Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Pediatr., 06 August 2018
Sec. Children and Health

Family Childcare Types and Conduct Problem Behaviors in Young Children: The Mediation Role of Caregiver-Child Interaction

\r\nLi LiuLi Liu1Lijun FanLijun Fan1Xiang-Yu HouXiang-Yu Hou2Chuan-An Wu*Chuan-An Wu3*Xiao-Na YinXiao-Na Yin3Guo-Min WenGuo-Min Wen3Dengli SunDengli Sun3Dan-Xia XianDan-Xia Xian3Hui JiangHui Jiang1Jin JingJin Jing4Yu JinYu Jin4Wei-Qing Chen,*Wei-Qing Chen1,5*
  • 1Guangzhou Key Laboratory of Environmental Pollution and Health Assessment, Guangdong Provincial Key Laboratory of Food, Nutrition and Health, Department of Medical Statistics and Epidemiology, School of Public Health, Sun Yat-sen University, Guangzhou, China
  • 2School of Public Health and Social Work, Australia China Centre for Public Health, Queensland University of Technology, Brisbane, QLD, Australia
  • 3Women's and Children's Hospital of Longhua New District of Shenzhen, Shenzhen, China
  • 4Department of Maternal and Child Health, School of Public Health, Sun Yat-sen University, Guangzhou, China
  • 5Department of Information Management, Xinhua College, Sun Yat-sen University, Guangzhou, China

Background: Previous studies have demonstrated the impacts of genetic, family, and community factors on child conduct problems (CPs). However, little is understood regarding the association between family childcare types and child conduct problem behaviors, as well as whether and to what extent caregiver-child interaction mediates the above association.

Methods: 9,289 children first entering kindergartens in the Longhua New District of Shenzhen, China were enrolled in this cross-sectional study. Primary caregivers were invited to fulfill a self-administered structured questionnaire containing data regarding socio-demographics, family childcare types, caregiver-child interaction, and child conduct problem behaviors (measured by the Conners' Conduct Problem Subscale). A series of multiple logistic and linear regression models were employed to assess the associations among family childcare, caregiver-child interaction, and child conduct problem behaviors.

Results: Family childcare types other than by parents together (i.e., mother alone, mother with others, grandparents, or changing caregivers) were all significantly associated with higher risks of conduct problem behaviors in young children (adjusted ORs ranged from 2.18 to 2.55, and adjusted βs ranged from 0.043 to 0.073; all p < 0.05), after adjusting for confounders including child age, gender, parental education level, parental age at pregnancy, marital status, and family income. The following family childcare types (mother alone, or grandparents, or changing caregivers) vs. the childcare by parents together showed significant relative indirect effects on conduct problem behaviors through caregiver-child interaction, indicating the significant mediation effect of caregiver-child interaction on the above associations. Mediation of caregiver-child interaction on the effect of being cared by mother with others relative to care by parents together on child conduct problem behaviors was yet non-significant.

Conclusions: Family childcare types other than by parents together are associated with increased risks for conduct problem behaviors in young children. Caregiver-child interaction may function as a potential mediator for the above association.

Introduction

Conduct problems (CPs) are among the most common psychological conditions in early childhood, characterized by common externalizing behaviors including troublesome, aggression, disruption, oppositionality, delinquency, and antisociality (13). Children with CP diagnoses or elevated conduct problem behaviors tend to encounter increased risks for a variety of academic (e.g., school failure and dropout), social (e.g., anti-society behaviors and violent crimes), and health (e.g., substance misuse and depression) problems, and they exert enormous economic burden to the education, health, community service and justice systems (15). Conduct problems can occur at an age of as early as three, and early starters (onset during childhood) often exhibit more persistent and detrimental outcomes extending to adolescence and adulthood, as compared with late starters (onset from mid- to late-adolescence) (69). Conduct problems may lead to a lifetime of dysfunction if without effective treatment in place (1, 4, 6), which thus emphasizes the pressing need of exploring the risk factors for conduct problem behaviors in young children as well as the potential mechanisms for developing effective prevention and intervention programs.

Causes for the development and persistence of child conduct problems include a wide range of genetic and environmental factors involving child, family, and community (3, 4, 6, 10, 11), among which the early experience with family and caregivers (e.g., use of childcare) has been documented to critically affect child development (6, 11, 12). Childcare is often categorized into parental, informal (untrained grandparents, extended family, relatives, friends or employed help in the home) or formal (childcare center or nursery) care (13, 14). Previous research into the association of different childcare with children's psychological well-being has been extensive but reached controversary conclusions. For instance, several studies demonstrated a protective role of increasing quantity of parental care on cognitive outcomes or externalizing behavior problems in children (1517). Another study, however, found that there was no increase in relative risk of severe externalizing behaviors for non-relative and center care children as compared with parental care children (14). Besides, father functioning in childcare was supported by prior research as associated with improvement for psychological, cognitive and academic development of preschool-aged children (14, 18, 19). Children receiving informal care from grandparents (20, 21) or relatives (14) were found to experience significantly more behavioral problems than their similar schoolmates. Many studies consistently showed that children who experienced multiple and concurrent childcare arrangements displayed more internalizing and externalizing behavior problems and fewer prosocial behaviors (12, 2224). There were also a large pool of research focusing on the role of formal care, which however, also concluded inconsistent findings regarding its association with psychobehavioral outcomes in children, with some studies favoring that childcare arrangement while others not (14, 22, 25, 26). Besides, previous research largely focused on non-parental childcare (especially formal care) arrangements in Western countries, while few consideration has been given to family or informal childcare common in the rest of globe, including in Asian countries like China (27). Given the aforementioned discrepancies in the impact of childcare on psychological health as well as the scarce evidence in Chinese population which has very unique social culture, more research is thus warranted toward childcare and child development in China.

The precise mechanism of the association between childcare and child psychological development including conduct problems remains unclear. However, previous evidence has shown that the varieties of childcare arrangement are associated with different interactions between childcare and child (18, 28, 29). For example, mothers engaged in childcare often provided nurturing care and emotional support, while fathers usually spent more time in playful and physically stimulating interactions with their children (18, 30, 31). Grandparents, by contrast, were likely to focus more on children's physiological demands (e.g., dieting, sleeping) rather than active interactions (e.g., emotional support, reading, traveling) (21, 29). Different types of caregivers seemed to engage in different amount and pattern of interacting activities with children (18, 28, 29). Moreover, a growing body of research proposed that incremental involvement in caregiver-child interaction (e.g., outdoor play, bookreading, etc.) would be linked to improved cognitive and behavioral development, language acquisition, and social abilities in children (28, 3234). It is thus of interest to test whether caregiver-child interaction may play a mediating role in the association between childcare and child development.

Referring to the aforementioned evidence, this study therefore, taking the advantage of a large population-representative sample, aims to explore the association of different family childcare types with conduct problem behaviors in young preschool children aged around three years, as well as whether and to what extent caregiver-child interaction mediates the above association. We hypothesize that: (1) different types of family childcare in China are associated with varied levels of risks for young children's behavioral problems, and childcare by parents together could be superior over other childcare types; (2) different types of family childcare have different levels of caregiver-child interaction, and an increasing caregiver-child interaction level is related to a decreased risk for child conduct problem behaviors; and (3) caregiver-child interaction serves as a mediator in the association between childcare and child behavioral development.

Materials and Methods

Study Population

Participants were recruited from the baseline survey of the Longhua Child Cohort Study (LCCS) taken place in September 2014. The LCCS was set up in the Longhua New District of Shenzhen, China, which aimed to investigate the influences of family and school environment surrounding children's early life on child psycho-behavioral development. We enrolled children aged around 3 years who were at their first entrance into all of the kindergartens located in the Longhua New District, and invited children's primary caregivers to fulfill a self-administered structured questionnaire. After excluding those who refused to respond or with incomplete required information, a total of 9,289 child-caregiver pairs were included for analysis. This study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the School of Public Health at Sun Yat-sen University (ethics clearance No.: 2015–016), and all participants provided their written informed consent.

Data Collection

Data were gathered through primary caregiver-reported structured questionnaires, which contained information regarding the parents' socio-demographic characteristics including education level, age at pregnancy, marital status, and family income, as well as the child's general information including gender, date of birth, single child or not, preterm birth (gestation < 36 weeks), low birth weight (the infant with birth weight ≤ 2,500 gram), family childcare arrangement, and conduct problem behaviors.

Measurement of Family Childcare Types

Family childcare types was determined from the question: “Who were the primary caregivers for the child at home during the following different ages: from child birth to 3 months, from 3 to 6 months, from 6 months to 1 year, from 1 to 2 years, and from 2 years till now, respectively?” The response choices were: “parents together,” “mother alone,” “mother with others (e.g., grandparents, relatives, friends, neighbors, or babysitters/nannies),” and “grandparents.” The primary childcare arrangement that we used was based on the type of childcare attended for the longest period of time.

Given that there were changes in the family childcare arrangement from child birth till the investigation date for some participants, in the present study, we further categorized all participants into the following five categories of family childcare types, including: “parents together,” “mother alone,” “mother with others,” “grandparents,” and “changing caregivers,” among which the first four categories consistently employed that particular kind of childcare arrangement throughout the whole investigated period.

Measurement of Caregiver-Child Interaction

Caregiver-child interaction was assessed based on the following questions: (1) “How often did the primary child caregivers perform specific activities with the target child from child birth to 1 year old, including: singing, chatting, playing games, body touch (e.g., hugging, kissing, caressing, etc.), and outdoor activities, respectively”; and (2) “How often did the primary child caregivers perform specific activities with the target child during 1–3 years old of the child, including: reading, singing, chatting, playing games, body touch (e.g., hugging, kissing, caressing, etc.), taking to parties, outdoor activities, and traveling, respectively”. The response choices were: “never,” “< 1 time/week,” “1–2 times/week,” and “more than 2 times/week,” which were accordingly allocated to a score of “0,” “1,” “2,” and “3,” respectively. We added up the score for each specific activity and then divided the total score by 13, to derive the final average score for measuring the overall level of caregiver-child interaction. The score for caregiver-child interaction ranged from 0 to 3, with a higher score indicating a higher level of caregiver-child interaction.

Measurement of Conduct Problem Behaviors

We assessed child conduct problem behaviors by a conduct problem (CP) subscale of the Conners' Parent Rating Scale-Revised (CPRS-48), an internationally disseminated and validated screening tool to assess behavioral difficulties in children aged between 3 and 16 years old (35). This tool had been translated into Chinese and showed a good reliability and validity as well (36). The Conners' CP measure comprises 13 items covering a range of defiant or aggressive behaviors. Each item was rated on a four-point scale according to the frequency of child's behaviors: never (score = 0), sometimes (score = 1), often (score = 2), and frequently (score = 3). The average score was then calculated, thereby we obtained a continuous variable ranging between 0 to 3, where a higher score indicated a higher level of conduct problem behaviors. In accordance with previous literature (36), we also transformed the average score into a categorical variable: children scoring larger than X-+2SD were classified as with conduct problem behaviors and were coded “1,” otherwise they were coded “0.” In the current study, the measurement of conduct problem behaviors was treated in both categorical and continuous formats for analysis.

Covariates

The potential covariates included child gender and age, single child, preterm birth, low birth weight, parents' education level, age at the time of pregnancy, marital status and family income. The variables that were significant at p < 0.1 in univariate analyses or widely reported in the literature (e.g., child gender and age) were adjusted in the later multiple regression models.

Statistical Analysis

Means and standard deviations (SD) were used to describe continuous variables, and absolute frequencies and proportions were used for categorical variables. Chi-square tests were applied to compare the socio-demographic characteristics between children with and without conduct problem behaviors. The associations between family childcare arrangement and child conduct problem behaviors were examined by logistic regression models (when the outcome measurement took a categorical format) or linear regression models (when the outcome measurement took a continuous format), under the circumstances of both with and without adjustment for the aforementioned confounders. We also performed multiple linear or logistic regression models, whichever appropriate, to explore the associations of caregiver-parent interaction with family childcare types and child conduct problem behaviors.

To further determine whether and to what extent caregiver-child interaction (Mediator, M) mediated the association between family childcare types (Independent variable, X) and child conduct problem behaviors (Dependent variable, Y), the simple mediation model (Figure 1) was conducted through a bootstrapping procedure with 5,000 resamples using Mplus (37). In this approach, we assessed the bias-corrected bootstrap confidence intervals (CI) of indirect effects, which were considered significant if the upper and lower bound of the 95% CI did not straddle zero. Given that the X (family childcare) in the study was a multicategorical variable, we obtained a relative indirect effect for each category (group Di) vs. the reference group, and evidence that at least one relative indirect effect was different from zero supported the conclusion that M mediated the effect of X on Y (38). All regression models adjusted for the aforementioned potential confounders. The proportion of mediation was then calculated as follows (39):

i) When Y was taken as a categorical variable: ORDE×(ORIE-1)ORDE× ORIE-1

ii) When Y was taken as a continuous variable: aib/ci

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. A simple mediation model in path diagram form. The upper figure (A): Direct effect of X on Y; The lower figure (B): Indirect effect of X on Y.

where in Equation (i): ORDE was the odds ratio of the coefficient of Xi on Y while holding M constant, and ORIE was the exponent of the product by the partial coefficient of M on Y and Xi on M; and in equation (ii) ai was the coefficient of Xi on Y, b was the coefficient of M on Y while controlling for X, ci was the coefficient of Xi on Y, and aib represented the relative indirect effects of Xi (being in group Di relative to the reference group) on Y through M.

All analyses were conducted with SPSS version 22.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL), and a two-tailed p below 0.05 was considered statistically significant.

Results

Socio-Demographic Characteristics of the Study Population

The socio-demographic characteristics of participants are shown in Table 1. Of the total sample, 309 (3.3%) young children were identified as having conduct problem behaviors according to the recommended cut-off value. The included children were all aged 2.5–4 years, with about half boys (54.5%) and half girls (45.5%). Between the children with and without conduct problem behaviors, significant differences were observed in terms of parents' education level, parents' age at the time of maternal pregnancy, marital status, and family income. However, other characteristics were quite comparable between them including child age and gender, single child, preterm birth, and low birth weight.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Socio-demographic comparisons of children with and without conduct problem behaviors.

Association Between Family Childcare Types and Child Conduct Problem Behaviors

Table 2 presents results on the association between family childcare types and conduct problem behaviors in the young children. In the univariate analyses, results from both logistic regression (considering the outcome variable in categorical format) and linear regression (considering the outcome variable in continuous format) models revealed that, compared with children cared by parents together, those cared by mother alone, mother with others, or changing caregivers encountered significantly elevated risks for conduct problem behaviors, while those cared by grandparents showed non-significantly increased risks. In further multiple regression models with adjustment for potential confounders including child age, gender, parental education level, parental age at pregnancy, marital status, and family income, when compared with family childcare by parents together, children primarily cared by mother alone, mother with others, grandparents, or changing caregivers since birth were all significantly associated with higher risks of conduct problem behaviors at an early age of around 3 years (adjusted ORs ranged from 2.18 to 2.55, and adjusted βs ranged from 0.043 to 0.073; all p < 0.05).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Logistic and linear regression models on the association between family childcare types and conduct problem behaviors in young children.

Association of Caregiver-Child Interaction With Family Childcare Types and Child Conduct Problem Behaviors

Table 3 depicts the association of caregiver-child interaction with family childcare types and child conduct problem behaviors. Compared with the childcare arrangement by parents together, childcare by grandparents was found to be significantly associated with a largest degree of decrease in the caregiver-child interaction level after adjusting for potential confounders (β = −0.168, 95%CI: −0.210, −0.126). Childcare by mother alone (β = −0.070, 95%CI: −0.110, −0.029) or changing caregivers (β = −0.067, 95%CI: −0.089, −0.046) were also associated with significant decreases in the levels of caregiver-child interaction, while the childcare by mother with others only non-significantly decreased the caregiver-child interaction (β = −0.024, 95%CI: −0.050, 0.002). Besides, an increasing level of caregiver-child interaction was found to be significantly associated with decreased risks of conduct problem behaviors in children, after controlling for potential confounders (OR = 0.45, 95%CI: 0.34, 0.59; β = −0.118, 95%CI: −0.138, −0.099).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Association of caregiver-child interaction with family childcare types and child conduct problem behaviors.

Mediation Effect of Caregiver-Child Interaction on the Association Between Family Childcare Types and Child Conduct Problem Behaviors

The mediation effect of caregiver-child interaction on the association between family childcare types and child conduct problem behaviors is illustrated in Table 4. Results showed that after controlling for aforementioned confounders, all of the following family childcare types (by mother alone, or by grandparents, or by changing caregivers) relative to the reference group of childcare by parents together showed significant relative indirect effects on child conduct problem behaviors through caregiver-child interaction (95% bias-corrected bootstrap CIs for all of their corresponding relative indirect effects did not overlap zero, regardless of whether taking the dependent variable as a categorical format or continuous format), and the relative proportion of mediation for these conditions were 8.9% (or 13.7%), 23.4% (or 44.9%), and 9.0% (or 10.8%), respectively. However, the mediation of caregiver-child interaction on the effect of being in the group of childcare by mother with others relative to the reference group of by parents together on child conduct problem behaviors was non-significant (when Y was taken as a categorical variable: 0.019, 95%CI = −0.005, 0.039; when Y was taken as a continuous variable: 0.003, 95%CI = −0.0002, 0.006).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Mediation effect of caregiver-child interaction on the association between family childcare types and child conduct problem behaviors.

Discussion

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study examining the association between family childcare and conduct problem behaviors in Chinese preschool children with caregiver-child interaction as a potential mediator. Findings from the current study support that compared with stable childcare by parents together since child birth, other family childcare types (including mother alone, mother with others expect father, grandparents, and changing caregivers) are all associated with augmented risks for conduct problem behaviors in preschool children. In addition, caregiver-child interaction plays a mediating role in the above associations, especially mediating a sizeable proportion of the effect of being cared by grandparents vs. by both parents on child conduct problem behaviors. Its mediation effect was only non-significant on the effect of being cared by mother with others relative to care by parents together.

An emerging body of research has examined the associations between family childcare and child behavioral development, but their conclusions still remained controversial. Several studies demonstrated a protective effect of parental care on cognitive outcomes or externalizing behavior problems in children (1517); while another study found that compared with parental care, children receiving non-relative and center care showed no increase in relative risk of severe externalizing behaviors (14). Most studies found that non-maternal childcare would impose adverse impact on child psychobehaviroal development, such as increasing aggression, troublesome and oppositionality, etc. (4042); however, the above association might not be true for those children who were from poor family with low-educated mothers but were receiving non-maternal care (25). Father involvement in childcare was widely supported by prior research as associated with improvement for psychological, cognitive, and academic development of children (14, 18, 19). Childcare from grandparents (20, 21) or relatives (14) were found to be significantly associated with more behavioral problems in children. Besides, the majority of studies consistently pointed out that children who experienced multiple and concurrent childcare arrangements displayed more internalizing and externalizing behavior problems and fewer prosocial behaviors (12, 2224); however, there were also a few studies supporting that childcare changes leading to higher quality or more developmentally appropriate care, such as transitioning from low socio-economic family childcare or low-educated mother's care to high quality child care (43) and transitioning from in-home care to center-based care during the preschool years (44, 45), were beneficial to improve outcomes including better school readiness, improved cognitive performance, and decreased behavioral problems. In the present study, our findings are consistent with several previous studies, which underscored the importance of stable childcare engagement from both parents during early childhood. Our study shows that family childcare types other than by parents together (i.e., mother alone, mother with others, grandparents, and changing caregivers) are all associated with elevated risks for conduct problem behaviors in young children. Discrepancies among previous studies may be owing to extensive differences in the social and cultural patterns or the demographic characteristics, as well as the varied definitions of childcare across studies.

The following explanations may account for the above association found in our study. That is, childcare by parents together represent a quite complementary and balanced caring style, with mothers providing more nurturing care and emotional support while fathers providing more playful and physically stimulating interactions (18, 30, 31). Such care itself may also imply a harmonized and well-functioned family environment or parental psychopathology (46), which may ultimately improve child development. The most current research based on animal studies of mice even demonstrated that early life experience with maternal care could alter DNA methylation at YY1 binding sites implicated in L1 activation, and would then affect expression of the de novo methyltransferase DNMT3a which appear to have substantial functional effects in the brain development and behavior (47, 48). Meanwhile, considerable evidence from Feldman et al. demonstrated that early parent-infant interactions are critical for shaping the children's development of moral stance, neurobehavioral, cognitive and social-emotional outcomes in the later years or across the lifespan (4951). However, when it comes to family childcare types other than parents together, for example, if mothers take the responsibility of childcare all by themselves alone, they may be overwhelmingly busy and unable to provide quality care. Childcare by mother with others could ensure enough functioning of mothers, yet lacks the involvement of paternal functioning and perhaps includes more conflict or competition in terms of parenting, education and life style between mother and other caregivers than mother and father (30). Grandparental care is often committed to mainly caring for the child's physiological demands, while ignoring necessary parenting, interaction and emotional caring (21, 29). Changes in care arrangement disrupt children's already attached or emerging relationships with caregivers, which in turn, may destroy the secure attachment and positive interactions with particular caregiver and may impede children's social-emotional adjustment and development, especially during infancy or young childhood (12, 52, 53). However, despite of the above presumed reasons, the exact explanation for the differences in associations between different childcare types and child psycho-behavioral development remains uncertain and requires further in-depth research.

Based on prior studies, different family childcare types are commonly known to be associated with varieties in caregiver-child interaction. For example, parents were more involved in interacting activities that actively stimulated the children's intellect and playfulness, while grandparents, who often devoted themselves in providing the basic caring in terms of dieting, dressing, sleeping and other living conditions, were less playfully interactive with the children that they cared (21, 29). In addition, mothers were often very emotionally supportive but less physically active, whereas fathers tended to spend more time in playing with their children or participating in adventurous and amusing interactive activities (18, 30, 31). Our findings also confirm that childcare by parents together is likely to have a highest level of caregiver-child interaction, followed by mother with others, changing caregivers, mother alone, and grandparents. In addition, previous research also demonstrated that more involvement in caregiver-child interaction such as reading and outdoor play would be likely to result in better cognitive and behavioral development, language acquisition and social abilities in young children (28, 3234). In accordance with the previous findings, our study observe that an elevated level of caregiver-child interaction is associated with lower risks for child conduct problem behaviors. The positive association between caregiver-child interaction and child behavioral outcomes may be owing to the fact that: a more active engagement in interaction with caregiver may help the child acquire ever-increasing training in communication, social adaption, intellectual stimulation and so on, all of which are likely to link to the healthy and all-round psychological development of child.

Extensive research has indicated that child conduct problem behaviors may be attributable to a complex combination of genetic and environmental factors involving child, family and community (3, 4, 6, 10, 11); however, the underlying mechanisms remain poorly investigated and understood. In the present study, we observe that caregiver-child interaction medicates 23.4% (or 44.9%, the difference in percentage was owing to the categorical or continuous format of the outcome variable that we used) of the effect of being cared by grandparents vs. by parents together on increasing the conduct problem risks. The proportion of mediation effect turns smaller in size but remains significant, when comparing childcare by mother alone (or by changing caregivers) with parents together. Our results indicate that caregiver-child interaction serve as a potential mediator in the association of family childcare with child conduct problem behaviors, especially mediating a sizeable proportion of the effect of being cared by grandparents vs. by both parents. To our best knowledge, there was no prior study that had ever explored the mediating role of caregiver-child interaction in the relationship of family childcare with child behavioral development. Our findings highlight the importance of maintaining stable parental engagement in childcare or increasing active caregiver-child interactions, thereby possibly improving child behavioral and psychological development.

Several limitations should be acknowledged when interpreting the findings in the study. First, we employed a cross-sectional study design that could not determine the causal relationship between childcare and child conduct problems. Results from the present study should be interpreted cautiously, and it is warranted that we conduct larger research in the future to investigate the exact causality and mediation effect. Second, the data was retrospectively collected using a structured questionnaire completed by the primary caregivers, which might result in an information bias. Thus interpretation of the results would require cautions and caveats. Third, clinical diagnosis of conduct problems in children was unavailable in this study, considering the infeasibility to diagnose by interview or direct observation in a large-scale survey. Instead, our study used the Conners' Conduct Problem Subscale, which was widely accepted in epidemiological research and proved to show good reliability and validity in the Chinese population. Fourth, given that formal childcare is rarely used in China, we could not compare the role of formal vs. informal childcare, which may vary in their effects on the development and progression of child's conduct problems. Fifth, we did not collect detailed information regarding the quality of childcare arrangement and caregiver-child interaction, which prevented us from understanding the more precise and sophisticated association between childcare and child development. Last, our study did not involve children who were absent in kindergarten. However, conduct problem behaviors may be more serious among that population (although that population size could be very minor).

Conclusions

Findings from the current study support that family childcare types other than by parents together are associated with elevated risks for conduct problem behaviors in young children. Caregiver-child interaction functions as a potential mediator for the above associations, especially mediating a sizeable proportion of the effect of being cared by grandparents vs. by both parents on child conduct problem behaviors. Our study provides encouraging insights that parents being involved together in family childcare are likely to introduce beneficial effects on the child's psycho-behavioral development, and also adds to the scarce knowledge concerning caregiver-child interaction as a plausible mechanism underlying the associations between family childcare types and child conduct problem behaviors.

Data availability

The datasets for this manuscript are not publicly available because: part of the data is included in other manuscripts under preparation. Requests to access the datasets should be directed to Wei-Qing Chen: chenwq@mail.sysu.edu.cn.

Author Contributions

W-QC, C-AW, LL, X-NY, G-MW, DS, D-XX, JJ, and YJ initiated and designed the study. W-QC led the research training. C-AW led the field investigation. LL, X-NH, G-MW, DS, D-XX, and HJ took part in the investigation team. LL, LF, and W-QC analyzed and interpreted the data. LL, LF, and W-QC wrote the manuscript. X-YH and W-QC revised it with suggestions from all authors.

Funding

The current research was funded by the Longhua New District Government of Shenzhen, China.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank the Longhua New District Government of Shenzhen, China for their funding and other supports. The authors are also grateful to the staff at the Women's and Children's Hospital of Longhua New District of Shenzhen, the staff from all kindergartens in the Longhua New District, as well as families who took part in the field investigation.

References

1. Kazdin AE. Treatment of conduct disorder: Progress and directions in psychotherapy research. Dev Psychopathol. (1993) 5:277–310. doi: 10.1017/S0954579400004399

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Loeber R, Farrington DP. Young children who commit crime: Epidemiology, developmental origins, risk factors, early interventions, and policy implications. Dev Psychopathol. (2000) 12:737–62. doi: 10.1017/S0954579400004107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Powell NR, Lochman JE, Boxmeyer CL. The prevention of conduct problems. Int Rev Psychiatry (2007) 19:597–605. doi: 10.1080/09540260701797738

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Hill J. Biological, psychological and social processes in the conduct disorders. J Child Psychol Psychol. (2002) 43:133–64. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Frick PJ, Morris AS. Temperament and developmental pathways to conduct problems. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. (2004) 33:54–68. doi: 10.1207/S15374424JCCP3301_6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Shaw DS. Future directions for research on the development and prevention of early conduct problems. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. (2013) 42:418–28. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2013.777918

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Shaw DS, Shelleby EC. Early-starting conduct problems: Intersection of conduct problems and poverty. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. (2014) 10:503–28. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032813-153650

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Loeber R, Burke JD, Lahey BB, Winters A, Zera M. Oppositional defiant and conduct disorder: a review of the past 10 years, part I. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry (2000) 39:1468–84. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200012000-00007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Burke JD, Loeber R, Birmaher B. Oppositional defiant disorder and conduct disorder: a review of the past 10 years, part II. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry (2002) 41:1275–93. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200211000-00009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Furlong M, McGilloway S, Bywater T, Hutchings J, Smith S, Donnelly M. Behavioral and cognitive-behavioural group-based parenting interventions for early-onset conduct problems in children age 3–12 years. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. (2012) 2: CD008225. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD008225

CrossRef Full Text

11. Vanderbilt-Adriance E, Shaw DS, Brennan LM, Dishion TJ, Gardner F, Wilson MN. Child, family, and community protective factors in the development of children's early conduct problems. Fam Relat. (2015) 64:64–79. doi: 10.1111/fare.12105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Morrissey TW. Multiple child-care arrangements and young children's behavioral outcomes. Child Dev. (2009) 80:59–76. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01246.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Lin SL, Leung GM, Hui LL, Lam TH, Schooling CM. Is informal child care associated with childhood obesity? Evidence from Hong Kong's “Children of 1997” birth cohort. Int J Epidemiol. (2011) 40:1238–46. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyr086

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Bacharach VR, Baumeister AA. Child care and severe externalizing behavior in kindergarten children. J Appl Dev Psychol. (2003) 23:527–37. doi: 10.1016/S0193-3973(02)00140-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Belsky J. Quantity of nonmaternal care and boys' problem behavior/adjustment at ages 3 and 5: exploring the mediating role of parenting. Psychiatry (1999) 62:1–20. doi: 10.1080/00332747.1999.11024848

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. Further Explorations of the Detected Effects of Quantity of Early Child Care on Socioemotional Adjustment. Minneapolis: MN (2001).

17. Ryan RM, Martin A, Brooks-Gunn J. Is one good parent good enough? Patterns of mother and father parenting and child cognitive outcomes at 24 and 36 months. Parenting. (2006) 6:211–28. doi: 10.1207/s15327922par0602&3_5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Ehrenberg MF, Gearing-Small M, Hunter MA, Small BJ. Childcare task division and shared parenting attitudes in dual-earner families with young children. Fam Relat. (2001) 50:143–53. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3729.2001.00143.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Choi JK, Jackson AP. Fathers' involvement and child behavior problems in poor African American single-mother families. Child Youth Serv Rev. (2011) 33:698–704. doi: 10.1016/j.childyouth.2010.11.013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Edwards OW. Teachers' perceptions of the emotional and behavioral functioning of children raised by grandparents. Psychol Sch. (2006) 43:565–72. doi: 10.1002/pits.20170

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Kelley SJ, Whitley DM, Campos PE. Behavior problems in children raised by grandmothers: the role of caregiver distress, family resources, and the home environment. Child Youth Serv Rev. (2011) 33:2138–45. doi: 10.1016/j.childyouth.2011.06.021

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Schipper J, IJzendoorn MHV, Tavecchio LW. Stability in center day care: Relations with children's well-being and problem behavior in day care. Soc Dev. (2004) 13:531–50. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2004.00282.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Pilarz AR, Hill HD. Unstable and multiple child care arrangements and young children's behavior. Early Child Res Q. (2014) 29:471–83. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2014.05.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Bratsch-Hines ME, Mokrova I, Vernon-Feagans L. Child care instability from 6 to 36 months and the social adjustment of children in prekindergarten. Early Child Res Q. (2015) 30:106–16. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2014.09.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Peng D, Robins PK. Who should care for our kids? The effects of infant child care on early child development. J Child Poverty (2010) 16:1–45. doi: 10.1080/10796120903575085

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Lekhal R. Do type of childcare and age of entry predict behavior problems during early childhood? Results from a large Norwegian longitudinal study. Int J Behav Dev. (2012) 36:197–204. doi: 10.1177/0165025411431409

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Li Y, Zhao Y. Early childcare and education: current status and policy selection in China. Population J. (2013) 35:31–41. doi: 10.1007/2288-6729-3-1-51

CrossRef Full Text

28. World Health Organization. The Importance of Caregiver-Child Interactions for the Survival and Healthy Development of Young Children: A Review. Geneva (2004).

Google Scholar

29. Williams MN. The changing roles of grandparents raising grandchildren. J Hum Behav Soc Environ. (2011) 21:948–62. doi: 10.1080/10911359.2011.588535

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. DeKlyen M, Speltz ML, Greenberg MT. Fathering and early onset conduct problems: positive and negative parenting, father-son attachment, and the marital context. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. (1998) 1:3–21. doi: 10.1023/A:1021844214633

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Hawkins AJ, Bradford KP, Palkovitz R, Christiansen SL, Day RD, Call VR. The inventory of father involvement: A pilot study of a new measure of father involvement. J Mens Stud. (2002) 10:183–96. doi: 10.3149/jms.1002.183

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Raikes H, Pan BA, Luze G, Tamis-Lemonda CS, Brooks-Gunn J, Constantine J, et al. Mother-child bookreading in low-income families: correlates and outcomes during the first three years of life. Child Dev. (2006) 77:924–53. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00911.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Hopwood-Stephens I. Outdoor Play for 1–3 Year Olds How to Set Up and Run Your Own Outdoor Toddler Group. Routledge: Chapman & Hall (2014).

Google Scholar

34. Brownlee F, Norris L. Promoting Attachment With a Wiggle, Giggle, Hug and Tickle: A Programme for Babies, Young Children and Carers. London: Jessica Kingsley Publishers (2015).

Google Scholar

35. Conners CK, Sitarenios G, Parker JD, Epstein JN. The revised Conners' Parent Rating Scale (CPRS-R): factor structure, reliability, and criterion validity. J Abnorm Child Psychol. (1998) 26:257–68. doi: 10.1023/A:1022602400621

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Fan J, Du Y, Wang W. The norm and reliability of the Conners Parent Symptom Questionnaire in Chinese urban children. Shanghai Arch Psychiatry (2005) 17:321–3.

37. Hayes AF. Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach. New York, NY: Guilford Publications (2017).

38. Hayes AF, Preacher KJ. Statistical mediation analysis with a multicategorical independent variable. Br J Math Stat Psychol. (2014) 67:451–70. doi: 10.1111/bmsp.12028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. VanderWeele TJ, Vansteelandt S. Odds ratios for mediation analysis for a dichotomous outcome. Am J Epidemiol. (2010) 172:1339–48. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwq332

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Jaffee SR, Van Hulle C, Rodgers JL. Effects of nonmaternal care in the first 3 years on children's academic skills and behavioral functioning in childhood and early adolescence: a sibling comparison study. Child Dev. (2011) 82:1076–91. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01611.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Jacob JI. The socio-emotional effects of non-maternal childcare on children in the USA: a critical review of recent studies. Early Child Dev Care (2009) 179:559–70. doi: 10.1080/03004430701292988

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Bradley RH, Vandell DL. Child care and the well-being of children. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. (2007) 161:669–76. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.161.7.669

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Côté SM, Boivin M, Nagin DS, Japel C, Xu Q, Zoccolillo M, et al. The role of maternal education and nonmaternal care services in the prevention of children's physical aggression problems. Arch Gen Psychiatry (2007) 64:1305–12. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.11.1305

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Ansari A, Winsler A. Stability and sequence of center-based and family childcare: links with low-income children's school readiness. Child Youth Serv Rev. (2013) 35:358–66. doi: 10.1016/j.childyouth.2012.11.017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Morrissey TW. Sequence of child care type and child development: what role does peer exposure play? Early Child Res Q. (2010) 25:33–50. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2009.08.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Ramchandani PG, Domoney J, Sethna V, Psychogiou L, Vlachos H, Murray L. Do early father-infant interactions predict the onset of externalising behaviours in young children? Findings from a longitudinal cohort study. J Child Psychol Psyc. (2013) 54:56–64. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02583.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Bedrosian TA, Quayle C, Novaresi N, Gage FH. Early life experience drives structural variation of neural genomes in mice. Science (2018) 359:1395–9. doi: 10.1126/science.aah3378

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Song S, Gleeson JG. Early life experience shapes neural genome. Science (2018) 359:1330–1. doi: 10.1126/science.aat3977

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Feldman R. Parent-infant synchrony and the construction of shared timing; physiological precursors, developmental outcomes, and risk conditions. J Child Psychol Psychiatry (2007) 48:329–54. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01701.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Feldman R. Mother-infant synchrony and the development of moral orientation in childhood and adolescence: Direct and indirect mechanisms of developmental continuity. Am J Orthopsychiatry (2007) 77: 582–97. doi: 10.1037/0002-9432.77.4.582

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Feldman R. The relational basis of adolescent adjustment: trajectories of mother-child interactive behaviors from infancy to adolescence shape adolescents' adaptation. Attach Hum Dev. (2010) 12:173–92. doi: 10.1080/14616730903282472

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Barnas MV, Cummings EM. Caregiver stability and toddlers' attachment-related behavior towards caregivers in day care. Infant Behav Dev. (1994) 17:141–7. doi: 10.1016/0163-6383(94)90049-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Fiese BH, Tomcho TJ, Douglas M, Josephs K, Poltrock S, Baker T. A review of 50 years of research on naturally occurring family routines and rituals: Cause for celebration? J Fam Psychol. (2002) 16:381. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.16.4.381

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: family childcare types, caregiver-child interaction, conduct problem behaviors, mediation, young children

Citation: Liu L, Fan L, Hou X-Y, Wu C-A, Yin X-N, Wen G-M, Sun D, Xian D-X, Jiang H, Jing J, Jin Y and Chen W-Q (2018) Family Childcare Types and Conduct Problem Behaviors in Young Children: The Mediation Role of Caregiver-Child Interaction. Front. Pediatr. 6:217. doi: 10.3389/fped.2018.00217

Received: 15 May 2018; Accepted: 16 July 2018;
Published: 06 August 2018.

Edited by:

Jean Marc Guile, University of Picardie Jules Verne, France

Reviewed by:

Xavier Benarous, Pitié-Salpêtrière Hospital, France
Laure Boissel, Centre Hospitalier Universitaire D'Amiens, France

Copyright © 2018 Liu, Fan, Hou, Wu, Yin, Wen, Sun, Xian, Jiang, Jing, Jin and Chen. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Chuan-An Wu, wuca@163.com
Wei-Qing Chen, chenwq@mail.sysu.edu.cn

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.