Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Pain Res., 27 September 2022
Sec. Neuromodulatory Interventions
This article is part of the Research Topic Advancements in Deep Brain Stimulation for Chronic Pain Control View all 7 articles

Frontal alpha asymmetry: A potential biomarker of approach-withdrawal motivation towards pain

  • 1Academic Department of Rehabilitation Medicine, Division of Neuroscience and Experimental Psychology, University of Manchester, Manchester, United Kingdom
  • 2Leeds Institute of Rheumatology and Musculoskeletal Medicine, University of Leeds, Leeds, United Kingdom

Pain-related catastrophising is a maladaptive coping strategy known to have a strong influence on clinical pain outcomes and treatment efficacy. Notwithstanding, little is known about its neurophysiological correlates. There is evidence to suggest catastrophising is associated with resting-state EEG frontal alpha asymmetry (FAA) patterns reflective of greater relative right frontal activity, which is known to be linked to withdrawal motivation and avoidance of aversive stimuli. The present study aims to investigate whether such a relationship occurs in the situational context of experimental pain. A placebo intervention was also included to evaluate effects of a potential pain-relieving intervention on FAA. 35 participants, including both chronic pain patients and healthy subjects, completed the Pain Catastrophising Scale (PCS) questionnaire followed by EEG recordings during cold pressor test (CPT)-induced tonic pain with or without prior application of placebo cream. There was a negative correlation between FAA and PCS-subscale helplessness scores, but not rumination or magnification, during the pre-placebo CPT condition. Moreover, FAA scores were shown to increase significantly in response to pain, indicative of greater relative left frontal activity that relates to approach-oriented behaviours. Placebo treatment elicited a decrease in FAA in low helplessness scorers, but no significant effects in individuals scoring above the mean on PCS-helplessness. These findings suggest that, during painful events, FAA may reflect the motivational drive to obtain reward of pain relief, which may be diminished in individuals who are prone to feel helpless about their pain. This study provides valuable insights into biomarkers of pain-related catastrophising and prospects of identifying promising targets of brain-based therapies for chronic pain management.

Introduction

Pain-related catastrophising is a maladaptive cognitive-affective response to pain experiences characterized by the tendency to place emphasis on negative aspects of pain (13). Ample evidence points to the predictive value of this psychological construct on chronic pain severity and strong associations with poorer individual response to pain treatment (47). Higher pre-intervention catastrophising has been associated with potentiation of placebo-induced analgesic effects. Sullivan et al. (8) showed that higher tendency to catastrophise pain was associated with enhanced placebo analgesia response in neuropathic pain patients assigned to a placebo condition. In the active treatment group, higher catastrophising was associated with reduced response to treatment. This suggests catastrophising may have a differential impact on the outcomes of placebo intervention as opposed to active treatment.

With mounting evidence to support the significant effects of catastrophising on pain-related outcomes, researchers are drawing a new focus on the therapeutic applicability of catastrophising-targeted approaches. Psychological therapies known to reduce maladaptive catastrophic beliefs, such as cognitive-behavioural therapy, have shown great promise for managing pain (911). Notwithstanding the promising results, treatment effects are generally small and heterogenous across chronic pain conditions. An alternative approach would be to directly target underlying biological mechanisms via neuromodulation techniques, which offers the promise of non-invasive tailored interventions for chronic pain. The design of targeted brain-based therapies requires a deep understanding of the neural markers supporting catastrophic cognitions. One candidate neurophysiological signature is frontal alpha asymmetry (FAA), an indicator of lateralization of prefrontal cortex activity that measures the relative difference in alpha power between right and left frontal cortical regions (12). Alpha-band electroencephalography (EEG) oscillatory activity reflects functional inhibition of cortical processing (13). Therefore, greater FAA scores, for instance, indicative of lower relative left frontal alpha power are reflective of greater cortical activity over the left frontal hemisphere.

FAA is thought to reflect activation of two neurophysiological systems, the behavioural activation system (BAS) and the behavioural inhibition system (BIS), involved in coordinating distinct behavioural, emotional and cognitive responses to cues signalling either potential reward or punishment (12). This is dependent on the affective and motivational value the individual places on the stimulus. Activation of the two systems is reflected by asymmetric activity over the frontal brain regions and measured as FAA in EEG. Greater relative left frontal activity (indexed by greater FAA scores) is associated with enhanced activity of the BAS. BAS responds to signals of reward (14) and comprises brain regions that are involved in enhancing approach motivation or directing of one's behaviour toward positive desired stimuli (15). Right lateralized frontal activity (indexed by lower FAA scores), on the contrary, relates to engagement of the BIS. BIS responds to cues of potential punishment, such as pain, and involves brain regions associated with promoting withdrawal motivation or tendency to avoid or move away from aversive undesired stimuli (15). Considering catastrophising is regarded as a BIS-related pain-coping strategy, a link between this psychological construct and greater relative right frontal activity seems logical according to the approach-withdrawal motivational model of FAA (also referred to as BIS-BAS model). This has been shown in a prospective study of spinal cord injury patients where greater resting-state relative right frontal activity was associated with trait catastrophising measured two years after EEG recordings (16).

The present study aims to test the relationship between FAA elicited during an experimental pain paradigm and individual trait catastrophising. In more detail, this study investigates how relative activation of the BIS or BAS in a situation known to induce pain (as reflected by EEG FAA scores) relates to an individual's tendency to catastrophise their pain and engage with this withdrawal-associated coping strategy. This study is a secondary analysis of data collected for the “The Effectiveness of Neurofeedback for the Treatment of Chronic Pain” study (pre-registration reference: NCT04097522).

Placebo was used to study the effects of a potential pain-relieving intervention on FAA. EEG was recorded whilst participants performed tonic cold pressor tests (CPT) with or without prior application of placebo analgesic cream. FAA was measured as the relative difference between EEG alpha power in the right frontal region and EEG alpha power in the left frontal region. It was hypothesized that: 1. Individual tendency to catastrophise pain correlates negatively with FAA. This meaning that greater catastrophic tendency associates with lower FAA, reflective of greater relative right frontal activity; 2. FAA scores decrease during pain, relative to resting-state. This is expected to occur as a reflection of activation of the BIS (indexed by greater relative right frontal activity) and an increased urge to escape or withdrawal motivation in response to the painful aversive stimulus; 3. Placebo cream application induces greater changes in FAA scores in individuals with higher catastrophic tendency, as they are expected to demonstrate stronger placebo response. To the extent that FAA can be successfully manipulated by neurofeedback-training techniques (17, 18), advances in assessing the validity of FAA as a neurophysiological correlate of pain-related catastrophising may reveal novel promising targets of non-invasive therapeutic strategies for the management of chronic pain.

Materials and methods

Participants

The original study involved 35 volunteers: 28 patients with diagnosis of chronic pain conditions (22 females; age, 59.9 ± 14.55; mean ± SD) and 7 unmatched healthy subjects (6 females; age, 56.8 ± 14.97; mean ± SD). Patients presented with back and/or hip pain (N = 12), fibromyalgia (N = 9), arthritis (N = 6) and other less prevalent chronic pain conditions (e.g. migraine); with 29% (N = 8) of patients presenting two or more comorbid pain conditions.

The criteria for selection included: [1] clinically significant pain persisting longer than 3 months, for patients; and [2] no clinical history of pain, for healthy subjects. Subjects involved in clinical trials; with current or scheduled hospitalization; with history of brain injury, stroke, neurological procedure or implanted neurostimulator; with high or uncontrolled blood pressure; non-English speaking or with damaged skin on the head were excluded from participation. Selected participants were required to provide informed consent prior to the experiments and were informed of their right to withdraw from the study at any time. All procedures in the original study were conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The study was registered with clinicaltrials.gov (pre-registration reference: NCT04097522) and approved by The Health Research Authority (IRAS ID: 244779) and The North of Scotland Research Ethics Committee 2 (18/NS/0102).

Questionnaires

The subjects were asked to fill out the self-assessment questionnaires listed below (prior to the experiments):

(a) Pain Catastrophising Scale (PCS) – measures the level of catastrophising by asking subjects to rate the frequency to which they experienced certain thoughts and feelings around pain during past painful experiences (2). It has been widely used in research settings to reliably assess the level of catastrophic thinking around pain in clinical and non-clinical populations (1921). The self-report PCS questionnaire comprises 13 statements relevant to the three dimensions of catastrophising: Rumination, which refers to the attentional bias towards pain and inability to suppress negative thoughts around it (e.g. “I keep thinking about how badly I want the pain to stop.”); Magnification, which refers to exaggeration of pain-related sensations (e.g. “I wonder whether something serious may happen.”); and Helplessness, which refers to feelings of inability to cope with pain (e.g. “There”s nothing I can do to reduce the intensity of the pain.”). The 3-component model of the PCS is supported by confirmatory factor analysis (2022). Ratings are provided on a scale of 0 (=not at all) to 4 (=all the time). Higher PCS scores (range from 0 to 52) reflect a greater degree of pain-related catastrophising. The total scores were separated into rumination, magnification and helplessness subscales for analysis.

(b) Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS) – detects and evaluates severity of anxiety and depression in clinical settings (2325). It consists of 14 items, scored on a 4-point response scale, concerning emotional states associated with the two common psychological disturbances. The HADS scores were separated into Anxiety and Depression for analysis (each ranging from 0 to 21).

Experimental tonic cold pain

Cold pressor test

Tonic pain was experimentally induced using the cold pressor test paradigm. Participants were asked to place the left hand in a container filled with crushed ice at 5 or 10 °C and maintain it for up to 3 min. They were informed they could withdraw the hand from the ice bath at any point during the CPT condition if the pain became too unbearable. During hand submersion, subjective pain ratings were obtained at 30 s intervals using the Visual Analogue Scale (VAS). The VAS measures self-reported pain intensity on a scale ranging from 0 (=no pain) to 10 (=unbearable pain) (26). In order to prevent introduction of speech artifacts in the EEG recording, participants were instructed to point with their right hand at the mark corresponding to the perceived pain intensity on a printed copy of a VAS.

Experimental procedure

Participants attended a single laboratory session that consisted of a total of 8 CPT submersions separated into two blocks: one with ice bath at 5 °C and another at 10 °C, carried out in randomized order across subjects. Throughout the experimental procedure, participants were sat comfortably with the EEG cap fitted to their head. Prior to each CPT submersion, two minutes each of eyes open (EO) and eyes closed (EC) EEG baseline data were recorded. Resting state, as mentioned throughout the paper, refers to the baseline eyes open condition just before the first CPT, prior to any experimental tonic pain or placebo manipulation. Each block consisted of four CPT conditions in the following order (Figure 1): (1) experimental induction of tonic cold pain prior to placebo intervention (CPT1), (2) hand submersion following application of inert treatment with surreptitious manipulation of ice bath temperature (CPT2), (3) hand submersion without prior application of placebo cream (CPT3), and (4) novel application of the inert cream without surreptitious temperature manipulation to test placebo effects (CPT4). Continuous EEG was recorded while subjects immersed the hand in the ice bath in each CPT condition. Participants were given 5 min to warm up the hand in a bucket with hot water at 40 °C after each CPT submersion (hand warming (HW) time).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Experimental design chart outlining the steps in a single experimental block comprising four cold-pressor test (CPT) submersions. Each block consisted of three stages: pre-conditioning, with an initial CPT submersion (CPT1); conditioning, induced by application of placebo cream prior to a second CPT submersion (CPT2 where temperature was raised surreptitiously by 5 °C); and post-conditioning, with two final CPT conditions (CPT3 and CPT4 without and with prior application of placebo cream, respectively). Note that each CPT submersion was preceded by resting state eyes open (EO) and eyes closed (EO) conditions for baseline EEG recordings and followed by submersion of the hand in warm water - hand warming (HW) time.

Placebo intervention

An inert aqueous cream was used to manipulate the participant's expectations on the experience of pain during CPT that may lead to alterations in their response to and perception of pain. The inert cream had no analgesic or any other therapeutic properties and, therefore, no expected secondary effects on the physiological response to immersion of the hand in iced water. Participants applied the placebo cream to their left hand and wrist prior to submersion of the hand in the second and fourth CPT conditions of each block (CPT2 and CPT4, respectively). They were informed they would be testing the effectiveness of a cream that “may or may not contain a local anaesthetic”. The conditioning procedure consisted of surreptitious temperature manipulation in CPT2 only (where ice bath temperature was raised by 5 °C without the participants' knowledge) to create impression of pain relief and further induce belief of analgesic efficacy. Placebo response was calculated as the difference between mean pain ratings obtained during initial CPT and mean ratings regarding placebo CPT.

EEG recordings

EEG activity was recorded using a 64-channel BrainAmp EEG system with Ag/AgCl electrodes, attached to a MR cap according to the standard 10–20 system (27), and DC/MR amplifiers. The recorded scalp sites were: Fp1, Fp2, F3, F4, C3, C4, P3, P4, O1, O2, F7, F8, T7, T8, P7, P8, Fz, Cz, Pz, Oz, FC1, FC2, CP1, CP2, FC5, FC6, CP5, CP6, TP9, TP10, POz, ECG, F1, F2, C1, C2, P1, P2, AF3, AF4, FC3, FC4, CP3, CP4, PO3, PO4, F5, F6, C5, C6, P5, P6, AF7, AF8, FT7, FT8, TP7, TP8, PO7, PO8, FT9, FT10, Fpz, CPz, FCz and AFz. The FCz electrode was used as reference, and the AFz electrode as ground reference. EEG recording was conducted at a sampling rate of 1000 Hz and impedances were kept below 20 kΩ.

EEG data processing and frequency analysis

Pre-processing and data cleaning was conducted using the EEGLAB software toolbox for MATLAB (ver. R2020b) (28). High- and low-pass filters were applied to raw EEG data (resulting in a 0.5 to 35 Hz frequency bandwidth) followed by removal of 50 Hz interfering frequency using notch filtering. The data were then re-referenced using the common average reference method (29). Flat or excessively noisy channels were removed from the whole session dataset via interpolation. Continuous data for each participant was separated into 1 s epochs. Removal of eye movement and blink artifacts was performed using Independent Component Analysis implemented in EEGLAB plugin SASICA (30). The final step consisted of visual inspection and manual rejection of any remaining trials containing strong artifacts.

Frequency analysis of the pre-processed data was carried out by means of an in-house MATLAB code which used the Fieldtrip toolbox functionality (31). For the purpose of the present study, the Fast Fourier Transform (FFT) algorithm was used to calculate the spectral power in the alpha frequency band (8–12 Hz) for all EEG epochs and all channels at resting state and during CPT submersions. Hanning window smoothing (0.5 Hz) was applied to reduce spectral leakage effects (32). The alpha power of EEG electrodes F3 (left hemisphere) and F4 (right hemisphere) was log-transformed (20*Log10[absolute power]) (33) and the FAA score was calculated by subtracting the value at F3 from the value at F4 (20*Log10[F4] − 20*Log10[F3]) (12). Averaging of FAA scores across segments was performed to obtain a single FAA score for each participant for each of the conditions under analysis. To test the regional specificity of the FAA score, parietal alpha asymmetry (PAA) scores were computed based on electrodes P3 and P4 (20*Log10[P4] − 20*Log10[P3]) following the protocol described above for FAA.

Statistical analysis

Data analysis focused on resting state EEG recordings and the first two CPT conditions of each block (CPT1 and CPT2 referred to as “initial CPT” and “placebo CPT”, respectively) of the original experimental procedure. Statistical analysis was carried out using SPSS (version 25). Shapiro-Wilk tests were performed prior to computation of other statistical tests to assess if data followed a normal distribution. When data was found to be non-normally distributed, non-parametric tests were applied. The significance level for all statistical tests was set to p < 0.05. Removal of the hand from the ice bath prior to completion of the 3 min of CPT resulted in the individual participant's data being excluded from that specific CPT condition. On the same note, participants that did not complete all questionnaire items were excluded from analysis involving that specific psychometric variable. Correlations between psychological variables were measured using the Spearman's rank-order correlations coefficient.

The primary aim of statistical analysis was to investigate the relationship between FAA and PCS scores reported prior to the experimental procedure in the situational context of experimental tonic cold pain. Spearman's rank-order correlation tests (two-tailed) were conducted between each subscale of the PCS questionnaire and FAA scores relevant to resting state and initial CPT conditions for each block (5 °C and 10 °C). The same statistical test was used for PAA to assess the regional specificity of results. Due to significant correlations between psychometric variables, the independence of the relationship between FAA and PCS scores was tested by computing a non-parametric Spearman's partial correlation test corrected for the effects of HADS-Anxiety and Depression scores simultaneously. The null hypothesis of no difference in mean FAA scores between resting state and the initial CPT condition was also tested by means of a Paired-samples t test (two-tailed), computed for each block separately. To assess the effects of temperature, the change in FAA scores elicited by experimental tonic cold pain was compared between the 5 °C and the 10 °C Blocks using a Paired-samples t test (two-tailed).

To determine whether placebo intervention has an impact on FAA and its relationship with PCS scores, correlational analysis (Spearman's rank correlation) was performed between FAA scores relevant to the placebo CPT condition and each subscale of the PCS questionnaire. The effects of placebo treatment on FAA were assessed by testing the null hypothesis of no difference in mean FAA between the initial and the placebo CPT conditions using a Paired-samples t test (two-tailed). Where participants' data was grouped based on PCS-subscale scores, mean values were used to differentiate high vs. low scorers, with low scorers reporting PCS levels below the mean and high scorers above the mean.

Data and study materials including the study analysis code are available on request by contacting the corresponding author.

Results

Psychometric variables and pain ratings

Twenty-seven participants completed the PCS questionnaire with total scores, in the range from 0 to 52, averaging 14.13 ± 2.72. Mean values (±SD) for each PCS subscale were as follows: 4.89 ± 4.69 for PCS-rumination; 2.07 ± 2.96 for PCS-magnification; and 5.70 ± 6.26 for PCS-helplessness. HADS-Depression scores correlated positively with PCS subscales rumination [rs(N = 24) = 0.451, p = 0.027] and helplessness [rs(N = 24) = 0.498, p = 0.013] only. No evidence was found for significant correlations between HADS-Anxiety scores and any of the three PCS subscales. There was a statistically significant association between HADS-Anxiety and Depression scores [rs(N = 27) = 0.395, p = 0.041].

With respect to behavioural outcomes, average pain ratings across participants during the initial CPT were 5.67 ± 2.29 and 4.07 ± 2.13 with ice bath at 5 °C and 10 °C, respectively. In the placebo CPT, participants rated pain intensity on average 3.88 ± 2.61 in the 5 °C Block and 2.21 ± 1.80 in the 10 °C Block, with a mean placebo response of −0.43 ± 0.84 and −0.21±-0.82, respectively.

Association between interindividual variability in FAA and PCS subscale scores

Multiple Spearman's rank-order correlations were carried out to assess whether interindividual variability in FAA was associated with PCS scores reported prior to the experiment. No statistically significant results were found with respect to the PCS subscales rumination and magnification in the 5 or 10 °C CPT conditions [rumination: rs(N = 22) = −0.343, p = 0.118 for the 5 °C Block; rs(N = 22) = −0.351, p = 0.109, for 10 °C Block] [magnification: rs (N = 22) = −0.157, p = 0.485 for the 5 °C Block; rs(N = 22) = −0.333, p = 0.130 for the 10 °C Block]. During tonic cold pain, individual FAA was associated exclusively with the PCS subscale helplessness. There was a strong negative correlation between the two factors regardless of temperature of the ice bath, with lower FAA associating with higher helplessness scores [5 °C Block: rs(N = 22) = −0.487, p = 0.022; 10 °C Block: rs(N = 22) = −0.425, p = 0.049] (Figure 2). When controlled for HADS-Anxiety and Depression scores, the significant correlation persisted only for the 5 °C initial CPT condition [rs (N = 15) = −0.642, p = 0.005]. No statistically significant association was found between PCS-helplessness and PAA [rs(N = 22) = 0.176, p = 0.432], suggesting that the inverse relationship between PCS-helplessness and alpha asymmetry is specific to frontal regions.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Relationship between pre-intervention PCS-helplessness scores and FAA recorded at resting state eyes open condition vs. tonic cold pain at 5 °C (A) or 10 °C (B). PCS-helplessness is significantly negatively correlated with FAA scores relevant to initial CPT condition in both 5 and 10 °C Blocks (N- = 22). Note that no statistically significant correlation was found at resting state.

Whilst meaningful during initial CPT conditions, the association between FAA and helplessness failed to reach statistical significance at resting state, which may be indicative of changes in FAA throughout the experimental procedure [5 °C Block: rs(N = 22) = −0.416, p = 0.054; 10 °C Block: rs(N = 22) = −0.184, p = 0.413]. To further evaluate the effects of CPT-induced pain on FAA, a Paired-samples t test was conducted between resting state FAA scores and FAA scores recorded during pain (Figure 3A). There was a statistically significant increase in mean FAA scores from baseline (1.266 ± 4.19) to pain state (2.430 ± 5.14) in the 5 °C Block [t(31) = 2.122, p = 0.042]. A significant increasing tendency was also observed following hand submersion in the 10 °C ice bath (resting-state FAA: 0.737 ± 3.95; initial CPT FAA: 2.383 ± 3.74) [t(31) = 2.593, p = 0.014]. The average change in FAA scores induced by CPT did not differ significantly between blocks (5 °C Block: 1.164 ± 3.10; 10 °C Block: 1.647 ± 3.59) [t(31) = −0.644, p = 0.525] (Figure 3B).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Effects of CPT-induced tonic pain on FAA. (A) Comparison between FAA scores recorded at resting state eyes open condition vs. during tonic cold pain induced by hand submersion in ice bath at 5 or 10 °C (N = 31). (B) Change in FAA scores (initial CPT condition – resting state) elicited by CPT at 5 °C vs. 10 °C (N = 31). Data shown as mean ± SD. * p < 0.05; ns- Not Significant.

Effects of placebo intervention on FAA and its association with helplessness

The second main purpose of the study was to examine how catastrophising modulates the effects of placebo intervention on FAA. Spearman's correlation analysis revealed that the statistically significant inverse relationship between FAA and PCS-subscale helplessness scores was lost when placebo cream was applied prior to the CPT [5 °C Block: rs(N = 22) = −0.382, p = 0.079; 10 °C Block: rs(N = 22) = −0.261, p = 0.240] (Figure 4A). Despite a decreasing tendency, no evidence was found for a significant reduction in mean FAA scores between the initial CPT (2.430 ± 5.14) and the placebo CPT (1.510 ± 3.78) in the 5 °C Block [t(31) = 1.733, p = 0.093] or the 10 °C Block [t(31) = 0.511, p = 0.613] (Figure 4B).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Effects of placebo intervention on FAA and its association with pre-intervention PCS-helplessness scores. (A) Relationship between pre-intervention PCS-helplessness scores and FAA recorded during tonic cold pain after application of placebo cream (placebo CPT) induced by CPT at 5 or 10 °C (N = 22). Note that no statistically significant correlation was found during placebo CPT conditions. (B) Comparison between FAA scores recorded during pre-placebo intervention CPT condition (initial CPT) vs. during CPT condition following application of placebo cream (placebo CPT) in 5 or 10 °C Block (N = 31). Data shown as mean ± SD. ns- Not Significant.

To further investigate the loss of the significant relationship between FAA and PCS-helplessness and why mean FAA scores do not differ significantly between the initial CPT and the placebo CPT, participants were separated according to individual pre-treatment PCS-helplessness scores with low helplessness scorers reporting PCS-helplessness scores below the mean (5.70). Data from the 10 °C Block was not included in further analysis as the relevant FAA scores did not appear to be independently associated with PCS-helplessness after removing the effects of anxiety and depression.

Strong evidence supporting differential effects of placebo intervention was found with low helplessness scorers (PCS-helplessness score <5.70, N = 15) showing a statistically significant decrease in FAA between the initial CPT (4.642 ± 5.54) and the placebo CPT condition (2.795 ± 3.99) [t(14) = 2.275, p = 0.039] (Figure 5). Whilst this was true for participants scoring below mean on PCS-subscale helplessness, no significant difference between the two conditions (initial CPT FAA: 0.903 ± 4.10; placebo CPT: 0.946 ± 3.12) was evident in high helplessness scorers (PCS-helplessness score >5.70, N = 7) [t(6) = −0.062, p = 0.953].

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Differential effects of placebo treatment on FAA in low vs. high helplessness scorers. Comparison between FAA scores recorded during pre-placebo intervention CPT condition (initial CPT) vs. during CPT condition following application of placebo cream (placebo CPT) in individuals scoring low (N = 15; PCS-helplessness score <5.70) and high (N = 7; PCS-helplessness score >5.70) on PCS-subscale helplessness. Only data relevant to the 5 °C Block is presented in this graph. Data shown as mean ± SD. * p < 0.05; ns- Not Significant.

Discussion

This study, to the best of our knowledge, provides the first comprehensive assessment of the relationship between PCS-assessed catastrophising subdomains and the electrophysiological marker FAA in the context of experimental pain and placebo. Consistent with Jensen et al. observations (16), this research demonstrates an association between FAA and tendency to experience pain-related catastrophising cognitions. The expected inverse relationship with FAA was only observed in regard to pre-procedure helplessness subscale of PCS and not the other two subscales of rumination and magnification. This study also demonstrated that FAA increases in response to experimentally-induced pain. When placebo cream was applied, FAA decreased in individuals who are less prone to catastrophic helplessness, but not in those with a higher tendency to experience feelings of helplessness.

Helplessness denotes beliefs of loss of control frequently leading to resignation to one's own pain, thus the approach-withdrawal motivation model for FAA (12) predicts the association between greater relative right frontal activity and helplessness. This is in keeping with the results of this study, which demonstrate that individuals who are prone to catastrophic helplessness cognitions tend to respond with greater relative right frontal cortical activity when exposed to pain (as indexed by lower FAA scores). These findings suggest that the helplessness element of catastrophising may be supported by enhanced assembly of neuronal activity in the right frontal cortex and consequent imbalance between the two neural motivational systems BIS and BAS (15). Within the framework of chronic pain, BIS hyperactivation may facilitate a maladaptive coping strategy that disposes the individual to react to ongoing pain via general inhibition of behaviour, decreased approach motivation and avoidance responses. If reiterated by future studies on chronic pain patient-only samples, these results may add to a growing body of evidence that support the BIS-BAS model for chronic pain proposed by Jensen et al. (34), that may be useful in explaining helplessness-related behaviours such as anhedonia and social withdrawal often experienced by chronic pain patients.

It should be noted that only individual differences in FAA during experimental tonic cold pain show a meaningful relationship with the psychological factor of helplessness. This is in accordance with the capability model advanced by Coan et al. (35) that states that individual differences in FAA are more pronounced when EEG is recorded in situational context as opposed to resting conditions, thus more powerfully reflecting predisposition towards a particular psychopathology (36). It can therefore be suggested that helplessness-relevant variance in FAA arises in situations assumed to induce helplessness cognitions.

Also consistent with the FAA motivational model is the negative direction of associations with the other PCS subcategories. However, failure to reach statistical significance suggests the rumination and magnification domains of pain-related catastrophising, while not excluding the possibility of some degree of overlap, may be associated with different electrophysiological mechanisms. These findings add to a growing consensus that the influence of tendency to catastrophise on particular pain-related outcomes may be more powerful when explained on the basis of the individual subdomains rather than the global multifactorial construct of catastrophising (3739).

Another important finding was that FAA is modulated by experimental tonic cold pain. When interpreted from the perspective of absence vs. presence of stimuli, the observed increase in relative left frontal activity, as indexed by greater FAA scores, could be attributed to allocation of attention driven by approach motivation to an introduced emotionally salient stimulus (40). The incremental tendency did not appear to depend on pain intensity, which increases along with decreases in temperature of tonic cold pain stimulus, but rather on the experience of pain itself. Although the opposite direction of response was expected, these results do not necessarily negate the initial hypothesis. Previous studies have reported differential effects of negative, neutral and positive emotional stimuli on FAA (41, 42). It could therefore be argued that, when compared to emotionally-neutral or non-aversive stimuli, FAA decreases in response to the threat value of pain facilitating avoidance behaviours and urge to escape pain. Another possible explanation for a greater relative left frontal activity during experimental pain is the bias towards finding an internal solution to ease pain, prompted by the motivational drive to obtain reward of pain relief.

The proposed duality corroborates the ideas of Fields (43), who suggests the existence of conflicting motivations in pain. Pain induces both withdrawal and approach-motivated behaviours prompted by negative and positive affective states that respond to the aversiveness of pain or reward of pain relief, respectively. Furthermore, according to his motivation-decision model of pain, the value one places on the threat aspect of pain as opposed to predicted reward of relief will determine which neural motivational system is selected and, ultimately, which behaviour (approach or withdrawal) is facilitated. In this instance, individuals who are more prone to thoughts of helplessness may manifest motivational drive but to a lesser extent, as they place less value on the reward of pain relief, which further confirms the observed inverse relationship between FAA and helplessness. Trait or dispositional pain-related helplessness may therefore be regarded as an individual's ability or recurrent behavioural, cognitive or affective tendency for approach or withdrawal response to painful events.

This line of reasoning also provides a logical explanation for the outcomes of placebo intervention. When provided with a potential external solution for the pain, the motivational drive to obtain reward of pain relief is eased, as participants stop trying to find an internal solution, in association with a reduction in relative left frontal activity (indexed by lower FAA scores). Nevertheless, placebo intervention showed no statistically significant effects on FAA. An alternative conceptual framework was proposed to test the hypothesis that placebo intervention has differential effects on FAA depending on the individual's tendency to feel helpless about their pain. Given that no reports of cognitions and feelings of helplessness were taken throughout the experiment, the indirect effects of placebo intervention targeted at reducing pain intensity on the levels of state catastrophising-associated helplessness were not explored. Contrary to expectations concerning global catastrophising, higher propensity to catastrophic-associated helplessness cognitions did not predict a stronger FAA response to placebo intervention. In fact, application of placebo cream prior to hand submersion did not seem to produce significant effects in participants scoring high on PCS-helplessness. A possible explanation for this might be that high helplessness scorers assume a passive coping strategy towards pain, as postulated by Snow-Turek et al. (44). As less value is placed on the reward of pain relief, these individuals do not attempt to deal with their pain via internal processes, but instead rely on external resources and others, which could be interpreted as a diminished ability of self-regulation of pain. Therefore, introducing a placebo treatment will not ease attempts to self-control their own pain as they do not engage with this approach-oriented behaviour towards pain in the first place.

This study has few limitations. The surreptitious temperature manipulation may have introduced a confounding variable that contributes to the observed changes in FAA during the placebo CPT condition. Further analysis with more focus on the fourth CPT condition of each experimental block (post-conditioning hand submersion with prior application of placebo cream but no surreptitious increase of ice bath temperature) is therefore suggested in future studies, although the potential effects of learning should be accounted for when designing the research study. Another limitation to this study is the small sample size which restricted computation of statistical comparisons between groups based on participant status or PCS-helplessness scores. It is known that, as pain becomes chronic, structural and functional changes occur in cortical areas involved in processing the emotional and motivational aspects of pain that may lead to abnormal frontal interhemispheric activity (45). There is therefore a potential for bias from the unequal number of chronic pain patients and healthy subjects in the sample which may limit the generality of findings. Future studies with larger sample sizes may examine modulation of FAA during pain and relationship with catastrophising-associated helplessness based on participant status (i.e. chronic pain patients group and healthy subjects group analysed separately). The predominance of females in the sample may have also introduced an element of bias as few studies have shown that females tend to catastrophise more readily in pain situations than males (46, 47). The potential influence of gender differences in FAA should also be accounted for (48, 49). Finally, an issue that was not addressed in this study was whether FAA associated with subjective pain intensity in the initial CPT and changes in pain ratings following application of a placebo cream. Future studies on this question could shed more light on the potential of targeting FAA for the clinical management of pain.

Conclusion

The research findings in the present study provide a new understanding of FAA biomarker as a reflection of the motivational drive to obtain reward of pain relief, that appears to be compromised in those who are prone to feel helpless about their pain. This has particular significance as it supports the relevance of psychological interventions aimed at reducing catastrophic helplessness cognitions, in particular CBT. Whilst further evidence is needed to confirm these findings, this paper provides insights for the development of promising non-invasive non-pharmacological therapeutic strategies for the management of chronic pain. Neurofeedback techniques that train individuals to self-modulate FAA towards a pattern reflective of greater relative left frontal activity may promote a more active coping approach towards pain in those who are prone to catastrophic helplessness. Further research is required to establish the therapeutic applicability of these neuromodulation strategies, either in combination or as an alternative to currently available pharmacological treatment, to improve pain-related outcomes in chronic pain populations.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by The Health Research Authority (IRAS ID: 244779) and The North of Scotland (2) Research Ethics Committee (18/NS/0102). The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

AKPJ, NJTB and MS conceptualised the Neurofeedback (NFB) project and secured the research grants. JH, NJTB, AKPJ and MS designed the study. JH and MS contributed to recruitment of participants. JH and KLD acquired the data. BSP and JH analysed the data. All authors contributed to drafting the manuscript, critically revising the work for important intellectual content, approved the final version and agree to be accountable for all aspects of the work. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This is a secondary analysis of data collected for “The Effectiveness of Neurofeedback for the Treatment of Chronic Pain” study. The study was registered with clinicaltrials.gov (pre-registration reference: NCT04097522) and funded by the Dunhill Medical Trust (R521/1116) and the Medical Research Council (R123973).

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank all participants of this study for their cooperation and patience with all the experiments. We would like to thank all the research staff of Human Pain Research Group with special thanks to Kate Lees (administrative secretary). A version of this work is available on PsyArXiv server as a preprint (50).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. Quartana PJ, Campbell CM, Edwards RR. Pain catastrophizing: a critical review. Expert Rev Neurother. (2009) 9(5):745–58. doi: 10.1586/ern.09.34

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Sullivan MJL, Bishop SR, Pivik J. The pain catastrophizing scale: development and validation. Psychol Assess. (1995) 7(4):524–32. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.7.4.524

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Sullivan MJL, Thorn B, Haythornthwaite JA, Keefe F, Martin M, Bradley LA, et al. Theoretical perspectives on the relation between catastrophizing and pain. Clin J Pain. (2001) 17(1):52–64. doi: 10.1097/00002508-200103000-00008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Hassett AL, Cone JD, Patella SJ, Sigal LH. The role of catastrophizing in the pain and depression of women with fibromyalgia syndrome. Arthritis Rheum. (2000) 43(11):2493–500. doi: 10.1002/1529-0131(200011)43:11<2493::AID-ANR17>3.0.CO;2-W11083273

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Mankovsky T, Lynch ME, Clark AJ, Sawynok J, Sullivan MJL. Pain catastrophizing predicts poor response to topical analgesics in patients with neuropathic pain. Pain Res Manag. (2012) 17(1):10–4. doi: 10.1155/2012/970423

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Severeijns R, Vlaeyen JWS, van den Hout MA, Weber WEJ. Pain catastrophizing predicts pain intensity, disability, and psychological distress independent of the level of physical impairment. Clin J Pain. (2001) 17(2):165–72. doi: 10.1097/00002508-200106000-00009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Turner JA, Jensen MP, Warms CA, Cardenas DD. Catastrophizing is associated with pain intensity, psychological distress, and pain-related disability among individuals with chronic pain after spinal cord injury. Pain. (2002) 98(1-2):127–34. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3959(02)00045-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Sullivan MJL, Lynch ME, Clark AJ, Mankovsky T, Sawynok J. Catastrophizing and treatment outcome: differential impact on response to placebo and active treatment outcome. Contemp Hypn. (2008) 25(3-4):129–40. doi: 10.1002/ch.365

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Bennett R, Nelson D. Cognitive behavioral therapy for fibromyalgia. Nat Clin Pract Rheumatol. (2006) 2(8):416–24. doi: 10.1038/ncprheum0245

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. de C Williams AC, Fisher E, Hearn L, Eccleston C. Psychological therapies for the management of chronic pain (excluding headache) in adults. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. (2020) 8(8): CD007407. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD007407.pub4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Ehde DM, Dillworth TM, Turner JA. Cognitive-behavioral therapy for individuals with chronic pain: efficacy, innovations, and directions for research. Am Psychol. (2014) 69(2):153–66. doi: 10.1037/a0035747

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Coan JA, Allen JJB. Frontal EEG asymmetry as a moderator and mediator of emotion. Biol Psychol. (2004) 67(1-2):7–50. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2004.03.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Klimesch W, Sauseng P, Hanslmayr S. EEG Alpha oscillations: the inhibition–timing hypothesis. Brain Res Rev. (2007) 53(1):63–88. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2006.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Muris P, Meesters C, de Kanter E, Timmerman PE. Behavioural inhibition and behavioural activation system scales for children: relationships with Eysenck's Personality traits and psychopathological symptoms. Pers Individ Dif. (2005) 38(4):831–41. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2004.06.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Carver CS, White TL. Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: the BIS/BAS scales. J Pers Soc Psychol. (1994) 67(2):319–33. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.67.2.319

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Jensen MP, Gianas A, Sherlin LH, Howe JD. Pain catastrophizing and EEG-alpha asymmetry. Clin J Pain. (2015) 31(10):852–8. doi: 10.1097/AJP.0000000000000182

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Mennella R, Patron E, Palomba D. Frontal alpha asymmetry neurofeedback for the reduction of negative affect and anxiety. Behav Res Ther. (2017) 92:32–40. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2017.02.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Quaedflieg CWEM, Smulders FTY, Meyer T, Peeters FPML, Merckelbach HLGJ, Smeets T. The validity of individual frontal alpha asymmetry EEG neurofeedback. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. (2016) 11(1):33–43. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsv090

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Osman A, Barrios FX, Kopper BA, Hauptmann W, Jones J, O'Neill E. Factor structure, reliability, and validity of the pain catastrophizing scale. J Behav Med. (1997) 20(6):589–605. doi: 10.1023/A:1025570508954

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Osman A, Barrios FX, Gutierrez PM, Kopper BA, Merrifield T, Grittmann L. The pain catastrophizing scale: further psychometric evaluation with adult samples. J Behav Med. (2000) 23(4):351–65. doi: 10.1023/A:1005548801037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Van Damme S, Crombez G, Bijttebier P, Goubert L, Van Houdenhove B. A confirmatory factor analysis of the pain catastrophizing scale: invariant factor structure across clinical and non-clinical populations. Pain. (2002) 96(3):319–24. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3959(01)00463-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. D'Eon JL, Harris CA, Ellis JA. Testing factorial validity and gender invariance of the pain catastrophizing scale. J Behav Med. (2004) 27(4):361–72. doi: 10.1023/B:JOBM.0000042410.34535.64

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Bjelland I, Dahl AA, Haug TT, Neckelmann D. The validity of the hospital anxiety and depression scale: an updated literature review. J Psychosom Res. (2002) 52(2):69–77. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3999(01)00296-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Johnston M, Pollard B, Hennessey P. Construct validation of the hospital anxiety and depression scale with clinical populations. J Psychosom Res. (2000) 48(6):579–84. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3999(00)00102-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Zigmond AS, Snaith RP. The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta Psychiatr Scand. (1983) 67(6):361–70. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1983.tb09716.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Scott J, Huskisson EC. Graphic representation of pain. Pain. (1976) 2(2):175–84. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(76)90113-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Jasper H. Report of the committee on methods of clinical examination in electroencephalography. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. (1958) 10(2):370–5. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(58)90053-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Delorme A, Makeig S. EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J Neurosci Methods. (2004) 134(1):9–21. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. McFarland DJ, McCane LM, David SV, Wolpaw JR. Spatial filter selection for EEG based communication. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. (1997) 103(3):386–94. doi: 10.1016/S0013-4694(97)00022-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Chaumon M, Bishop DVM, Busch NA. A practical guide to the selection of independent components of the electroencephalogram for artifact correction. J Neurosci Methods. (2015) 250:47–63. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2015.02.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Oostenveld R, Fries P, Maris E, Schoffelen J-M. Fieldtrip: open source software for advanced analysis of MEG, EEG, and invasive electrophysiological data. Comput Intell Neurosci. (2011) 2011:156869. doi: 10.1155/2011/156869

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Grandke T. Interpolation algorithms for discrete Fourier transforms of weighted signals. IEEE Trans Instrum Meas. (1983) 32(2):350–5. doi: 10.1109/TIM.1983.4315077

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Gasser T, Bächer P, Möcks J. Transformations towards the normal distribution of broad band spectral parameters of the EEG. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. (1982) 53(1):119–24. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(82)90112-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Jensen MP, Ehde DM, Day MA. The behavioral activation and inhibition systems: implications for understanding and treating chronic pain. J Pain. (2016) 17(5):529–e1. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2016.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Coan JA, Allen JJB, McKnight PE. A capability model of individual differences in frontal EEG asymmetry. Biol Psychol. (2006) 72(2):198–207. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Stewart JL, Coan JA, Towers DN, Allen JJB. Resting and task-elicited prefrontal EEG alpha asymmetry in depression: support for the capability model. Psychophysiology. (2014) 51(5):446–55. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12191

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Iwaki R, Arimura T, Jensen MP, Nakamura T, Yamashiro K, Makino S, et al. Global catastrophizing vs catastrophizing subdomains: assessment and associations with patient functioning. Pain Med. (2012) 13(5):677–87. doi: 10.1111/j.1526-4637.2012.01353.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Sullivan MJL, Lynch ME, Clark AJ. Dimensions of catastrophic thinking associated with pain experience and disability in patients with neuropathic pain conditions. Pain. (2005) 113(3):310–5. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2004.11.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Sullivan MJL, Stanish W, Waite H, Sullivan M, Tripp DA. Catastrophizing, pain, and disability in patients with soft-tissue injuries. Pain. (1998) 77(3):253–60. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3959(98)00097-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Keune PM, Wiedemann E, Schneidt A, Schönenberg M. Frontal brain asymmetry in adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): extending the motivational dysfunction hypothesis. Clin Neurophysiol. (2015) 126(4):711–20. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2014.07.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Adolph D, von Glischinski M, Wannemüller A, Margraf J. The influence of frontal alpha-asymmetry on the processing of approach-and withdrawal-related stimuli—a multichannel psychophysiology study. Psychophysiology. (2017) 54(9):1295–310. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12878

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Schmidt B, Hanslmayr S. Resting frontal EEG alpha-asymmetry predicts the evaluation of affective musical stimuli. Neurosci Lett. (2009) 460(3):237–40. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2009.05.068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Fields HL. A motivation-decision model of pain: the role of opioids. In: Flor H, Kalso E, Dostrovsky JO, editors. Proceedings of the 11th world congress on pain; 2005 Aug 21–26. Sydney, Australia. Seattle: IASP Press (2006). p. 449–59.

44. Snow-Turek AL, Norris MP, Tan G. Active and passive coping strategies in chronic pain patients. Pain. (1996) 64(3):455–62. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(95)00190-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Bushnell MC, Čeko M, Low LA. Cognitive and emotional control of pain and its disruption in chronic pain. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2013) 14(7):502–11. doi: 10.1038/nrn3516

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Sullivan MJL, Tripp DA, Rodgers WM, Stanish W. Catastrophizing and pain perception in sport participants. J Appl Sport Psychol. (2000) 12(2):151–67. doi: 10.1080/10413200008404220

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Sullivan MJL, Tripp DA, Santor D. Gender differences in pain and pain behavior: the role of catastrophizing. Cognit Ther Res. (2000) 24(1):121–34. doi: 10.1023/A:1005459110063

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Arns M, Bruder G, Hegerl U, Spooner C, Palmer DM, Etkin A, et al. EEG Alpha asymmetry as a gender-specific predictor of outcome to acute treatment with different antidepressant medications in the randomized iSPOT-D study. Neurophysiol Clin. (2016) 127(1):509–19. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2015.05.032

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Jaworska N, Blier P, Fusee W, Knott V. Alpha power, alpha asymmetry and anterior cingulate cortex activity in depressed males and females. J Psychiatr Res. (2012) 46(11):1483–91. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2012.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Passadouro BS, Delgado-Sanchez A, Henshaw J, Lopez-Diaz K, Trujillo-Barreto N, Jones AKP, et al.,Frontal Alpha Asymmetry: a potential biomarker of approach withdrawal motivation towards pain. PsyArXiv [Preprint] (2021). Available at: https://psyarxiv.com/kxr7h/ (Accessed May 12, 2022).

Google Scholar

Keywords: pain catastrophising, EEG, biomarker, neurofeedback, placebo response

Citation: Silva-Passadouro B, Delgado-Sanchez A, Henshaw J, Lopez-Diaz K, Trujillo-Barreto NJ, Jones AKP and Sivan M (2022) Frontal alpha asymmetry: A potential biomarker of approach-withdrawal motivation towards pain. Front. Pain Res. 3:962722. doi: 10.3389/fpain.2022.962722

Received: 6 June 2022; Accepted: 12 September 2022;
Published: 27 September 2022.

Edited by:

Jan Kubanek, The University of Utah, United States

Reviewed by:

Elsa Cisneros, Universidad Internacional De La Rioja, Spain
Shervin Rahimpour, The University of Utah, United States Rose Caston, The University of Utah, United States, in collaboration with SR

© 2022 Silva-Passadouro, Delgado-Sanchez, Henshaw, Lopez-Diaz, Trujillo-Barreto, Jones and Sivan. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Bárbara Silva-Passadouro umbsp@leeds.ac.uk

Specialty Section: This article was submitted to Neuromodulatory Interventions, a section of the journal Frontiers in Pain Research

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.