Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Pediatr., 03 May 2018
Sec. Pediatric Neurology

Overview of Social Cognitive Dysfunctions in Rare Developmental Syndromes With Psychiatric Phenotype

  • 1Scientific Brain Training, Reference Center for Rare Diseases GénoPsy, CH Le Vinatier, UMR 5229, Université Lyon 1, CNRS, Lyon, France
  • 2Reference Center for Rare Diseases GénoPsy, SUR/CL3R: Service Universitaire de Réhabilitation, CH Le Vinatier, UMR 5229, Université Lyon 1, CNRS, Lyon, France
  • 3Centre des Sciences du Goût et de l’Alimentation, AgroSup Dijon, INRA, Université Bourgogne Franche-Comté, CNRS, Dijon, France
  • 4Reference Center for Rare Diseases GénoPsy, CH Le Vinatier, UMR 5229, Université Lyon 1, CNRS, Lyon, France
  • 5Centre ressource de réhabilitation psychosociale et de remédiation cognitive, CH Le Vinatier, Lyon et UMR 5229 (CNRS and Université Lyon), Lyon, France

Rare neurodevelopmental syndromes often present social cognitive deficits that may underlie difficulties in social interactions and increase the risk of psychosis or autism spectrum disorders. However, little is known regarding the specificities of social cognitive impairment across syndromes while it remains a major challenge for the care. Our review provides an overview of social cognitive dysfunctions in rare diseases associated with psychiatric symptoms (with a prevalence estimated between 1 in 1,200 and 1 in 25,000 live births: 22q11.2 deletion syndrome, Angelman syndrome, Fragile X syndrome, Klinefelter syndrome, Prader–Willi syndrome, Rett syndrome, Smith–Magenis syndrome, Turner syndrome, and Williams syndrome) and shed some light on the specific mechanisms that may underlie these skills in each clinical presentation. We first detail the different processes included in the generic expression “social cognition” before summarizing the genotype, psychiatric phenotype, and non-social cognitive profile in each syndrome. Then, we offer a systematic review of the social cognitive abilities and the disturbed mechanisms they are likely associated with. We followed the PRISMA process, including the definition of the relevant search terms, the selection of studies based on clear inclusion, and exclusion criteria and the quality appraisal of papers. We finally provide insights that may have considerable influence on the development of adapted therapeutic interventions such as social cognitive training (SCT) therapies specifically designed to target the psychiatric phenotype. The results of this review suggest that social cognition impairments share some similarities across syndromes. We propose that social cognitive impairments are strongly involved in behavioral symptoms regardless of the overall cognitive level measured by intelligence quotient. Better understanding the mechanisms underlying impaired social cognition may lead to adapt therapeutic interventions. The studies targeting social cognition processes offer new thoughts about the development of specific cognitive training programs, as they highlight the importance of connecting neurocognitive and SCT techniques.

Introduction

Neurodevelopmental syndromes are genetic abnormalities frequently associated with behavioral and/or psychiatric phenotypes. Notably, these syndromes increase the difficulties in social interactions and present a high risk to develop psychosis or autism spectrum disorders (ASDs) (13). While social difficulties are generally associated with several comorbidities of genetic conditions, such as intellectual impairments, facial dysmorphology, speech problems, and psychotic symptoms, the high prevalence of social cognitive disorders recently received a growing interest. The aim here is thus to provide an overview of social cognitive abilities in developmental syndromes associated with psychiatric symptoms and shed some light on the specific mechanisms that may underlie these skills in each clinical presentation.

We focus on syndromes with a psychiatric phenotype (psychosis and/or ASD) and a prevalence estimated between 1 in 1,200 and 1 in 25,000 live births: 22q11.2 deletion syndrome (22q11.2DS) (46), Angelman syndrome (AS), Fragile X syndrome (FXS) (7), Klinefelter syndrome (KS), Prader–Willi syndrome (PWS) (8, 9), Rett syndrome (RS), Smith–Magenis syndrome (SMS) (10), Turner syndrome (TS), and Williams syndrome (WS) (11). We first detail the different processes included in the generic expression “social cognition” before summarizing the genotype, psychiatric phenotype, and non-social cognitive profile in each syndrome. Then, we offer a systematic review of the social cognitive abilities and the disturbed mechanisms they are likely associated with. We finally provide insights that may have considerable influence on the development of adapted therapeutic interventions such as social cognitive training (SCT) therapies specifically designed to target the psychiatric phenotype.

Social Cognition

Social cognition is defined as the ability to understand, perceive, and interpret information about other people and ourselves in a social context. This includes abilities such as emotion recognition, theory of mind (ToM), attributional style, and social perception and knowledge (Figure 1). Broadly speaking, social cognition includes a wide range of processes that allow people to rapidly, effortlessly, and flexibly perceive and interpret rapidly changing social information, and respond appropriately to social stimuli. Besides, this ability gives meaning to the actions of others. More specifically, social cognition is an “umbrella concept” that includes many heterogeneous cognitive dimensions, such as emotional information processing, social perception and knowledge, ToM, and attributional bias (12).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Organizational decision chart of social cognitive training in rare neurodevelopmental syndromes with psychiatric phenotype.

Emotional Information Processing

Emotional information about others is conveyed by complex signals such as prosody and emotional expressions from the face or the body (13). Regarding emotional expressions, Ekman (14) described six basic emotions (happiness, sadness, fear, disgust, anger, and surprise) that can be easily discriminated from one another depending on different facial patterns (14, 15). Such categorical perception requires the processing of fine face changes and specific observation strategies.

Theory of Mind

An individual has a ToM if he imputes mental states to himself and others. A system of inferences of this kind is properly viewed as a theory because such states are not directly observable, and the system can be used to make predictions about the behavior of others (1618). This theory comprises two dimensions: cognitive ToM (beliefs, intentions, etc.) and affective ToM (emotional state, knowledge of emotion, etc.) (19, 20). The ToM is subtended by first- and second-order representations. The first-order representation is defined as the ability to understand another person’s mental state. The second-order representation is the ability to understand what one person thinks about another person’s thoughts. These levels are both supported by the primary ability to understand how and why an individual does something (e.g., understanding motor action and eye direction) (16).

Attributional Style

Attribution refers to the way people explain their own and other people’s behaviors (21), but individuals are not objective perceivers and sometimes attribution does not properly match reality. Some individuals suffer from attribution bias, i.e., they systematically over- and under-use the available information, which leads to a misinterpretation of the world they live in.

Social Perception and Knowledge

Social perception refers to the understanding of social roles and rules that typically appear in social situations. Social knowledge is the awareness of the behavior expected in different social contexts and interactions (12). This knowledge consists of social experience, education, and ritualized practices that are not necessarily explicitly communicated. These processes are the basis of an accurate analysis of another person’s intentions, desires, or emotional states; more generally, it is linked to ToM.

Selected Neurodevelopmental Syndromes

The Table 1 presents a summary of the main medical, psychiatric, and social characteristics of the neurodevelopmental syndromes cited in this review.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Summary presentation of the main medical and social characteristics of the neurodevelopmental syndromes cited in this review.

Materials and Methods

This systematic review follows the process developed by the PRISMA statement, including the definition of the relevant search terms, the selection of studies based on clear inclusions and exclusion criteria, and the quality appraisal of papers.

Search Strategy

A search of the literature was conducted using the electronic database PubMed and Google Scholar, covering the period between first January 1990 and December 2016 and focusing on articles published in English. Broad search terms were used, including “social cognition,” “theory of mind,” “mind reading,” “mentalizing,” “mentalising,” “emotion recognition,” “emotion perception,” “emotion processing,” “affect recognition,” “affect perception,” “affect processing,” “social knowledge,” “attributional style,” “facial emotion,” “auditory emotion,” “gaze processing,” “body posture,” “social cues” in combination with the diagnostic terms “22q11.2 deletion syndrome” (22q11.2DS), “Angelman syndrome,” “Fragile X syndrome” (FXS), “Klinefelter syndrome” (KS), “Prader-Willi syndrome” (PWS), Rett syndrome” (RS), “Smith-Magenis syndrome” (SMS), “Turner syndrome” (TS) and “Williams syndrome” (WS). Reference lists of the retrieved articles were also manually searched for relevant publications. All articles investigating the perception and/or recognition of affective stimuli (facial expression, prosody, and body language), ToM, social knowledge and attributional style in patients with rare neurodevelopmental syndromes were evaluated. Only studies that involved a behavioral task were included. We excluded from our evaluation the studies that only included neuroimaging data, the studies with non-humans and the reviews, and studies that did not directly assessed social cognition such as studies using only a questionnaire assessing social behavior.

A total of 256 articles were initially identified as potential papers for inclusion. After a comprehensive reading by the first author (Aurore Morel), 136 articles were excluded from the analysis (e.g., studies not focused on behavior or duplicates). A total of 120 papers met the inclusion criteria and were eligible for quality appraisal (40 duplicates).

All titles and abstracts of the identified studies were independently assessed by two authors (Elodie Peyroux and Caroline Demily) for inclusion in or exclusion from the systematic review. Papers were excluded when inclusion criteria were not met (disagreements were resolved by discussion).

Data Analysis

We extracted relevant data from the 132 studies. A meta-analysis of the studies was not feasible given the variations among study designs, social cognitive assessments used, and populations.

A thematic analysis was performed to qualitatively synthesize the data, aiming at identifying specific syndrome patterns. Data were presented in a narrative form. The analytic process was carried on through discussion between the authors (Aurore Morel, Arnaud Leleu, Elodie Peyroux, Emilie Favre, Nicolas Franck, and Caroline Demily).

Patients

We proposed a review analysis, reporting the number of subjects included, the nature of the control population, the statistical method used, and the results obtained for all current published studies (Tables 25).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Fourteen studies examining social cognition in 22q11.2 deletion syndrome (22q11.2DS).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Studies examining social cognition in Fragile X syndrome (FXS).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Studies examining social cognition in Turner syndrome (TS).

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Studies examining social cognition in Williams syndrome (WS).

Results

The large majority of studies assessed either emotion recognition or ToM whereas fewer studies documented attributional style and social knowledge. Moreover, in some area of social cognition, very few studies or no study at all were available for some syndromes, especially AS, RS, and SMS.

Emotional Information Processing

Recognition of Facial Emotion

Basic emotions defined by Ekman (14), namely joy, sadness, anger, fear, and surprise, are the first emotions recognized in faces by children with typical development (117). No obvious deficits in basic emotion perception were reported in females with FXS (37, 79, 81, 84). However, males with FXS looked significantly longer at disgusted faces compared to neutral faces (75), with more difficulties to recognize sadness and fear (83). The same pattern of results was revealed in children with ASD. By contrast, individuals with RS spend less time than controls exploring key facial features (eye, nose, and mouth) for all emotions (118) and have difficulties to recognize emotional expressions (happiness, fear, and sadness) in general. These results may be due to increased attentional or informative value of negative over neutral (and positive) expressions (75).

People with 22q11.2DS have difficulties to discriminate facial emotions when they are moderately expressed (66), which can explain major deficits in social interactions, especially during childhood. Moreover, they have more difficulties to perceive negative emotions such as anger, fear, and sadness and to label emotions (64, 69, 71) while happiness, sadness, and surprise recognition seems efficient (59, 67, 68). A recent study using the CANTAB ERT test (morphed images with fast cover up times to avoid ceiling effects) showed that 22q11.2DS patients are significantly less accurate in detecting sadness and happiness (71). The same results are observed in males with FXS (73, 74, 78, 80, 83, 119).

Patients with WS showed emotion recognition levels similar to controls with the same mental age or individuals with developmental disabilities (99101, 104, 106, 107, 110, 114). They are more accurate in perceiving happiness, sadness, anger, and surprise (107) but perform more poorly on auditive stimuli for all emotions except for happiness, which seems relatively surprising considering the classical hyperacusis associated with the syndrome (103). Abnormalities in auditory processing in WS seem to be restricted to the perception of negative affective vocalizations, such as scream or gasp (96). However, children with WS had no difficulty to recognize vocal emotions compared to controls (94, 95). Emotional information can be transmitted via both visual (e.g., facial expressions and gestures) and auditory (e.g., affective prosody) channels. In social interactions, both channels are competing and require the integration of relevant multisensory emotional information. When identifying facial affects, patients with WS are as accurate as controls in both congruent and incongruent conditions. But they are less efficient when identifying non-social affects (97). These results confirmed that emotionally evocative music facilitates the ability of patients with WS to recognize emotional faces. They spend more time staring at the salient features of the face—particularly the eyes (106, 108). Interestingly, the ability to discriminate and match faces with emotions was assessed in children with WS syndrome compared to PWS syndrome, or a non-specific intellectual deficiency. The three matched groups performed at a similar level (115) without significant correlation between chronological age (CA) and performance on face perception tasks (107). Overall, neurocognitive deficits do not seem to be the only explanation of expression recognition impairments across syndromes, as illustrated in TS (88).

Adults with WS demonstrated a normal level of performance in the identification of emotions in unimodal conditions with face stimuli alone and in congruent face-voice multisensory contexts. However, they are less accurate to identify emotions in unimodal conditions with vocal emotions alone and even worse in multisensory incongruent stimuli conditions (95, 98). This difference seems to be highly specific and central to understand the hypersociability of WS (Table 1). Patients with WS may be specifically attracted by facial information, decreasing their attention to vocal information. Interestingly, when adults with WS are asked to identify complex mental states (such as worry, disinterest, etc.) from the whole dynamic face, their performance is similar to controls matched on CA (57). However, they have difficulties with “relief” and “distrust,” which may be related to hypersociability.

The deficit is more pronounced in PWS and WS, with difficulties to recognize all basic emotions (8, 99, 101). A global emotion identification impairment was documented in 22q11.2DS, FXS, PWS, and WS but the recognition of happiness seems preserved in these syndromes (101). Regarding the recognition of complex emotions, patients with PWS are less efficient than intelligence quotient (IQ)-matched individuals with WS. This result is consistent with the distinct behavioral symptoms associated to these syndromes (complex psychological disorder with maladaptive behaviors vs. frequent hypersociability).

During a task assessing the recognition of anger, fear, disgust, or sadness, individuals with KS were less accurate in recognizing angry faces, unlike other facial expressions (120). Children and women with TS show a specific impairment for the recognition of fearful and angry faces, both when the stimulus is a whole face or the upper face only (8589, 91, 92). More specifically, in TS, fear recognition is significantly weaker compared to other emotions, including anger (88), with difficulties to correctly identify affect tones (91). In a study using dynamic facial expressions of different emotional intensities, patients with TS, 22q11.2DS and controls did not show any deficit in recognizing fear (62, 90). But patients with 22q11.2 are slower to recognize emotions (62) with a normal level of emotion recognition in auditory stimuli (70). By contrast, dynamic stimuli in WS do not change the accuracy of emotion recognition (114).

During an emotion recognition task using photographs, patients with 22q11.2 (59) spent less time than controls observing the eye region, as observed in FXS and ASD. They spent more time looking at the mouth (63, 68), which may explain why they have less difficulty to recognize happiness, surprise and, to a lesser extent, disgust. This result is correlated with the high prevalence of ASD in 22q11.2DS and FXS. By contrast, women with TS stare longer at the mouth, but only when looking at fearful faces. This result suggests a possible mechanism for selective fear deficit (89) across syndromes. Facial emotion recognition impairment may be related to an atypical observation of faces (121). Fewer fixations on the eyes region are related to difficulties to recognize specific emotions. This deficit is a core symptom in ASD controls who stare longer at the mouth for joy, fear, and disgust; at the eye for sadness; and at the eye and nose for anger (122).

ToM in Developmental Syndromes With Psychiatric Phenotype

Ability to Infer False-Belief in Others

An important mechanism of ToM is the ability to understand that other people’s beliefs or representations about the world may differ from reality.

Children with 22q11.2 did not show major difficulties in ToM tasks (60). When it comes to PWS and WS, children had difficulties attributing mental states and understanding first-order beliefs (98, 116, 123). These results were confirmed in a non-verbal picture-sequencing task, but with a preservation of the understanding of intention, social script knowledge, and physical cause-and-effect reasoning (105). These results suggest that language abilities do not influence performance on ToM in WS (116), considering the heterogeneity of the process in the syndrome (105).

Patients with FXS are less accurate in ToM tasks (76, 77) and perform at a similar level than individuals with Down syndrome (DS) (73, 74) or ASD (77), with a limited impact of autistic symptoms (124). Altogether, these findings suggest that a ToM disorder in FXS may not result solely from the high comorbidity of ASD (77).

Children with WS, PWS, or a non-specific intellectual deficiency, perform at a similar level in standard ToM tasks (115, 125). However, children with PWS had specific difficulties to attribute second-order beliefs (123, 126).

Interestingly, 22q11.2DS seems specifically associated with a poor performance on false-belief tasks (60, 61), this deficit being highly correlated with psychotic symptoms.

Ability to Infer Mental States in Others

Children with 22q11.2 seem to have difficulties to link contextual and social relevant cues when the scene is visually complex (61) or based on circumstances (61). This deficit may be related to attentional and visuospatial disorders and requires families and therapists to clearly verbalize emotions during social interactions. Moreover, the comprehension of misunderstanding, persuasion, pretending, sarcasm, and white lie seems impaired in 22q11.2DS (25, 60, 71). These findings suggest that a cognitive ToM disorder causes a delay rather than a deficit (60). From this perspective, impairments in the spontaneous attribution of seducing, mocking, playing with one another, getting frightened, or elated mental states to abstract visual stimuli were reported in 22q11.2DS (65), suggesting that gaze direction is also a weakness in the syndrome (60).

As expected, patients with WS were proficient during a verbal task (111, 126) but their performance decreased when the ToM task was presented on a visual medium (111) with difficulties inferring mental states from visual cues, especially for negative intentions (93) and gaze direction (109). These results suggest that they may present a positive bias in the interpretation of social cues. An impaired social-perceptual ability may play a role in increased approachability and a deficit in the processing of basic social cues may have possible repercussions on ToM. The ability to observe and recognize other people’s actions contributes to understand goals and intentions (127) and may help people with WS improve their recognition of action process (113). Indeed, in the presence of context cues, children with WS were as accurate as controls (112, 113).

Overall, patients with 22q11.2DS, FXS, or WS had difficulties determining whether someone is looking at them or elsewhere (60, 82, 102, 128) but individuals with PWS seem to yield scores within the normal range (129). When patients with FXS were asked to judge the direction of eye gazes (direct or averted), they performed at the same level than patients with similar general cognitive abilities and autism symptoms (72), and patients with a developmental delay (82). Complex emotion recognition requires knowledge and analysis of interpersonal relationships, unlike basic emotion recognition. Thus, further research on the potential dissociation between the recognition of complex mental states and basic emotions, and the influence of the nature of the stimuli (static or dynamic) is needed.

Discussion

Interventions and Perspectives

This systematic review shows that social cognition deficits are present in neurodevelopmental disorders at different levels and remain strongly correlated with psychiatric phenotype. Emotion recognition and ToM skills impairments seem to be a core deficit in rare developmental syndromes, otherwise associated with psychosis and ASD. Although more research needs to be done to assess social cognition phenotypes in these syndromes, it is crucial to develop and evaluate appropriate therapeutic interventions. In this section, we briefly discuss therapeutic approaches and present future keys to improve social deficits in that indication.

In daily life, facial expressions are typically embedded in a rich context with many distractors (e.g., noises and visual stimuli) and social information from multiple sensory channels (e.g., prosody, gestures, and change in facial expression). Therefore, social interactions necessitate fast and efficient cognitive skills. They include the ability to identify visual and spatial relationships in emotion recognition, to integrate and manipulate such perceptions, to select relevant information, to inhibit irrelevant stimuli and alternate between several sensitive channels. Emotion recognition is also a complex process involving visual attention (130), visuospatial abilities (130), working memory (131), divided attention and executive functions (132). As a consequence, improvement of these functions may have a positive impact on social cognition and behavior. Visuospatial and visuoperceptual skills play a key role in everyday life. Visual information and complex visual stimuli are analyzed with a complete unawareness of the visuoperceptual process or the complexities of the stimuli involved. This process becomes conscious in a context of learning. Repetition and familiarity enable a more spontaneous approach and turn the conscious and effortful process into an automatic one. If this ability is impaired, many types of deficits can occur, ranking from a failure to process the basic elements of a visual stimulus (i.e., colors, lines, and orientation) to more complex and integrative features, such as object identification, faces, or familiar scenes. These deficits can include social cognition defects, especially in the area of facial emotion recognition, acquired slowly during childhood and reach adult levels in late adolescence (133).

Currently, two programs, named “Cognitus & Moi” (Cognitus & Me) and “Vis-à-Vis” have considered the link between neuropsychological functions and social cognition (facial emotion recognition). “Cognitus & Moi” is a cognitive remediation program with SCT, designed for 5- to 13-year-old children with developmental disorders. The program is specifically focused on attentional, visuospatial functions and emotion recognition (134). It involves a variety of exercises in a paper and/or pencil (n = 30) or a computerized format (n = 29) and a strategy coaching approach. “Vis-à-Vis” is a computerized training program based on a trial-and-error approach. It can be performed at home with parents and targets social cognition difficulties and working memory (135).

The ability to explore face efficiently develops during childhood. The eyes, nose, and mouth are the preferred attentional targets in facial exploration (121). These facial features play an important role in perception and recognition of emotion. However, the visual exploration developed by individuals with neurodevelopmental syndrome is mainly different, with a lack of eye contact according to a specific pattern previously observed in ASD. Individuals with neurodevelopmental syndrome may benefit from educational solutions targeting this impairment. In that regard, an important point is that dynamic information facilitates the perception of facial expressions (136, 137). Individuals with autism appear to benefit from slow dynamic information when categorizing emotional expressions (138). Considering the possibility of overlap between children with neurogenetic disorders and children with typical ASD (139), the question of a similar benefit in rare diseases should also be addressed in the future. More generally, the beneficial effect of motion may play a role in therapeutic intervention.

Some current treatment plans which are based on phenotype rather than etiology may be adapted for individual with neurodevelopmental syndrome.

For example, considering the well-established association between 22q11.2DS and schizophrenia, SCT, a program based on the cognitive enhancement therapy developed for individuals with schizophrenia (140) may be of interest in 22q11.2DS. This program targets general and high-level social skills such as the ability to practice calming techniques, to identify the main idea in a conversation, to determine expected behaviors in different situations (social perception and social knowledge), or to clearly express one’s own thoughts (ToM) (140).

It is necessary to define strengths and weaknesses of each participant and the exact nature of deficits. Indeed this review highlights the implication of different cognitive processes involved in social interaction deficits, which appear to have different effects across syndromes (Tables 6 and 7). Here, we show that social cognition impairments are not uniquely caused by syndrome comorbidities such as IQ. Understanding the differences in the social cognitive abilities may become a distinctive clinical tool to develop special trainings. In several cases, the severe cognitive impairment associated with rare diseases necessitates individual adaptation of therapeutic interventions. Individual may present severe limitations in their ability to communicate through speech and gestures. Single case reports have demonstrated that patients can develop communicative skills and social cognition, particularly affective and cognitive ToM with appropriate interventions. For example, communication by composing words on an alphabetic table may be improved by training communicative skills in RS (141). With a training focused on emotion, discrimination of facial emotion (joy, sadness, and anger) may be improved in RS (142). Moreover, although patients do not develop functional verbal skills, the eye-tracking technology has increased the possibilities to understand patients (143) and facilitated the access to augmentative and alternative communication (144146).

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Emotion processing among rare neurodevelopmental syndrome.

TABLE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Table 7. Theory of mind among rare neurodevelopmental syndrome.

Face recognition and ToM have an important role in social problem solving (14, 147, 148). These capacities allow to understand and interpret other’s emotion (149). In the social interaction, recognition of expressions leads to adapt behavior (150, 151).

Limitations

This systematic review highlights some limits. Some rare syndromes received only little interest, particularly AS, KS, RS, and SMS. The lack of results regarding some areas of social cognition in these syndromes must be interpreted with caution, as the number of published studies is limited. Furthermore, very few studies have directly compared patients considering anxiety levels. Moreover, psychiatric diagnosis has not been considered and very few studies have proposed to establish a link between social cognition and behavior in rare neurodevelopmental syndromes. In fact, the psychiatric phenotype has not yet been extensively studied. Many data are available for facial emotion recognition and ToM, but a lack of research in social perception/knowledge and attributional style has been highlighted. Thus, it remains unclear whether individuals with a rare developmental syndrome show similar impairments in these areas. Moreover, impairments may be specific to tasks and emotions, rather than all social cognitive processes. The use of relatively consistent methodology across study designs would help compare these impairments.

Summary and Conclusion

To sum up, some social trainings, such as the use of the eye-tracking technology in RS or the focus on visuospatial functions in 22q11.2DS, are created among the specific profile of individuals with a neurodevelopmental syndrome. Understanding the differences in the social cognitive abilities of people with rare neurodevelopmental disorders may lead to a more personalized medicine. This review suggests that cognitive remediation therapy and SCT may be a viable way to improve the social and functional outcomes, although many questions on its effectiveness still need to be answered.

The studies targeting social cognition processes offer new thoughts about the development of specific cognitive training programs, as they highlight the importance of connecting neurocognitive and social cognitive training techniques. They point out that individuals with social cognitive impairments may need more help in terms of social interactions. They provide keys for caregivers, who may better adjust their communication with explicit social cues by mentioning what they are thinking or feeling and why. Understanding and adjusting attitudes to individuals with social cognition impairments are required to prevent frustration and maladaptive behaviors.

Availability of Data and Supporting Materials Section

Data sharing not applicable to this article as no datasets were generated or analyzed during the current study.

Author Contributions

AM elaborated the PRISMA review. AM and CD wrote the paper and collected the data. EP, AL, EF, and NF collected the data. All authors approved the final version of the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Abbreviations

22q11.2DS, 22q11.2 deletion syndrome; AAC, augmentative and alternative communication; ASD, Autism spectrum disorder; AS, Angelman syndrome; CA, chronological age; DS, Down syndrome; FXS, Fragile X syndrome; IQ, intelligence quotient; KS, Klinefelter Syndrome; MA, Mental Age; NA, non available; PWS, Prader–Willi syndrome; RS, Rett Syndrome; SMS, Smith–Magenis syndrome; TD, typically developing; TDCA, typically developing matched on chronological age; TDMA, typically developing matched on mental age; ToM, theory of mind; TS, Turner syndrome; VIQ, verbal intelligence quotient; VMA, Verbal Mental Age; WS, Williams syndrome.

References

1. Crespi B. Genomic imprinting in the development and evolution of psychotic spectrum conditions. Biol Rev Camb Philos Soc (2008) 83(4):441–93. doi:10.1111/j.1469-185X.2008.00050.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Crespi B, Badcock C. Psychosis and autism as diametrical disorders of the social brain. Behav Brain Sci (2008) 31(3):241–60. doi:10.1017/S0140525X08004214

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Crespi B, Summers K, Dorus S. Genomic sister-disorders of neurodevelopment: an evolutionary approach. Evol Appl (2009) 2(1):81–100. doi:10.1111/j.1752-4571.2008.00056.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Norkett EM, Lincoln SH, Gonzalez-Heydrich J, D’Angelo EJ. Social cognitive impairment in 22q11 deletion syndrome: A review. Psychiatry Res (2017) 253:99–106. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2017.01.103

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Biswas AB, Furnis F. Cognitive phenotype and psychiatric disorder in 22q11.2 deletion syndrome: A review. Res Dev Disabil (2016) 53(54):242–57. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2016.02.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Swillen A. The importance of understanding cognitive trajectories: the case of 22q11.2 deletion syndrome. Curr Opin Psychiatry (2016) 29(2):133–7. doi:10.1097/YCO.0000000000000231

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Bostrom C, Yau SY, Majaess N, Vetrici M, Gil-Mohapel J, Christie BR. Hippocampal dysfunction and cognitive impairment in Fragile-X Syndrome. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2016) 68:563–74. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.06.033

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Whittington J, Holland T. Recognition of emotion in facial expression by people with Parder-Willi syndrome. J Intellect Disabil Res (2011) 55(1):75–84. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2788.2010.01348.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Rice LJ, Einfeld SL. Cognitive and behavioural aspects of Prader-Willi syndrome. Curr Opin Psychiatry (2015) 28(2):102–6. doi:10.1097/YCO.0000000000000135

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Poisson A, Nicolas A, Cochat P, Sanlaville D, Rigard C, de Leersnyder H, et al. Behavioral disturbance and treatment strategies in Smith-Magenis syndrome. Orphanet J Rare Dis (2015) 4(10):111. doi:10.1186/s13023-015-0330-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Fisher MH, Morin L. Addressing social skills deficits in adults with Williams syndrome. Res Dev Disabil (2017) 71:77–87. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2017.10.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Green MF, Olivier B, Crawley JN, Penn DL, Silverstein S. Social cognition in schizophrenia: recommendations from the measurement and treatment research to improve cognition in schizophrenia. New approaches conference. Schizophr Bull (2005) 31(4):882–7. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbi049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Elfenbein HA, Ambady N. On the universality and cultural specificity of emotion recognition: a meta-analysis. Psychol Bull (2002) 128(2):203–35. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.128.2.203

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Ekman P. Universal and Cultural Differences in Facial Expression of Emotion. Lincoln, NB: Nebraska University Press (1972).

Google Scholar

15. Calder AJ, Young AW, Perrett DI, Etcoff NL, Rowland D. Categorical perception of morphed facial expressions. Vis cogn (1996) 3(2):81–117. doi:10.1080/713756735

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Baron-Cohen S. Mindblindness: An Essay on Autism and Theory of Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press (1995).

Google Scholar

17. Premack D, Woodruff G. Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behav Brain Sci (1978) 1(4):515–26. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00076512

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Wimmer H, Perner J. Beliefs about beliefs: representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition (1983) 13(1):103–28. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(83)90004-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Kalbe E, Schlegel M, Sack AT, Nowak DA, Dafotakis M, Bangard C, et al. Dissociating cognitive from affective theory of mind: a TMS study. Cortex (2010) 46(6):769–80. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2009.07.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Shamay-Tsoory SG, Aharon-Peretz J. Dissociable prefrontal networks for cognitive and affective theory of mind: a lesion study. Neuropsychologia (2007) 45(13):3054–67. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.05.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Fiske ST, Taylor SE. Social Cognition. 2nd ed. New York: McGraw-Hill Book Co (1991).

Google Scholar

22. Oskarsdottir S, Vujic M, Fasth A. Incidence and prevalence of the 22q11 deletion syndrome: a population-based study in Western Sweden. Arch Dis Child (2004) 89(2):148–51. doi:10.1136/adc.2003.026880

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Shprintzen RJ. Velo-cardio-facial syndrome: 30 years of study. Dev Disabil Res Rev (2008) 14(1):3–10. doi:10.1002/ddrr.2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Angkustsiri K, Goodlin-Jones B, Deprey L, Brahmbhatt K, Harris S, Simon TJ. Social impairments in chromosome 22q11.2 deletion syndrome (22q11.2DS): autism spectrum disorder or a different endophenotype? J Autism Dev Disord (2014) 44(4):739–46. doi:10.1007/s10803-013-1920-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Jalbrzikowski M, Carter C, Senturk D, Chow C, Hopkins JM, Green MF, et al. Social cognition in 22q11.2 microdeletion syndrome: relevance to psychosis. Schizophr Res (2012) 142(1–3):99–107. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2012.10.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Debbané M, Glaser B, David MK, Feinstein C, Eliez S. Psychotic symptoms in children and adolescents with 22q11.2 deletion syndrome: neuropsychological and behavioral implications. Schizophr Res (2006) 84(2–3):187–93. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2006.01.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Buckley RH, Dinno N, Weber P. Angelman syndrome: are the estimates too low? Am J Med Genet (1998) 80(4):385–90. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1096-8628(19981204)80:4<385::AID-AJMG15>3.0.CO;2-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Petersen MB, Brondum-Nielsen K, Hansen LK, Wulff K. Clinical, cytogenetic, and molecular diagnosis of Angelman syndrome: estimated prevalence rate in a Danish county. Am J Med Genet (1995) 60(3):261–2. doi:10.1002/ajmg.1320600317

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Mount R, Oliver C, Berg K, Horsler K. Effects of adult familiarity on social approach behaviours in Angelman syndrome. J Intellect Disabil Res (2011) 55:339–50. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2788.2010.01364.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Adams D, Horsler K, Oliver C. Age related change in social behaviour in children with Angelman syndrome. Am J Med Genet A (2011) 155:1290–7. doi:10.1002/ajmg.a.33964

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Walz NC. Parent report of stereotyped behaviors, social interaction, and developmental disturbances in individuals with Angelman syndrome. J Autism Dev Disord (2007) 37:940–7. doi:10.1007/s10803-006-0233-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Steffenburg S, Gillberg CL, Steffenburg U, Kyllerman M. Autism in Angelman syndrome: a population-based study. Pediatr Neurol (1996) 14:131–6. doi:10.1016/0887-8994(96)00011-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Crawford DC, Acuña JM, Sherman SL. FMR1 and the Fragile X syndrome: human genome epidemiology review. Genet Med (2001) 3(5):359–71. doi:10.1097/00125817-200109000-00006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Hall SS, Frank MC, Pusiol GT, Farzin F, Lightbody AA, Reiss AL. Quantifying Naturalistic social gaze in Fragile X syndrome using a novel eye tracking paradigm. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet (2015) 168(7):564–72. doi:10.1002/ajmg.b.32331

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Murphy MM, Abbeduto L, Schroeder S, Serlin R. Contribution of social and information-processing factors to eye-gaze avoidance in Fragile X syndrome. Am J Ment Retard (2007) 112:349–60. doi:10.1352/0895-8017(2007)112[0349:COSAIF]2.0.CO;2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Baumgardner TL, Reiss AL, Freund LS, Abrams MT. Specification of the neurobehavioral phenotype in males with Fragile X syndrome. Paediatrics (1995) 5:744–52.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

37. Turk J, Cornish K. Face recognition and emotional perception in boys with Fragile X syndrome. J Intellect Disabil Res (1998) 42(6):490–9. doi:10.1046/j.1365-2788.1998.4260490.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Bojesen A, Juul S, Gravholt C. Prenatal and postnatal prevalence of Klinefelter syndrome: a national registry study. J Clin Endocrinol Metab (2003) 88(2):622–6. doi:10.1210/jc.2002-021491

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Bishop DV, Jacobs PA, Lachlan K, Wellesley D, Barnicoat A, Boyd PA, et al. Autism, language and communication in children with sex chromosome trisomies. Arch Dis Child (2011) 96:954–9. doi:10.1136/adc.2009.179747

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Cordeiro L, Tartaglia N, Roeltgen D, Ross J. Social deficits in male children and adolescents with sex chromosome aneuploidy: a comparison of XXY, XYY, and XXYY syndromes. Res Dev Disabil (2012) 33:1254–63. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2012.02.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Whittington J, Holland A, Webb T, Butler J, Clarke D, Boer H. Population prevalence and estimated birth incidence and mortality rate for people with Prader-Willi syndrome in one UK health region. J Med Genet (2001) 38(11):792–8. doi:10.1136/jmg.38.11.792

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Kerr AM, Witt Engerström I. The clinical background to the Rett disorder. In: Ingegerd WE, Kerr AM, editors. Rett Disorder and the Developing Brain. Oxford: Oxford University Press (2001). p. 1–26.

Google Scholar

43. Bartl-Pokorny KD, Marschik PB, Sigafoos J, Tager-Flusberg H, Kaufmann WE, Grossmann T, et al. Early socio-communicative forms and functions in typical Rett syndrome. Res Dev Disabil (2013) 34:3133–8. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2013.06.040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Didden R, Korzilius H, Smeets E, Green VA, Lang R, Lancioni GE, et al. Communication in individuals with Rett syndrome: an assessment of forms and functions. J Dev Phys Disabil (2010) 22:105–18. doi:10.1007/s10882-009-9168-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Urbanowicz A, Leonard H, Girdler S, Ciccone N, Downs J. Parental perspectives on the communication abilities of their daughters with Rett syndrome. Dev Neurorehabil (2016) 19:17–25. doi:10.3109/17518423.2013.879940

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Juyal RC, Figuera LE, Hauge X, Elsea SH, Lupski JR, Greenberg F, et al. Molecular analyses of 17p11.2 deletions in 62 Smith-Magenis syndrome patients. Am J Hum Genet (1996) 58(5):998–1007.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

47. Martin SC, Wolters PL, Smith ACM. Adaptive and maladaptive behavior in children with Smith-Magenis syndrome. J Autism Dev Disord (2006) 36:541–52. doi:10.1007/s10803-006-0093-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Sarimski K. Communicative competence and behavioral phenotype in children with Smith–Magenis syndrome. Genet Couns (2004) 15:347–55.

Google Scholar

49. Stochholm K, Juul S, Juel K, Naeraa RW, Gravholt CH. Prevalence, incidence, diagnostic delay, and mortality in Turner syndrome. J Clin Endocrinol Metab (2006) 91(10):3897–902. doi:10.1210/jc.2006-0558

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Lagrou K, Froidecoeur C, Verlinde F, Craen M, De Schepper J, François I, et al. Psychosocial functioning, selfperception and body image and their auxologic correlates in growth hormone and oestrogen-treated young adult women with Turner syndrome. Horm Res (2006) 66:277–84. doi:10.1159/000095547

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. McCauley E, Feuillan P, Kushner H, Ross JL. Psychosocial development in adolescents with Turner syndrome. J Dev Behav Pediatr (2001) 22:360–5. doi:10.1097/00004703-200112000-00003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Hong DS, Dunkin B, Reiss AL. Psychosocial functioning and social cognitive processing in girls with Turner syndrome. J Dev Behav Pediatr (2011) 32:512–20. doi:10.1097/DBP.0b013e3182255301

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Lepage JF, Dunkin B, Hong DS, Reiss AL. Impact of cognitive profile on social functioning in prepubescent females with Turner syndrome. Child Neuropsychol (2013) 19:161–72. doi:10.1080/09297049.2011.647900

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Suzigan LZ, de Paiva e Silva RB, Guerra-Júnior G, Marini SH, Maciel-Guerra AT. Social skills in women with Turner syndrome. Scand J Psychol (2011) 52:440–7. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9450.2011.00887.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Strømme P, Bjornstad PG, Ramstad K. Prevalence estimation of Williams syndrome. J Child Neurol (2002) 17(4):269–71. doi:10.1177/088307380201700406

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Klein-Tasman BP, Mervis CB, Lord C, Phillip K. Socio-communicative deficits in young children with Williams syndrome: performance on the autism diagnostic observation schedule. Child Neuropsychol (2007) 13:444–67. doi:10.1080/09297040601033680

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Hanley M, Riby DM, Caswell S, Rooney S, Back E. Looking and thinking: how individuals with Williams syndrome make judgments about mental states. Res Dev Disabil (2013) 34(12):4466–76. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2013.09.026

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Porter MA, Coltheart M. Cognitive heterogeneity in Williams syndrome. Dev Neuropsychol (2005) 27:275–306. doi:10.1207/s15326942dn2702_5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Campbell L, McCabe K, Leadbeater K, Schall U, Loughland C, Rich D. Visual scanning of faces in 22q11.2 deletion syndrome: attention to the mouth or the eyes? Psychiatry Res (2010) 177(1–2):211–5. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2009.06.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Campbell LE, Stevens AF, McCabe K, Cruickshank L, Morris RG, Murphy DGM, et al. Is theory of mind related to social dysfunction and emotional problems in 22q11.2 deletion syndrome (velo-cardio-facial syndrome)? J Neurodev Disord (2011) 3(2):152–61. doi:10.1007/s11689-011-9082-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Campbell LE, McCabe KL, Melville JL, Strutt PA, Schall U. Social cognition dysfunction in adolescents with 22q11.2 deletion syndrome (velo-cardio-facial syndrome): relationship with executive functioning and social competence/functioning. J Intellect Disabil Res (2015) 59(9):845–59. doi:10.1111/jir.12183

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Franchini M, Schaer M, Glaser B, Kott-Radecka M, Debanné M, Schneider M, et al. Visual processing of emotional dynamic faces in 22q11.2 deletion syndrome. J Intellect Disabil Res (2016) 60(4):308–21. doi:10.1111/jir.12250

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Glaser B, Debban M, Ottet MC, Vuilleumier P, Zesiger P, Antonarakis SE, et al. Eye gaze during face processing in children and adolescents with 22q11.2 deletion syndrome. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry (2010) 49(7):665–74. doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2010.04.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Goldenberg PC, Calkins ME, Richard J, McDonald-McGinn D, Zackai E, Mitra N, et al. Computerized neurocognitive profile in young people with 22q11.2 deletion syndrome compared to youths with schizophrenia and at-risk for psychosis. Am J Med Genet (2012) 159B(1):87–93. doi:10.1002/ajmg.b.32005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Ho JS, Radoeva PD, Jalbrzikowski M, Chow C, Hopkins J, Tran W-C, et al. Deficits in mental state attributions in individuals with 22q11.2 deletion syndrome (Velo-cardio-facial syndrome). Autism Res (2012) 5(6):407–18. doi:10.1002/aur.1252

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Leleu A, Saucourt G, Rigard C, Chesnoy G, Baudouin JY, Rossi M, et al. Facial emotion perception by intensity in children and adolescents with 22q11.2 deletion syndrome. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry (2016) 25(3):297–310. doi:10.1007/s00787-015-0741-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. McCabe K, Rich D, Loughland CM, Schall U, Campbell LE. Visual scanpath abnormalities in 22q11.2 deletion syndrome: is this a face specific deficit? Psychiatry Res (2011) 189(2):292–8. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2011.06.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. McCabe KL, Melville JL, Rich D, Strutt PA, Cooper G, Loughland CM, et al. Divergent patterns of social cognition performance in autism and 22q11.2 deletion syndrome (22q11DS). J Autism Dev Disord (2013) 43(8):1926–34. doi:10.1007/s10803-012-1742-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. McCabe KL, Marlin S, Cooper G, Morris R, Schall U, Murphy DG, et al. Visual perception and processing in children with 22q11.2 deletion syndrome: associations with social cognition measures of face identity and emotion recognition. J Neurodev Disord (2016) 8:30. doi:10.1186/s11689-016-9164-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Shashi V, Veerapandiyan A, Schoch K, Kwapil T, Keshavan M, Ip E, et al. Social skills and associated psychopathology in children with chromosome 22q11.2 deletion syndrome: implications for interventions. J Intellect Disabil Res (2012) 56(9):865–78. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2788.2011.01477.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Vangkilde A, Jepsen JRM, Schmock H, Olesen C, Arnarsdóttir S, Baaré WF, et al. Associations between social cognition, skills, and function and subclinical negative and positive symptoms in 22q11.2 deletion syndrome. J Neurodev Disord (2016) 8:42. doi:10.1186/s11689-016-9175-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Bruno JL, Garrett AS, Quintin E-M, Mazaika PK, Reiss AL. Aberrant face and gaze habituation in Fragile X syndrome. Am J Psychiatry (2014) 171(10):1099–106. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.13111464

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Cornish K, Kogan C, Turk J, Manly T, James N, Mills A, et al. The emerging Fragile X premutation phenotype: evidence from the domain of social cognition. Brain Cogn (2005) 57(1):53–60. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2004.08.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Cornish KM, Burack JA, Rahman A, Munir F, Russo N, Grant C. Theory of mind deficits in children with Fragile X syndrome. J Intellect Disabil Res (2005) 79(Pt 5):372–8. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2788.2005.00678.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Crawford H, Moss J, Anderson G, Oliver C, McCleery JP. Implicit discrimination of basic facial expressions of positive/negative emotion in Fragile X syndrome and autism spectrum disorder. Am J Intellect Dev Disabil (2015) 120(4):328–45. doi:10.1352/1944-7558-120.4.328

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Garner C, Callias M, Turk J. Executive function and theory of mind performance of boys with Fragile-X syndrome. J Intellect Disabil Res (1999) 43(Pt6):466–74. doi:10.1046/j.1365-2788.1999.00207.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Grant C, Apperly I, Oliver C. Is theory of mind understanding impaired in males with Fragile X syndrome? J Abnorm Child Psychol (2007) 35(1):17–28. doi:10.1007/s10802-006-9077-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Hagan CC, Hoeft F, Mackey A, Mobbs D, Reiss AL. Aberrant neural function during emotion attribution in female subjects with Fragile X syndrome. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry (2008) 47(12):1443–54. doi:10.1097/CHI.0b013e3181886e92

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Mazzocco MM, Pennington BF, Hagerman R. Social cognition skills among females with Fragile X. J Autism Dev Disord (1994) 24(4):473–85. doi:10.1007/BF02172129

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Shaw TA, Porter MA. Emotion recognition and visual-scan paths in Fragile X syndrome. J Autism Dev Disord (2013) 43(5):1119–39. doi:10.1007/s10803-012-1654-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Simon EW, Finucane BM. Facial emotion identification in males with Fragile X syndrome. Am J Med Genet (1996) 67(1):77–80. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1096-8628(19960216)67:1<77::AID-AJMG13>3.0.CO;2-M

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Watson C, Hoeft F, Garrett AS, Hall SS, Reiss AL. Aberrant brain activation during gaze processing in boys with Fragile X syndrome. Arch Gen Psychiatry (2008) 65(11):1315–23. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.65.11.1315

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Williams TA, Porter MA, Langdon R. Viewing social scenes: a visual scan-path study comparing Fragile X syndrome and Williams syndrome. J Autism Dev Disord (2013) 43(8):1880–94. doi:10.1007/s10803-012-1737-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Wishart JG, Cebula KR, Willis DS, Pitcairn TK. Understanding of facial expressions of emotion by children with intellectual disabilities of differing aetiology. J Intellect Disabil Res (2007) 51(Pt7):551–63. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2788.2006.00947.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Anaki D, Zadikov Mor T, Gepstein V, Hochberg Z. Face perception in women with Turner syndrome and its underlying factors. Neuropsychologia (2016) 90:274–85. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.08.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Good CD, Lawrence K, Thomas NS, Price CJ, Ashburner J, Friston KJ, et al. Dosage-sensitive X-linked locus influences the development of amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex, and fear recognition in humans. Brain (2003) 126(Pt 11):2431–46. doi:10.1093/brain/awg242

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Hong DS, Bray S, Haas BW, Hoeft F, Reiss AL. Aberrant neurocognitive processing of fear in young girls with Turner syndrome. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci (2014) 9(3):255–64. doi:10.1093/scan/nss133

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Lawrence K, Kuntsi J, Coleman M, Campbell R, Skuse D. Face and emotion recognition deficits in Turner syndrome: a possible role for X-linked genes in amygdala development. Neuropsychology (2003) 17(1):39–49. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.17.1.39

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Mazzola F, Seigal A, MacAskill A, Corden B, Lawrence K, Skuse DH. Eye tracking and fear recognition deficits in Turner syndrome. Soc Neurosci (2006) 1(3–4):259–69. doi:10.1080/17470910600989912

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Roelofs RL, Wingbermühle E, Freriks K, Verhaak CM, Kessels RP, Egger JI. Alexithymia, emotion perception, and social assertiveness in adult women with Noonan and Turner syndromes. Am J Med Genet A (2015) 167A(4):768–76. doi:10.1002/ajmg.a.37006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Ross JL, Stefanatos GA, Kushner H, Zinn A, Bondy C, Roeltgen D. Persistent cognitive deficits in adult women with Turner syndrome. Neurology (2002) 58(2):218–25. doi:10.1212/WNL.58.2.218

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Ross JL, Stefanatos GA, Kushner H, Bondy C, Nelson L, Zinn A, et al. The effect of genetic differences and ovarian failure: intact cognitive function in adult women with premature ovarian failure versus turner syndrome. J Clin Endocrinol Metab (2004) 89(4):1817–22. doi:10.1210/jc.2003-031463

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Godbee K, Porter MA. Attribution of negative intention in Williams syndrome. Res Dev Disabil (2013) 34(5):1602–12. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2013.01.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Järvinen A, Ng R, Crivelli D, Arnold AJ, Woo-VonHoogenstyn N, Bellugi U. Relations between social-perceptual ability in multi- and unisensory contexts, autonomic reactivity, and social functioning in individuals with Williams syndrome. Neuropsychologia (2015) 73:127–40. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.04.035

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Järvinen A, Ng R, Crivelli D, Neumann D, Arnold AJ, Woo-VonHoogenstyn N, et al. Social functioning and autonomic nervous system sensitivity across vocal and musical emotion in Williams syndrome and autism spectrum disorder. Dev Psychobiol (2015) 58(1):17–26. doi:10.1002/dev.21335

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

96. Järvinen-Pasley A, Pollak SD, Yam A, Hill KJ, Grichanik M, Mills D, et al. Atypical hemispheric asymmetry in the perception of negative human vocalizations in individuals with Williams syndrome. Neuropsychologia (2010) 48(4):1047–52. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.12.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

97. Järvinen-Pasley A, Vines BW, Hill KJ, Yam A, Grichanik M, Mills D, et al. Cross-modal influences of affect across social and non-social domains in individuals with Williams syndrome. Neuropsychologia (2010) 48(2):456–66. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.10.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

98. John AE, Rowe ML, Mervis CB. Referential communication skills of children with Williams Syndrome: understanding when messages are not adequate. Am J Intellect Dev Disabil (2009) 114(2):85–99. doi:10.1352/2009.114.85-99

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

99. Lacroix A, Guidetti M, Rogé B, Reilly J. Recognition of emotional and nonemotional facial expressions: a comparison between Williams syndrome and autism. Res Dev Disabil (2009) 30(5):976–85. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2009.02.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

100. Little K, Riby DM, Janes E, Clark F, Fleck R, Rodgers J. Heterogeneity of social approach behaviour in Williams syndrome: the role of response inhibition. Res Dev Disabil (2013) 34(3):959–67. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2012.11.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

101. Martínez-Castilla P, Burt M, Borgatti R, Gagliardi C. Facial emotion recognition in Williams syndrome and Down syndrome: a matching and developmental study. Child Neuropsychol (2014) 21(5):668–92. doi:10.1080/09297049.2014.945408

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

102. Mobbs D, Garrett AS, Menon V, Rose FE, Bellugi U, Reiss AL. Anomalous brain activation during face and gaze processing in Williams syndrome. Neurology (2004) 62(11):2070–6. doi:10.1212/01.WNL.0000129536.95274.DC

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

103. Plesa-Skwerer D, Faja S, Schofield C, Verbalis A, Tager-Flusberg H. Perceiving facial and vocal expressions of emotion in individuals with Williams syndrome. Am J Ment Retard (2006) 111(1):15–26. doi:10.1352/0895-8017(2006)111[15:PFAVEO]2.0.CO;2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

104. Porter M, Coltheart M, Langdon R. The neuropsychological basis of hypersociability in Williams and Down syndrome. Neuropsychologia (2007) 45(12):2839–49. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.05.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

105. Porter MA, Coltheart M, Langdon R. Theory of mind in Williams syndrome assessed using a nonverbal task. J Autism Dev Disord (2008) 38(5):806–14. doi:10.1007/s10803-007-0447-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

106. Porter MA, Shaw TA, Marsh PJ. An unusual attraction to the eyes in Williams-Beuren syndrome: a manipulation of facial affect while measuring face scanpaths. Cogn Neuropsychiatry (2010) 15(6):505–30. doi:10.1080/13546801003644486

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

107. Riby DM, Doherty-Sneddon G, Bruce V. Exploring face perception in disorders of development: evidence from Williams syndrome and autism. J Neuropsychol (2008) 2(Pt1):47–64. doi:10.1348/174866407X255690

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

108. Riby DM, Hancock PJ. Do faces capture the attention of individuals with Williams syndrome or autism? Evidence from tracking eye movements. J Autism Dev Disord (2009) 39(3):421–31. doi:10.1007/s10803-008-0641-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

109. Riby DM, Hancock PJ, Jones N, Hanley M. Spontaneous and cued gaze-following in autism and Williams syndrome. J Neurodev Disord (2013) 5(1):13. doi:10.1186/1866-1955-5-13

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

110. Santos A, Rosset D, Deruelle C. Human versus non-human face processing: evidence from Williams syndrome. J Autism Dev Disord (2009) 39(11):1552–9. doi:10.1007/s10803-009-0789-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

111. Santos A, Deruelle C. verbal peaks and visual valleys in theory of mind ability in Williams syndrome. J Autism Dev Disord (2009) 39(4):651–9. doi:10.1007/s10803-008-0669-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

112. Sparaci L, Stefanini S, Marotta L, Vicari S, Rizzolatti G. Understanding motor acts and motor intentions in Williams syndrome. Neuropsychologia (2012) 50(7):1639–49. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.03.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

113. Sparaci L, Stefanini S, D’Elia L, Vicari S, Rizzolatti G. What and why understanding in autism spectrum disorders and Williams syndrome: similarities and differences. Autism Res (2014) 7(4):421–32. doi:10.1002/aur.1370

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

114. Skwerer DP, Borum L, Verbalis A, Schofield C, Crawford N, Ciciolla L, et al. Autonomic responses to dynamic displays of facial expressions in adolescents and adults with Williams syndrome. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci (2009) 4(1):93–100. doi:10.1093/scan/nsn041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

115. Tager-Flusberg H, Sullivan K. A componential view of theory of mind: evidence from Williams syndrome. Cognition (2000) 76(Pt1):59–90. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00069-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

116. Van Herwegen J, Dimitriou D, Rundblad G. Performance on verbal and low-verbal false belief tasks: evidence from children with Williams syndrome. J Commun Disord (2013) 46(5–6):440–8. doi:10.1016/j.jcomdis.2013.10.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

117. Widen SC, Russell JA. A closer look at preschoolers’ freely produced labels for facial expressions. Dev Psychol (2003) 39(1):114–28. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.39.1.114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

118. Djukic A, Rose SA, Jankowski JJ, Feldman JF. Rett syndrome: recognition of facial expression and its relation to scanning patterns. Pediatr Neurol (2014) 51(5):650–6. doi:10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2014.07.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

119. Williams TA, Porter MA, Langdon R. Social approach and emotion recognition in Fragile X syndrome. Am J Intellect Dev Disabil (2014) 119(2):133–50. doi:10.1352/1944-7558-119.2.133

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

120. Van Rijn S, Stockmann L, van Buggenhout G, van Ravenswaaij-Arts C, Swaab H. Social cognition and underlying cognitive mechanisms in children with an extra X chromosome: a comparison with autism spectrum disorder. Genes Brain Behav (2014) 13(5):459–67. doi:10.1111/gbb.12134

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

121. Hernandez N, Metzger A, Magné R, Bonnet-Brilhault F, Roux S, Barthélémy C, et al. Exploration of core features of a human face by healthy and autistic adults analyzed by visual scanning. Neuropsychologia (2009) 47(4):1004–12. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.10.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

122. Schurgin MW, Nelson J, Iida S, Ohira H, Chiao JY, Franconeri SL. Eye movements during emotion recognition in faces. J Vis (2014) 14(13):14. doi:10.1167/14.13.14

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

123. Lo ST, Siemensma E, Collin P, Hokken-Koelega A. Impaired theory of mind and symptoms of autism spectrum disorder in children with Prader-Willi syndrome. Res Dev Disabil (2013) 34(9):2764–73. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2013.05.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

124. Lewis P, Abbeduto L, Murphy M, Richmond E, Giles N, Bruno L, et al. Cognitive, language and social-cognitive skills of individuals with Fragile X syndrome with and without autism. J Intellect Disabil Res (2006) 50(Pt 7):532–45. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2788.2006.00803.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

125. Sullivan K, Tager-Flusberg H. Second-order belief attribution in Williams syndrome: intact or impaired? Am J Ment Retard (1999) 104:523–32. doi:10.1352/0895-8017(1999)104<0523:SBAIWS>2.0.CO;2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

126. Steerneman P, Meesters C. ToM Test-R Handleiding. Antwerpen-Apeldoorn: GarantUitgevers (2009).

Google Scholar

127. Iacoboni M, Molnar-Szakacs I, Gallese V, Buccino G, Mazziotta JC, Rizzolatti G. Grasping the intentions of others with one’s own mirror neuron system. PLoS Biol (2005) 3(3):e79. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.0030079

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

128. Riby DM, Hanley M, Kirk H, Clark F, Little K, Fleck R, et al. The interplay between anxiety and social functioning in Williams syndrome. J Autism Dev Disord (2014) 44:1220–9. doi:10.1007/s10803-013-1984-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

129. Hali H, Grice SJ, Bolton R, Johnson MH. Face and gaze processing in Prader-Willi syndrome. J Neuropsychol (2008) 2(Pt 1):65–77. doi:10.1348/174866407X243305

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

130. Thakkar KN, Park S. Empathy, schizotypy, and visuospatial transformations. Cogn Neuropsychiatry (2010) 15(5):477–500. doi:10.1080/13546801003711350

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

131. Spreng RN. Examining the role of memory in social cognition. Front Psychol (2013) 4:437. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00437

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

132. Carlson SM, Moses LJ. Individual differences in inhibitory control and children’s theory of mind. Child Dev (2001) 72(4):1032–53. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00333

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

133. Hobson RP. Autism and the Development of Mind. London: Psychology Press (1995). 246 p.

Google Scholar

134. Demily C, Rigard C, Peyroux E, Chesnoy-Servanin G, Morel A, Franck N. «Cognitus & Moi»: a computer-based cognitive remediation program for children with intellectual disability. Front Psychiatry (2016) 7:10. doi:10.3389/FPSYT.2016.00010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

135. Glaser B, Lothe A, Chabloz M, Dukes D, Pasca C, Redoute J, et al. Candidate socioemotional remediation program for individuals with intellectual disability. Am J Intellect Dev Disabil (2012) 117(5):368–83. doi:10.1352/1944-7558-117.5.368

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

136. Ambadar Z, Schooler JW, Cohn JF. Deciphering the enigmatic face: the importance of facial dynamics in interpreting subtle facial expressions. Psychol Sci (2005) 16(5):403–10. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01548.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

137. Furl N, Hadj-Bouziane F, Liu N, Averbeck BB, Ungerleider LG. Dynamic and static facial expressions decoded from motion-sensitive areas in the macaque monkey. J Neurosci (2012) 32(45):15952–62. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1992-12.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

138. Gepner B, Deruelle C, Grynfeltt S. Motion and emotion: a novel approach to the study of face processing by autistic children. J Autism Dev Disord (2001) 31:37–45. doi:10.1023/A:1005609629218

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

139. Klein-Tasman BP, Phillips KD, Lord CE, Mervis CB, Gallo F. Overlap with the autism spectrum in young children with Williams syndrome. J Dev Behav Pediatr (2009) 30(4):289–99. doi:10.1097/DBP.0b013e3181ad1f9a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

140. Shashi V, Harrell W, Eack S, Sanders C, McConkie-Rosell A, Keshavan MS, et al. Social cognitive training in adolescents with chromosome 22q11.2 deletion syndrome: feasibility and preliminary effects of the intervention. J Intellect Disabil Res (2015) 59(10):902–13. doi:10.1111/jir.12192

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

141. Fabio RA, Castelli I, Marchetti A, Antonietti A. Training communication abilities in Rett syndrome through reading and writing. Front Psychol (2013) 4:911. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00911

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

142. Antonietti A, Castelli I, Fabio RA, Marchetti A. Understanding emotions and mental states from faces and pictures in Rett syndrome. In: Balconi M, editor. Emotional Face Comprehension Neuropsychological Perspectives. New York: Nova Science Publishers (2008). p. 205–32.

Google Scholar

143. Djukic A, Valicenti McDermott M, Mavrommatis K, Martins CL. Rett syndrome: basic features of visual processing-a pilot study of eye-tracking. Pediatr Neurol (2012) 47(1):25–9. doi:10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2012.04.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

144. Lariviere J. Eye Gaze Technology For Girls With Rette Syndrome: From Trials to Conversations. (2014). Available from: http://www.dbmhresource.org/uploads/2/2/5/7/225717

Google Scholar

145. Lariviere JA. Eye tracking: eye-gaze technology. 2nd ed. In: Söderback I, editor. International Handbook of Occupational Therapy Interventions. Switzerland: Springer International Publishing (2015). p. 339–62.

Google Scholar

146. Townend GS, Marschik PB, Smeets E, van de Berg R, van den Berg M, Curfs LMG. Eye gaze technology as a form of augmentative and alternative communication for individuals with Rett syndrome: experiences of families in the Netherlands. J Dev Phys Disabil (2016) 28:101–12. doi:10.1007/s10882-015-9455-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

147. Matsumoto D, Keltner D, Shiota MN, O’Sullivan M, Frank M. Facial expressions of emotion. 3rd ed. In: Lewis M, Haviland-Jones JM, Barrett LF, editors. Handbook of Emotions. New York: Guilford (2008). p. 211–34.

Google Scholar

148. Tooby J, Cosmides L. The psychological foundations of culture. In: Barkow J, Cosmides L, Tooby J, editors. The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture. New York: Oxford University Press (1992). p. 19–136.

Google Scholar

149. Dezecache G, Mercier H, Scott-Phillips TC. An evolutionary approach to emotional communication. J Pragmatics (2013) 59:221–33. doi:10.1016/j.pragma.2013.06.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

150. Marsh AA, Ambady N, Kleck RE. The effects of fear and anger facial expressions on approach- and avoidance-related behaviors. Emotion (2005) 5(1):119–24. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.5.1.119

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

151. Winkielman P, Berridge KC, Wilbarger JL. Unconscious affective reactions to masked happy versus angry faces influence consumption behavior and judgments of value. Pers Soc Psychol Bull (2005) 31(1):121–35. doi:10.1177/0146167204271309

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: social cognition, facial emotion recognition, theory of mind, systematic review, genetics, neurodevelopmental disorders

Citation: Morel A, Peyroux E, Leleu A, Favre E, Franck N and Demily C (2018) Overview of Social Cognitive Dysfunctions in Rare Developmental Syndromes With Psychiatric Phenotype. Front. Pediatr. 6:102. doi: 10.3389/fped.2018.00102

Received: 25 July 2017; Accepted: 27 March 2018;
Published: 03 May 2018

Edited by:

Alberto Spalice, Policlinico Umberto I, Italy

Reviewed by:

Wei-Liang Chen, Medical College of Wisconsin, United States
Alain Dervaux, Centre Hospitalier Universitaire d’Amiens, France

Copyright: © 2018 Morel, Peyroux, Leleu, Favre, Franck and Demily. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Caroline Demily, caroline.demily@ch-le-vinatier.fr

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.