpose of enhancing your cognitive performance during studying, have you used substances which are only available in a pharmacy, can be prescribed by a doctor or can be bought on the black marked during the last 12 month (examples)” (Dietz et al., 2013a,b). One objection against this definition might be that it excludes substances that can only be bought in a pharmacy (e.g., high dosages of vitamins). Therefore, we provided concrete examples of substances in order to increase the comprehension of our question. It is likely that prevalence estimates are influenced by how such questions are posed. For example, the expression “prescription drugs for cognitive-enhancement” seems ambivalent for readers and hence may produce prevalence estimates that are too small compared to the true prevalence. Another limitation of the study might be that we were not able to identify specific substances for PN by using the present RRT design to investigate an overall prevalence estimate of PN. This gap should be closed by future RRT studies that are tailored to address specific substance prevalences.

Unrelated question model was used in the present study to assess the PN prevalence estimate. UQM is one technique among several RRTs [e.g., the forced response method, the item count technique, the crosswise method, the cheater detection model (CDM)]. In their meta-analysis of 38 randomized response validation studies, Lensvelt-Mulders et al. (2005a) have concluded that although RRT results are more valid compared to results of conventional survey techniques, there is still room for improvement (Lensvelt-Mulders et al., 2005a). Using the UQM participants get randomly assigned to answer one of two questions, the sensitive or a neutral one. However, a participant that has been assigned to answer the sensitive answer may avoid this question and respond to the neutral question instead. As shown by Ulrich et al. (2018, Supplementary Material, Section 4.3), this non-compliance does not meaningfully distort the estimate obtained under the standard UQM assumptions. Moreover, a previous study has shown that UQM and CDM, a model taking potential cheating of participants into account, delivered comparable prevalence estimates for a sensitive item (Schröter et al., 2016).

Conclusion

This study revealed that the PN prevalence estimate increases when caffeine tablets are included in the definition of PN. Therefore, future studies investigating the prevalence of, and predictors for, PN should be performed and interpreted with respect to potential framing effects. This study further revealed that the PN prevalence estimate is increased in students with a higher health-related risk attitude compared to students with a lower one. Therefore, future education and prevention programs addressing PN in the collective of students should not only inform about potential side effects of its use but also address the limited effects on cognition and potential alternatives of PN.

Author Contributions

PD, BI, ES, RU, and MS made substantial contributions to the conception and design of the study, the data acquisition, analysis, interpretation of data, and the preparation of the manuscript. MvP made substantial contributions to the interpretation of data and the preparation of the manuscript. All authors proofread and accepted the final version of the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The reviewer JD and handling Editor declared their shared affiliation.

Acknowledgments

Special thanks to the volunteers that distributed the questionnaires during classes and enabled us to collect a large number of questionnaires in short time: Stefan Florjancic, Philipp Klaindl, Christian Gerhard Petrovcic, and Peter Stueger. We also thank the lecturers allowing us to visit their classes and collect data for this survey. We acknowledge the help of Dr. Estelle Watson from the University of the Witwatersrand, Johannesburg, South Africa for improving the readability of the manuscript.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fphar.2018.00494/full#supplementary-material

References

Altman, D. G., and Bland, J. M. (1999). How to randomise. BMJ 319, 703–704. doi: 10.1136/bmj.319.7211.703

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arria, A. M., Garnier-Dykstra, L. M., Caldeira, K. M., Vincent, K. B., O’Grady, K. E., and Wish, E. D. (2011). Persistent nonmedical use of prescription stimulants among college students: possible association with ADHD symptoms. J. Atten. Disord. 15, 347–356. doi: 10.1177/1087054710367621

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Banerjee, P., Ali, Z., Levine, B., and Fowler, D. R. (2014). Fatal caffeine intoxication: a series of eight cases from 1999 to 2009. J. Forensic Sci. 59, 865–868. doi: 10.1111/1556-4029.12387

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benson, K., Flory, K., Humphreys, K. L., and Lee, S. S. (2015). Misuse of stimulant medication among college students: a comprehensive review and meta-analysis. Clin. Child Fam. Psychol. Rev. 18, 50–76. doi: 10.1007/s10567-014-0177-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blais, A.-R., and Weber, E. U. (2006). A Domain-Specific Risk-Taking (DOSPERT) scale for adult populations. Judgm. Decis. Mak. 1, 33–47.

Google Scholar

Castaldi, S., Gelatti, U., Orizio, G., Hartung, U., Moreno-Londono, A. M., Nobile, M., et al. (2012). Use of cognitive enhancement medication among northern Italian university students. J. Addict. Med. 6, 112–117. doi: 10.1097/ADM.0b013e3182479584

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (1997). Youth risk behavior surveillance: national college health risk behavior survey - United States. MMWR CDC Surveill. Summ. 46, 1–56.

Google Scholar

Chung, Y.-C., Park, C.-H., Kwon, H.-K., Park, Y.-M., Kim, Y. S., Doo, J.-K., et al. (2012). Improved cognitive performance following supplementation with a mixed-grain diet in high school students: a randomized controlled trial. Nutrition 28, 165–172. doi: 10.1016/j.nut.2011.05.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DAK (2009). Gesundheitsreport 2009 - Analysen der Arbeitsunfähigkeitsdaten. Schwerpunktthema Doping am Arbeitsplatz. Available at: http://www.dak.de/content/filesopen/Gesundheitsreport_2009.pdf

DeSantis, A. D., Webb, E. M., and Noar, S. M. (2008). Illicit use of prescription ADHD medications on a college campus: a multimethodological approach. J. Am. Coll. Health 57, 315–324. doi: 10.3200/JACH.57.3.315-324

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diekelmann, S., and Born, J. (2010). The memory function of sleep. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 114–126. doi: 10.1038/nrn2762

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dietz, P. (2013). “The influence of sports on cognitive task performance – a critical overview,” in Cognitive Enhancement – An Interdisciplinary Perspective, eds E. Hildt and A. G. Franke (Dordrecht: Springer), 67–72.

Google Scholar

Dietz, P., Dalaker, R., Letzel, S., Ulrich, R., and Simon, P. (2016a). Analgesics use in competitive triathletes: its relationship to doping and on predicting its usage. J. Sports Sci. 34, 1965–1969. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2016.1149214

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dietz, P., Soyka, M., and Franke, A. G. (2016b). Pharmacological neuroenhancement in the field of economics-poll results from an online survey. Front. Psychol. 7:520. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00520

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dietz, P., Striegel, H., Franke, A. G., Lieb, K., Simon, P., and Ulrich, R. (2013a). Randomized response estimates for the 12-month prevalence of cognitive-enhancing drug use in university students. Pharmacotherapy 33, 44–50. doi: 10.1002/phar.1166

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dietz, P., Ulrich, R., Dalaker, R., Striegel, H., Franke, A. G., Lieb, K., et al. (2013b). Associations between physical and cognitive doping - A cross-sectional study in 2.997 triathletes. PLoS One 8:e78702. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0078702

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Franke, A. G., Bagusat, C., Dietz, P., Hoffmann, I., Simon, P., Ulrich, R., et al. (2013). Use of illicit and prescription drugs for cognitive or mood enhancement among surgeons. BMC Med. 11:102. doi: 10.1186/1741-7015-11-102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Franke, A. G., Bagusat, C., McFarlane, C., Tassone-Steiger, T., Kneist, W., and Lieb, K. (2015). The use of caffeinated substances by surgeons for cognitive enhancement. Ann. Surg. 261, 1091–1095. doi: 10.1097/SLA.0000000000000830

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Franke, A. G., Bonertz, C., Christmann, M., Huss, M., Fellgiebel, A., Hildt, E., et al. (2011a). Non-medical use of prescription stimulants and illicit use of stimulants for cognitive enhancement in pupils and students in Germany. Pharmacopsychiatry 44, 60–66. doi: 10.1055/s-0030-1268417

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Franke, A. G., Christmann, M., Bonertz, C., Fellgiebel, A., Huss, M., and Lieb, K. (2011b). Use of coffee, caffeinated drinks and caffeine tablets for cognitive enhancement in pupils and students in Germany. Pharmacopsychiatry 44, 331–338. doi: 10.1055/s-0031-1286347

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Franke, A. G., Dietz, P., Ranft, K., Balló, H., Simon, P., and Lieb, K. (2017). The use of pharmacologic cognitive enhancers in competitive chess. Epidemiology 28, e57–e58. doi: 10.1097/EDE.0000000000000737

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Franke, A. G., and Lieb, K. (2010). [Pharmacological neuroenhancement and brain doping: chances and risks]. Bundesgesundheitsblatt Gesundheitsforschung Gesundheitsschutz 53, 853–859. doi: 10.1007/s00103-010-1105-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Greenberg, B. G., Abulela, A. L. A., Simmons, W. R., and Horvitz, D. G. (1969). Unrelated question randomized response model - Theoretical framework. J. Am. Stat. Assoc. 64, 520–536. doi: 10.1037/a0029314

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jackson, D. A. E., Cotter, B. V., Merchant, R. C., Babu, K. M., Baird, J. R., Nirenberg, T., et al. (2013). Behavioral and physiologic adverse effects in adolescent and young adult emergency department patients reporting use of energy drinks and caffeine. Clin. Toxicol. 51, 557–565. doi: 10.3109/15563650.2013.820311

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kann, L., Kinchen, S., Shaklin, S. L., Flint, K. H., Hawkins, J., Harris, W. A., et al. (2013). (63). Youth risk behavior surveillance - United States, 2013. MMWR Surveil. Summ. 61, 1–168.

Google Scholar

Khan, M. R., Berger, A. T., Wells, B. E., and Cleland, C. M. (2012). Longitudinal associations between adolescent alcohol use and adulthood sexual risk behavior and sexually transmitted infection in the United States: assessment of differences by race. Am. J. Public Health 102, 867–876. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2011.300373

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kumar, R. (2008). Approved and investigational uses of modafinil: an evidence-based review. Drugs 68, 1803–1839. doi: 10.2165/00003495-200868130-00003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

LaBotz, M., and Griesemer, B. A. (2016). Use of Performance-enhancing substances. Pediatrics 138:e20161300. doi: 10.1542/peds.2016-1300

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lambourne, K., and Tomporowski, P. (2010). The effect of exercise-induced arousal on cognitive task performance: a meta-regression analysis. Brain Res. 1341, 12–24. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.03.091

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, R. M., and Renzetti, C. M. (1990). The problems of researching sensitive topics: an overview and introduction. Am. Behav. Sci. 33, 510–528. doi: 10.1177/0002764290033005002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lensvelt-Mulders, G. J. L. M., Hox, J. J., and van der Heijden, P. G. M. (2005a). How to improve the efficiency of randomised response designs. Qual. Quant. 39, 253–265. doi: 10.1007/s11135-004-0432-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lensvelt-Mulders, G. J. L. M., Hox, J. J., van der Heijden, P. G. M., and Maas, C. J. M. (2005b). Meta-analysis of randomized response research thirty-five years of validation. Sociol. Methods Res. 33, 319–348. doi: 10.1177/0049124104268664

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liakoni, E., Schaub, M. P., Maier, L. J., Glauser, G.-V., and Liechti, M. E. (2015). The use of prescription drugs, recreational drugs, and ”Soft Enhancers” for cognitive enhancement among Swiss secondary school students. PLoS One 10:e0141289. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0141289

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lutz, A., Slagter, H. A., Dunne, J. D., and Davidson, R. J. (2008). Attention regulation and monitoring in meditation. Trends Cogn. Sci. 12, 163–169. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.01.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mache, S., Eickenhorst, P., Vitzthum, K., Klapp, B. F., and Groneberg, D. A. (2012). Cognitive-enhancing substance use at German universities: frequency, reasons and gender differences. Wien. Med. Wochenschr. 162, 262–271. doi: 10.1007/s10354-012-0115-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maher, B. (2008). Poll results: look who’s doping. Nature 452, 674–675. doi: 10.1038/452674a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maier, L. J., Liechti, M. E., Herzig, F., and Schaub, M. P. (2013). To dope or not to dope: neuroenhancement with prescription drugs and drugs of abuse among Swiss university students. PLoS One 8:e77967. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0077967

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maier, L. J., and Schaub, M. P. (2015). The use of prescription drugs and drugs of abuse for neuroenhancement in Europe. Eur. Psychol. 20, 155–166. doi: 10.1027/1016-9040/a000228

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maier, L. J., Wunderli, M. D., Vonmoos, M., Römmelt, A. T., Baumgartner, M. R., Seifritz, E., et al. (2015). Pharmacological cognitive enhancement in healthy individuals: A compensation for cognitive deficits or a question of personality? PLoS One 10:e0129805. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0129805

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCabe, S. E., Knight, J. R., Teter, C. J., and Wechsler, H. (2005). Non-medical use of prescription stimulants among US college students: prevalence and correlates from a national survey. Addiction 100, 96–106. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2005.00944.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nicholls, A. R., Cope, E., Bailey, R., Koenen, K., Dumon, D., Theodorou, N. C., et al. (2017). Children’s first experience of taking anabolic-androgenic steroids can occur before their 10th birthday: a systematic review identifying 9 factors that predicted doping among young people. Front. Psychol. 8:1015. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Noble, N., Paul, C., Turon, H., and Oldmeadow, C. (2015). Which modifiable health risk behaviours are related? A systematic review of the clustering of smoking, nutrition, alcohol and physical activity (’SNAP’) health risk factors. Prev. Med. 81, 16–41. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2015.07.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Partridge, B., Bell, S., Lucke, J., and Hall, W. (2013). Australian university students’ attitudes towards the use of prescription stimulants as cognitive enhancers: perceived patterns of use, efficacy and safety. Drug Alcohol Rev. 32, 295–302. doi: 10.1111/dar.12005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Plous, S. (2007). The Psychology of Judgment and Decision Making. New York, NY: Mcgraw-Hill Higher Education.

Google Scholar

Repantis, D., Schlattmann, P., Laisney, O., and Heuser, I. (2010). Modafinil and methylphenidate for neuroenhancement in healthy individuals: a systematic review. Pharmacol. Res. 62, 187–206. doi: 10.1016/j.phrs.2010.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rothman, A. J., Salovey, P., Antone, C., Keough, K., and Martin, C. D. (1993). The influence of message framing on intentions to perform health behaviors. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 29, 408–433. doi: 10.1006/jesp.1993.1019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sattler, S. (2016). “Cognitive enhancement in Germany,” in Cognitive enhancement: Ethical and Policy Implications in International Perspectives, eds F. Jotterand and V. Dubljeviæ (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 159–180. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199396818.003.0011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sattler, S., Mehlkop, G., Graeff, P., and Sauer, C. (2014). Evaluating the drivers of and obstacles to the willingness to use cognitive enhancement drugs: the influence of drug characteristics, social environment, and personal characteristics. Subst. Abuse Treat. Prev. Policy 9:8. doi: 10.1186/1747-597X-9-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sattler, S., and Wiegel, C. (2013). Cognitive test anxiety and cognitive enhancement: the influence of students’ worries on their use of performance-enhancing drugs. Subst. Use Misuse 48, 220–232. doi: 10.3109/10826084.2012.751426

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schelle, K. J., Olthof, B. M. J., Reintjes, W., Bundt, C., Gusman-Vermeer, J., and van Mil, A. C. C. M. (2015). A survey of substance use for cognitive enhancement by university students in the Netherlands. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 9:10. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2015.00010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schleim, S., and Quednow, B. B. (2017). “Debunking the ethical neuroenhancement debate,” in Rethinking Cognitive Enhancement, eds R. H. J. T. Meulen, A. D. Mohamed, and W. Hall (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 164–176. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198727392.003.0010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schröter, H., Studzinski, B., Dietz, P., Ulrich, R., Striegel, H., and Simon, P. (2016). A Comparison of the cheater detection and the unrelated question models: a randomized response survey on physical and cognitive doping in recreational triathletes. PLoS One 11:e0155765. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0155765

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schulz, K. F., and Grimes, D. A. (2007). Reihe Epidemiologi 6. Z. Ärztl. Fortbild. Qual. Gesundh. 101, 419–426. doi: 10.1016/j.zgesun.2007.05.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sedlmeier, P., Eberth, J., Schwarz, M., Zimmermann, D., Haarig, F., Jaeger, S., et al. (2012). The psychological effects of meditation: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 138, 1139–1171. doi: 10.1037/a0028168

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sieber, J. E., and Stanley, B. (1988). Ethical and professional dimensions of socially sensitive research. Am. Psychol. 43, 49–55. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.43.1.49

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Silva, A. C., de Oliveira Ribeiro, N. P., de Mello Schier, A. R., Pereira, V. M., Vilarim, M. M., Pessoa, T. M., et al. (2014). Caffeine and suicide: a systematic review. CNS Neurol. Disord. Drug Targets 13, 937–944. doi: 10.2174/1871527313666140612123656

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simon, P., Striegel, H., Aust, F., Dietz, K., and Ulrich, R. (2006). Doping in fitness sports: estimated number of unreported cases and individual probability of doping. Addiction 101, 1640–1644. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2006.01568.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Singh, I., Bard, I., and Jackson, J. (2014). Robust resilience and substantial interest: a survey of pharmacological cognitive enhancement among university students in the UK and Ireland. PLoS One 9:e105969. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0105969

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steiner, H., and van Waes, V. (2013). Addiction-related gene regulation: risks of exposure to cognitive enhancers vs. other psychostimulants. Prog. Neurobiol. 100, 60–80. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2012.10.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steptoe, A., and Wardle, J. (2001). Health behaviour, risk awareness and emotional well-being in students from Eastern Europe and Western Europe. Soc. Sci. Med. 53, 1621–1630. doi: 10.1016/S0277-9536(00)00446-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steptoe, A., Wardle, J., Cui, W., Bellisle, F., Zotti, A.-M., Baranyai, R., et al. (2002). Trends in smoking, diet, physical exercise, and attitudes toward health in European university students from 13 countries, 1990-2000. Prev. Med. 35, 97–104. doi: 10.1006/pmed.2002.1048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stocké, V. (2002). Framing und Rationalität: Die Bedeutung der Informationsdarstellung für das Entscheidungsverhalten. Munich: R. Oldenbourg Verlag. doi: 10.1524/9783486833263

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Temple, J. L., Bernard, C., Lipshultz, S. E., Czachor, J. D., Westphal, J. A., and Mestre, M. A. (2017). The safety of ingested caffeine: a comprehensive review. Front. Psychiatry 8:80. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00080

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Teter, C. J., McCabe, S. E., LaGrange, K., Cranford, J. A., and Boyd, C. J. (2006). Illicit use of specific prescription stimulants among college students: prevalence, motives, and routes of administration. Pharmacotherapy 26, 1501–1510. doi: 10.1592/phco.26.10.1501

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomporowski, P. D. (2003). Effects of acute bouts of exercise on cognition. Acta Psychol. 112, 297–324. doi: 10.1016/S0001-6918(02)00134-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ullrich, S., de Vries, Y. C., Kühn, S., Repantis, D., Dresler, M., and Ohla, K. (2015). Feeling smart: effects of caffeine and glucose on cognition, mood and self-judgment. Physiol. Behav. 151, 629–637. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.08.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ulrich, R., Pope, H. G. Jr., Cléret, L., Petróczi, A., Nepusz, T., Schaffer, J., et al. (2018). Doping in two elite athletics competitions assessed by randomized-response surveys. Sports Med. 48, 211–219. doi: 10.1007/s40279-017-0765-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ulrich, R., Schröter, H., Striegel, H., and Simon, P. (2012). Asking sensitive questions: a statistical power analysis of randomized response models. Psychol. Methods 17, 623–641. doi: 10.1037/a0029314

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Von Ah, D., Ebert, S., Ngamvitroj, A., Park, N., and Kang, D.-H. (2004). Predictors of health behaviours in college students. J. Adv. Nurs. 48, 463–474. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2648.2004.03229.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Warner, S. L. (1965). Randomized response: a survey technique for eliminating evasive answer bias. J. Am. Stat. Assoc. 60, 63–66. doi: 10.1080/01621459.1965.10480775

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Warner, S. L. (1971). The linear randomized response model. J. Am. Stat. Assoc. 66, 884–888. doi: 10.2307/2284247

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilens, T. E., Adler, L. A., Adams, J., Sgambati, S., Rotrosen, J., Sawtelle, R., et al. (2008). Misuse and diversion of stimulants prescribed for ADHD: a systematic review of the literature. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 47, 21–31. doi: 10.1097/chi.0b013e31815a56f1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolff, W., and Brand, R. (2013). Subjective stressors in school and their relation to neuroenhancement: a behavioral perspective on students’ everyday life “doping”. Subst. Abuse Treat. Prev. Policy 8:23. doi: 10.1186/1747-597X-8-23

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: message frame, cognitive bias, cognitive enhancing drugs, risk behavior, substance abuse detection, statistical distributions, epidemiologic methods, randomized response technique

Citation: Dietz P, Iberl B, Schuett E, van Poppel M, Ulrich R and Sattler MC (2018) Prevalence Estimates for Pharmacological Neuroenhancement in Austrian University Students: Its Relation to Health-Related Risk Attitude and the Framing Effect of Caffeine Tablets. Front. Pharmacol. 9:494. doi: 10.3389/fphar.2018.00494

Received: 15 December 2017; Accepted: 25 April 2018;
Published: 12 June 2018.

Edited by:

Francisco Lopez-Munoz, Universidad Camilo José Cela, Spain

Reviewed by:

Boris B. Quednow, Universität Zürich, Switzerland
Carina Rodrigues Boeck, Centro Universitário Franciscano, Brazil
Juan Del Coso, Universidad Camilo José Cela, Spain

Copyright © 2018 Dietz, Iberl, Schuett, van Poppel, Ulrich and Sattler. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Pavel Dietz, cGF2ZWwuZGlldHpAdW5pLWdyYXouYXQ=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.