Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Pharmacol., 27 April 2018
Sec. Neuropharmacology
This article is part of the Research Topic Psychedelic Drug Research in the 21st Century View all 27 articles

Ibogaine Acute Administration in Rats Promotes Wakefulness, Long-Lasting REM Sleep Suppression, and a Distinctive Motor Profile

\r\nJoaquín GonzlezJoaquín González1Jos P. PrietoJosé P. Prieto2Paola RodríguezPaola Rodríguez3Matías CavelliMatías Cavelli1Luciana BenedettoLuciana Benedetto1Alejandra MondinoAlejandra Mondino1Mariana PazosMariana Pazos3Gustavo SeoaneGustavo Seoane3Ignacio Carrera*Ignacio Carrera3*Cecilia Scorza*Cecilia Scorza2*Pablo TorteroloPablo Torterolo1
  • 1Laboratorio de Neurobiología del Sueño, Departamento de Fisiología, Facultad de Medicina, Universidad de la República, Montevideo, Uruguay
  • 2Departamento de Neurofarmacología Experimental, Instituto de Investigaciones Biológicas Clemente Estable, Montevideo, Uruguay
  • 3Laboratorio de Síntesis Orgánica, Departamento de Química Orgánica, Facultad de Química, Universidad de la República, Montevideo, Uruguay

Ibogaine is a potent psychedelic alkaloid that has been the focus of intense research because of its intriguing anti-addictive properties. According to anecdotic reports, ibogaine has been originally classified as an oneirogenic psychedelic; i.e., induces a dream-like cognitive activity while awake. However, the effects of ibogaine administration on wakefulness (W) and sleep have not been thoroughly assessed. The main aim of our study was to characterize the acute effects of ibogaine administration on W and sleep. For this purpose, polysomnographic recordings on chronically prepared rats were performed in the light phase during 6 h. Animals were treated with ibogaine (20 and 40 mg/kg) or vehicle, immediately before the beginning of the recordings. Furthermore, in order to evaluate associated motor behaviors during the W period, a different group of animals was tested for 2 h after ibogaine treatment on an open field with video-tracking software. Compared to control, animals treated with ibogaine showed an increase in time spent in W. This effect was accompanied by a decrease in slow wave sleep (SWS) and rapid-eye movements (REM) sleep time. REM sleep latency was significantly increased in animals treated with the higher ibogaine dose. While the effects on W and SWS were observed during the first 2 h of recordings, the decrement in REM sleep time was observed throughout the recording time. Accordingly, ibogaine treatment with the lower dose promoted an increase on locomotion, while tremor and flat body posture were observed only with the higher dose in a time-dependent manner. In contrast, head shake response, a behavior which has been associated in rats with the 5HT2A receptor activation by hallucinogens, was not modified. We conclude that ibogaine promotes a waking state that is accompanied by a robust and long-lasting REM sleep suppression. In addition, it produces a dose-dependent unusual motor profile along with other serotonin-related behaviors. Since ibogaine is metabolized to produce noribogaine, further experiments are needed to elucidate if the metabolite and/or the parent drug produced these effects.

Introduction

Ibogaine is a natural occurring indole alkaloid found in the root bark of the shrub Tabernanthe iboga, originally from Congo and Gabon, as well as in other plants of the Apocynaceae family (Lavaud and Massiot, 2017). It has become widely known in the western world because of its claimed properties to reduce addiction to drugs of abuse and craving (Alper et al., 1999). These effects have been confirmed in pre-clinical studies in rodents, where ibogaine decreases self-administration of a variety of drugs such as opioids (Lotsof, 1995), cocaine (Cappendijk and Dzoljic, 1993), alcohol (Rezvani et al., 1995), and nicotine (Glick et al., 1998). In humans, the reduction of addiction and craving by ibogaine has been highlighted in many anecdotal reports and observational studies (Schenberg et al., 2014; Brown and Alper, 2017).

Ibogaine has been classified as an oneirogenic psychedelic drug for its ability to induce vivid dream-like episodes while awake with eyes closed, without loss of contact with the environment (Naranjo, 1973). This dream-like activity does not produce the typical interferences in thinking, identity distortions, and space–time alteration produced by the traditional psychedelics drugs (also known as hallucinogens) such as lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD), mescaline, and dimethyltryptamine (DMT), which are pharmacologically classified as 5-HT2A receptor agonists (Naranjo, 1973; Glennon, 1990; Lotsof, 1995; Nichols, 2016). However, the effect of ibogaine on wakefulness (W) and sleep remains unclear, existing only a few early studies regarding this issue. An early report in cats described that the administration of ibogaine (2–10 mg/kg doses) produced an activation of the electroencephalogram (EEG) that resembles the effect of the electrical stimulation of the activating reticular system (Schneider and Sigg, 1957). This effect was reduced in the classical “decerebrate” animal model, suggesting that the reticular formation plays a key role in the ibogaine effects. Moreover, they demonstrated that atropine, a muscarinic cholinergic antagonist, blocked the EEG-activating effect induced by ibogaine. Because of this result, the authors suggested that ibogaine effect depends on the activation of the cholinergic system. Da Costa et al. (1980), Da Costa-Rochette et al. (1981) analyzed the effects of others iboga alkaloids on chronically implanted cats. They found that tabernanthine tartrate and tabernanthine p-chlorophenoxyacetate provoked an increase in W and a reduction of both slow wave sleep (SWS) as well as rapid-eye movements (REM) sleep. However, the opposite effect was found when other tabernanthine derivatives (claimed as methoxy-16 ibogaine tartrate and methoxy-16 tabernanthine tartrate) were administered (Da Costa et al., 1980; Da Costa-Rochette et al., 1981).

When considering the behavioral responses induced by ibogaine in animal models, different effects have been reported depending on the dose, time points assayed, and length of the recordings. In one of the early reports in cats, it has been described that ibogaine administration (2–10 mg/kg) immediately produced an unusual excitatory effect that evolved into reactions of rage and fear (Schneider and Sigg, 1957). A more recent study in rats showed that ibogaine (1 and 10 mg/kg, i.v.) promoted a dose-dependent increase in locomotor activity during 30 min after administration (Baumann et al., 2001a). For higher doses [10–40 mg/kg, intraperitoneal (i.p.)], ibogaine produced deleterious effects in the vestibular function and a dose-dependent reduction in the detection of sensory stimuli in rats (Kesner, 1995). In addition, rats treated with 30 and 40 mg/kg were very inactive and appeared to be in a state of “suspension” (Kesner, 1995). These facts resemble subjective reports in humans, where ibogaine (4–5 mg/kg) promotes the desire to lie down because of a loss of equilibrium while trying to walk and directs the attention inwards (Naranjo, 2016). Previous studies also demonstrated that during the first 3–4 h after administration in animals, ibogaine (30–40 mg/kg or higher doses) produced serotonin syndrome-like behaviors such as tremor, piloerection, flat body posture, and forepaw tapping (Schneider and Sigg, 1957; Glick et al., 1994; Pearl et al., 1997; Baumann et al., 2001a; Haberzettl et al., 2013). It is well known that hallucinogens induce paroxysmal rotational movement of the head in rodents, that is mediated by 5-HT2A receptor activation (Halberstadt et al., 2011; Nichols, 2016). This behavior is called head twitch response in mice and head shake response (HSR) in rats (Corne and Pickering, 1967; Yamamoto and Ueki, 1975; Bedard and Pycock, 1977; Halberstadt et al., 2011). To the best of our knowledge, no previous studies explored if ibogaine promotes this behavior in rodents.

The main aim of the present study was to characterize ibogaine effects on sleep and W using rat as a model system. For this purpose, we performed polysomnographic recordings in chronically prepared rats and studied the acute effects of ibogaine, at doses that have been previously employed in drug self-administration studies (20 and 40 mg/kg). In addition, in order to analyze the animal behavior during the waking state, we characterized the motor effects induced by the same doses of ibogaine in naive animals, using an open-field (OF) assay.

Materials and Methods

Ibogaine

Total iboga alkaloid extract from the root bark of T. iboga was obtained from IbogaWorld and purified as follows. The material was suspended in aqueous 10% NaOH solution, which was extracted with ethyl acetate (4 × 200 ml). The combined organic layers were dried with Na2SO4 and evaporated in vacuo. The residue was purified by column chromatography (SiO2, CH2Cl2:MeOH 9:1 + 0.1% NH4OH). The obtained free base was further crystallized from ethanol. Ibogaine HCl was prepared dissolving the free base in dried acetone under Argon atmosphere and the equivalent amount of HCl (aq, 36%) was added. Ibogaine hydrochloride was filtered, washed with cold acetone, dried under vacuum, and characterized by 1H- and 13C-NMR (see Supplementary Materials for details). Purity was determined as 96.4% by GC–MS (see Supplementary Materials for details). Dissolution of ibogaine-HCl to prepare the samples for i.p. injection was carried out using warm saline that was previously degassed by nitrogen bubbling.

Experimental Animals

Wistar adult rats were maintained on a 12-h light/dark cycle (lights on at 07.00 h) and housed four–six per cage before behavioral testing. Food and water were freely available. Twenty-six animals (270–300 g) were used for all performed studies: eight animals were used for sleep recordings and 18 rats were used for the evaluation of the motor activity. The animals were determined to be in good health by veterinarians of the institution. All experimental procedures were conducted in agreement with the National Animal Care Law (No. 18611) and with the “Guide to the care and use of laboratory animals” (8th edition, National Academy Press, Washington DC, 2010). Furthermore, the Institutional Animal Care Committee approved the experimental procedures. Adequate measures were taken to minimize pain, discomfort, or stress of the animals, and all efforts were made to use the minimal number of animals necessary to obtain reliable scientific data.

Surgical Procedures

Eight animals selected for sleep experiments were chronically implanted with electrodes to monitor the states of sleep and W. We employed similar surgical procedures as in our previous studies (Benedetto et al., 2013; Cavelli et al., 2015, 2017b). Anesthesia was induced with a mixture of ketamine–xylazine (90 mg/kg; 5 mg/kg i.p., respectively). The rat was positioned in a stereotaxic frame and the skull was exposed. To record the EEG, stainless steel screw electrodes were placed on the skull above frontal, parietal, occipital cortices (bilateral), the right olfactory bulb, and cerebellum (reference electrode).

To record the electromyogram (EMG), two electrodes were inserted into the neck muscle. The electrodes were soldered into a 12-pin socket and fixed onto the skull with acrylic cement. At the end of the surgical procedures, an analgesic (Ketoprofen, 1 mg/kg, s.c.) was administered. After the animals had recovered from the preceding surgical procedures, they were adapted to the recording chamber for 1 week.

Experimental Sessions

Sleep Recordings

Animals were housed individually in transparent cages (40 × 30 × 20 cm) containing wood shaving material in a temperature-controlled (21–24°C) room, with water and food ad libitum. Experimental sessions were conducted during the light period, between 10 AM and 4 PM in a sound-attenuated chamber with Faraday shield. The recordings were performed through a rotating connector, to allow the rats to move freely within the recording box.

Polysomnographic data were acquired and stored in a computer for further analysis using Spike 2 software (CED, Cambridge, United Kingdom). The states of sleep and W were determined in 10 s epochs. W was defined as low voltage fast waves in frontal cortex, a mixed theta rhythm (4–7 Hz) in occipital cortex, and relatively high EMG activity. Light sleep (LS) as high voltage slow cortical waves interrupted by low voltage fast EEG activity. SWS was defined as continuous high amplitude slow waves and sleep spindles in frontal, parietal, and occipital cortices associated with a reduced EMG amplitude; while REM sleep as low voltage fast frontal waves, a regular theta rhythm in the occipital cortex, and a silent EMG except for occasional twitches. Total time spent in W, LS, SWS, and REM sleep, as well as the duration and the number of episodes over a 6 h recording period was analyzed. Sleep latencies were also evaluated. Besides, the time spent in each state was analyzed separately in blocks of 2 h (0–2, 2–4, and 4–6 h) (Monti et al., 2015).

In order to study the effect of ibogaine on sleep and W, at the beginning of the recordings each rat received ibogaine 20 mg/kg (I20), 40 mg/kg (I40), or vehicle (saline) i.p., in different days in a counterbalance order; the wash-out period between doses was 3 days. These doses have been extensively used in preclinical addiction studies (Glick et al., 1991, 1992; Cappendijk and Dzoljic, 1993).

Motor Behavior

Eighteen naive (not operated) rats were used in these experiments. Animals were brought to the experimental room in their home cages, identified, and weighed prior to the behavioral test. An OF apparatus consisting of a square area (45 cm wide × 45 cm long × 40 cm high) with transparent plastic walls indirectly illuminated (35 luxes) to avoid reflection and shadows were employed. The OF was placed in a quiet experimental room with controlled temperature (22 ± 2°C). As rats were not habituated to the OF before drug or vehicle administration, novelty-induced motor activity was automatically recorded by a camera connected to a computer equipped with the Ethovision XT 12.0 software (Noldus, Netherlands) located above the OF. Using this video tracking software, we specifically measured the total distance traveled in meters (m) during 120 min (5 min bin), starting immediately after the drug or vehicle administration. Animals were randomly assigned to different experimental groups, where each animal received I20, I40, or saline (n = 6 per group), and were used only once. Specific behaviors were assessed by a trained investigator every 30 min during 2 h after ibogaine administration. Each evaluation session lasted 5 min. The number of rearings was taken as an index of the vertical exploratory behavior to evaluate the animal habituation to the environment. Serotonin syndrome-like continuous behaviors such as tremor, flat body posture, piloerection, hind limb abduction, and Straub tail (Haberzettl et al., 2013) were scored using a graded scale: 0, absent; 1, equivocal; 2, present; and 3, intense (Spanos and Yamamoto, 1989; Reyes-Parada et al., 1996; Baumann et al., 2001b). In the case of forepaw treading (an intermittent behavior), an additive score of 1 was given every time the animal displayed this conduct. However, to allow a comparison with the continuous behaviors, a score of 5 (five times the animal displayed a FPT behavior) was considered as intense (3, in the abovementioned scale). Finally, the number of HSR defined as short and firm movement of the head in any direction was also recorded (Haberzettl et al., 2014).

During all experiments, the OF was cleaned with alcohol 30% before placing the following rat. All experiments were done between 9 AM and 3 PM.

Data Analysis

Sleep Recordings

All values are presented as mean ± SEM. The experimental design for the sleep analysis was a within-subject design, where statistical significance of the differences among groups (ibogaine 0, 20, and 40 mg/kg) was evaluated utilizing one-way repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) and Bonferroni as a post hoc test (Monti et al., 2015). When sphericity criteria were not accomplished (tested by Mauchly’s test), the Greenhouse–Geisser correction was applied. Statistical significance was set at p < 0.05.

Motor Behavior

Depending on the comparison performed, data from motor activity were analyzed by two-way (treatment, time, and interaction between factors) ANOVA for repeated measures followed by Newman–Keuls multiple comparison post hoc test; or by one-way (treatment) ANOVA for independent measures followed by Newman–Keuls multiple comparison test. In all cases, statistical significance was set at p < 0.05.

Results

Ibogaine’s Effect on Wakefulness and Sleep

Figure 1 shows a typical hypnogram and spectrogram of a representative animal following saline, I20, and I40 administration. Compared to control, I20 [F(1.1,8.3) = 10.7, p < 0.01] and I40 [F(1.1,8.3) = 10.7, p < 0.05] increased the time spent in W (Figure 1 and Table 1). This effect was accompanied by a decrease in SWS time, I20 [F(2,14) = 14.7, p < 0.01], I40 [F(2,14) = 14.7, p = 0.01]. In addition, the total amount of REM sleep was diminished in animals treated with I20 [F(2,14) = 19.3, p < 0.01] and I40 [F(2,14) = 19.3, p < 0.005]. No differences on the total amount of LS were observed.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. Hypnograms and spectrograms (0.1–20 Hz) from the parietal cortical recordings of a representative animal are shown after saline, ibogaine 20 and 40 mg/kg. Arrows in the hypnogram indicate SWS and REM sleep latencies. During wakefulness (W) and REM sleep, theta activity (4–9 Hz) in the spectrograms can be readily observed. During SWS sleep, delta activity (0.5–4 Hz) is more prominent and there are intermittent episodes of sigma activity (9–15 Hz), which correspond to the presence of sleep spindles. Color calibration of the spectrogram is not exhibited (larger power is exhibited in red). Ibogaine increased W and reduced REM sleep time. LS, light sleep; REM, rapid eyes movements sleep; SWS, slow wave sleep.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Effects of intraperitoneal injections of ibogaine on sleep and waking parameters during total recording time.

When considering the duration and number of episodes (Table 1), we found that compared to control, there was a significant increase in the duration of the individual W episodes after I20 [F(1.1,7.9) = 6.4, p < 0.05], and a decrease in the duration of SWS episodes for I40 [F(2,14) = 9.5, p < 0.005]. Regarding REM sleep, both doses of ibogaine reduced the total number of episodes [F(1.2,8.5) = 10.5; p < 0.05 for I20 and I40] without affecting the episodes’ duration. Finally, REM sleep latency increased following I40 administration [F(2,14) = 9.6, p < 0.05] (Table 1), while the latency to LS and SWS was not affected.

Ibogaine effects were also analyzed in 2-h blocks (Figure 2). Compared to control, the time of W was significantly increased for both doses in the first 2 h of the recording [F(2,14) = 9.1, p < 0.05 for I20 and I40]. This increment was accompanied by a decrease in SWS [F(2,14) = 10.2, p < 0.05 for I20, and p < 0.01 for I40] and REM sleep [F(2,14) = 17.8, p < 0.05 for I20, and p = 0.001 for I40] without any appreciable change in LS. Within the second 2 h, a decrease in the REM sleep was observed following I40 [F(2,14) = 8.7, p < 0.05], while in the last 2 h, both I20 and I40 decreased REM sleep time [F(1.1,7.8) = 8.3, p < 0.005 for I20, and p < 0.05 for I40].

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. Effects of ibogaine administration on sleep and W. Graphic chart shows the mean ± SEM time spent in (A) W, (B) light sleep, (C) slow wave sleep, and (D) REM sleep after administration of saline and ibogaine (20 and 40 mg/kg), analyzed in 2-h blocks. Group mean differences were determined by one-way ANOVA repeated measures and Bonferroni as post hoc test; significant differences (p < 0.05) between groups are indicated by asterisks. Eight animals were employed in this analysis.

Ibogaine’s Effect on Motor Behavior

In order to obtain further insights about the animal behavior during the ibogaine-induced waking state, a detailed study of motor behaviors was carried out (Figures 3, 4). Compared to the control group, horizontal locomotion was slightly altered by both doses of ibogaine (Figure 3A). Two-way ANOVA revealed a significant effect of the treatment [F(2,15) = 8.7, p < 0.01], time [F(23,345) = 12.2, p < 0.001], and treatment × time interaction [F(46,345) = 1.43, p < 0.05]. Newman–Keuls test showed that compared to control and I20, I40 elicited a significant decrease (p < 0.001) in the distance that the animals moved during the first 5 min; the animals injected with I20 or saline exhibited a very similar level of locomotor activity (Figure 3A). When the total locomotor activity was analyzed (Figure 3A inset), one-way ANOVA revealed a significant difference between groups [F(2,15) = 8.7, p < 0.01]. Post hoc analysis showed that I20 elicited a significant increment in the locomotor activity compared to control (p < 0.01) and I40 (p < 0.05) groups. The abovementioned decrease in novelty-induced locomotor activity observed in the I40-treated animals at the beginning of the recording was not evidenced when the total activity period was considered (Figure 3A inset).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 3. Effect of ibogaine administration (20 and 40 mg/kg) on locomotor activity (A) and exploratory activity (B; number of rearings) recorded in the OF test during 120 min after ibogaine administration. The inset graphs represent the total locomotor activity and the total number of rearings (0–120 min). Data are expressed as mean ± SEM. The data were analyzed by the two-way ANOVA of repeated measured followed by Newman–Keuls test, and one-way ANOVA of independent measures followed by Newman–Keuls test (insets). , respective to saline group; +, respective to ibogaine. ∗∗∗, +++p < 0.001; ∗∗p < 0.01; +p < 0.05. N = 6 per group.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 4. Effects of ibogaine on behavior. Specific behaviors such as tremor, flat body posture forepaw treading, piloerection (A), and head shake response (HSR) (B) were shown during the first 5 min after ibogaine administration. In A, behaviors were scored using the graded scale: 0, absent; 1, equivocal; 2, present; and 3, intense while in B, the number of HSR was scored. Data are expressed as mean ± SEM and analyzed by one-way ANOVA of independent measures followed by Newman–Keuls test. , respective to saline group and +, respective to ibogaine. ∗∗∗, +++p < 0.001; ∗∗, ++p < 0.01. N = 6 per group.

Rearing is a component of the natural exploratory behavior directly related to the environment novelty (Fink and Smith, 1980; Geyer et al., 1986; Bardo et al., 1990). In Figure 3B, we can observe that naive rats were more active mainly in the first period of the recording session; in fact, the control group seems to be rapidly habituated to the OF apparatus and becoming virtually inactive at the end of the 60-min recording time. Statistical two-way ANOVA analysis revealed a significant effect of the treatment [F(2,15) = 4.0, p < 0.05], time [F(4,60) = 14.6, p < 0.001], and the treatment × time interaction [F(8,60) = 2.4, p < 0.05]. Newman–Keuls test showed that animal injected with I40 (p < 0.001), but not I20, significantly reduced the ability to induce rearing behavior during the first 5 min, suggesting an alteration in the environment habituation response (Figure 3B). When the total rearing activity was considered (Figure 3B inset), one-way ANOVA did not reveal a significant difference between groups [F(2,15) = 3.4, p = 0.06], although this behavior was still not correctly restored.

Figures 4A,B show the effect of the ibogaine on the induction of serotonin syndrome-like behaviors and HSR, respectively. In order to simplify, only the first 5 min session is shown. One-way ANOVA analysis revealed significant differences between experimental groups for tremor [F(2,15) = 35.9, p < 0.0001] and flat body posture [F(2,15) = 9.3, p < 0.01] but not for forepaw treading [F(2,15) = 1.2, p = 0.3)] or piloerection [F(2,15) = 1.2, p = 0.1]. Straub tail and hind limb abduction were not seen (data not shown). Post hoc analysis showed that tremor and flat body posture were significantly present immediately after I40 injection (p < 0.001 and p < 0.01, respectively, Figure 4A), while these behaviors were completely absent at the end of the 60 min session (data not shown). Animal treated with I20 did not significantly elicit these behavioral signs. Since only these two behaviors were significantly elicited by I40, an overall serotonin syndrome (Haberzettl et al., 2013) cannot be referred as induced by ibogaine in these conditions. Additionally, one-way ANOVA for the HSR did not reveal significant difference [F(2,15) = 1.1, p = 0.36] neither for I20 nor for I40 during the total 120 min session (Figure 4B).

Discussion

In the present study, we showed that administration of 20 and 40 mg/kg of ibogaine produced a robust effect on sleep and W, promoting a waking state that is accompanied by a robust and long-lasting REM sleep suppressive effect. The higher dose (I40) showed, in addition, a time-dependent disability to explore a novel environment, as well as disabling behaviors like tremor and flat body posture. It is well-established that ibogaine is rapidly metabolized to its long-lived metabolite noribogaine, so both substances should be taken into account to explain the findings of this study. According to previous reports in rats using i.p. administration (Baumann et al., 2001a), ibogaine concentration in blood rapidly decreases in the first hour while noribogaine concentration is maximum at 2.4 h and lasts up to 24 h. Since it is known that ibogaine induces tremors while noribogaine does not (Baumann et al., 2001a), the fact that we only found tremors during the first hour of recordings for I40 seems to be in accordance with this pharmacokinetic profile.

Ibogaine administration promoted W; this effect was accompanied by a decrease in the total amount of SWS and REM sleep. While the effects on W and SWS were observed only in first 2 h, the effects on REM sleep lasted through the entire recording. These results may resemble observational studies in humans where ibogaine administration in multiple doses produced difficulties in sleep onset and maintenance immediately after each intake (Wilkins et al., 2017). Ibogaine’s W-promoting effect, in addition, is in accordance with a previous report in cats (Schneider and Sigg, 1957), suggesting that ibogaine induces W in rats, cats, and probably in humans.

Interestingly, a similar impact upon sleep architecture has been reported for traditional psychedelics (5HT2A agonists), such as LSD (also a 5HT2C receptor agonist) and 2,5-dimethoxy-4-iodoamphetamine (DOI, also a 5HT2C agonist) (Depoortere and Loew, 1971; Monti and Jantos, 2006). Chronic LSD treatment in rats promotes an increase in W and a reduction in SWS and REM sleep. Similar to ibogaine, the W-promoting effect was more prominent during the first hour, while it disappeared after 4 h. The authors also found qualitative changes in REM sleep, reporting an increase in phasic episodes as well as an increase in rapid eye movements. Subcutaneous administration of DOI also increases W and decrease SWS and REM sleep. Together these results suggest that ibogaine and traditional psychedelics induce similar effects on sleep architecture. Regarding selective antagonists of 5HT2A and 5HT2C receptors, it was found that their administration promotes SWS; while surprisingly decreases REM sleep time (Monti and Jantos, 2006; Monti et al., 2018). These unexpected results have not been explained yet.

Since dreams are the cognitive counterpart of REM sleep (although dreams under NREM sleep may also occur), the long-lasting REM sleep suppression induced by ibogaine administration seems at first sight to be against its previously mentioned oneirogenic properties. However, this may not be the case, since a quantitative analysis of the EEG during W could provide evidence of an altered W pattern. For example, ibogaine-induced W may present a decrease in gamma band coherence, which is a well-known electrophysiological signature of REM sleep (Castro et al., 2013; Cavelli et al., 2015, 2017a). In other words, this pharmacologically-induced W state could have subtle electrophysiological traits of REM sleep that could explain the oneirogenic cognitive effect of the drug. With the aim of answering this question, a quantitative analysis of the EEG (power, coherence, co-modulation, entropy) is under process and will be reported in due course. In addition, this type of analysis would be helpful to compare the quality of W produced by ibogaine to traditional psychedelics.

Which are the underlying mechanisms of the W-promoting and REM sleep suppressive effects? Experiments in synaptosomes have shown that ibogaine inhibits serotonin re-uptake, which increases the synaptic levels of this neurotransmitter (Wells et al., 1999). In addition, Wei et al. (1998) showed that ibogaine elicited a large increase in serotonin levels (up to 25-fold in the Nucleus Accumbens, NAC and 10-fold in the striatum, STR) while noribogaine produced a moderate increase (up to eightfold in NAC and fivefold in STR) (Wei et al., 1998). In contrast, Baumann et al. (2001b) showed that noribogaine was more potent in increasing serotonin levels at the NAC than ibogaine, which correlates with the ability of both compounds to inhibit SERT (IC50 of 3.85 and 0.18 μM for ibogaine and noribogaine, respectively). These authors suggested that ibogaine and noribogaine are serotonin-reuptake inhibitors with a mechanism of action similar to fluoxetine. Serotonin increases W and suppress the generation of REM sleep (Oniani and Akhvlediani, 1988; Monti and Jantos, 2005). Hence, the ability of ibogaine and noribogaine to increase synaptic serotonin concentration could account for the increment in W time, as well as the long-lasting REM suppressive effect. The fact that most antidepressant drugs share this REM sleep suppressing effect (Palagini et al., 2012), insinuate that ibogaine might also have antidepressant properties as suggested by human reports (Mash et al., 2000).

Schneider and Sigg (1957) also proposed that the activation of the cholinergic pathways should be involved in the effects produced by ibogaine. In this regard, mesopontine and basal forebrain cholinergic neurons are involved in the generation and maintenance of W (Torterolo et al., 2016). Hence, it is likely that by modulation of these neurochemical systems ibogaine can promote W and suppress sleep. Nevertheless, because of its complex pharmacology, the interactions between other neurotransmitters, as well as the still unknown effects of ibogaine on REM sleep promoting neurons such as the melanin-concentrating hormone (MCH) containing neurons, should be taken into account when interpreting these results (Torterolo et al., 2011; Monti et al., 2013).

Another possibility is that the increase in W could be caused by some unspecific effect, such as irritation or pain. In this regard, we did not observe any behavior suggesting this kind of effect. In fact, it is interesting to consider that there is evidence of ibogaine as an anti-nociceptive agent (Olney, 1995, 1997). Moreover, both ibogaine and noribogaine enhance morphine anti-nociception (Bagal et al., 1996), and to our knowledge, there are no observational studies which reported pain or inflammation after ibogaine administration in humans.

When considering the results seen in each 2-h block, we hypothesized that the different effects seen along the entire recording could be attributed not just to ibogaine itself, but also to its principal metabolite, noribogaine. As mentioned before, according to previous reports using i.p. administration in rats, ibogaine concentration in blood rapidly decreases in the first hour (with a T-max of approximately 0.1 h), while noribogaine is detectable in blood up to 24 h after ibogaine administration (with a T-max of approximately 2.4 h) (Baumann et al., 2001b). Hence, the increased W found in the first 2-h block could be correlated to the peak concentration of ibogaine in addition to increasing amounts of noribogaine, while the extended REM suppression seen through the entire recording could be attributed to the long-lasting noribogaine. Further experiments are needed to confirm this hypothesis.

Regarding motor behavior, a higher total locomotor activity was found after I20 administration suggesting a more vigilant animal response. This stimulant effect is in accordance with previous findings that showed that 1 and 10 mg/kg i.v. doses promoted a dose-dependent increase in locomotor activity in rats (Baumann et al., 2001b). In contrast, I40 administration did not show a substantial increase in locomotor activity, producing serotonin syndrome-like behaviors such as tremor and flat body posture mainly during the first part of the recording session. It is likely that the appearance of these behaviors would explain, at least in part, the abnormal environment animal habituation response to the OF (Figure 3A), and reduced the ability to induce rearing behavior that was found after the administration of this dose (Figure 3B). Taken together, these results clearly indicate that I40 induces a kind of W that is different than the one produced by the lower dose. These results may resemble subjective experience of natives from Congo and Gabon who used low doses of T. iboga root bark as a powerful stimulant to combat fatigue and tiredness, while larger doses were chosen to produce visions in ritual settings (Schneider and Sigg, 1957; Pope, 1969).

The ibogaine induced tremor and flat body posture (Figure 4A), suggest a putative interaction with serotonin transmission. However, Baumann et al. (2001b) have postulated that a serotonergic mechanism may not be involved in the locomotor effects of ibogaine, since according to their results noribogaine (which is not tremorgenic in rats) is more potent in increasing serotonin levels than ibogaine. In this manner, they speculate that sigma or NMDA receptors might also explain these behaviors (since noribogaine has less affinity for this sites) (Baumann et al., 2001b). Regarding the induction of HSR, no changes were found for both ibogaine treatments (Figure 4B). As mentioned before, this behavior is exacerbated by the systemic administration of 5HT2A receptor agonists (like classical hallucinogens). This constitutes a putative behavioral difference for animals treated with hallucinogens (LSD, DOI, etc.) versus ibogaine, which is considered a non-traditional psychedelic. Pharmacological data also favor this difference, while hallucinogens interact with the 5-HT2A receptor in the nanomolar range, ibogaine affinity for this receptor is in the micromolar range (Ki 4.8–92.5 μM depending the study) (Repke et al., 1994; Sweetnam et al., 1995; Helsley et al., 1998; Glick et al., 1999) or negligible (Deecher et al., 1992; Staley et al., 1996). At last, this difference is also supported by the differences between the subjective experiences in humans where ibogaine does not produce the typical interferences in thinking, identity distortions, and space–time alteration produced by the traditional psychedelics drugs (Naranjo, 1973). In summary, the sleep and motor effects induced by ibogaine, and its differences with classical hallucinogens, can only be explained by the interaction of ibogaine with several neurochemical systems.

Conclusion and Future Perspectives

In this study, we observed that intraperitoneal administration of ibogaine in rats produced an increase in W, a decrease in SWS along with a robust suppression of REM sleep. Although these effects on sleep and W were observed for both doses of ibogaine (20 and 40 mg/kg), locomotor studies indicated differences in the behavioral outcomes for both treatments. While the I20 dose had a stimulant profile, the I40 dose generated an abnormal environment habituation where significant tremor and flat body posture were detected. Hence, given that ibogaine is considered an oneirogenic psychedelic, the next step would be to analyze the specific electroencephalographic characteristics of the pharmacologically induced W, such as the power spectrum and spectral coherence between different cortical areas. This kind of analysis could provide further insights of the cognitive activity induced by this drug.

Ethics Statement

This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of the National Animal Care Law (No. 18611) and with the “Guide to the care and use of laboratory animals” (8th edition, National Academy Press, Washington DC, 2010). Furthermore, the Institutional Animal Care Committee (Facultad de Medicina – Universidad de la República; Instituto de Investigaciones Biológicas Clemente Estable) approved the experimental protocols. Adequate measures were taken to minimize pain, discomfort, or stress of the animals, and all efforts were made to use the minimal number of animals necessary to obtain reliable scientific data.

Author Contributions

IC, GS, PT, and CS provided the financial support. IC, PT, and CS performed the experimental design. JG, JP, PR, MC, LB, AM, and MP performed the experimental procedures. JG, MC, JP, IC, PT, CS, and GS were involved in the analysis of the data. JG, MC, JP, GS, IC, PT, and CS were involved in the discussion and interpretation of the data. JG, IC, PT, and CS wrote the manuscript. All the authors participated in the critical revision of the manuscript, added important intellectual content, and approved the definitive version.

Funding

This work was funded by Agencia Nacional de Investigación e Innovación (ANII, Montevideo – Uruguay) Project Fondo María Viñas 103488, Comisión Sectorial de Investigación Científica (UdelaR) – Project Grupos I+D 981, and Programa de Desarrollo de las Ciencia Básicas (PEDECIBA).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank Agencia Nacional de Investigación e Innovación (ANII) and Comisión Sectorial de Investigación Científica (CSIC-UdelaR) for financial support. We also thank Eleuterio Umpiérrez and Bruno González for their help in the GC–MS analysis. We are grateful to Dr. Charles Nichols for the discussions regarding the psychedelic nature of ibogaine and Dr. Ines Carrera for english proofreading.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fphar.2018.00374/full#supplementary-material

References

Alper, K. R., Lotsof, H. S., Frenken, G. M., Luciano, D. J., and Bastiaans, J. (1999). Treatment of acute opioid withdrawal with ibogaine. Am. J. Addict. 8, 234–242. doi: 10.1080/105504999305848

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bagal, A. A., Hough, L. B., Nalwalk, J. W., and Glick, S. D. (1996). Modulation of morphine-induced antinociception by ibogaine and noribogaine. Brain Res. 741, 258–262. doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(96)00938-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bardo, M. T., Lacy, M., and Mattingly, B. A. (1990). Effects of apomorphine on novelty-induced place preference behavior in rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 37, 89–93. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(90)90046-K

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baumann, M. H., Pablo, J., Ali, S. F., Rothman, R. B., and Mash, D. C. (2001a). Comparative neuropharmacology of ibogaine and its O-desmethyl metabolite, noribogaine. Alkaloids Chem. Biol. 56, 79–113. doi: 10.1016/S0099-9598(01)56009-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baumann, M. H., Rothman, R. B., Pablo, J. P., and Mash, D. C. (2001b). In vivo neurobiological effects of ibogaine and its O-desmethyl metabolite, 12-hydroxyibogamine (noribogaine), in rats. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 297, 531–539.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Bedard, P., and Pycock, C. J. (1977). &quotWet-dog&quot; shake behaviour in the rat: a possible quantitative model of central 5-hydroxytryptamine activity. Neuropharmacology 16, 663–670. doi: 10.1016/0028-3908(77)90117-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benedetto, L., Rodriguez-Servetti, Z., Lagos, P., D’Almeida, V., Monti, J. M., and Torterolo, P. (2013). Microinjection of melanin concentrating hormone into the lateral preoptic area promotes non-REM sleep in the rat. Peptides 39, 11–15. doi: 10.1016/j.peptides.2012.10.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, T. K., and Alper, K. (2017). Treatment of opioid use disorder with ibogaine: detoxification and drug use outcomes. Am. J. Drug Alcohol Abuse 44, 24–36. doi: 10.1080/00952990.2017.1320802

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cappendijk, S. L. T., and Dzoljic, M. R. (1993). Inhibitory effects of ibogaine on cocaine self-administration in rats. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 241, 261–265. doi: 10.1016/0014-2999(93)90212-Z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Castro, S., Falconi, A., Chase, M. H., and Torterolo, P. (2013). Coherent neocortical 40-Hz oscillations are not present during REM sleep. Eur. J. Neurosci. 37, 1330–1339. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12143

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cavelli, M., Castro, S., Schwarzkopf, N., Chase, M. H., Falconi, A., and Torterolo, P. (2015). Coherent neocortical gamma oscillations decrease during REM sleep in the rat. Behav. Brain Res. 281, 318–325. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.12.050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cavelli, M., Castro-Zaballa, S., Mondino, A., Gonzalez, J., Falconi, A., and Torterolo, P. (2017a). Translational brain rhythmicity absence of EEG gamma coherence in a local activated cortical state: a conserved trait of REM sleep. Transl. Brain Rhythm. 2, 1–13. doi: 10.15761/TBR.1000115

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cavelli, M., Rojas-Líbano, D., Schwarzkopf, N., Castro-Zaballa, S., Gonzalez, J., Mondino, A., et al. (2017b). Power and coherence of cortical high-frequency oscillations during wakefulness and sleep. Eur. J. Neurosci. doi: 10.1111/ejn.13718 [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Corne, S. J., and Pickering, R. W. (1967). A possible correlation between drug-induced hallucinations in man and a behavioural response in mice. Psychopharmacologia 11, 65–78. doi: 10.1007/BF00401509

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Da Costa, L., Sulklaper, I., and Naquet, R. (1980). Modifications de l’equilibre veille-sommeil du chat par la tabernanthine et oueloues-uns de ses derives. Rev. Electroencéphalogr. Neurophysiol. Clin. 1, 105–112. doi: 10.1016/S0370-4475(80)80027-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Da Costa-Rochette, L., Sulklaper, I., Tomei, C., and Naquet, R. (1981). Le retour du sommeil chez le chat pretraite par le p-chlorophenoxyacetate de tabernanthine (SAD 103). Rev. Electroencéphalogr. Neurophysiol. Clin. 11, 147–154. doi: 10.1016/S0370-4475(81)80046-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deecher, D. C., Teitler, M., Soderlund, D. M., Bornmann, W. G., Kuehne, M. E., and Glick, S. D. (1992). Mechanisms of action of ibogaine and harmaline congeners based on radioligand binding studies. Brain Res. 571, 242–247. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(92)90661-R

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Depoortere, H., and Loew, D. M. (1971). Alterations in sleep-wakefulness cycle in rats following treatment with (+)-lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD-25). Br. J. Pharmacol. 41, 402–403.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Fink, J. S., and Smith, G. P. (1980). Mesolimbic and mesocortical dopaminergic neurons are necessary for normal exploratory behavior in rats. Neurosci. Lett. 17, 61–65. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(80)90062-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geyer, M., Russo, P., and Masten, V. (1986). Multivariate assessment of locomotor behavior: pharmacological and behavioral analyses. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 25, 277–288. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(86)90266-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glennon, R. A. (1990). Do classical hallucinogens act as 5-HT2 agonists or antagonists? Neuropsychopharmacology 3, 509–517.

Google Scholar

Glick, S. D., Kuehne, M. E., Raucci, J., Wilson, T. E., Larson, D., Keller, R. W., et al. (1994). Effects of iboga alkaloids on morphine and cocaine self-administration in rats: relationship to tremorigenic effects and to effects on dopamine release in nucleus accumbens and striatum. Brain Res. 657, 14–22. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(94)90948-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glick, S. D., Maisonneuve, I. M., Hough, L. B., Kuehne, M. E., and Bandarage, U. K. (1999). ( ± )-18-Methoxycoronaridine: a novel iboga alkaloid congener having potential anti-addictive efficacy. CNS Drug Rev. 5, 27–42. doi: 10.1111/j.1527-3458.1999.tb00084.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glick, S. D., Maisonneuve, I. M., Visker, K. E., Fritz, K. A., Bandarage, U. K., and Kuehne, M. E. (1998). 18-Methoxycoronardine attenuates nicotine-induced dopamine release and nicotine preferences in rats. Psychopharmacology 139, 274–280. doi: 10.1007/s002130050716

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glick, S. D., Rossman, K., Rao, N. C., Maisonneuve, I. M., and Carlson, J. N. (1992). Effects of ibogaine on acute signs of morphine withdrawal in rats: independence from tremor. Neuropharmacology 31, 497–500. doi: 10.1016/0028-3908(92)90089-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glick, S. D., Rossman, K., Steindorf, S., Maisonneuve, I. M., and Carlson, J. N. (1991). Effects and aftereffects of ibogaine on morphine self-administration in rats. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 195, 341–345. doi: 10.1016/0014-2999(91)90474-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haberzettl, R., Bert, B., Fink, H., and Fox, M. A. (2013). Animal models of the serotonin syndrome: a systematic review. Behav. Brain Res. 256, 328–345. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2013.08.045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haberzettl, R., Fink, H., and Bert, B. (2014). Role of 5-HT1A- and 5-HT2A receptors for the murine model of the serotonin syndrome. J. Pharmacol. Toxicol. Methods 70, 129–133. doi: 10.1016/j.vascn.2014.07.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Halberstadt, A. L., Koedood, L., Powell, S. B., and Geyer, M. A. (2011). Differential contributions of serotonin receptors to the behavioral effects of indoleamine hallucinogens in mice. J. Psychopharmacol. 25, 1548–1561. doi: 10.1177/0269881110388326

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Helsley, S., Fiorella, D., Rabin, R. A., and Winter, J. C. (1998). Behavioral and biochemical evidence for a nonessential 5-HT2A component of the ibogaine-induced discriminative stimulus. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 59, 419–425. doi: 10.1016/S0091-3057(97)00451-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kesner, R. (1995). Effects of ibogaine on sensory-motor function, activity, and spatial learning in rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 51, 103–109. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(94)00367-R

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lavaud, C., and Massiot, G. (2017). “The Iboga Alkaloids. Prog. Chem. Org. Nat. Prod., 105, 89–136. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-49712-9_2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lotsof, H. S. (1995). ibogaine in the treatment of chemical dependence disorders: clinical perspectives. Bull. Multidiscip. Assoc. Psychedelic Stud. 5, 16–20.

Google Scholar

Mash, D. C., Kovera, C. A., Pablo, J., Tyndale, R. F., Ervin, F. D., Williams, I. C., et al. (2000). Ibogaine: complex pharmacokinetics, concerns for safety, and preliminary efficacy measures. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 914, 394–401. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2000.tb05213.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Monti, J. M., and Jantos, H. (2005). A study of the brain structures involved in the acute effects of fluoxetine on REM sleep in the rat. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 8, 75–86. doi: 10.1017/S1461145704004511

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Monti, J. M., and Jantos, H. (2006). Effects of the serotonin 5-HT2A/2C receptor agonist DOI and of the selective 5-HT2A or 5-HT2C receptor antagonists EMD 281014 and SB-243213, respectively, on sleep and waking in the rat. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 553, 163–170. doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2006.09.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Monti, J. M., Lagos, P., Jantos, H., and Torterolo, P. (2015). Increased REM sleep after intra-locus coeruleus nucleus microinjection of melanin-concentrating hormone (MCH) in the rat. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 56, 185–188. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2014.09.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Monti, J. M., Pandi, S. R., Spence, D. W., Torterolo, P., Monti, J. M., Pandi Perumal, S. R., et al. (2018). “The involvement of 5-HT2A receptor in the regulation of sleep and wakefulness, and the potential therapeutic use of selective 5-HT2A receptor antagonists and inverse agonists for the treatment of an insomnia disorder,” 5-HT2A Receptors in the Central Nervous System, eds B. P. Guiard, and G. Di Giovanni (Berlin: Springer). doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-70474-6_13

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Monti, J. M., Torterolo, P., and Lagos, P. (2013). Melanin-concentrating hormone control of sleep–wake behavior. Sleep Med. Rev. 17, 293–298. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2012.10.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Naranjo, C. (1973). The Healing Journey. New York, NY: Random House.

Google Scholar

Naranjo, C. (2016). Exploraciones Psicodélicas: Para la Transformacioìn Colectiva de la Conciencia. Barcelona: Ediciones la Llave.

Google Scholar

Nichols, D. E. (2016). Psychedelics. Pharmacol. Rev. 68, 264–355. doi: 10.1124/pr.115.011478

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Olney, J. W. (1995). Use of ibogaine in reducing excitotoxic brain damage.

Google Scholar

Olney, J. W. (1997). Use of ibogaine for treating neuropathic pain.

Google Scholar

Oniani, T. N., and Akhvlediani, G. R. (1988). Influence of some monoamine oxidase inhibitors on the sleep-wakefulness cycle of the cat. Neurosci. Behav. Physiol. 18, 301–306. doi: 10.1007/BF01185522

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Palagini, L., Baglioni, C., Ciapparelli, A., Gemignani, A., and Riemann, D. (2012). REM sleep dysregulation in depression: state of the art. Sleep Med. Rev. 17, 377–390 doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2012.11.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pearl, S. M., Hough, L. B., Boyd, D. L., and Glick, S. D. (1997). Sex differences in ibogaine antagonism of morphine-induced locomotor activity and in ibogaine brain levels and metabolism. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 57, 809–815. doi: 10.1016/S0091-3057(96)00383-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pope, H. G. (1969). Tabernanthe iboga: an African narcotic plant of social importance. Econ. Bot. 23, 174–184. doi: 10.1007/BF02860623

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Repke, D. B., Artis, D. R., Nelson, J. T., and Wong, E. H. F. (1994). Abbreviated ibogaine congeners. Synthesis and reactions of tropan-3-yl-2- and -3-indoles. Investigation of an unusual isomerization of 2-substituted indoles using computational and spectroscopic techniques. J. Org. Chem. 59, 2164–2171. doi: 10.1021/jo00087a037

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reyes-Parada, M., Scorza, C., Romero, V., Silveira, R., Medina, J. H., Andrus, D., et al. (1996). (+/-)-1-(2,5-Dimethoxy-4-ethylthiophenyl)-2-aminopropane (ALEPH-2), a novel putative anxiolytic agent lacking affinity for benzodiazepine sites and serotonin-1A receptors. Naunyn. Schmiedebergs. Arch. Pharmacol. 354, 579–585. doi: 10.1007/BF00170831

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rezvani, A. H., Overstreet, D. H., and Lee, Y. W. (1995). Attenuation of alcohol intake by ibogaine in three strains of alcohol-preferring rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 52, 615–620. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(95)00152-M

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schenberg, E. E., de Castro Comis, M. A., Chaves, B. R., and da Silveira, D. X. (2014). Treating drug dependence with the aid of ibogaine: a retrospective study. J. Psychopharmacol. 28, 993–1000. doi: 10.1177/0269881114552713

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneider, J. A., and Sigg, E. B. (1957). Neuropharmacological studies on ibogaine, an indole alkaloid with central-stimulant properties. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 66, 765–776. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1957.tb40765.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spanos, L. J., and Yamamoto, B. K. (1989). Acute and subchronic effects of methylenedioxymethamphetamine [( + )MDMA] on locomotion and serotonin syndrome behavior in the rat. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. U.S.A. 32, 835–840. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(89)90044-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Staley, J. K., Ouyang, Q., Pablo, J., Hearn, W. L., Flynn, D. D., Rothman, R. B., et al. (1996). Pharmacological screen for activities of 12-hydroxyibogamine: a primary metabolite of the indole alkaloid ibogaine. Psychopharmacology 127, 10–18. doi: 10.1007/BF02805969

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sweetnam, P. M., Lancaster, J., Snowman, A., Collins, J. L., Perschke, S., Bauer, C., et al. (1995). Receptor binding profile suggests multiple mechanisms of action are responsible for ibogaine’s putative anti-addictive activity. Psychopharmacology 118, 369–376. doi: 10.1007/BF02245936

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Torterolo, P., Lagos, P., and Monti, J. M. (2011). Melanin-concentrating hormone: a new sleep factor? Front. Neurol. 2:14. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2011.00014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Torterolo, P., Monti, J. M., and Pandi-Perumal, S. R. (2016). “Neuroanatomy and neuropharmacology of sleep and wakefulness,” in Synopsis of Sleep Medicine, ed. S. R. Pandi-Perumal (Oakville: Apple Academic Press).

Google Scholar

Wei, D., Maisonneuve, I. M., Kuehne, M. E., and Glick, S. D. (1998). Acute iboga alkaloid effects on extracellular serotonin (5-HT) levels in nucleus accumbens and striatum in rats. Brain Res. 800, 260–268. doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(98)00527-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wells, G. B., Lopez, M. C., and Tanaka, J. C. (1999). The effects of ibogaine on dopamine and serotonin transport in rat brain synaptosomes. Brain Res. Bull. 48, 641–647. doi: 10.1016/S0361-9230(99)00053-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilkins, C., dos Santos, R. G., Solá, J., Aixalá, M., Cura, P., Moreno, E., et al. (2017). Detoxification from methadone using low, repeated, and increasing doses of ibogaine: a case report. J. Psychedelic Stud. 1, 29–34. doi: 10.1556/2054.01.2017.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yamamoto, T., and Ueki, S. (1975). Behavioral effects of 2,5-dimethoxy-4-methylamphetamine (DOM) in rats and mice. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 32, 156–162. doi: 10.1016/0014-2999(75)90278-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: REM sleep, wakefulness, ibogaine, psychedelics, hallucinogens

Citation: González J, Prieto JP, Rodríguez P, Cavelli M, Benedetto L, Mondino A, Pazos M, Seoane G, Carrera I, Scorza C and Torterolo P (2018) Ibogaine Acute Administration in Rats Promotes Wakefulness, Long-Lasting REM Sleep Suppression, and a Distinctive Motor Profile. Front. Pharmacol. 9:374. doi: 10.3389/fphar.2018.00374

Received: 27 November 2017; Accepted: 03 April 2018;
Published: 27 April 2018.

Edited by:

Rick Strassman, University of New Mexico School of Medicine, United States

Reviewed by:

Michael H. Baumann, National Institute on Drug Abuse (NIH), United States
Deborah Carmen Mash, University of Miami, United States

Copyright © 2018 González, Prieto, Rodríguez, Cavelli, Benedetto, Mondino, Pazos, Seoane, Carrera, Scorza and Torterolo. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Ignacio Carrera, aWNhcnJlcmFAZnEuZWR1LnV5 Cecilia Scorza, Y3Njb3J6YUBpaWJjZS5lZHUudXk=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.