Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Pediatr., 19 April 2024
Sec. Children and Health

Evaluating the influence of feedback on motor skill learning and motor performance for children with developmental coordination disorder: a systematic review

\r\nEllana Welsby
&#x;Ellana Welsby1*Brenton Hordacre,&#x;Brenton Hordacre1,†David Hobbs,,&#x;David Hobbs1,2,†Joanne Bouckley,&#x;Joanne Bouckley1,†Emily Ward,&#x;Emily Ward1,†Susan Hillier,&#x;\r\nSusan Hillier1,†
  • 1Innovation, Implementation, and Clinical Translation (IIMACT) in Health, Allied Health & Human Performance, University of South Australia, Adelaide, SA, Australia
  • 2College of Science and Engineering, Medical Device Research Institute, Flinders University, Adelaide, SA, Australia

Introduction: Children with developmental coordination disorder (DCD) have difficulties with learning and performing physical tasks. It is well known that task-specific practice is effective in improving motor skills. Additional feedback during practice may function as a quality improvement mechanism and therefore enhance motor skill outcomes.

Aims: To investigate the effect of different forms of feedback on motor learning and motor performance in children with DCD.

Methods: A systematic review was conducted (registration CRD42020175118) to investigate the effectiveness of different types of feedback, compared to other forms of feedback, or no additional feedback, on motor learning and motor performance outcomes in children with DCD. The search was run across six electronic databases (last search January 2024). Two reviewers independently screened studies for inclusion, assessed the quality of included studies, and extracted relevant data. A narrative synthesis was performed and included studies that assessed motor learning and/or performance outcomes following an intervention that delivered a specific form of feedback in comparison to another form of feedback or no specific feedback.

Results: 14 articles from 13 trials were included in this review. Feedback was delivered by providing various forms of feedback, including: knowledge of results, focus of attention and augmented feedback delivered via technology. No significant differences were found between different forms of feedback for motor learning or performance outcomes for children with DCD. Interventions that used technology (with augmented feedback) to deliver the intervention were found to be as effective as traditional therapy. All groups who participated in therapy, regardless of the presence or type of feedback received, improved in overall scores on a motor performance outcome assessment.

Conclusion: Despite the clear rationale for using feedback-oriented interventions for children with DCD, there is surprisingly limited and low-quality research. There is no clear evidence that one form of feedback is more effective than another, although it appears that feedback delivered via technology may be as effective as feedback delivered in traditional therapy interventions for children with DCD. Further exploration is required from appropriately powered and well-designed trials.

Systematic Review Registration: https://www.crd.york.ac.uk/prospero/display_record.php?RecordID=175118, identifier (CRD42020175118).

1 Introduction

Children with Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD) typically experience issues with motor performance that restrict participation in activities of daily living, impact academic performance, and increase risk of psycho-social difficulties (14). Clumsiness, together with slow and inaccurate gross and fine motor skills, are defining features of DCD. Long-term consequences of DCD include lower self-concept and self-efficacy, reduced physical and social participation, and increased rates of anxiety and depression that extend into adolescence and adulthood (38). Without intervention or support, between 50%–70% of individuals with DCD will continue to have motor and subsequent psychosocial difficulties into adolescence and adulthood (9).

It is well established that any form of physical activity and/or intervention is important for motor learning and performance in children with and without motor difficulties (10, 11). Children with DCD have difficulty acquiring, retaining, executing, and transferring fundamental motor skills (9, 12). Importantly, like typically developing (TD) children, research suggests children with DCD can successfully learn new motor skills and tasks, however, task type and difficulty significantly moderate motor performance and learning outcomes beyond their TD peers (13, 14). For example, children with DCD can learn simple motor tasks just as quickly and accurately as their TD peers, however they perform more slowly and less accurately on more complex motor tasks (14). It has been hypothesized this might be due to their reduced ability to use internal and/or external feedback to predict and update their stored motor plan, and that children with DCD may benefit from amplified feedback mechanisms to facilitate the motor learning process.

To produce movement, an individual must analyse the environment and generate movement to meet the demands of the task. This is typically facilitated via feedforward and feedback control processes (15). Before movement execution, sensory information of our surroundings and prior experience of the movement are used to create a predicted motor plan of the desired movement (feedforward control). As a movement is executed, sensory feedback is analysed during [knowledge of performance (KOP)] and after [knowledge of results (KOR)] the movement and compared to the desired motor plan (feedback control). Mismatches between the predicted and executed movement then result in modification and updating of the motor plan that occurs in real time. The updated and stored motor plan is then available for the prediction and planning of future movement (15) (Figure 1).

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Traditional motor loop.

Feedback is both a fundamental and crucial element to the motor control process (1619). In motor learning, feedback can be defined as movement related information that is “fed-back” to the learner and can be provided before, during (performance) or after (results) a movement. Feedback information can be provided intrinsically by specific body movements and sensation of movement within the learner's body (internal feedback), or extrinsically from different environmental cues and task relevant cues from outside the learner's body, such as via visual, auditory, or tactile feedback (external feedback). Common methods of delivering feedback can be through guided discovery and instruction (KOR and KOP), providing internal and external cues by directing the learners focus of attention (FOA), and by manipulating constraints of the task or environment. Each method can be delivered at any point in the motor control process (20).

Augmented feedback is also of particular interest in motor learning and performance as it can provide an individual with amplified internal and/or external knowledge of an action over and above the natural information given in the environment (demanding higher attention). Numerous studies have investigated the effect of different forms of augmented feedback, including feedback delivered by visual, auditory, and tactile modalities, in clinical and non-clinical adults and children (16, 2123). Research outcomes suggest augmented feedback that is delivered using various methods, and at different frequencies and quantities, both have advantages depending on the population group and skill being learnt (2326).

Traditional therapy and training is typically directed by the interaction of one or more of the three systems in motor control—action, perception, and cognition (PCA)—to promote motor learning. Augmented feedback may be particularly useful to amplify PCA demands of the task. Figure 2 demonstrates where augmented feedback may be manipulated, changed and/or enhanced beyond the traditional therapy models and natural environment to facilitate the learner's ability to recognise and update their motor plan more effectively and refine their motor control loop (27, 28). For example, using the Canadian Occupational Performance Model where goal setting, planning and decision making are a large focus of motor performance, is largely centered within the Cognitive domain of performance (see Figure 2) (29). Incorporating ideas of augmenting feedback in this area could include enhancing KOR by encouraging the implicit explanation and/or exploration of the results of the action through guided discovery and instruction, or by tacitly facilitating memory and experience to support motor learning.

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Demonstration of how augmented feedback may be used to manipulate change and /or enhance feedback beyond traditional training methods. This figure combines therapeutic, neuroscience and motor learning theory to display the unidirectional nation of the use of feedback for motor skill training, using the perception, cognitive, and action model.

Training can also be directed in the Action phase where the execution of movement and repetitive practice can increase exposure to a motor skill (for example task-specific training). It may be possible to incorporate or enhance augmented feedback in the action phase by promoting KOP and FOA to kinaesthetic cues during movement.

Within the Perception domain, the use of perceptual cues from the learner's environment can also assist the learner—potentially by updating their body schema to support their feedforward learning and therefore their motor plan. Interventions here can also incorporate training via attending to specific sensory stimuli by highlighting one sense over another, by directing one's FOA to their body within their environment, or by manipulating environmental constraints. As demonstrated in Figure 2, it is important to recognise that these systems or domains do not work in isolation, nor are they mutually exclusive.

In addition to traditional training methods, technology appears to be a promising way to deliver augmented feedback at all stages of the motor control loop due to its usability, adaptability, and popularity. Technology in clinical practice has become a popular option for play and rehabilitation for children with DCD (9, 3032), however, research is limited. Technology can be used to enhance the feedback the learner receives (visual, auditory, haptic) in real time, both during (concurrent or KOP) and after (terminal or KOR) the movement. Providing real-time concurrent feedback may further assist intrinsic and extrinsic feedback mechanisms, that may ultimately help to facilitate successful motor learning through the motor control loop. Promisingly, positive outcomes from augmented feedback using technology have been found in children and adults with low motor abilities, for example cerebral palsy and brain injury (3338). However, it remains unclear whether augmented feedback using technology has the same motor skill benefits for children with DCD as other clinical populations.

While feedback is important for learning, it remains unclear if any form of feedback is superior to another for motor learning and/or motor performance for children with DCD. Therefore, this systematic review aimed to investigate the effectiveness of different types of feedback, compared to other forms of feedback, or no additional feedback, on motor learning and motor performance outcomes in children with DCD. Findings may lead to a better understanding of the influence of feedback on motor learning and motor performance and could inform the best feedback modalities for future research and clinical interventions for the DCD population.

2 Methods

2.1 Protocol and registration

The systematic review protocol was registered with the PROSPERO international database and was accepted on 5th July 2020 (registration number CRD42020175118).

2.2 Search strategy

The Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines were used to structure the review (39). The following databases were searched on 24th January 2024: MEDLINE, Web of Science, EMBASE, Cochrane, PEDro and OTseeker. The search strategy was developed and trialled in MEDLINE and was adapted to be used in all included electronic databases. The search strategy is available in Supplementary document 1. Two researchers (EW and JB) independently reviewed titles, abstracts, and full texts of the searches to find included studies. Conflicts were reviewed by the two researchers and if a decision could not be reached, a third independent reviewer was consulted (SH). Reference lists of included studies were pearled to identify any missing studies that were not identified in the initial search that met the inclusion criteria.

2.3 Inclusion criteria

This review employed the PICOS criteria (population, intervention, comparator, outcome, and study design) to inform the inclusion criteria. Studies had to meet the following criteria: (1) children aged 5–12 years with probable or diagnosed DCD (intervention and control groups), as indicated by a score ≤ 16% percentile on the MABC-2 or low percentile quadrant on other motor performance assessments (e.g., DCD-Q), (2) participants needed to receive a motor skill intervention that authors state the use of one of more forms of feedback, (3) a control group was required who were either receiving an active intervention, that was either a different form of feedback or a traditional training method without feedback, and, (4) included trials needed to include a motor learning and/or performance outcome measure. No restrictions were placed on study design. Conference abstracts and protocols were excluded. Included studies were limited to primary peer-reviewed studies, with no grey literature included.

This review included studies that used and reported on any form of feedback, including but not limited to, internal and/or external feedback, FOA (internal or external), KOR, KOP, or augmented feedback. Interventions that did not specify the use of a specific form of feedback were excluded, as feedback was not isolated, and therefore, it would not be possible to distinguish the effect of feedback as opposed to repeated practice. Motor learning was defined as the ability to acquire, retain, and execute a motor task, due to practice of a particular motor skill. Motor performance was defined as the ability to perform a motor task. The categorisation of articles either assessing motor learning or motor performance was determined by what the authors reported on assessing. For full inclusion and exclusion criteria, see Table 1.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Inclusion and exclusion criteria.

Due to the increase in popularity of technology in therapy, interventions delivered via technology in comparison to a traditional training group were included due to the technologies' stated visual, auditory, and/or haptic feedback properties. A technology intervention was defined to be one that used a gaming console device to deliver motor skill practice as the therapeutic intervention, for example using the Nintendo Wii Console. Games that did not have a motor skill focus e.g., a focus on cognition such as Brain training for Nintendo Switch, were not included.

2.4 Data extraction and synthesis

Data from all included studies were extracted into a customized Microsoft Excel spreadsheet by one researcher (EW) and a second reviewer (JB) randomly selected and independently reviewed 50% of included studies to ensure no errors were made. Data that were extracted included: study design, participant characteristics (including sample size, mean age, male:female ratio, diagnosis criteria), intervention type (including type of feedback, duration and frequency, adherence to protocol), comparators, outcomes assessed (motor skill outcome) and results. There was not sufficient homogeneity within the included studies (interventions, outcomes, or feedback type) to conduct a meta-analysis, hence, data were synthesised narratively.

2.5 Quality assessment

The Cochrane Collaboration's revised tool was used to assess risk of bias in randomized controlled trials, with additional considerations for crossover trials (40). Additionally, the Cochrane risk of bias in non-randomized studies—of interventions (ROBINS-I)—was used to critically appraise the remaining non-randomized included studies (41). For each tool, studies were rated as having high, low, or unclear risk of bias in each of the relevant categories. Two researchers (EW, JB) critically appraised each included study.

3 Results

3.1 Study selection

The database search yielded a total of 1,924 results. Four hundred and ninety-five duplicates were removed. After title and abstract screening, 309 full texts were retrieved and independently reviewed by two researchers. A total of 14 published articles from 13 unique trials were included in this review (Figure 3). One trial reported different outcomes in two separate papers, therefore, both articles were included in this review (42, 43).

Figure 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. PRISMA flow diagram of included studies.

3.2 Study characteristics

Six RCTs, represented in seven articles (4248), four randomized crossover design trials (4952) and three quasi-experimental designs (31, 53, 54) were included in the systematic review. Eight trials assessed internal FOA compared to external FOA, one trial assessed KOR and the remaining four trials were included due to providing augmented feedback delivered via technology in the intervention group, which was tested against traditional therapy.

Data from a combined 392 children with DCD (mean age 5–12 years, mean individual study sample size 5–91) were included. Two articles did not provide the mean age of participants (44, 51), however, ages of participants were restricted by inclusion criteria to between seven and twelve years of age. Six trials used a version of the diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM), relevant to the year the studies were conducted, to confirm a diagnosis of DCD. Two trials used the DSM-IV (31, 44) and four trials used the DSM-V (42, 43, 5254). The remaining seven trials did not state the use of a diagnostic manual for the confirmation of a DCD diagnosis (4551). Eleven trials used the MABC-2 assessment to diagnose a motor impairment (DSM-V criterion A), where participants were included if they scored ≤16th-20th percentile (31, 4249, 5254). The remaining two trials used other measures to assess criteria for DCD (50, 51). Five trials did not exclude participants based on an indication of ADHD or hyperactivity (31, 46, 50, 53, 54).

While all included articles reported motor learning and/or performance outcomes pre and post intervention, only four articles conducted follow up assessments post intervention, at 48 h, 6–8 weeks and 3 months respectively (4648, 53). Three of the fourteen articles reported on levels of participant adherence to the prescribed interventions (31, 42, 45) with all reporting adherence of above 80%.

Six of the included trials provided an intervention that was comparable to a traditional therapeutic intervention directed by a therapist (31, 42, 43, 46, 48, 50, 53). The remaining seven trials completed interventions that followed a strict protocol to perform a motor skill (44, 45, 47, 49, 51, 52, 54). Intervention dosage and frequency of included multi-session trials, differed significantly, ranging from 10 to 60 min in duration, over 3–12 weeks for controlled trials. Table 2 lists the characteristics of the individual studies.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Study characteristics.

3.2.1 Quality of included studies: risk of bias

The risk of bias assessment is presented in Figure 4. All but three articles (42, 43) had domains that were either unclear or had a high risk of bias. Four of the RCT papers were poorly reported and consequently, the risk of bias was unclear in several domains (4447). Poor methodology in all four crossover trials resulted in a moderate-high risk of bias (4952). The non-randomized trials revealed unclear reporting of methods with some concerns about the fidelity of the interventions provided, however, most other domains revealed a low risk of bias (31, 53, 54). Due to poor reporting of methods and results, authors of three included articles were contacted, with no response received.

Figure 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Risk of bias for individual studies. (A) RCT's, (B) crossover trials, (C) non-randomized trials.

3.2.2 Intervention duration and intensity of included studies

Four of the eight trials that explored internal FOA and external FOA were single session protocols (45, 46, 51, 52), with only one trial including a follow up retention session (46). The remaining four trial interventions ranged between 4 and 12 weeks, consisting of 4–12 sessions (4749, 53, 54).

One trial examined the effect of increased KOR during a 12-week therapy program (53). Both groups received one 30-min session per week consisting of the same treatment, however, the intervention group received increased positive and specific feedback on progress towards achieving participant goals throughout the treatment sessions.

The five trials that investigated feedback delivered by technology varied in intervention duration for the technology groups and traditional training groups. Cavalcante Neto et al. (42, 43) matched group duration and frequency, consisting of 2 × 60-min sessions per week for up to 8 weeks. Bartov et al. (49) also matched group duration and frequency, each group completing eight 20-minute sessions per week for 8 weeks. Ferguson et al. (31) reported the Wii intervention group participated in 3 × 30-min sessions per week for 6 weeks, compared to the neuromotor task training group who completed 2 × 45–60-min sessions per week over 9 weeks. Lastly, Hammond et al. (50) reported the Wii group participated in 3 × 10-min sessions over 4 weeks, compared to the Jump Ahead Program group who participated in 1 × 60-min sessions per week, for 4 weeks. Table 3 lists the outcome measures and conclusions of each included article.

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Outcomes of included studies.

3.2.3 Types of feedback

Eight trials assessed internal FOA compared to external FOA for motor learning and performance (4448, 51, 52, 54). One trial examined the effect of increased KOR during a 12-week therapy program (53).

Five trials, represented in four articles, investigated the effect of augmented feedback delivered via technology compared to traditional training methods (31, 42, 43, 49, 50). Three trials used the Nintendo Wii to augment feedback and one used an iPad to deliver immediate visual feedback (49). Control participants received a no feedback handwriting task (49), task specific training (42, 43), neuromotor task training (31) and the Jump Ahead Program (50).

No other articles were found by this review that investigated other forms of feedback for motor learning or performance for children with DCD.

3.3 Impact of feedback on motor performance and motor learning

3.3.1 Motor performance

Eleven articles reported on the impact of different forms of feedback on motor performance. Seven of these articles investigated an internal FOA compared to an external FOA (4548, 51, 52, 54). Only two of these trials including a “no specific FOA” feedback groups on motor performance (45, 52). Six of these seven articles did not find a significant difference between an internal FOA, external FOA or no FOA on motor performance outcomes, including the single session trials (45, 52), multi session trials (46, 48, 51, 54), and trials that included follow up measures (46, 48). The remaining article found a significant difference between visual gaze direction feedback and no gaze direction for a bimanual wrist coordination task (p = 0.001) (47).

The two single session trials (45, 52) were the only two trials to include a no feedback group, and both reported no difference between internal FOA, external FOA or no direction feedback groups. Interestingly, Li et al. (45) revealed 70.97% of participants in the no direction group, reported adopting internal FOA feedback principles. Although no significant results were found, it may be important to note that Psotta et al. (52), found a trend for benefits of external FOA on jump performance for children with DCD (p = 0.063) compared to internal FOA and no direction groups. Finally, it is noteworthy that the six multi-session articles did not include a “no feedback” group, and that all articles reported improvements in motor performance from baseline (45, 48, 5154).

Noordstar et al. (53) investigated the effect of using increased specific KOR feedback compared to usual care on motor learning outcomes. There was no significant difference reported between groups for motor performance outcomes at the end of the 12-week intervention period. However, it was reported that all participants, regardless of group, improved in their motor performance due to participation in the intervention (p = 0.005), and that this was maintained at a 3-month follow up (p = 0.003).

The remaining three articles investigated the effect of augmented feedback, delivered via technology, on motor performance (31, 42, 50). Ferguson et al. (31) reported an overall improvement in motor performance for both groups (p = <0.01). Cavalcante Neto et al. (42) revealed a significant pre-post change for the total standard score (p = <0.01) and for the balance component score (p = <0.001) on the MABC-2 assessment for both groups. Hammond et al. (50) also revealed a significant time effect for motor performance in both groups (p = <0.02). Altogether, no significant differences were reported between intervention methods, but again, all three articles revealed overall significant improvements from baseline.

3.3.2 Motor learning

Three articles reported on internal FOA and external FOA for motor learning (44, 46, 48). Additionally, two articles reported on augmented feedback delivered via technology for motor learning (43). All five articles were included as assessing motor learning if the authors stated motor learning as the outcome. All five articles completed outcome assessments for skill acquisition and retention. Interestingly, only two of the five articles reported using skill acquisition, retention, and transfer tests to determine motor learning effects (44, 49).

All three articles that investigated internal FOA compared to external FOA showed that there was no difference between the two methods for motor learning outcomes (44, 46, 48). Two articles that included a follow up assessment reported significant results for maintenance of motor skills on standardized quantitative outcome measures for both groups, suggesting acquisition and maintenance of the learned motor skill despite type of feedback modality (46, 48). Wood et al. (48), revealed a significant improvement in qualitative catching scores for the quiet eye training (QET) group at follow up (p = 0.032), revealing the QET group (external FOA) were subjectively observed to have significantly improved their catching technique on delayed retention, whereas the traditional training group (internal FOA) had not. Additionally, Miles et al. (46), who also used the QET training method, revealed a trend for significance for the external FOA group for qualitative catching scores at delayed retention (p = 0.068).

Bartov et al. (49) revealed a significant time × condition interaction (p = 0.001) in favour of the immediate visual feedback group with handwriting, however, we note that there was a significant difference at baseline between the feedback and no feedback group, which when corrected for, revealed no significant differences between groups. The remaining article investigated the effect of the Nintendo Wii compared to task specific training for motor learning in children with DCD (43). No significant difference between groups for motor learning was reported. However, the authors stated the Wii training group showed the highest percentage change in phase 1 to phase 3 for frisbee and bowling, compared to the task specific training group.

4 Discussion

The aim of this systematic review was to investigate the effect of different forms of feedback, compared to other forms of feedback, or no additional feedback, for motor learning and/or performance in children with DCD. Interestingly, eight of the fourteen included articles used either internal or external FOA as feedback during intervention. The remaining articles reported on KOR (one) and intervention delivered via technology (five). This review found that the use of any feedback training strategy (internal FOA, external FOA, KOR, augmented feedback, and no additional feedback), with any intervention (traditional or technology), appeared beneficial to improve motor outcomes in children with DCD. These findings are consistent with previous research that indicates any kind of training is better than none (9, 55). This review also found that augmented feedback delivered by technology may provide equivalent motor skill outcomes as traditional training methods for children with DCD.

4.1 Feedback for motor learning and performance

Motor performance appeared to benefit from internal FOA, external FOA, KOR, no additional feedback and augmented feedback modalities (31, 42, 4548, 5054). Specifically, visual feedback delivered by QET, appeared to be indicative of a better performance with a specific motor task for children with DCD. This result should be interpreted with caution as only one of three articles using the QET method found a statistical significance between groups (47). Additionally, this review found that no individual article reported statistically significant differences in their sample between internal or external FOA or augmented feedback delivered by technology for motor learning outcomes for children with DCD.

An interesting finding of this review was that only three articles included a “no feedback” group (45, 52). Two of the trials investigated motor performance and were a single session design. Within the trials, participants only completed three and five repetitions of the task and neither trial found a difference between internal FOA, external FOA or no additional feedback groups. The remaining article investigated motor learning and compared external visual feedback to no additional feedback (49). Authors revealed a significant decrease in writing duration, and an improvement with spatial features of the letters for both groups, indicating no difference between additional external feedback and no feedback for motor learning in handwriting. All other multi-session trials did not include a “no feedback” control group and/or included a comparison to a TD group (which was not the focus of this review). From the findings of this review, only one multi-session intervention trial exists where a form of specific feedback is compared to no additional feedback, making it impossible to form a conclusion about the use of a specific form of feedback provided during therapeutic intervention. It appears that all participants who participate in a motor skill intervention, regardless of type of feedback or intervention completed, improved their motor performance outcomes.

Some evidence proposes that children with DCD have increased difficulty with internal feedback information (sensory perception), due to poorer acquisition, retention, and transfer under implicit conditions (unconscious automatic learning), than their traditional developing peers, which may result in less “learning by experience” (20, 44, 56). One hypothesis is that due to the proposed difficulty with implicit learning (44), and potentially limited experience with motor tasks, children with DCD may benefit from a greater focus on an internal approach to encourage the automatic update of feedforward control to enhance motor skill learning outcomes (16). Contrary to this, some researchers suggest that a focus on controlling one's movements (internal feedback) may suppress the automatic update of the motor plan. Therefore it is proposed that an external method to feedback may encourage the implicit learning process by taking the focus away from specific movements of the body, therefore promoting unconscious learning (16, 57, 58). Interestingly, in a single session design, Li et al. (45) revealed that 70.97% of DCD participants in the “no feedback” group reported spontaneously adopting an internal FOA method to assist them to perform the movement, however, no differences were found in this review between internal or external FOA. No other studies that included a “no feedback” group reported on the participants approach to feedback during a task. An external approach to feedback has been shown to be more beneficial for motor learning in adults, however as this review has shown, limited and variable research exists for the DCD population. This review found very limited evidence to detect a difference between forms of feedback for motor learning or performance for children with DCD.

Within traditional therapeutic methods, therapists aim to provide external feedback to supplement naturally occurring task-intrinsic (sensory) information to improve the learner's motor performance, and to consequently encourage increased experience of the skill to improve motor learning. Typically, this is delivered by KOR and KOP. This review only found one article that investigated the effect of enhanced KOR on motor performance for children with DCD (53). No significant improvement with KOR was found, suggesting this additional external feedback, provided during a traditional motor skill program, did not offer benefits for motor performance beyond traditional training for children with DCD (53). This finding should be considered with care due to the limited evidence found and the methods employed in the included trial. Therapists who participated in the usual-care group, although blinded to the true outcomes of the trial, were not guided by standardized protocols, nor were their therapeutic methods collected, meaning it is highly possible additional feedback was also provided in the usual-care group. Additionally, therapists delivering the enhanced KOR were not guided by feedback schedules nor required to report the level of feedback provided. This finding is surprising, as the benefits of KOR and KOP are well documented as being beneficial for motor learning and performance in other populations, however, we did not find sufficient articles that focused on these forms of feedback for the DCD population. Additionally, no trials were found that investigated KOR and/or KOP for motor learning. More research is required, which employs standardized protocols and reporting, to investigate the influence of KOR and/or KOP for motor learning and performance for children with DCD.

Despite the importance of feedback during intervention tasks, none of the included articles reported on feedback frequency and intensity. Most articles reported the use of a feedback schedule in their methods; however, this was not explored further. Existing literature proposes the importance of feedback frequency during intervention to improve motor skills. It has been shown that for TD children, a lower frequency of feedback (33%) was more beneficial for motor skill learning on a simple task, however a higher frequency (100%) of feedback was more beneficial for a complex task (24). In children with CP, a moderate frequency of feedback (50%–62%) has been shown to be more beneficial during tasks, however, this may be largely influenced by task complexity and individual preferences (23). These findings appear consistent with children with DCD, where more feedback and repetitive practice may be required to achieve similar motor skill outcomes as their typically developing peers, and that task type and complexity may significantly alter acquisition and retention of motor skills (13, 14, 25). Additionally, literature suggests self-controlled feedback scheduling may be a good way to facilitate improved motor learning across all age groups, including in children (59, 60). Surprisingly, this review did not find any articles that reported on these forms of feedback scheduling for children with DCD. Articles included in this review did not have adequate reporting of feedback frequency to determine its effect on motor learning or performance for children with DCD and is an important consideration for future trials.

4.2 Augmented feedback delivered via technology vs. traditional training

Despite the positive applications and outcomes seen in children and adults with motor impairments from the use of virtual and augmented reality systems, this review only identified four trials, represented in five articles, that investigated the effect of augmented feedback via technology for children with DCD (31, 42, 43, 50). Promisingly, results identified that the use of technology interventions to deliver augmented feedback appear to be equivalent to traditional therapy, suggesting that technology could be beneficial as part of a therapeutic program from children with DCD. Technology may introduce options to train with or without a therapist and/or with greater intensity at home or in the clinic. Additionally, it may provide a platform that individuals can readily identify with and be used as a tool to enhance motivation within therapy. Positive effects on motor function using technology have been evidenced for other clinical and non-clinical populations, such as in cerebral palsy (33). This may support a rationale for the potential benefits for children with DCD who may share similar internal feedback processing challenges.

Current clinical practice recommendations suggest that children with DCD need better ways to self-evaluate their performance (9, 12), and instant or concurrent feedback of a person's movement pattern via technology has been popular due to its ability to provide enhanced multisensory feedback (auditory, visual, haptic). However, evidence is still mixed about the true effects of this style of feedback on motor learning in all populations (61). While results are promising for the use of technology in this review, three of the four trials rated a moderate-high risk of bias (31, 49, 50). These studies were heterogeneous in nature, with mixed methods differing significantly in their motor skills training, duration, and frequency of interventions between the Wii and traditional training intervention groups (31, 50). Only two trials matched the motor skills being learnt between intervention groups (42, 43, 49). Further research needs to be conducted to investigate the influence of augmented feedback on motor learning and performance in all populations and better-quality evidence needs to be conducted for the use of technology in the DCD population.

4.3 Limitations

This review was completed using a narrative synthesis due to the low quality and heterogeneity of the included trials, including low sample sizes, low quality reporting of intervention, variability of study designs, interventions and outcome measures used. Diagnostic criteria and motor skill outcome measures for DCD varied between trials. Trials that did not use the DSM-IV or DSM-V to make a diagnosis of DCD or probable DCD were not excluded from this review. Additionally, reporting of co-morbidities was poor, making some data at risk of being confounded and reducing the reliability or validity of outcomes. This may mean that some of the data cannot be generalized to the DCD population alone due to a mixed sample. Included trials varied significantly in motor skills and interventions used, and surprisingly, most articles had low quality of reporting intervention and outcome measures. Trials also varied in intervention frequency, intensity and duration, and the risk of bias was found to be moderate to high for the majority of included studies. Only four of the included trials included a follow up to the intervention, meaning it cannot be concluded whether reported effects remain long term. The potential inclusion of the above-mentioned factors means that we cannot confidently draw a conclusion on the effect of different forms of feedback for motor learning or performance for children with DCD.

4.4 Implications and recommendations for future research

This review revealed there is a limited amount of high-quality research investigating the influence of different forms of feedback on motor learning and motor performance for children with DCD. We identified several gaps in the literature that require investigation for the DCD population. Further high-quality research needs to be conducted to investigate the best feedback modality, frequency, and approach options for the DCD population (62). Future studies should be appropriately powered, ensure motor skills being learnt are consistent between the intervention and comparator groups, and include appropriate acquisition, retention, and transfer assessments to determine how different forms of feedback may affect motor learning and performance in children with DCD. A “no feedback” comparator group should also be included to determine the true effects of feedback during intervention across different tasks. Studies also need to ensure they have clear aims and standardized method of delivering different forms of feedback, and at what frequency, within interventions. Future trials also need to include a standard method for classifying DCD, such as using the DSM-V criteria.

5 Conclusion

It is well known that children with DCD who engage in therapeutic interventions have significantly better motor performance outcomes than those who do not (10, 55). However, limited evidence exists about the effect of an internal or external approach to feedback during an intervention and its influence on motor learning and performance outcomes. This review identified that both internal and external FOA and KOR during an intervention were beneficial for motor learning and performance outcomes for children with DCD, with no superior effect for one method over the other. It was also found that interventions delivered via the use of technology, which delivered augmented feedback, were equivalent to traditional therapy for motor learning and performance outcomes. It appears that any intervention is better than no intervention, and that technology may be a promising avenue to deliver more independent interventions of therapeutic equivalence to traditional therapy for children with DCD. More research is required to clarify the role of feedback in DCD interventions.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Author contributions

EW: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal Analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. BH: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal Analysis, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. DH: Conceptualization, Investigation, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. JB: Data curation, Formal Analysis, Methodology, Writing – review & editing. EW: Conceptualization, Investigation, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. SH: Formal Analysis, Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

EJW is a recipient of a Research Training Program domestic (RTPd) stipend as a postgraduate student at the university of South Australia.

Acknowledgments

We would like to acknowledge the academic librarians at the University of South Australia who assisted in the development and execution of the search strategy for this review.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fped.2024.1327445/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Harrowell I, Hollén L, Lingam R, Emond A. The impact of developmental coordination disorder on educational achievement in secondary school. Res Dev Disabil. (2018) 72:13–22. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2017.10.014

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Kirby A, Sugden DA. Children with developmental coordination disorders. J R Soc Med. (2007) 100(4):182–6. doi: 10.1177/014107680710011414

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Lingam R, Jongmans MJ, Ellis M, Hunt LP, Golding J, Emond A. Mental health difficulties in children with developmental coordination disorder. Pediatrics. (2012) 129(4):e882–891. doi: 10.1542/peds.2011-1556

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Zwicker JG, Harris SR, Klassen AF. Quality of life domains affected in children with developmental coordination disorder: a systematic review. Child Care Health Dev. (2013) 39(4):562–80. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2214.2012.01379.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Caçola P. Physical and mental health of children with developmental coordination disorder. Front Public Health. (2016) 4:224–224. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2016.00224

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Karras HC, Morin DN, Gill K, Izadi-Najafabadi S, Zwicker JG. Health-related quality of life of children with developmental coordination disorder. Res Dev Disabil. (2019) 84:85–95. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2018.05.012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Missiuna C, Cairney J, Pollock N, Campbell W, Russell DJ, Macdonald K, et al. Psychological distress in children with developmental coordination disorder and attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Res Dev Disabil. (2014) 35(5):1198–207. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2014.01.007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Missiuna C, Campbell WN. Psychological aspects of developmental coordination disorder: can we establish causality? Curr Dev Disord Rep. (2014) 1(2):125–31. doi: 10.1007/s40474-014-0012-8

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Blank R, Barnett AL, Cairney J, Green D, Kirby A, Polatajko H, et al. International clinical practice recommendations on the definition, diagnosis, assessment, intervention, and psychosocial aspects of developmental coordination disorder. Dev Med Child Neurol. (2019) 61(3):242–85. doi: 10.1111/dmcn.14132

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Smits-Engelsman BC, Blank R, van der Kaay AC, Mosterd-van der Meijs R, Vlugt-van den Brand E, Polatajko HJ, et al. Efficacy of interventions to improve motor performance in children with developmental coordination disorder: a combined systematic review and meta-analysis. Dev Med Child Neurol. (2013) 55(3):229–37. doi: 10.1111/dmcn.12008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Zhang D, Soh KG, Chan YM, Zaremohzzabieh Z. Effect of intervention programs to promote fundamental motor skills among typically developing children: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Child Youth Serv Rev. (2024) 156:107320. doi: 10.1016/j.childyouth.2023.107320

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Miyahara M, Hillier SL, Pridham L, Nakagawa S. Task-oriented interventions for children with developmental co-ordination disorder. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. (2017) 7(7):Cd010914. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD010914.pub2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Biotteau M, Chaix Y, Albaret J-M. What do we really know about motor learning in children with developmental coordination disorder? Curr Dev Disord Rep. (2016) 3(2):152–60. doi: 10.1007/s40474-016-0084-8

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Wilson PH, Smits-Engelsman B, Caeyenberghs K, Steenbergen B, Sugden D, Clark J, et al. Cognitive and neuroimaging findings in developmental coordination disorder: new insights from a systematic review of recent research. Dev Med Child Neurol. (2017) 59(11):1117–29. doi: 10.1111/dmcn.13530

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Seidler RD, Noll DC, Thiers G. Feedforward and feedback processes in motor control. NeuroImage. (2004) 22(4):1775–83. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.05.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Emanuel M, Jarus T, Bart O. Effect of focus of attention and age on motor acquisition, retention, and transfer: a randomized trial. Phys Ther. (2008) 88(2):251–60. doi: 10.2522/ptj.20060174

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Sharma DA, Chevidikunnan MF, Khan FR, Gaowgzeh RA. Effectiveness of knowledge of result and knowledge of performance in the learning of a skilled motor activity by healthy young adults. J Phys Ther Sci. (2016) 28(5):1482–6. doi: 10.1589/jpts.28.1482

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Winstein CJ. Knowledge of results and motor learning–implications for physical therapy. Phys Ther. (1991) 71(2):140–9. doi: 10.1093/ptj/71.2.140

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Zwicker JG, Harris SR. A reflection on motor learning theory in pediatric occupational therapy practice. Can J Occup Ther. (2009) 76(1):29–37. doi: 10.1177/000841740907600108

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Wilson PH, Adams ILJ, Caeyenberghs K, Thomas P, Smits-Engelsman B, Steenbergen B. Motor imagery training enhances motor skill in children with DCD: a replication study. Res Dev Disabil. (2016) 57:54–62. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2016.06.014

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Fujii S, Lulic T, Chen JL. More feedback is better than less: learning a novel upper limb joint coordination pattern with augmented auditory feedback. Front Neurosci. (2016) 10:251. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2016.00251

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Moinuddin A, Goel A, Sethi Y. The role of augmented feedback on motor learning: a systematic review. Cureus. (2021) 13(11):e19695. doi: 10.7759/cureus.19695

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Schoenmaker J, Houdijk H, Steenbergen B, Reinders-Messelink HA, Schoemaker MM. Effectiveness of different extrinsic feedback forms on motor learning in children with cerebral palsy: a systematic review. Disabil Rehabil. (2023) 45(8):1271–84. doi: 10.1080/09638288.2022.2060333

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Sidaway B, Bates J, Occhiogrosso B, Schlagenhaufer J, Wilkes D. Interaction of feedback frequency and task difficulty in children’s motor skill learning. Phys Ther. (2012) 92(7):948–57. doi: 10.2522/ptj.20110378

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Sullivan KJ, Kantak SS, Burtner PA. Motor learning in children: feedback effects on skill acquisition. Phys Ther. (2008) 88(6):720–32. doi: 10.2522/ptj.20070196

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Wulf G, Chiviacowsky S, Schiller E, Ávila L. Frequent external focus feedback enhances motor learning. Front Psychol. (2010) 1:190. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00190

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Seidler RD, Kwak Y, Fling BW, Bernard JA. Neurocognitive mechanisms of error-based motor learning. Adv Exp Med Biol. (2013) 782:39–60. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4614-5465-6_3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Shadmehr R, Smith MA, Krakauer JW. Error correction, sensory prediction, and adaptation in motor control. Annu Rev Neurosci. (2010) 33(1):89–108. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-060909-153135

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Dunford C. Goal-orientated group intervention for children with developmental coordination disorder. Phys Occup Ther Pediatr. (2011) 31(3):288–300. doi: 10.3109/01942638.2011.565864

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Bonney E, Ferguson G, Smits-Engelsman B. The efficacy of two activity-based interventions in adolescents with developmental coordination disorder [with consumer summary]. Res Dev Disabil. (2017) 71:223–36. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2017.10.013

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Ferguson GD, Jelsma D, Jelsma J, Smits-Engelsman BC. The efficacy of two task-orientated interventions for children with developmental coordination disorder: neuromotor task training and nintendo wii fit training. Res Dev Disabil. (2013) 34(9):2449–61. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2013.05.007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Smits-Engelsman BC, Jelsma LD, Ferguson GD, Geuze RH. Motor learning: an analysis of 100 trials of a ski slalom game in children with and without developmental coordination disorder. PLoS One. (2015) 10(10):e0140470. Available online at: https://access.library.unisa.edu.au/login?url=http://ovidsp.ovid.com/ovidweb.cgi?T=JS&CSC=Y&NEWS=N&PAGE=fulltext&D=med12&AN=26466324 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0140470

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Chen Y, Fanchiang HD, Howard A. Effectiveness of virtual reality in children with cerebral palsy: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Phys Ther. (2018) 98(1):63–77. doi: 10.1093/ptj/pzx107

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Duckworth J, Bayliss JD, Thomas PR, Shum D, Mumford N, Wilson PH. Tabletop Computer Game Mechanics for Group Rehabilitation of Individuals with Brain Injury. In A. M. Stephanidis C. (Ed.), Universal Access in Human-Computer Interaction. Universal Access to Information and Knowledge. Vol. 8514. Cham: Springer International Publishing (2014). p. 501–12.

Google Scholar

35. Green D, Wilson P. Applications of VR technologies for childhood disability. In: Weiss PL, Keshner EA, Levin MF, United States of America: Springer (2014). p. 203–16.

Google Scholar

36. Green D, Wilson PH. Use of virtual reality in rehabilitation of movement in children with hemiplegia–a multiple case study evaluation. Disabil Rehabil. (2012) 34(7):593–604. doi: 10.3109/09638288.2011.613520

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Lee CM, Bo J. Error argumentation enhance adaptability in adults with low motor ability. J Mot Behav. (2016) 48(4):297–308. doi: 10.1080/00222895.2015.1092937

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Mumford NHP, Duckworth JP, Wilson PHP. The application of enhanced virtual environments for co-located childhood movement disorder rehabilitation. Paper presented at the Proceedings of the 10th International Conference on Disability, Virtual Reality and Associated Technologies (ICDVRAT 2014), United Kingdom: University of Reading (2014).

Google Scholar

39. Page MJ, McKenzie JE, Bossuyt PM, Boutron I, Hoffmann TC, Mulrow CD, et al. The PRISMA 2020 statement: an updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. Br Med J. (2021) 372:n71. doi: 10.1136/bmj.n71

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Sterne JAC, Savović J, Page MJ, Elbers RG, Blencowe NS, Boutron I, et al. Rob 2: a revised tool for assessing risk of bias in randomised trials. Br Med J. (2019) 366:l4898. doi: 10.1136/bmj.l4898

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Sterne JAC, Hernan MA, Reeves BC, Savovic J, Berkman ND, Viswanathan M, et al. ROBINS-I: a tool for assessing risk of bias in non-randomised studies of interventions. Br Med J. (2016) 355:i4919. doi: 10.1136/bmj.i4919

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Cavalcante Neto JL, Steenbergen B, Wilson P, Zamuner AR, Tudella E. Is wii-based motor training better than task-specific matched training for children with developmental coordination disorder? A randomized controlled trial. Disabil Rehabil. (2020) 42(18):2611–20. doi: 10.1080/09638288.2019.1572794

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Cavalcante Neto JL, Steenbergen B, Zamunér AR, Tudella E. Wii training versus non-Wii task-specific training on motor learning in children with developmental coordination disorder: A randomized controlled trial. Ann Phys Rehabil Med. (2021) 64(2):101390. doi: 10.1016/j.rehab.2020.03.013

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Jarus T, Ghanouni P, Abel RL, Fomenoff SL, Lundberg J, Davidson S, et al. Effect of internal versus external focus of attention on implicit motor learning in children with developmental coordination disorder. Res Dev Disabil. (2015) 37:119–26. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2014.11.009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Li LL, Li YC, Chu CH, Pan CY, Chen FC. External focus of attention concurrently elicits optimal performance of suprapostural pole-holding task and postural stability in children with developmental coordination disorder. Neurosci Lett. (2019) 703:32–7. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2019.03.011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Miles CAL, Wood G, Vine SJ, Vickers JN, Wilson MR. Quiet eye training facilitates visuomotor coordination in children with developmental coordination disorder. Res Dev Disabil. (2015) 40:31–41. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2015.01.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Norouzi Seyed Hosseini R, Norouzi E, Soleymani M. Effects of quiet eye training on performance of bimanual coordination in children with DCD. Iran J Child Neurol. (2021) 15(4):43–54. doi: 10.22037/ijcn.v15i4.18926

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Wood G, Miles CA, Coyles G, Alizadehkhaiyat O, Vine SJ, Vickers JN, et al. A randomized controlled trial of a group-based gaze training intervention for children with developmental coordination disorder. PLoS One. (2017) 12(2):e0171782. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0171782

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Bartov R., Wagner M., Shvalb N., Hochhauser M. (2023). Enhancing handwriting performance of children with developmental coordination disorder (DCD) using computerized visual feedback. Children, 10(9), 1534. doi: 10.3390/children10091534

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Hammond J, Jones V, Hill EL, Green D, Male I. An investigation of the impact of regular use of the wii fit to improve motor and psychosocial outcomes in children with movement difficulties: a pilot study. Child Care Health Dev. (2014) 40(2):165–75. doi: 10.1111/cch.12029

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Khatab SF, Ghasemi A, Sadati SKM. The effect of focus instructions on dart throwing performance in children with and without developmental coordination disorder. Ann Appl Sport Sci. (2018) 6(2):55–60. doi: 10.29252/aassjournal.6.2.55

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Psotta R, Abdollahipour R, Janura M. The effects of attentional focus instruction on the performance of a whole-body coordination task in children with developmental coordination disorder. Res Dev Disabil. (2020) 101:103654. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2020.103654

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Noordstar JJ, van der Net J, Voerman L, Helders PJ, Jongmans MJ. The effect of an integrated perceived competence and motor intervention in children with developmental coordination disorder. Res Dev Disabil. (2017) 60:162–75. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2016.12.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

54. van Cappellen-van Maldegem SJM, van Abswoude F, Krajenbrink H, Steenbergen B. Motor learning in children with developmental coordination disorder: the role of focus of attention and working memory. Hum Mov Sci. (2018) 62:211–20. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2018.11.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Hillier S. Intervention for children with developmental coordination disorder: a systematic review. Int J Allied Health Sci Pract. (2007) 5:7. doi: 10.46743/1540-580X/2007.1159

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Gheysen F, Van Waelvelde H, Fias W. Impaired visuo-motor sequence learning in developmental coordination disorder. Res Dev Disabil. (2011) 32(2):749–56. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2010.11.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Wulf G, McNevin N, Shea CH. The automaticity of complex motor skill learning as a function of attentional focus. Q J Exp Psychol A. (2001) 54(4):1143–54. doi: 10.1080/713756012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Wulf G, Shea C, Lewthwaite R. Motor skill learning and performance: a review of influential factors. Med Educ. (2010) 44(1):75–84. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2923.2009.03421.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Jimenez-Diaz J, Chaves-Castro K, Morera-Castro M. Effect of self-controlled and regulated feedback on motor skill performance and learning: a meta-analytic study. J Mot Behav. (2021) 53(3):385–98. doi: 10.1080/00222895.2020.1782825

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

60. van der Veer IPA, Verbecque E, Rameckers EAA, Bastiaenen CHG, Klingels K. How can instructions and feedback with external focus be shaped to enhance motor learning in children? A systematic review. PLoS One. (2022) 17(8):e0264873. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0264873

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Wilson PH, Green D, Caeyenberghs K, Steenbergen B, Duckworth J. Integrating new technologies into the treatment of CP and DCD. Curr Dev Disord Rep. (2016) 3:138–51. doi: 10.1007/s40474-016-0083-9

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Miyahara M, Lagisz M, Nakagawa S, Henderson S. Intervention for children with developmental coordination disorder: how robust is our recent evidence? Child Care Health Dev. (2020) 46(4):397–406. doi: 10.1111/cch.12763

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: developmental coordination disorder (DCD), motor performance, motor learning, internal feedback, external feedback, augmented feedback, technology

Citation: Welsby E, Hordacre B, Hobbs D, Bouckley J, Ward E and Hillier S (2024) Evaluating the influence of feedback on motor skill learning and motor performance for children with developmental coordination disorder: a systematic review. Front. Pediatr. 12:1327445. doi: 10.3389/fped.2024.1327445

Received: 25 October 2023; Accepted: 1 April 2024;
Published: 19 April 2024.

Edited by:

Steven Hirschfeld, Uniformed Services University of the Health Sciences, United States

Reviewed by:

Giuseppe Battaglia, University of Palermo, Italy
Emmanuel Bonney, University of Minnesota Twin Cities, United States

© 2024 Welsby, Hordacre, Hobbs, Bouckley, Ward and Hillier. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Ellana Welsby ZWxsYW5hLndlbHNieUBteW1haWwudW5pc2EuZWR1LmF1

ORCID Ellana Welsby orcid.org/0000-0002-9901-7800 Brenton Hordacre orcid.org/0000-0002-7129-6684 David Hobbs orcid.org/0000-0003-1164-3899 Joanne Bouckley orcid.org/0000-0001-8988-0849 Emily Ward orcid.org/0000-0002-3177-0820 Susan Hillier orcid.org/0000-0002-6071-6137

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.