Skip to main content

MINI REVIEW article

Front. Pediatr., 26 March 2021
Sec. Pediatric Gastroenterology, Hepatology and Nutrition

First Bites—Why, When, and What Solid Foods to Feed Infants

  • Division of Pediatric Gastroenterology, Hepatology and Nutrition, University of Virginia, Charlottesville, VA, United States

Humans are the only mammals who feed our young special complementary foods before weaning and we are the only primates that wean our young before they can forage independently. There appears to be a sensitive period in the first several months of life when infants readily accept a wide variety of tastes and this period overlaps with a critical window for oral tolerance. As a result, infants should be exposed to a wide variety of flavors while mother is pregnant, while mother is nursing and beginning at an early age. There also appears to be a sensitive period between 4 and 9 months when infants are most receptive to different food textures. There remains debate about when it is best to begin introducing solid foods into an infant's diet however, the available evidence suggests that provided the water and food supply are free of contamination, and the infant is provided adequate nutrition, there are no clear contraindications to feeding infants complementary foods at any age. There is emerging evidence that introduction of solid foods into an infant's diet by 4 months may increase their willingness to eat a variety of fruits and vegetables later in life, decrease their risk of having feeding problems later in life, and decrease their risk of developing food allergies, and the early introduction of solid foods into an infant's diet does not appear to increase their risk of obesity later in childhood.

Introduction

Humans are the only mammals that feed our young complementary foods before weaning and the only primates that wean our offspring before they can forage independently (1). The human anomaly of early weaning is likely due to our slow post-natal growth and maturation which in turn are likely due to our brain size. The human brain is more than three times larger than our closest relative the chimpanzee. In human adults, the brain accounts for 2% of body mass but consumes 25% of resting energy expenditure. An average adult needs 600 kilocalories per day to feed his or her brain (2). In other primates, the brain consumes <10% of resting energy expenditure (3).

In non-human primates, time spent feeding increases with body mass; the bigger the animal, the more time spent feeding. On a raw diet similar to what non-human primates eat, humans would need to feed 12 h every day to meet our energy needs however, the actual value is only an hour (4). We solved this paradox with cooking; cooking enables us to eat a diet much lower in fiber than any other ape. The move from raw food to cooked food was associated with a significant reduction in gut size as compared to our ancestors and allowed us to divert more energy to brain growth (5) and increased the time available for social and other cognitively demanding activities which in turn imposed positive pressure for increasing brain size (6).

Humans have the longest period of juvenile dependency of all primates, and the shortest period of lactation. If humans followed the great ape pattern of supplementing our offspring via lactation and not conceiving again until juveniles were nutritionally independent, our interbirth interval would be between 10 and 15 years. Early weaning allows human mothers to resume reproduction much sooner than expected. Moreover, human mothers are unique in that they simultaneously care for multiple dependent offspring at different stages of maturity (7). Cooking and making food more digestible allows children to be weaned onto adult foods much more quickly and allows mothers to stop nursing sooner; all human societies prepare some form of weaning food (7, 8).

When Do We Start Feeding Infants Solid Foods?

As a result of feeding our young complementary foods, there does not appear to be a specific age for weaning in humans, and weaning is variable across cultures. In pre-industrial populations, complementary foods are usually started between 4 and 6 months and breast-feeding has completely ceased by 30 months however, in some cultures solids are started substantially earlier (1). In general, the more active women are in subsistence activities, the earlier supplementary foods are started however, there is variation that seems the result of the work women are expected to do (9).

Since 2002, the WHO has recommended infants be exclusively breast fed for the first 6 months of life to optimize their growth, development and health (10). The American Academy of Pediatrics, the American College of Obstetrics and Gynecology and the American Academy of Family Physicians all recommend solid foods be introduced at approximately six months of age (1113) whereas, the European Society for Pediatric Gastroenterology, Hepatology and Nutrition (ESPGHAN) recommends “complementary foods should not be introduced before 4 months but should not be delayed beyond 6 months” (14). Despite these recommendations, in the U.S., 40% of mothers introduce solids before 4 months of age, and in those who start solids before 4 months, the average age is 12 weeks, and 9% of mothers start solids before 4 weeks of age (15). In Australia, 50% of mothers have fed their babies solids by 4 months of age, and 90% of mothers feed solids before 6 months (16). Comparable patterns are reported in the U.K. and Italy (17). In all these studies, the most commonly cited reasons for starting solids early are the baby was hungry all the time, the baby seemed interested in eating solids and/or starting solids would help the baby sleep (1517). There are studies that confirm that the early introduction of solid foods is associated with longer infant sleep duration and less frequent waking at night, (18, 19). It is worth noting that most of the studies examining when infants commence eating solid foods describe feeding practices before 2010, and the increased emphasis on the benefits of exclusive breast feeding over the past decade may have changed mothers' behaviors.

What Solid Foods Should We Feed Infants?

Fetuses and newborn infants appear to have an innate preference for sweet and an innate distaste for bitter and other taste preferences appear to be learned through exposure and experience (20, 21). In a study at the Pennsylvania Hospital, newborns were offered water or sucrose solution and allowed to drink for 5 minutes. The infants took nearly three times as much of the sucrose solutions as water. At 6 months, the investigators managed to recall 131 of 151 infants. At the time, many parents fed infants water sweetened with sucrose, honey or Karo syrup as a supplement. The infants who had been fed sweetened water as newborns drank significantly more of the sucrose solution than infants who had not been getting sweetened water as newborns (22).

In utero events and exposures appear to influence taste and flavor preferences (23). What mothers eat during pregnancy influences the flavors the fetus is exposed to in the amniotic sac (24). In one study, prior to undergoing amniocentesis, women were randomized to eat garlic capsules or placebo. The smell of garlic was easily detected in the fluid of the women who had eaten garlic but not in those who had not (25). Fetal swallowing frequency increases when they are exposed to sweet solutions in the amniotic fluid and swallowing frequency decreases when they are exposed to bitter solutions, and similar behaviors are observed within hours of birth (26). Newborn infants orient and begin mouthing when exposed to anise or garlic if their mothers had consumed anise or garlic during pregnancy (23, 27).

These prenatal exposures may have long lasting effects (28). At 35 weeks gestation, Irish mothers were randomized to eat four meals a week containing fresh garlic or to not eat any fresh garlic. Eight years later, their children were given a test meal containing two portions of potato gratin, one which contained garlic and one which did not. Remarkably, the children whose mothers ate garlic during the last month of pregnancy ate twice as much of the garlic containing potatoes as did the control group (29). Analogously, what mother eats and drinks while she is nursing influences the flavor of her breast milk (30) and these taste experiences appear to influence her infant's acceptance of new flavors and what flavors they will accept and like later in life (31).

There appears to be a sensitive period during the first several months of life when infants are most receptive to a wide diversity of flavors, and what they taste during this sensitive period can influence their taste preferences later in life (32). Most infants <4 months of age readily drink formulas that contain hydrolyzed casein which are extremely bitter, however beyond 6 months, infants who have never been exposed to these formulas refuse to drink them (33). Moreover, infants fed hydrolysate formulas in the first several months of life are more willing to eat savory, sour or bitter foods than are infants fed standard milk-based formulas and as compared to children who were never fed a hydrolysate formula, 5 year old children who were fed a hydrolysate formula during infancy more readily eat foods with sour or bitter tastes or aromas (34).

Repetition of taste exposures seems to matter as well. In one study, mothers of seven month old infants were asked to identify a vegetable their infant disliked and were instructed to offer that vegetable on alternate days for 16 days, and to offer a well-liked vegetable, typically something sweet like carrots, sweet potatoes or squash, on the other days. On the first day, infants ate substantially less of the disliked vegetable than the well-liked vegetable however by the eighth repetition, the intake of the liked and unliked vegetables were identical (35). A number of studies have demonstrated similar findings with anywhere between five and ten repetitions (36, 37).

In another study 5 month old infants were randomized to three patterns of introduction of complementary foods. A third were randomized to a “no variety group” and got carrot puree every day for 12 days, a third were randomized to a “low variety group” and got puree of carrot, artichoke, green bean, and pumpkin each for 3 days, and the final third were randomized to a “high variety group” and got carrot puree the first day, followed by artichoke, green beans and pumpkin and back to carrot, etc. When the children were six, they were offered a test meal and the children who had experienced a wider variety of vegetables at weaning ate far more new vegetables than those who had experienced little or no variety at weaning (38).

There also appears to be a sensitive period when infants are particularly receptive to different food textures. Infants and young children typically prefer smooth foods over foods with lumps or chunks however, most fruits and vegetables have complex textures which require the infant to use their tongues to move the food around their mouths in preparation for swallow. Some authors suggest infants are not ready to eat solids before 6 months of age when they have developed a phasic bite and release, have good head control, have good trunk stability and are sitting with minimal support, and are bringing their hands and other objects to their mouth however, the ability of infants to use their tongues and move food around their mouth appears to be more dependent upon their experiences with textured food than on any particular age or developmental stage (39). When infants are exposed to a variety of textures, they are more willing to eat and enjoy chopped or chunky foods at 12 months (40), are more likely to eat a variety of fruits and vegetables at 7 years, and are less likely to suffer from feeding problems during childhood (41). In contrast, children who are not introduced to solids until after a year are more likely to develop oral defensiveness and refuse more highly textured foods (42).

Potential Risks of Early Introduction of Solids

Allergies

It has long been proposed that early introduction of solids increases the risk of allergies later in life (43) however, evidence is accumulating that early introduction of solids may decrease the risk of food allergies (44). In the LEAP study, 640 infants with eczema, egg allergy or both were randomized to avoid peanuts or to consume a minimum amount of peanut containing foods. At 5 years of age, the proportion of children who had peanut allergy as assessed by oral food challenge was substantially lower in the peanut consumption group (45). In a follow-up study a year later, the findings were unchanged (46). In the EAT study 1,303 exclusively breastfed infants were introduced to six allergenic foods (peanut, cooked egg, cow's milk, sesame, whitefish and wheat) at 4 months, or were exclusively breast fed through 6 months. At 36 months, 2.4% of the early exposure group was allergic to one or more food as compared to 7.3% of the group who were introduced to solids after 6 months, and the early exposure group had significantly lower rates of peanut, egg, and milk allergy (47).

These findings suggest there is a window for oral tolerance during early infancy that begins to close somewhere around 6 months of age. This comports with animal models suggesting tolerance is dependent upon exposure to exogenous proteins coupled with development of a healthy intestinal microbiome during a critical early window in life (48).

Obesity

There is little evidence early introduction into an infant's diet increases their subsequent risk of obesity (49). In one trial, 165 formula fed infants were randomized to be fed solid foods beginning at 3 months as compared to 6 months of age. Weights were identical at 3, 6, and 12 months of age (50). In a Scandinavian study, 100 breast-fed infants were randomized to receive complementary foods beginning at 4 months or to continue to exclusively breast feed through 6 months. Weights at 9, 12, and 36 months were identical in the two groups (51). In the EAT study described above, exclusively breastfed infants in the UK were introduced to six allergenic foods at 4 months, or were exclusively breast fed through 6 months and the weights of the two groups were identical at 3, 12, and 36 months (47). The majority of studies show no association between introduction of solids at 4 months and an increased risk of obesity later in childhood. There are several retrospective observational studies suggesting there may be an increased risk of obesity when solids are introduced before 4 months however, it is important to consider the possibility of reverse causality as rapid weight gain and size at 6 months are independent risk factors for future obesity (52). Moreover, it is important to remember that the most common reason parents cite for starting solids early is because they perceive the baby as being hungry much of the time, whether this is right or wrong (1517).

The Intestinal Microbiome

During exclusive breast or formula feeding, the infant's intestinal microbiome contains a pre-ponderance of bifidobacteria and enterobacteriacae with smaller numbers of streptocci, lachnospiracaie, lactobacilli, and clostridial species. With the introduction of solids into the diet, bifidiobacteriae, enterobacterial, lactobacilli and clostridial species decline and the fiber fermenters lachnospiracaie, bacteriodes, and ruminococcace increase. There is also a fairly marked increase in the diversity of the fecal microbiome with the introduction of solids, and these changes appear independent of geographic location, mode of delivery, and whether the infant is breast and/or formula fed (52). Moreover, low microbial diversity early in life has been associated with an increased incidence of infantile colic, eczema, asthma and type 1 diabetes (53).

Conclusions

There appears to be a sensitive period in the first several months of life when infants readily accept a wide variety of tastes and this period overlaps with a critical window for oral tolerance. As a result, it makes sense to expose infants to a wide variety of flavors while mother is pregnant, while mother is nursing and beginning at an early age. There also appears to be a sensitive period between 4 and 9 months when infants are most receptive to different food textures.

There remains debate as to when it is best to start introducing solid foods into an infant's diet however, the available evidence suggests provided the water and food supply are free of contamination, and the infant is provided adequate nutrition, there are no clear contraindications to feeding infants complementary foods at any age. There is emerging evidence that introduction of solid foods into an infant's diet by 4 months may increase their willingness to eat a variety of fruits and vegetables later in life, decrease their risk of having feeding problems later in life, and decrease their risk of developing food allergies, and the early introduction of solid foods into an infant's diet does not appear to increase their risk of obesity later in childhood.

Author Contributions

SB is solely responsible for this manuscript. He conceived the paper, he solely wrote and edited the manuscript, and he has read and approved the final manuscript.

Conflict of Interest

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

1. Sellen DW. Evolution of infant and young child feeding: implications for contemporary public health. Ann Rev Nutr. (2007) 27:123–48. doi: 10.1146/annurev.nutr.25.050304.092557

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Herculano-Houzel S. The remarkable, yet not extraordinary, human brain as a scaled-up primate brain and its associated cost. PNAS. (2012) 109:10661–8. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1201895109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Fonseca-Azevedo K, Herculano-Houzel S. Metabolic constraint imposes tradeoff between body size and number of brain neurons in human evolution. PNAS. (2012) 109:18571–6. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1206390109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Organ C Nunn CL, Machanda Z, Wrangham RW. Phylogenetic rate shifts in feeding time during evolution nof Homo. PNAS. (2011) 108:14555–9. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1107806108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Wrangham RW, Jones JH, Laden G, Pilbeam D, Conklin-Brittain N. The raw and the stolen. Cooking and the ecology of human origins. Curr Anthropol. (1999) 40:567–94. doi: 10.1086/300083

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Herculano-Houzel S, Kaas JH. Gorilla and orangutan brain conform to the primate cellular scaling rules: implications for human evoluation. Brain Behav Evol. (2011) 77:33–44. doi: 10.1159/000322729

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Humphrey LT. Weaning behavior in human evolution. Semin Cell Dev Biol. (2010) 21:453–61. doi: 10.1016/j.semcdb.2009.11.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Knott C. Reproductive ecology and human evolution. In: Ellison, editor. Hawthorne, NY: Aldine De Gruyter (2001). p. 429–63.

Google Scholar

9. Sellen DW. Comparison of infant feeding patterns reported for nonindustrial populations with current recommendations. J Nutr. (2001) 131:2707–15. doi: 10.1093/jn/131.10.2707

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. WHO. Exclusive Breast Feeding for Six Months Best for Babies Everywhere. (2011). Available online at: https://www.who.int/mediacentre/news/statements/2011/breastfeeding_20110115/en/ (accessed March 24, 2020).

Google Scholar

11. AAP Section on Breastfeeding. Breastfeeding and the use of human milk. Pediatrics. (2012). 129:e827–e841. doi: 10.1542/peds.2011-3552

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. AAFP. Breastfeeding Policy Statement. (2017). https://www.aafp.org/about/policies/all/breastfeeding.html (accessed March 24, 2020).

Google Scholar

13. ACOG Committee on Health Care for Underserved Women. ACOG committee opinion no. 361: breastfeeding: maternal and infant aspects. Obstet Gynecol. (2007) 109:479–80. doi: 10.1097/00006250-200702000-00064

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Fewtrell M, Bronsky J, Campoy C, Domellof M, Embleton N, Fidler Mis N, et al. Complementary feeding: a position paper by the european society for paediatric gastroenterology, hepatology, and nutrition (ESPGHAN) committee on nutrition. J Pediatr Gastro Nutr. (2017) 64:119–32. doi: 10.1097/MPG.0000000000001454

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Clayton HR, Perrine LR, Scanlon KS. Prevalence and reasons for introducing infants early to solid foods: variations by milk feeding type. Pediatrics. (2013) 131:e1108–14. doi: 10.1542/peds.2012-2265

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Scott JA. Predictors of the early introduction of solid foods in infants: results of a cohort study. BMC Pediatr. (2009) 9:60. doi: 10.1186/1471-2431-9-60

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Costantini C, Harris H, Reddy V, Akehurst L, Fasulo A. Introducing complementary foods to infants: does age really matter? A look at feeding practices in two European Communities: British and Italian. Child Care Pract. (2019) 25:326–41. doi: 10.1080/13575279.2017.1414033

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Orenstein SR, Magill HL, Brooks P. Thickening of infant feedings for therapy of gastroesophageal reflux. J Pediatr. (1987) 110:181–6. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3476(87)80150-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Perkins MR, Bahnson HT, Logan K, Marrs T, Radulovic S, Craven J, et al. Association of early introduction of solids with infant sleep: a secondary analysis of a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Pediatr. (2018) 172:e180739. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2018.0739

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Rosenstein D, Oster H. Differential facial responses to 4 basic tastes in newborns. Child Dev. (1988) 59:1555–68. doi: 10.2307/1130670

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Cook CK. Taste perception in the newborn infant. Infant Behav Dev. (1978) 1:52–69. doi: 10.1016/S0163-6383(78)80009-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Beauchamp GK, Moran M. Dietary experience and sweet taste preference in human infants. Appetite. (1982) 3:139. doi: 10.1016/S0195-6663(82)80007-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Schaal B, Marlier L, Soussignan R. Human fetuses learn odours from their pregnant mother's diet. Chem Senses. (2000) 25:729–37. doi: 10.1093/chemse/25.6.729

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Hauser GJ, Chitayat D, Berns L, Braver D, Muhlbauer B. Peculiar odors in newborns and prenatal ingestion of spicy food. Eur J Pediatr. (1985) 144:403. doi: 10.1007/BF00441788

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Menella JA, Johnson A, Beauchamp GK. Garlic ingestion by pregnant women alters the odor of amniotic fluid. Chem Senses. (1995) 20:207–9. doi: 10.1093/chemse/20.2.207

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Ross M, Nijland MJ. Fetal swallowing: relation to amniotic fluid regulation. Clin Obstet Gynecol. (1997) 40:352–65. doi: 10.1097/00003081-199706000-00011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Hepper PG. Human fetal olfactory learning. Int J Prenat Perinat Psychol Med. (1995) 7:145–51.

Google Scholar

28. Menella JA, Jagnow CP, Beauchamp GK. Prenatal and postnatal flavor learning by human infants. Pediatrics. (2001) 107:e88. doi: 10.1542/peds.107.6.e88

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Hepper PG, Wells DL, Dorman JC, Lynch C. Long-term flavor recognition in humans with prenatal garlic experience. Dev Psychobiol. (2013) 55:568. doi: 10.1002/dev.21059

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Menella JA, Beauchamp GK. Maternal diet alters the sensory qualities of human milk and the nursling's behavior. Pediatrics. (1991) 88:737–44.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

31. Maier AS, Chabanet C, Schaal B, Leathwood PD, Issanchou SN. Breastfeeding and experience with variety early in weaning increases infants' acceptance of new foods for up to two months. Clin Nutr. (2008) 27:849–57. doi: 10.1016/j.clnu.2008.08.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Harris G, Mason S. Are there sensitive periods for food acceptancy in infancy? Curr Nutr Rep. (2017) 6:190–96. doi: 10.1007/s13668-017-0203-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Mennella J, Beauchamp GK. Developmental changes in the acceptance of protein hydrolysate formula. J Dev Behav Pediatr. (1996) 17:386–91. doi: 10.1097/00004703-199612000-00003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Beauchamp GK, Mennella J. Early flavor learning and its impact on later feeding behavior. J Ped Gastro Nutr. (2009) 43:S25–30. doi: 10.1097/MPG.0b013e31819774a5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Maier A, Chabanet C, Schaal B, Issanchou S, Leathwood P. Effects of repeated exposure on acceptance of initially disliked vegetables in 7-month old infants. Food Qual and Pref. (2007) 18:1023–32. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2007.04.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Remy E, Issanchou S, Chabanet C, Nicklaus S. Repeated exposure of infants at complementary feeding to a vegetable puree increases acceptance as effectively as flavor-flavor learning and more effectively than flavor-nutrient learning. J Nutr. (2013) 143:1194–200. doi: 10.3945/jn.113.175646

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Caton SJ, Ahern SM, Remy E, Nicklaus S. Repetition counts: repeated exposure increases intake of a novel vegetable in UK pre-school children compared to flavor-flavour and flavor-nutrient learning. Br J Nutr. (2013) 109:2089–97. doi: 10.1017/S0007114512004126

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Maier-Noth A, Schaal B, Leathwood P, Issanchou S. The lasting influences of early food-related variety experience: a longitudinal study of vegetable acceptance from 5 months to 6 years in two populations. PLoS ONE. (2016) 11:e0151356. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0151356

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Geisel EG. Effect of food texture on the development of chewing of children between six months and two years of age. Dev Med Child Neurol. (1991) 3:69–79. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.1991.tb14786.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Blossfeld I, Collins A, Kiely M, Delahunty C. Texture preferences of 12-month-old infants and role of early experiences. Food Qual Pref. (2007) 18:396–404. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2006.03.022

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Northstone K, Emmett P, Nethersole F. The effect of age of introduction to lumpy solids on foods eaten and reported feeding difficulties at 6 and 15 months. J Hum Nutr Diet. (2001) 14:43–54. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-277X.2001.00264.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Harris G, Coulthard H. Early eating behaviours and food acceptance revisited: breastfeeding and introduction of complementary foods as predictive of food acceptance. Curr Obes Rep. (2016) 5:113–20. doi: 10.1007/s13679-016-0202-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Fiocchi A, Assa'ad A, Bahna S. Food allergy and the introduction of solid foods to infants: a consensus document. Ann Allergy Asthma Allergy Immunol. (2006) 97:10–21. doi: 10.1016/S1081-1206(10)61364-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Abrams EM, Greehawt M, Fleischer DM, Chan ES. Early solid food introduction: role in food allergy prevention and implications for breastfeeding. J Pediatr. (2017) 184:13–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2017.01.053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Du Toit G, Roberts G, Sayre PH, Bahnson HT, Radulovic S, Santos AF, et al. Randomized trial of peanut consumption in infants at risk for peanut allergy. NEJM. (2015) 372:803–13. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa1414850

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Du Toit G, Sayre PH, Graham-Roberts DM, Sever ML, Lawson K, Bahnson HT, et al. Effect of avoidance on peanut allergy after early peanut consumption. NEJM. (2016) 374:1435–43. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa1514209

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Perkins MR, Logan K, Tsent A, Raji B, Ayis S, Peacock J, et al. Randomized trial of introduction of allergenic foods in breast-fed infants. NEJM. (2016) 374:1733–43. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa1514210

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Prescott SL, Smith P, Tang M, et al. he importance of early complementary feeding in the development of oral tolerance: concerns and controversies. Pediatr Allergy Immunol. (2008). 19:375–80.

Google Scholar

49. Daniels L, Mallan KM, Fildes A, Wilson J. The timing of solid introduction in an “obesogenic” environment: a narrative review of the evidence and methodologic issues. Austr and NZ J Pub Health. (2015) 39:366–73. doi: 10.1111/1753-6405.12376

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Mehta KC, Specker BL, Bartholomy S, Giddens J, Ho ML. Trial on timing of introduction of solids and food type on infant growth. Pediatr. (1998) 102:569–73. doi: 10.1542/peds.102.3.569

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Jonsdottir OH, Kleinman RE, Wells JC, Fewtrell MS, Hibbard PL, Gunnlaugsson G, et al. Exclusive breastfeeding for 4 versus 6 months and growth in early childhood. Acta Paediatr. (2014) 103:105–11. doi: 10.1111/apa.12433

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Leunissen RW, Kerkhof GF, Stijnen T. Timing and tempo of first-year rapid growth in relation to cardiovascular and metabolic risk profile in early adulthood. JAMA. (2009) 301:2234–42. doi: 10.1001/jama.2009.761

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Laursen MF, Bahl MI, Michaelsen KF, Licht TR. First foods and gut microbes. Front Microbiol. (2017) 8:356. doi: 10.3389/fmicb.2017.00356

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: infant feeding, weaning, infant nutrition, complementary feeding, taste, solid foods, food preferences

Citation: Borowitz SM (2021) First Bites—Why, When, and What Solid Foods to Feed Infants. Front. Pediatr. 9:654171. doi: 10.3389/fped.2021.654171

Received: 15 January 2021; Accepted: 01 March 2021;
Published: 26 March 2021.

Edited by:

Mauro Fisberg, Federal University of São Paulo, Brazil

Reviewed by:

Duška Tješić-Drinković, University of Zagreb, Croatia
Tudor Lucian Pop, Iuliu Haţieganu University of Medicine and Pharmacy, Romania

Copyright © 2021 Borowitz. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Stephen M. Borowitz, Witz@virginia.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.