Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Pediatr., 07 April 2022
Sec. Children and Health
This article is part of the Research Topic The importance of assessing health status and health behavioral characteristics in children View all 16 articles

Parents' Beliefs and Behaviors About Their Children's Literacy Development: A Cross-Sectional Study in Saudi Arabia

  • Department of Pediatrics, King Faisal Specialist Hospital and Research Center, Riyadh, Saudi Arabia

Background: A home environment that promotes learning is a significant factor in school performance in which children's parents are involved in their education. However, little study has focused on parents' reading-related beliefs or the relationship between parents' literacy beliefs and behaviors.

Methods: This cross-sectional study describes the range of parents' reported literacy beliefs and behaviors and examines whether an association exists in Saudi Arabia. It was conducted on a convenient sample of 100 parents using a web-based self-administrated shared with families in Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics clinics at multiple sites utilizing the Parents' Perceptions of Literacy Learning Interview Schedule (PPLLIS).

Results: There were 83 mothers and 17 fathers; 90% had a university degree or higher. Parents' beliefs ranged between 87 and 123, with a mean of 103.54 ± 8.05, indicating more holistic perceptions than skills-based ones. There was only a positive significant correlation between parents' beliefs and behaviors regarding literacy activities. However, those participating in literacy activities with their kids were more holistic parents and scored the top one-third on the PPLLIS.

Conclusion: Parents' beliefs and their reported behaviors are found to be significantly impacted by their educational levels. Therefore, future investigations and national campaigns are encouraged to improve parents' educational levels, especially in urban areas.

Introduction

Literacy is defined as having the ability to read and write as introduced within a significant context (1). Many techniques to improve literacy skills and enhance the significance of outcomes among children have been previously reported. For instance, exposure to storybooks and reading shared books are some of the commonly studied literacy aspects. However, evidence regarding the efficacy of these modalities in enhancing literacy skills is controversial among studies in the literature (2, 3). Among children, it has been previously reported that early acquisition of literacy skills within the first 5 years of life can significantly enhance the academic performance of these children (4, 5).

On the other hand, it has also been reported that lacking these skills can negatively impact the child's academic performance and significantly persist through adolescence (6, 7). However, parents' involvement in literacy development in their children is a fruitful practice with favorable outcomes on education (8). Leslie et al. (9) previously showed that parents' involvement in children's literacy activities was associated with a significant enhancement in the educational outcomes in elementary school children. It has been reported that less involvement with preschool children and reduced supervision periods from the caregivers was associated with the reduced acquisition of essential phenomics and vocabulary skills (10). Parents' beliefs may fluctuate on how children can become literate regarding their study and literacy education knowledge. Previous evidence revealed that when parents were questioned about their beliefs about learning, reading, and writing, it mainly occurred when children were already in regular education (11, 12).

The role of the family has become of great importance in educating preschool children, almost equivalent to the educational services offered at schools. However, little study has focused on parents' reading-related beliefs or the relationship between parents' literacy beliefs and behaviors. Understanding parents' beliefs about literacy is vital in identifying the children's home environment and activities parents engage in. In Saudi Arabia, reports show that many families have recently begun to be interested in enrolling their children in kindergarten at least 1 year earlier, despite being non-mandatory at that age group (13). However, no previous studies have been conducted in Saudi Arabia to assess parents' beliefs and attitudes toward literacy enhancement practices. Therefore, this study describes the range of parents' reported literacy beliefs and behaviors and examines whether an association exists in Saudi Arabia.

Methods

Study Design and Data Collection

This cross-sectional study was conducted on a convenient sample of 100 parents using a web-based self-administrated shared with families at Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics clinics in various private or governmental institutes around Saudi Arabia. Initially, the sample size was obtained through cluster sampling methods with an expected response rate of 90% and a sample size of 145. However, only 100 parents were involved in the final study after applying the inclusion and exclusion criteria. Inclusion criteria were all the children attending the behavioral pediatric clinics between March and June 2021 and who are regularly following up while those who are not known to regularly follow-up in the clinic or have an incomplete file were excluded. Data were collected by medical student volunteers from secondary and tertiary hospitals between March and June of 2021. All parents consenting to undertake the survey were included, and couples with no children were excluded. The survey utilized the Parents' Perceptions of Literacy Learning Interview Schedule (PPLLIS) (14, 15) questionnaire, and answers were collected in an excel sheet. This tool evaluated parents' beliefs on how children learn to read and write and gave information on the varieties of literacy exercises parents are involved in with their kids. Given the responses, parents' perceptions of literacy were described as more emergent or more traditional. We modified the PPLLIS score by excluding three questions (items 3, 4, and 27) (Supplementary Material 1). All the questions were scored on a 5-point Likert scale (strongly agree to strongly disagree), with a greater score given for answers that represented a more emergent or holistic literacy perspective (strongly agree scored 1 and strongly disagree 5 except for questions: 1, 2, 3, 6, 7, 8, 16, 17, 19, 20, 22, 23, 24, 25, 26, and 28 where strongly agree scored 5 and strongly disagree scored 1). Scores were calculated by summing the raw scores, which means the lowest possible score on the questionnaire is 30 and the highest possible score is 150.

One question included in the questionnaire to assess “parents' beliefs” was asking the parents to name the five most important ways they help their children learn to read and write. The responses were scored based on the frequency and were categorized into five categories based on the answers. The first category: teaching literacy skills, included parents' direct attempts to instruct children about reading and writing. Second category: being involved in supporting children's literacy by encouraging and valuing children's literacy development. Third category: participating in literacy activities included literacy events in which parents and children collaboratively participated. Fourth category: knowledge development had parents' attempts to promote children's general intellectual or cognitive development. Fifth category: involving those who did not fit any of the previous categories.

Statistical Analysis

Reports were collected, then coded and revised, and data were introduced on statistical software IBM SPSS version 26.0. All the statistical analysis was done using two-tailed tests and an alpha error of 0.05. A P-value < 0.05 was considered to be statistically significant. Frequencies and percentages were used to describe the distribution of items and scale. Finally, the questions were assessed and scored. The Cronbach's alpha test was used for overall ranking and objects and the Cronbach's alpha coefficient of more than 0.70 was considered acceptable. In this study, the Cronbach's alpha was 0.682, indicating an average internal consistency for our scale with this specific sample.

To facilitate data analysis, the included parents were grouped based on their PPLLIS score. Out of the 100 parents that completed the questionnaire, 32 parents scored the lowest on the PPLLIS and were placed in group 1. The following 33 parents whose scores fell in the middle were placed in group 2. Meanwhile, the 35 parents whose holistic beliefs were high and scored in the top third on the PPLLIS were placed in group 3. Finally, the groups were as follows; group 1: more skills-based group with the lowest scores, group 2: combination group of both the skills-oriented and holistic beliefs, in comparison to the other groups, group 3: more holistic orientation to literacy learning with the highest score.

The Shapiro–Wilk test was used to verify the normal distribution of continuous variables. For normally distributed variables, the ANOVA test was used to examine whether parents' education was examined differed in parents' beliefs. The Pearson's chi-squared was used to assess the differences between categorical variables. The Fisher's exact test was used in the cases where the conditions were not satisfied (≥20% of the expected values are <5 and the minimum is <1). In addition, partial correlations were used to examine relationships between parents' beliefs and their behaviors, controlling for children's age (15). The control for the age was done because parents' behaviors may vary with children's age (16).

Results

Participant's Characteristics

Overall, 100 participants completed the data. Table 1 shows participants' sociodemographic characteristics. There were 83 mothers and 17 fathers involved in this study and most of them were from the eastern province of Saudi Arabia (78%). Their education level ranged from high school completion to doctorate degrees, with 90% having a university degree and higher education. On average, the children in this study had the older sibling's mean age of 12.33 ± 7.75 years, while the younger sibling was 4.49 ± 3.82 years. In addition, 66 participants spoke Arabic in the house, while 34% of parents spoke another language.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Socio-demographic and educational characteristics of the participants.

Parent's Beliefs

Table 2 describes the overall participants' scores. Parents' beliefs ranged between 87 and 123, with a mean of 103.54 ± 8.05, indicating more holistic perceptions than skills-based ones (a higher score). The following five statements on the PPLLIS were those on which parents scored the lowest: real reading begins only when a child starts to say the words as they are printed on the page, a child can begin to write before she has learned the correct spelling of the terms, a child can begin to write (e.g., notes and stories) before she knows how to read, only gifted children learn to read and write before receiving formal instruction in preschool or elementary school, schools should be responsible for teaching children to learn to read and write, and children have to be of a certain age before they can begin to learn to read and write.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. The Parents' Beliefs of Literacy Learning Interview Schedule (PPLLIS) score.

Parents scored highest on the following five statements of the PPLLIS: a child benefits from hearing favorite stories that she has memorized read, again and again, you are helping a child learn to read by encouraging her to discuss what is being read, talking to them, having them pretend to write grocery lists with you, and reading to them. However, when testing the associations between the PPLLIS score and different sociodemographic characteristics, none of the variables was significantly associated with parents; perceptions, as shown in Table 3.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Associations between PPLLIS and different socio-demographic characteristics.

Parents' Behaviors

In the questionnaire, parents were asked about the most important things to help the child learn to read and write. We categorized the answers into five categories, each containing different responses, as shown in Table 4. There was a significant relationship between parents' total score on the PPLLIS and parents' participation in literacy activities (N = 62, P = 0.002). This suggests that those participating in literacy activities with their kids were more holistic parents, scoring in the top one-third on the PPLLIS, as shown in Table 5.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. The Parents' Behaviors of Literacy Learning Interview Schedule (PPLLIS) score.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Partial correlations between parents' behaviors and parents' total score on the PPLLIS.

The division of parents' beliefs into the three groups showed that parents with more skills-based beliefs were less likely to participate in activities grouped in the “participation in literacy activities” category, such as reading to children, playing games with them, and writing with them (Figure 1). Nearly twice as many parents in the groups 2 and 3 reported these activities types compared to the group 1. Within our sample, no clear trends could be identified.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Parents' behaviors and the grouping of parents' beliefs.

Discussion

Our aim in this investigation is to assess the behaviors and beliefs of parents about their children's literacy development in Saudi Arabia. Our results showed that the estimated PPLLIS scores for the included participants were higher, indicating overall holistic beliefs than the overall scores of skills focused on perception. We also found that the place of residency (province) and the high educational status of the parents were the only two significant factors associated with having high PPLLIS scores.

Our results are consistent with the previous investigation by Lynch et al. (16) that showed that higher educational levels were also associated with firmer holistic beliefs. Earlier research by Fitzgerald et al. (17) also reported that having more skill-based beliefs were significantly related to well-educated parents. On the other hand, Stipek et al. (18) reported that parents with higher educational levels were significantly associated with less support for didactic methods, including flashcards. In comparison, the opposite was noticed with parents with lower educational levels. Moreover, according to a previous report by the Organization for Economic Cooperation and Development, the parenteral different socioeconomic factors were not as significant as reading enjoyment in achieving higher educational success rates among children (19). The most reported literacy behaviors were reading stories, talking/discussing with them, and using picture clues grouped in the “Participation in literacy activities” category. In the same context, the current evidence supports graph phonemic (phonics, sounding out words, books with structured vocabulary, and spelling) and constructivist (readers rely on their general knowledge, the language, pictures, and the context of the text) as primary dimensions of beliefs about how children should be taught to read (20).

Our results also showed that participants with the highest scores believed that being more involved with their children in reading, writing, and making shopping lists can enhance literacy according to the PPLLIS score. Senechal and LeFevre previously reported that reading was the most sensitive subject that can be affected by the involvement of parents with their children (21). Furthermore, it has been reported that successful reading practices can, in turn, enhance the educational outcomes of other school subjects (22). Previous investigations have demonstrated that daily activities are significantly associated with many benefits on children in the home, such as language comprehension, reading achievement, more frequent expression of the acquired linguistic skills, and enhanced attitude about reading in schools (23, 24). Besides, it has also been previously reported that being involved with the child's literacy-relevant practices was a more significant predictor for educational success than parenteral family size, social class, and level of education (25). However, we did not find a significant correlation between direct teaching activities or valuing or encouraging literacy practices. On the other hand, Feinstein and Symons reported that achievement and success at 16 years of age were significantly associated with parents' enthusiasm and interest in their children's education and related outcomes (26).

Previous investigations have also reported that the early involvement of parents in literacy practices with their children can significantly enhance the educational outcomes and is usually associated with more lasting effects (27). However, it should be noted that although the early inauguration of parents' involvement with children is essential, it is also necessary that participation in the successful habits should also continue in the latter years, even during the adult and teenage years (27). In addition to the educational outcomes, evidence also shows that parents' involvement with children can also positively impact their emotional status, including more enhanced mental health and self-control, in addition to reduced delinquent behaviors (27, 28). Furthermore, the effect of dynamic interaction between the parents and their children cannot be ignored. Previous investigations reported that mother book reading had a positive association with children's development of early literacy skills (16).

We believe that our findings would imply future national investigations and campaigns that should enhance educational outcomes among both the parents and children. Such campaigns should raise awareness about reading for pleasures practices associated with favorable results on all the education parameters, especially for children in need. Moreover, it has been previously reported that children to parents who value reading are more frequently associated with higher motivations for reading for pleasure and enhanced educational outcomes (26). In the same context, a previous investigation by Baker and Scher also reported that parents who read to entertain their children were more likely to have children who enjoy, value reading, and feel sufficient in their reading activities (29).

It is worth reporting that our findings might be limited. First, the study sample and selection of participants might not adequately represent the general population in Saudi Arabia. Therefore, further studies with larger sample sizes and a random broad selection of the participants are encouraged. Moreover, the lack of temporality associated with the cross-sectional design and the potential social desirability bias is another limitation to this study that future investigations should consider. In addition, the majority of the respondents were mothers which do not reflect the actual population of fathers in the whole population.

Conclusion

Our findings indicate the overall holistic beliefs than the overall scores of skills that focused on the perception among our Saidu population. Parents' beliefs can influence their behaviors toward their children and higher educational levels can significantly influence such outcomes. We also found that parents' participation in literacy activities with their children was correlated considerably with good behavioral scores. For further validation of our evidence, future national studies are encouraged, together with campaigns aiming to increase the degree of awareness among parents about the benefits of reading for joy in enhancing their children's literacy outcomes.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary Material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

Written informed consent was obtained from the individual(s) for the publication of any potentially identifiable images or data included in this article.

Author Contributions

FA contributed to conceptualizing, drafting, data collecting, writing, editing, and rechecking references accordingly.

Conflict of Interest

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

The author would like to thank King Faisal Specialist Hospital in Riyadh's Statistical Department for providing the data analysis.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fped.2022.708217/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Stahl S, Miller PD. Whole language and language experience approaches for beginning reading: a quantitative research synthesis. Rev Educ Res. (1989) 59:116–87. doi: 10.3102/00346543059001087

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Bus A, van Ijzendoorn M, Pellegrini A. Joint book reading makes for success in learning to read: a meta-analysis on intergenerational transmission of literacy. Rev Educ Res. (1995) 65:1–21. doi: 10.3102/00346543065001001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Scarborough HS, Dobrich W. On the efficacy of reading to preschoolers. Dev Rev. (1994) 14:245–302. doi: 10.1006/drev.1994.1010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Goodrich JM, Lonigan CJ, Farver JM. Do early literacy skills in children's first language promote development of skills in their second language? An experimental evaluation of transfer. J Educ Psychol. (2013) 105:414–26. doi: 10.1037/a0031780

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Roberts J, Jurgens J, Burchinal M. The role of home literacy practices in preschool children's language and emergent literacy skills. J Speech Lang Hear Res. (2005) 48:345–59. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2005/024)

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Catts HW, Fey ME, Tomblin JB, Zhang X. A longitudinal investigation of reading outcomes in children with language impairments. J Speech Lang Hear Res. (2002) 45:1142–57. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2002/093)

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Farquharson K. Promoting early literacy for a child in foster care. Perspect ASHA Spec Interest Groups. (2017) 2:117. doi: 10.1044/persp2.SIG1.117

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Ece Demir-Lira Ö, Applebaum LR, Goldin-Meadow S, Levine SC. Parents' early book reading to children: relation to children's later language and literacy outcomes controlling for other parent language input. Dev Sci. (2019) 22:e12764. doi: 10.1111/desc.12764

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Leslie L, Allen L. Factors that predict success in an early literacy intervention project. Read Res Quart. (1999) 34:404–24. doi: 10.1598/RRQ.34.4.2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Rush KL. Caregiver-child interactions and early literacy development of preschool children from low-income environments. Topics Early Child Spec Educ. (1999) 19:3–14. doi: 10.1177/027112149901900101

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Evans M, Fox M, Cremaso L, McKinnon L. Beginning reading: the views of parents and teachers of young children. J Educ Psychol. (2004) 96:130–41. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.96.1.130

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. DeBaryshe BD, Binder JC, Buell MJ. Mothers' implicit theories of early literacy instruction: Implications for children's reading and writing. Early Child Dev Care. (2000) 160:119–31. doi: 10.1080/0030443001600111

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Alqassem R, Doaa D, Alzahrani A. Early childhood education in Saudi Arabia: report. World J Educ. (2016) 6:v6n5p1. doi: 10.5430/wje.v6n5p1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Anderson J. Emergent literacy: the beliefs and practices of parents of preschoolers. In: Annual Conference of the Canadian Society for the Study of Education. Charlottetown, PE (1992).

Google Scholar

15. Lynch J, Anderson J, Anderson A, Shapiro J. Parents' beliefs about young children's literacy development and parents' literacy behaviors. Read Psychol. (2006) 27:1–20. doi: 10.1080/02702710500468708

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Bus AG, van Ijzendoorn MH. Mother-child interactions, attachment, and emergent literacy: a cross-sectional study. Child Dev. (1988) 59:1262–72. doi: 10.2307/1130489

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Fitzgerald J, Spiegel D, Cunningham J. The relationship between parental literacy level and perceptions of emergent literacy. J Lit Res. (1991) 23:191–213. doi: 10.1080/10862969109547736

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Stipek D, Milburn S, Clements D, Daniels DH. Parents' beliefs about appropriate education for young children. J Appl Dev Psychol. (1992) 13:293–310. doi: 10.1016/0193-3973(92)90034-F

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Development OOFEC-OA. Reading for Change: Performance and Engagement Across Countries PISA 20002000. USA.

Google Scholar

20. Hindman A, Skibbe L, Foster T. Exploring the variety of parental talk during shared book reading and its contributions to preschool language and literacy: evidence from the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study-Birth Cohort. Read Writing. (2014) 27:4. doi: 10.1007/s11145-013-9445-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Sénéchal M, LeFevre JA. Parental involvement in the development of children's reading skill: a five-year longitudinal study. Child Dev. (2002) 73:445–60. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00417

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Jordan G, Snow C, Porche M. Project EASE: the effect of a family literacy project on kindergarten students' early literacy skills. Read Res Quart. (2000) 35:524–46. doi: 10.1598/RRQ.35.4.5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Gest SD, Freeman NR, Domitrovich CE, Welsh JA. Shared book reading and children's language comprehension skills: the moderating role of parental discipline practices. Early Child Res Q. (2004) 19:319–36. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2004.04.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Rowe K. The influence of reading activity at home on students' attitudes towards reading, classroom attentiveness and reading achievement: an application of structural equation modelling. Br J Educ Psychol. (2011) 61:19–35. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8279.1991.tb00958.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Flouri E, Buchanan A. Early father's and mother's involvement and child's later educational outcomes. Br J Educ Psychol. (2004) 74:141–53. doi: 10.1348/000709904773839806

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Feinstein L, Symons J. Attainment in secondary school. Oxf Econ Pap. (1999) 51:300–21. doi: 10.1093/oep/51.2.300

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Desforges C. The impact of parental involvement, parental support and family education on pupil achievements and adjustment: a literature review. Education. (2003) 30:1–108. Available online at: https://www.nationalnumeracy.org.uk/sites/default/files/documents/impact_of_parental_involvement/the_impact_of_parental_involvement.pdf

Google Scholar

28. Allen SM Daly KJ Father Involvement Research A University University of G Centre for Families W Well B. The Effects of Father Involvement : An Updated Research Summary of the Evidence (2007).

Google Scholar

29. Baker L, Scher D. Beginning readers' motivation for reading in relation to parental beliefs and home reading experiences. Read Psychol. (2002) 23:239–69. doi: 10.1080/713775283

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: literacy beliefs, Saudi Arabia, literacy behaviors, children literacy, Perceptions of Literacy Learning Interview Schedule

Citation: AlAhmari FS (2022) Parents' Beliefs and Behaviors About Their Children's Literacy Development: A Cross-Sectional Study in Saudi Arabia. Front. Pediatr. 10:708217. doi: 10.3389/fped.2022.708217

Received: 11 May 2021; Accepted: 07 March 2022;
Published: 07 April 2022.

Edited by:

Arlinda Cerga Pashoja, University of London, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Bijaya Kumar Padhi, Post Graduate Institute of Medical Education and Research (PGIMER), India
Achyut Kumar Pandey, Banaras Hindu University, India

Copyright © 2022 AlAhmari. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Fatimah Saeed AlAhmari, ZmF0aW1haHNhJiN4MDAwNDA7aG90bWFpbC5jb20=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.