Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Pediatr., 14 March 2024
Sec. Neonatology
This article is part of the Research Topic What is new on the Horizon in Neonatology? Recent Advances in Monitoring, Diagnostics, and Therapeutics in Neonatal Care. View all 12 articles

Multisensory stimulation and its effect on breast milk volume production in mothers of premature infants

\r\nCarla Madeleine Cuya
Carla Madeleine Cuya1*Carlos BarrigaCarlos Barriga2Maria del Carmen GrafMaria del Carmen Graf3Mirta CardeaMirta Cardeña1María del Pilar BorjaMaría del Pilar Borja1Richard CondoriRichard Condori4Moises AzocarMoises Azocar5Carlos Cuya\r\nCarlos Cuya4
  • 1Nursing Faculty, Universidad Católica de Santa María, Arequipa, Peru
  • 2Social Sciences and Technologies and Humanities Faculty, Universidad Católica de Santa María, Arequipa, Peru
  • 3Nursing, University of Wisconsin Oshkosh, Oshkosh, WI, United States
  • 4Medicine Faculty, Universidad Católica de Santa María, Arequipa, Peru
  • 5Manuel de Torres Muñoz Hospital (EsSalud), Arequipa, Peru

Introduction: In a significant number of NICUs, mothers are unable to provide enough maternal milk to feed their premature babies, so healthcare workers rely on human milk banks. Unfortunately, this service is not available in many countries, such as Peru, where premature infants receive formula. The aim of this study was to determine the effectiveness of multisensory stimulation on mother's own milk production.

Methods: Participants in this study were postpartum mothers of preterm infants 27–37 weeks gestational age. The participants were assigned to three groups: (1) audiovisual stimulation (SAV) (n = 17), (2) audiovisual and olfactory stimulation (SAVO) (n = 17), and (3) control (n = 16). A questionnaire was used to collect demographic and obstetric data, including a record of mother's own milk volume.

Results: There was no significant difference between the SAV, SAVO and control groups regarding age, marital status, education level, occupation, number of children, mode of delivery, Apgar and birth weight. On the other hand, a significant difference was observed between the SAV and SAVO groups regarding the amount of milk produced, with higher production between the fourth and seventh day (Tukey p < 0.05). Similarly, milk volume was significantly greater in the SAVO group compared to the SAV and control groups (OR = 1.032, 95% CI = 1.0036–1.062, p < 0.027).

Conclusion: Multisensory stimulation in postpartum mothers of preterm infants caused an increase in the volume of mother's own milk production. However, more research is needed to explain the findings presented in this study.

1 Introduction

Mother's own milk is undoubtedly the ideal and first food to promote the healthy growth and development of newborns, constituting a fundamental pillar at the beginning of their lives (1). It is rich in essential nutrients such as proteins, fats, carbohydrates, vitamins and minerals that promote the mental and physical development of premature infants (2). Therefore, the implementation of policies that actively encourage and promote mother's own milk feeding in this context becomes an unavoidable priority for health institutions (3, 4). Although preterm infants face developmental challenges due to their physiological immaturity, making them susceptible to various health conditions (5), mother's own milk acts as a protective factor due to its abundant immunoglobulins and other bioactive components (6).

One of the most common conditions faced by premature infants is necrotizing enterocolitis (2), an inflammation of the colon that occurs in response to the premature development of the digestive system. Necrotizing enterocolitis can have serious and, in some cases, life-threatening consequences, underscoring the importance of ensuring adequate nutrition from the first moments of life. Despite this, several neonatal intensive care units (NICUs) face a critical challenge: access and availability of mother's own milk (7).

Furthermore, the lack of availability of mother's own milk, in neonatal intensive care units, due to the fact that mothers often do not produce enough maternal milk (8), could be considered as a public health problem. Therefore, the unavailability of mother's own milk for immediate feeding of preterm infants, a common situation in many neonatal intensive care units (NICUs), leads health care providers to rely on donor human milk (2, 8, 9). In this case, the limited number of human milk banks aggravates the problem of neonatal care, as these centers could provide an alternative for feeding premature infants (10). Due to the difficulties in the implementation and operation of this type of basic health service, which could be attributed to the lack of an adequate government management and legal regulation in countries such as Peru (1113). However, in the absence of a human milk bank, health care institutions feel the need to administer infant formula, which several studies have shown to be detrimental to infant growth and development (1416). In the study by Moreira et al. (2) it was stated that the main complications of the use of formula in premature neonates in the NICU was the presence of necrotizing enterocolitis, which can be explained from the intestinal microbiota, where babies fed with mother's ow milk have a lower amount of enterobacteria than babies who receive formula or donor human milk. It also showed that babies fed with formula presented other complications, such as sepsis and bronchopulmonary dysplasia, unlike babies who received donor human milk or mother's own milk. This is because mother's own milk has metabolic advantages over formula, resulting in better protein absorption and more significant tissue growth in the newborn (17).

Another difference that has been observed between mother's own milk vs. formula, is the fat mass index, which is lower in newborns who are mother's own milk-fed than in those who are fed formula (17). Thus, mother's own milk should be the feeding source of choice for preterm infants.

In this context, the insufficient amount of milk produced by the mothers of preterm infants is one of the main reasons for the lack of mother's own milk in the NICU (8), a problem that may be due to multiple causes (12, 18). This condition can be explained by failed or delayed lactogenesis II, the second stage of milk production, which occurs after birth (18, 19). Underlying neurophysiological mechanisms such as endocrine, metabolic and obstetric factors are involved in lactogenesis II.

On the endocrine aspect, prolactin, insulin, adrenal cortisol and thyroid hormones can act directly on lactotropic cells and indirectly alter the endocrine response and the supply of nutrients to the mammary gland for milk production (2022). In addition to these factors, oxytocin plays a pivotal role in both the synthesis and ejection of maternal milk. Oxytocin is synthesized in magnocellular neurons and primarily accumulates in the paraventricular nuclei (PVN) and supraoptic nuclei (23). Its presence extends from the median eminence internal zone to the neurohypophysis (24), as well as collateral projections to the central amygdala and nucleus accumbens (25).

In terms of metabolic and obstetric factors that may delay lactogenesis II in mothers of premature infants, various conditions should be considered, such as retained placenta, cesarean section, type 2 diabetes, gestational diabetes, childbirth stress, polycystic ovarian syndrome, and postpartum hemorrhage, among others (18). Obese women are also more likely to have delayed lactogenesis II due to hormonal influences on milk production (26). Limitations have also been described in the maintenance of breastfeeding in preterm infants (27), which may be due to the immaturity of the sucking and swallowing reflexes of the newborn, who cannot be fed directly from the mother's breast, requiring manual extraction and collection of milk for later delivery to health care personnel (20).

Regarding the problem of insufficient mother's own milk production, several studies have demonstrated the importance of intervening with external and environmental stimuli. For example, the study by Cohen et al. (28), in lactating female mice showed that exposure to the odor of their pups resulted in significant changes in the cortical area of the mice's brains, leading to improved mother-pup behavior. Similarly, Embarek-Hernández et al. (29), report that multisensory stimulation improves feeding behavior in children and may include visual, tactile, oral, vestibular, auditory, or kinesthetic stimulation. Other similar studies, such as Hernández-Gutiérrez et al. (30), show that a combination of tactile, kinesthetic, and oral stimulation stimulates feeding responses in preterm infants compared with oral stimulation alone.

The production of mother's own milk can be influenced by multisensory stimuli at the level of oxytocin neurons (16) in the central amygdala and nucleus accumbens (25). Therefore, multisensory stimuli could induce an emotional and sentimental response (amygdala and hypothalamus) (31), which would result in the production of oxytocin through the action of oxytocinergic nerves originating in the PVN (32), which increase the blood flow of milk to the nipple during breastfeeding (23).

To summarize, the present study aimed to determine the efficacy of these stimulating processes on milk production in mothers of preterm infants.

2 Materials and methods

2.1 Study design

The design of the present study corresponds to case–control study, in accordance with the CONSORT 2010 guidelines (33). The study was conducted in the facilities of the Hospital Goyeneche, located in the city of Arequipa, in the southern region of Peru. Study participants were postpartum mothers with preterm infants born between 27 and 37 weeks of gestation. The study period was between June and December 2022. It should be noted that during this period there were sanitary restrictions in all Peruvian health facilities due to the COVID-19 pandemic (34). Therefore, maternal contact with the newborns was minimal. Even the kangaroo mother care method could not be applied during the study period due to the health restrictions that existed at the time.

The Institutional Research Ethics Committee of the Catholic University of Santa Maria approved the research project in the city of Arequipa, Peru (code: 099-2022). Prior to data collection, the researchers provided all participants with a detailed verbal and written explanation of the objectives and scope of the study, the data collection process, and their rights and responsibilities. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants. All information obtained during the study was kept strictly confidential. The results were presented anonymously to prevent the disclosure of any personal information about the participants. Participants were also free to withdraw or terminate their participation in the study at any time. However, there were no dropouts during the study.

In the case of the inclusion criteria, the selection of participants was postpartum mothers with premature infants born within 27–37 weeks of gestation, who had not received galactagogues, who did not have comorbidity or health complications in the breasts, and who wished to participate in the study, as well as premature infants who did not have malformations or health complications, who were born with an Apgar score greater than 7, at 5 min, with a weight between 1,000 and 3,900 g. Candidates who did not meet the above criteria were excluded from the study.

2.2 Sampling and recruitment

In the hospital where this study was conducted, 250 premature births were registered from June to December 2022, among which nine newborns presented malformations. According to the inclusion criteria, the mothers were selected in the delivery room. They were asked to sign the informed consent form to confirm their participation. Of the 241 potential participants, 191 mothers did not meet the inclusion criteria. Therefore, the sample was reduced to 50 participants who were recruited and randomly assigned (online random number generator) (35, 36) to three groups: 17 mothers in the visual-auditory stimulation (SAV) group, 17 in the visual-auditory olfactory stimulation (SAVO) group, and 16 mothers who received general care (control group) (see Figure 1).

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. A flow diagram summarizing the participant selection process. SAV, audiovisual stimulation; SAVO, audiovisual and olfactory stimulation; NICU, neonatal intensive care unit; MV, breast milk volume.

2.3 Data collection

Two instruments were used to collect data. The first instrument was a questionnaire to collect personal data from the participants (demographics and obstetric history). The second instrument was a form to record daily mother's own milk production. The maternal data collected in the questionnaire included age, marital status, education level, occupation, number of children, comorbidities, mode of delivery, and infant information such as gestational age, sex, birth weight, and Apgar. Sampling was performed for 15 min on each breast for seven consecutive days. All groups in this study received general care from the NICU staff, including educational sessions to teach participants about the importance of mother's own milk, breast massage before extraction, and guidelines on the safe method of expressing and storing breast milk.

The stimuli used in this study took place only in the area designated as a lactation room, an environment designed for the extraction and collection of mother's own milk within the facilities of the Goyeneche Hospital, the stimuli were carried out once a day.

The SAV and SAVO groups watched a video with images and sounds of babies breastfeeding, smiling and crying during the extraction of mother's own milk. The above-mentioned video served as an audiovisual stimulus that was previously validated by two health professionals (a neonatal intensive care specialist and a neonatal nurse) and an expert in social communication. The Delphi method was used and an Aiken V coefficient of 1 was obtained. The video is about 10 min long and includes instrumental music and neonatal crying. The video begins by showing a newborn's hands, feet, and face, then shows babies being breastfed, and ends with an image of a calm newborn. In the video, we included only the faces of healthy babies who were breastfeeding, relaxed, or crying, and excluded the faces of mothers and hospitalized newborns. The video was displayed on a high-definition (HD) 65-inch television screen.

During human milk extraction, the SAVO group, in addition to watching the video, also received an olfactory stimulus consisting of inhaling for approximately 3 min a cotton swab previously rubbed on the newborn's neck, armpits, and groin. NICU staff obtained the cotton swab after the neonate was bathed. They then placed the swabs individually in a hermetically sealed bottle. The control group received only general care from the NICU staff. The amount of milk collected from both breasts was recorded for seven consecutive days after delivery. It is noteworthy that throughout the process of stimulation and milk extraction, the mothers selected for this study did not directly breastfeed their newborns.

2.4 Data analysis

In the case of the statistical analysis of the selected sample of mothers (Figure 1), the power analysis was taken as a reference, according to a comparative study between the effectiveness of massage and the use of hot compresses on the breasts to stimulate milk production, which considers a power of 80% and a significance of 5% (37). Descriptive statistics (averages, standard deviations) are used for analysis of sample characteristics, and inferential statistics are used for analysis of continuous variables for estimation of normality. To evaluate the differences between the groups, they were compared using the ANOVA test for continuous data with the use of Tukey's post hoc test and the Pearson's χ2 test for categorical data. Additionally, multinomial logistic regression analysis was conducted. All p-values were two-tailed and significance was set at alpha <0.05. The JAMOVI statistical program, version 2.3.21, was used to analyze all data.

3 Results

3.1 Demographic and obstetric data

Demographic data such as age, marital status, education level, occupation, number of children, and obstetric data such as mode of delivery, Apgar, and birth weight were not significantly different between SAV, SAVO, and the control group (Table 1).

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic and obstetric data of participants.

3.2 Mother's own milk volume according to stimulus type during days 1–7 postpartum, between the control group, SAV, and SAVO (N = 50)

Depending on the type of stimulation, an increase in mother's own milk volume was observed, with a greater volume from day 4 to day 7 (Table 2) in the SAV and SAVO stimulation groups compared to the control group (Figure 2).

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Comparison of breast milk volume between control group, SAV and SAVO, days 1–7 postpartum (N = 50).

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Comparison of breast milk volume between the control group, SAV and SAVO.

3.3 One-way ANOVA for between-subject and within-subject comparisons of milk volume between the SAV, SAVO, and control groups

According to mother's own milk volume, significant variance is observed between study groups on day 0 (4 h postpartum) and from day 3 to day 7 (Table 3).

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. One-way ANOVA to compare milk volume, between subjects and within subjects between the SAV, the SAVO and the control groups.

There was a highly significant difference (p < 0.05) in the increase of mother's own milk volume in the SAVO group over the 7-day period (Tukey's posthoc test) (Table 4).

Table 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Multiple comparisons regarding the type of stimulation (SAV, SAVO and the control group) in the average volume of breast milk, over a period of 7 days.

In the multinomial logistic regression analysis, the audiovisual and olfactory stimulation (SAVO) group was more likely to have an increase in average mother's own milk volume than the control group. No significant changes were observed in the audiovisual stimulation and control groups. There were also no significant differences between the SAV and SAVO groups (Table 5).

Table 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Multinomial logistic regression according to stimulus and average milk volume.

On the other hand, the multivariate linear regression analysis between the sociodemographic and obstetric data of the mothers, the type of stimulus and the volume of milk production showed that the relationship between the age of the mother and the multisensory stimulus could intervene in the production of mother's own milk, especially during the first two days (Table 6).

Table 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Multivariate linear regression for sociodemographic and obstetric data according to the volume of breast milk production.

4 Discussion

The present study evaluated the effectiveness of using multisensory stimuli (audiovisual and olfactory) to increase milk production in mothers of preterm infants. The research was based on the neuroscientific basis of the activation of oxytocin, the main hormone in mother's own milk production, as demonstrated by Uvnäs-Moberg et al. (32), who found that this hormone can be activated by non-noxious sensory stimulation (PVN).

In this context, neuroscience (38) emphasizes that external stimuli from the environment intervene in the activation of the limbic system (39) and take advantage of the integration of different sensory channels (kinesthetic, tactile, visual, gustatory, olfactory, auditory and vestibular) for the development of a comprehensive experience (40). The simultaneous use of the senses creates a stimulating experience that effectively captures the recipient's attention and produces more intense emotions. As a result, information retention is enhanced (41) and a lasting experience is generated, creating an emotional connection with the recipient (31). Furthermore, a study by Rolls et al. (38), reported a link between visual, olfactory and auditory stimuli and the learning process, as both the amygdala and the hippocampus interact with the emotion produced by the stimulus, generating learning that is demonstrated in social and emotional behavior (42).

The literature found so far includes studies with ad hoc protocols of multisensory stimulation of preterm and term infants in the areas of mother's own milk production (43), weight gain, length of hospital stay (44) and neuromotor development (45, 46). However, we did not find any studies on mothers of preterm infants related to measurement of milk volume.

It is worth noting that there are only three human milk banks in Peru, none of which are located in the city of this study (Arequipa). However, the Neonatal Intensive Care Unit where the research took place has highly specialized personnel who take care of premature babies and their nutritional needs. Similarly, a multidisciplinary team educates mothers about the importance of feeding their milk to their babies to promote their growth and development.

The results of the present study suggest that the group of mothers who received visual-auditory olfactory stimulation (SAVO) positively increased the volume of milk production (Table 2). This increase could be due to the hormonal role in pregnancy and childbirth. During pregnancy, hormones intervene to facilitate childbirth (47). According to Larsen et al. (48), prolactin regulates neurogenesis in the mother's brain during pregnancy, creating new neuronal networks in the lateral subventricular zone of the brain that enhance the sense of smell. Similarly, Uvnäs-Moberg et al. (32), and Olza et al. (47), reported that oxytocin affects neuroendocrinological, physiological, and psychological processes during labor, delivery, and the early postpartum period and determines maternal behavior.

On the other hand, our study found an increase in milk volume on day 4 in mothers who received visual, auditory, and olfactory stimulation, in contrast to the control group (Table 3). Therefore, it was suggested that multisensory stimulation may positively influence milk production in mothers of preterm infants from the fourth day of lactation. These findings were in agreement with Yu et al. (18), who indicated that lactogenesis II is delayed in mothers of preterm infants, and milk volume is lower in the first 24 h after birth. Therefore, for a higher increase in volume, the stimuli should be applied 48 h after delivery.

Conversely, Mullen et al. (19), suggest that early breastfeeding (before 6 h after birth) is a primary factor in milk production in mothers of preterm infants. In this study, there was no significant difference in the type of multisensory stimulus used (Table 6). Yu et al. (18), found that late lactogenesis II was associated with the type of delivery, particularly cesarean delivery. However, Heller et al. (49), pointed out that this information is inconclusive for preterm birth. Regarding the type of delivery, there was no significant differences between stimulation type and milk production (Table 6).

The parity of the mother is a controversial factor, as some studies, such as Zachariassen et al. (50), suggest a significant association between being primiparous and successful initiation of breastfeeding. On the contrary, Maas-trup et al. (51), suggests that multiparity is associated with successful breastfeeding. However, in this study, no significant differences were found in stimulation and mother's own milk volume with parity of the mother. However, a significant difference was found between multisensory stimulation, mother's own milk volume on days 1 and 2, and maternal age. There were no conclusive studies on the association between maternal age and mother's own milk production. However, we found evidence of an association between maternal age and successful breastfeeding after discharge of a preterm infant. In this regard, Pineda et al. (52), found that young mothers were less successful in maintaining breastfeeding. Similarly, Casey et al. (53), indicated that maternal age is a predictor of failure to maintain exclusive breastfeeding.

Other sociodemographic factors, such as maternal marital status, education level, and occupation, did not show significant differences in stimulation with mother's own milk volume. Nevertheless, several studies have demonstrated the association of the above-mentioned variables with the success of maintaining exclusive breastfeeding in preterm infants after discharge from hospital (49, 52).

According to Mitha et al. (54), the factors that promote breastfeeding in preterm infants in the NICU are skin-to-skin contact using the kangaroo position during the first week of life, continuous information to mothers about the importance of breastfeeding, expressing their milk within 6 h of birth, the provision of a designated room (in the NICU) for expressing their milk, and support from staff trained in breastfeeding. In contrast, Heller et al. (49), did not find a significant association between the duration of breastfeeding in preterm infants and health care provider information, availability of a lactation room and staff trained in breastfeeding. Therefore, further studies are needed to determine the relationship between these factors and mother's own milk production.

Due to the hospital's COVID-19 protocol, postpartum mothers with preterm infants in the NICU were not able to perform skin-to-skin contact (kangaroo method). The absence or delay of this method is considered to be a risk factor for the successful establishment of exclusive breastfeeding at discharge (5456). Therefore, it would be essential to implement the multisensory stimuli proposed in this study with mothers who use the kangaroo method, in order to evaluate its impact on mother's own milk production.

Despite the limitations imposed by the context of the COVID-19 pandemic, the study was rigorously conducted. Thus, the limitations did not affect our findings.

Finally, the results found in this study may have relevant clinical implications for neonatal intensive care units that are faced with insufficient mother's own milk to feed premature infants and lack access to human milk banks. The incorporation of multisensory stimulation would represent an innovative approach to mother's own milk production and mother-infant care.

5 Conclusion

Multisensory stimulation in postpartum mothers of preterm infants may influence an increase in mother's own milk production, according to the findings of this study. However, more research is needed in this area to clarify the reported findings.

Data availability statement

The datasets presented in this study can be found in online repositories. The names of the repository/repositories and accession number(s) can be found in the article/Supplementary Material.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Ethics Committee of the Universidad Catolica de Santa María, Arequipa, Peru (protocol code 099-2022; 18 July 2022). The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study. Written informed consent was obtained from the individual(s) for the publication of any potentially identifiable images or data included in this article.

Author contributions

CC: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal Analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft. CB: Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – review & editing. MG: Data curation, Investigation, Validation, Writing – review & editing. MC: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Writing – review & editing. MB: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Writing – review & editing. RC: Investigation, Resources, Writing – review & editing. MA: Investigation, Resources, Validation, Writing – review & editing. CC: Investigation, Resources, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The authors declare that financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

This research was funded by the Universidad Católica de Santa María Internal Research Fund “R No 26617-R-2019”.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. North K, Gao M, Allen G, Lee AC. Breastfeeding in a global context: epidemiology, impact, and future directions. Clin Ther. (2022) 44(2):228–44. doi: 10.1016/j.clinthera.2021.11.017

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Moreira-Monteagudo M, Leirós-Rodríguez R, Marqués-Sánchez P. Effects of formula milk feeding in premature infants: a systematic review. Children. (2022) 9:150. doi: 10.3390/children9020150

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Llorente-Pulido S, Custodio E, López-Giménez M, Otero-García L. Barriers and facilitators for exclusive breastfeeding within the health system and public policies from in-depth interviews to primary care midwives in tenerife (canary Islands, Spain). Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2022) 19:128. doi: 10.3390/ijerph19010128

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Kinshella ML, Prasad S, Hiwa T, Vidler M, Nyondo-Mipando A, Dube Q, et al. Barriers and facilitators for early and exclusive breastfeeding in health facilities in sub-Saharan Africa: a systematic review. Glob Heal Res Policy. (2021) 6(1):21. doi: 10.1186/s41256-021-00206-2

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Lehman IM, Broers B, Czosnykowska-Lukacka M, Wesolowska W, Swiderska L, Krolak-Olejnik B. Donor human milk in neonatal intensive care unit—to whom, how much and how long? Ginekol Pol. (2019) 90(9):534–8. doi: 10.5603/GP.2019.0092

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

6. World Health Organization. Preguntas frecuentes: lactancia materna y COVID-19 para trabajadores de la salud (2020). Available online at: https://www.who.int/docs/default-source/coronaviruse/breastfeeding-covid-who-faqs-es-12may2020.pdf?sfvrsn=f1fdf92c_8 (accessed June 30, 2023).

7. Mantri N, Goel A, Joshi N, Bhardwaj P, Gautam V, Gupta M. Challenges in implementation of mother milk banks in Rajasthan: a situational analysis. J Mother Child. (2022) 25(2):86–94. doi: 10.34763/jmotherandchild.20212502.d-21-00009/

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Neves P, Vaz J, Ricardo L, Armenta-Paulino N, Barros AJ, Richter L, et al. Disparities in early initiation of breast feeding and prelacteal feeding: a study of low- and middle-income countries. PaeDAYtr Perinat Epidemiol. (2022) 36(5):741–9. doi: 10.1111/ppe.12871

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Manurung HR. The effectiveness of neck massage to increasing the total of postpartum breast milk on the first until third days at PMB Medan city. Sci Midwifery. (2022) 10(4):3458–64. doi: 10.35335/midwifery.v10i4.852

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Lee S, Kelleher S. Biological underpinnings of breastfeeding challenges: the role of genetics, diet, and environment on lactation physiology. Am J Physiol Metab. (2016) 311(2):E405–22. doi: 10.1152/ajpendo.00495.2015

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Klotz D, Wesołowska A, Bertino E, Moro G, Picaud J, Gayà A, et al. The legislative framework of donor human milk and human milk banking in Europe. Matern Child Nutr. (2022) 18(2):2–3. doi: 10.1111/mcn.13310

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Smith J, Iellamo A, Nguyen T, Mathisen R. The volume and monetary value of human milk produced by the world’s breastfeeding mothers: results from a new tool. Front Public Health. (2023) 11:2–8. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2023.1152659

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Espina-Jerez B, Romera-Álvarez L, de Dios-Aguado M, Cunha-Oliveira A, Siles-Gonzalez J, Gómez-Cantarino S. Wet nurse or milk bank? Evolution in the model of human lactation: new challenges for the islamic population. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2022) 19(15):9742. doi: 10.3390/ijerph19159742

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Chong H, Tan L, Hong K. Exploring the potential of human milk and formula milk on infants’ gut and health. Nutrients. (2022) 14(17):3554. doi: 10.3390/nu14173554

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Poulimeneas D, Bathrellou E, Antonogeorgos G, Mamalaki E, Kouvari M, Kuligowski J, et al. Feeding the preterm infant: an overview of the evidence. Int J Food Sci Nutr. (2021) 72(1):4–13. doi: 10.1080/09637486.2020.1754352

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Quigley M, Embleton N, McGuire W. Formula versus donor breast milk for feeding preterm or low birth weight infants. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. (2018) 20(6):CD002971. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD002971.pub4

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Gianni M, Roggero P, Mosca F. Human milk protein vs. formula protein and their use in preterm infants. Curr Opin Clin Nutr Metab Care. (2019) 22(1):76–81. doi: 10.1097/MCO.0000000000000528

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Yu X, Li J, Lin X, Luan D. Association between delayed lactogenesis Ⅱ and early milk volume among mothers of preterm infants. Asian Nurs Res. (2019) 13(2):93–8. doi: 10.1016/j.anr.2019.02.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Mullen A, O’Connor D, Hanley A, Piedimonte G, Wallace M, Ley S. Associations of metabolic and obstetric risk parameters with timing of lactogenesis II. Nutrients. (2022) 14(4):876. doi: 10.3390/nu14040876

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Oliva-Pérez J, Oliver-Roig A. Relación del retraso de la lactogénesis II con la percepción materna de leche insuficiente: un estudio longitudinal. Enfermería Clínica. (2022) 32(6):413–22. doi: 10.1016/j.enfcli.2022.07.005

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Freeman M, Kanyicska B, Lerant A, Nagy G. Prolactin: structure, function, and regulation of secretion. Physiol Rev. (2000) 80(4):1523–631. doi: 10.1152/physrev.2000.80.4.1523

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Laporta J, Peñagaricano F, Hernandez L. Transcriptomic analysis of the mouse mammary gland reveals new insights for the role of serotonin in lactation. PLoS One. (2015) 10(10):e0140425. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0140425

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Carter S, Kenkel W, MacLean E, Wilson S, Perkeybile A, Yee JR, et al. Is oxytocin “nature’s medicine”? Pharmacol Ver. (2020) 72(4):829–61. doi: 10.1124/pr.120.019398

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Liu CM, Spaulding MO, Rea JJ, Noble EE, Kanoski SE. Oxytocin and food intake control: neural, behavioral, and signaling mechanisms. Int J Mol Sci. (2021) 22(19):2–4. doi: 10.3390/ijms221910859

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Moller M. Chapter 2—vasopressin and oxytocin beyond the pituitary in the human brain. Handb Clin Neurol. (2021) 180:7–24. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-820107-7.00002-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Lian W, Ding J, Xiong T, Liuding J, Nie L. Determinants of delayed onset of lactogenesis II among women who delivered via cesarean section at a tertiary hospital in China: a prospective cohort study. Int Breastfeed J. (2022) 17(1):81. doi: 10.1186/s13006-022-00523-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Fewtrell M, Kennedy K, Ahluwalia J, Nicholl R, Lucas A, Burton P. Predictors of expressed breast milk volume in mothers expressing milk for their preterm infant. Arch Dis Child - Fetal Neonatal Edition. (2016) 101(6):F502–6. doi: 10.1136/archdischild-2015-308321

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Cohen L, Rothschild G, Mizrahi A. Multisensory integration of natural odors and sounds in the auditory cortex. Neuron. (2011) 72(2):357–69. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.08.019

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Embarek-Hernández M, Güeita-Rodríguez J, Molina-Rueda F. Multisensory stimulation to promote feeding and psychomotor development in preterm infants: a systematic review. PeDAYtr Neonatol. (2022) 63(5):452–61. doi: 10.1016/j.pedneo.2022.07.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Gutiérrez MH, Díaz-Gómez M, Jiménez A, Díaz JM, Domenech E. Effectiveness of 2 interventions for independent oral feeding in preterms. An PeDAYtría (English Ed). (2022) 96(2):97–105. doi: 10.1016/j.anpede.2020.12.020

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Baldassano C, Chen J, Zadbood A, Pillow J, Hasson U, Norman KA. Discovering event structure in continuous narrative perception and memory. Neuron. (2017) 95(3):709–21.e5. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.06.041

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Uvnäs-Moberg K, Handlin L, Petersson M. Self-soothing behaviors with particular reference to oxytocin release induced by non-noxious sensory stimulation. Front Psychol. (2015) 5:1529. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01529

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Cuschieri S. The CONSORT statement. Saudi J Anaesth. (2019) 13(5):27. doi: 10.4103/sja.SJA_559_18

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Republica del Perú. Decreto Supremo No 003-2023-SA. El Peruano (2023). Available online at: https://busquedas.elperuano.pe/normaslegales/decreto-supremo-que-prorroga-la-emergencia-sanitaria-declara-decreto-supremo-no-003-2023-sa-2154787-1/ (accessed June 20, 2023).

35. Random.org. Random integer generatos. Available online at: https://www.random.org/integers/ (accessed May 25, 2023).

36. Fernández R, Cao R. Técnicas de Simulación y Remuestreo. (2023). Available online at: https://rubenfcasal.github.io/simbook/Simulacion.pdf (accessed June 1, 2023).

37. Khoonphet C, Sopen C, Sasitorn P. Effects of southern Thai traditional massage with warm compression on lactation and breast engorgement: a randomized controlled trial. Pac Rim Int J Nurs Res. (2022) 26(3):390–403.

Google Scholar

38. Rolls E. Limbic systems for emotion and for memory, but no single limbic system. Cortex. (2015) 62:119–57. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2013.12.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

39. RajMohan V, Mohandas E. The limbic system. InDAYn J Psychiatry. (2007) 49(2):132. doi: 10.4103/0019-5545.33264

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Setti F, Handjaras G, Bottari D, Leo A, Diano M, Bruno V, et al. A modality-independent proto-organization of human multisensory areas. Nat Hum Behav. (2023) 7(3):397–410. doi: 10.1038/s41562-022-01507-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Gori M, Price S, Newell F, Berthouze N, Volpe G. Multisensory perception and learning: linking pedagogy, psychophysics, and human–computer interaction. Multisens Res. (2022) 35(4):335–66. doi: 10.1163/22134808-bja10072

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Yang Y, Wang JZ. From structure to behavior in basolateral amygdala-hippocampus circuits. Front Neural Circuits. (2017) 11:4–5. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2017.00086

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Fontana C, Menis C, Pesenti N, Passera S, Liotto N, Mosca F, et al. Effects of early intervention on feeding behavior in preterm infants: a randomized controlled trial. Early Hum Dev. (2018) 121:15–20. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2018.04.016

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Rodovanski G, Réus BA, Neves dos Santos A. The effects of multisensory stimulation on the length of hospital stay and weight gain in hospitalized preterm infants: a systematic review with meta-analysis. Brazilian J Phys Ther. (2023) 27(1):100468. doi: 10.1016/j.bjpt.2022.100468

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Kanagasabai P, Mohan D, Lewis L, Kamath A, Rao B. Effect of multisensory stimulation on neuromotor development in preterm infants. InDAYn J PeDAYtr. (2013) 80(6):460–4. doi: 10.1007/s12098-012-0945-z

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Yang XY, Zhang BB, Zhao L, Chen X. Effect of multi-sensory stimulation on children with global developmental delay. Asian J Surg. (2021) 44(10):1308–9. doi: 10.1016/j.asjsur.2021.06.051

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Olza I, Uvnas-Moberg K, Ekström-Bergström A, Leahy-Warren P, Karlsdottir SI, Nieuwenhuijze M, et al. Birth as a neuro-psycho-social event: an integrative model of maternal experiences and their relation to neurohormonal events during childbirth. PLoS One. (2020) 15(7):e0230992. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0230992

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Larsen C, Grattan D. Prolactin-Induced mitogenesis in the subventricular zone of the maternal brain during early pregnancy is essential for normal postpartum behavioral responses in the mother. Endocrinology. (2010) 151(8):3805–14. doi: 10.1210/en.2009-1385

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Heller N, Rüdiger M, Hoffmeister V, Mense L. Mother’s own milk feeding in preterm newborns admitted to the neonatal intensive care unit or special-care nursery: obstacles, interventions, risk calculation. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2021) 18(8):4140. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18084140

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Zachariassen G, Faerk J, Grytter C, Esberg B, Juvonen P, Halken S. Factors associated with successful establishment of breastfeeding in very preterm infants. Acta PaeDAYtr. (2010) 99(7):1000–4. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2010.01721.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Maastrup R, Hansen B, Kronborg H, Bojesen S, Hallum K, Frandsen A, et al. Factors associated with exclusive breastfeeding of preterm infants. Results from a prospective national cohort study. PLoS One. (2014) 9(2):e89077. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0089077

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Pineda R. Predictors of breastfeeding and breastmilk feeding among very low birth weight infants. Breastfeed Med. (2011) 6(1):15–9. doi: 10.1089/bfm.2010.0010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Casey L, Fucile S, Dow K. Determinants of successful direct breastfeeding at hospital discharge in high-risk premature infants. Breastfeed Med. (2018) 13(5):346–51. doi: 10.1089/bfm.2017.0209

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Mitha A, Piedvache A, Glorieux I, Zeitlin J, Roué J, Blondel B, et al. Unit policies and breast milk feeding at discharge of very preterm infants: the EPIPAGE-2 cohort study. PaeDAYtr Perinat Epidemiol. (2019) 33(1):59–69. doi: 10.1111/ppe.12536

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Mekonnen A, Yehualashet S, Bayleyegn A. The effects of kangaroo mother care on the time to breastfeeding initiation among preterm and LBW infants: a meta-analysis of published studies. Int Breastfeed J. (2019) 14(1):12. doi: 10.1186/s13006-019-0206-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Li L, Wang L, Niu C, Liu C, Lv T, Ji F, et al. Early skin contact combined with mother’s breastfeeding to shorten the process of premature infants ≤ 30 weeks of gestation to achieve full oral feeding: the study protocol of a randomized controlled trial. Trials. (2021) 22(1):637. doi: 10.1186/s13063-021-05605-x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: breastfeeding, premature birth, emotions, sensory system, limbic systems

Citation: Cuya CM, Barriga C, Graf MdC, Cardeña M, Borja MdP, Condori R, Azocar M and Cuya C (2024) Multisensory stimulation and its effect on breast milk volume production in mothers of premature infants. Front. Pediatr. 12:1331310. doi: 10.3389/fped.2024.1331310

Received: 31 October 2023; Accepted: 1 March 2024;
Published: 14 March 2024.

Edited by:

Karel Allegaert, KU Leuven, Belgium

Reviewed by:

An Eerdekens, University Hospitals Leuven, Belgium
Enrique Gomez-Pomar, University of Kentucky, United States

© 2024 Cuya, Barriga, Graf, Cardeña, Borja, Condori, Azocar and Cuya. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Carla Madeleine Cuya ccuya@ucsm.edu.pe

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.