Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Pain Res., 05 January 2022
Sec. Non-Pharmacological Treatment of Pain
This article is part of the Research Topic Mechanisms Underlying the Interactions Between Stress and Pain View all 7 articles

When Less Is More: Investigating Factors Influencing the Distraction Effect of Virtual Reality From Pain

\nKatharina BarcattaKatharina BarcattaElisabeth HollElisabeth HollLayla BattistuttaLayla BattistuttaMarian van der MeulenMarian van der MeulenKatharina M. Rischer
Katharina M. Rischer*
  • Department of Behavioral and Cognitive Sciences, University of Luxembourg, Esch-sur-Alzette, Luxembourg

Virtual reality (VR) is a powerful method of redirecting attention away from pain. Yet, little is known about which factors modulate the size of this distraction effect. The aim of this study was to investigate the role of cognitive load and inter-individual differences in the cognitive and affective domain on heat pain thresholds during a VR game. Ninety healthy participants (mean age ± SD: 23.46 ± 3.28; 50% identified as male and 50% as female) played a low and high load version of a VR game while heat pain thresholds and heart rate were recorded. The effects of cognitive load were assessed by computing the difference in pain thresholds between the high and low load condition for each participant. In addition, we computed the difference in heart rate variability (HRV) measures between both conditions to explore whether these would be correlated with the difference in heat pain thresholds. Prior to the VR session, participants completed questionnaires about their emotional distress, pain-related cognitions, and different executive functioning tasks. Contrary to our expectations, not all participants benefitted from a higher load in terms of distraction from pain. Logistic regression analysis revealed that participants who reported more emotional distress were more likely to exhibit higher pain thresholds in the low relative to the high load condition. Accordingly, these participants tended to show marginally higher HRV in the low compared to the high load condition. Our study demonstrates that the potential benefits of an increased cognitive load in VR on pain sensitivity depends on individual differences in affective state.

Introduction

Virtual reality (VR) has proven to be a powerful tool for attentional diversion and is increasingly used in clinical settings for pain management (14). The distractive, hypoalgesic effects of VR are assumed to result from a competition for limited shared attentional resources between the immersive, sensory properties of VR and incoming nociceptive signals (5).

Previous research suggests that several factors influence the efficacy of cognitive distraction from pain in non-VR settings, yet little is known about their role in VR (3). For example, tasks placing a high demand on central executive resources are particularly effective in diverting attention away from pain (6, 7). However, studies investigating the effects of increased task difficulty in VR often rely on the manipulation of game settings, such as the speed or number of hit targets, and are therefore likely to be confounded by the participant's gaming experience and motor coordination skills (810).

Other factors that may influence pain modulation are stress level (1113) and negative pain-related cognitions, such as the tendency to catastrophize about pain (14, 15). Furthermore, a growing body of studies suggests that better executive functions (EFs), specifically cognitive inhibition abilities, are associated with reduced pain sensitivity (16, 17) and may play a role in cognitive distraction from pain (18). However, while there is a growing body of research on the effects of virtual reality on pain, and (pain-related) anxiety and distress (19, 20), it is less well known how these factors influence the efficacy of virtual reality in reducing pain.

The aim of the present study was to examine whether increasing cognitive load through the implementation of a memory task in VR would enhance the hypoalgesic effect of VR when compared to a low load condition, as demonstrated in non-VR settings. Furthermore, we investigated whether emotional distress, pain-related cognitions and executive functions would predict the likelihood to benefit from the higher cognitive load. To account for individual differences in previous gaming experience, we also assessed gaming skills and simulator sickness symptoms.

We expected that an additional load in the VR game would lead to decreased pain sensitivity, i.e., higher heat pain thresholds, when compared to a low load condition. We also expected that individuals with better executive functions would be more likely to benefit from the additional cognitive load, as they might be able to better ignore the pain and selectively focus attention on the VR and memory task. We had no clear hypotheses about the influence of pain-related cognitions or emotional distress on the effect of an additional cognitive load. Throughout each condition, we recorded electrocardiograms to obtain heart rate variability (HRV) measures to explore whether pain modulation by cognitive load would be associated with changes in HRV. We had no clear hypothesis about the direction of the association: while a recent study by Colloca et al. (21) reported that decreased pain sensitivity in a relaxing VR environment correlated with higher HRV, pain relief by cognitive distraction has been associated with decreased HRV (22), which is consistent with studies that have linked higher mental stress to lower HRV (23, 24). In the present study, we wanted to assess whether changes in HRV would also reflect changes in pain thresholds in a more cognitively challenging VR environment.

Materials and Methods

Participants

A total of 101 participants were recruited via student forums, posters, social media, and a radio interview to reach participants outside of the university environment and tested in the MExLab at the University of Luxembourg between September and December 2019. Participants were healthy young adults between 18 and 35 years old and fluent in either German or English. Exclusion criteria were acute or chronic pain, intake of pain medication, a diagnosis of photosensitive epilepsy and neurodermatitis or other skin-related medical conditions on the participants' non-dominant leg (details about dropouts can be found in Descriptive Statistics). Each participant was compensated with a 25€ gift voucher and optional course credit. The study was approved by the Ethics Review Panel of the University of Luxembourg and conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. Measures were taken to ensure that an equal number of male and female participants were recruited.

Procedure

Participants first provided written informed consent and received standard information about the set up without being informed about the actual aim of the study (i.e., to investigate VR-induced distraction from pain). Participants then completed questionnaires (either in English or German) assessing their demographic and health status (including simulator sickness symptoms to establish a baseline), video gaming skills, emotional distress (i.e., stress, anxiety, and depression symptoms), and negative pain-related cognitions, in addition to three computerized executive function tasks.

Following this, participants underwent the VR paradigm. Thermal pain thresholds were recorded during a baseline phase to familiarize them with the protocol, as well as during two interactive VR gaming conditions with different levels of cognitive load. The order of the latter two conditions was counterbalanced (see Pain Thresholds and VR Paradigm). After each of the two interactive conditions, participants completed questionnaires assessing spatial presence, simulator sickness symptoms, and perceived task difficulty. Breaks between all VR sessions lasted at least 5 min. The experimenter continuously monitored participants and, if necessary, interrupted the session if participants were showing or reporting symptoms of simulator sickness (e.g., paleness, sweating, and vertigo). We also measured participants' heart rate and electrodermal activity throughout the VR session, but only analyzed heart rate variability in the present study. The complete experimental procedure lasted approximately 100 min. For a schematic illustration of the experimental setup, see Figure 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Experimental Set-Up. (A) Laptop operating the thermal stimulator; (B) PATHWAY 30 x 30 mm ATS thermal stimulator (Medoc, Ltd.); (C) foot switch; (D) htc Vive; (E) ECG electrodes; (F) Xbox controller; (G) thermal stimulator attached to the non-dominant leg (calf); (H) EDA electrodes; (I) gaming computer streaming Subnautica to the head mounted display; (J) BIOPAC modules; (K) computer running AcqKnowledge to record the psychophysiological data.

Questionnaires

Gaming and Virtual Reality

Gaming skill was assessed using a seven-item version of the Game Playing Skill Scale [GaPS; (25)]. Participants also indicated their average gaming time (gaming hours/day), rated their computer and VR experience (no experience, basic experience, advanced experience, expert experience) and self-evaluated their gaming expertise (no gamer, casual gamer, regular gamer, expert gamer). To assess the level of presence, i.e., the feeling of “being there” (26) in the VR, participants completed two subscales of the Spatial Presence Experience Scale (SPES) (27) after each interactive VR condition. They, furthermore, completed the Simulator Sickness Questionnaire (SSQ) (28) before the baseline condition and after each interactive VR condition. To assess the degree of perceived task difficulty, participants also rated the perceived cognitive load after each interactive condition (29).

Pain-Related Cognitions

The Fear of Pain Questionnaire-III [FPQ; (30)] was used to assess the participants' general fear of pain. The FPQ-III is a 30-item self-report inventory with items being rated on a 5-point scale (1 = not at all, to 5 = extreme).

The participants' level of pain catastrophizing was measured with the Pain Catastrophizing Scale [PCS; (31)]. The PCS consists of 13 items that are rated on a 5-point scale (0 = not at all, to 4 = all the time).

Attention to pain was assessed with the Pain Vigilance and Awareness Questionnaire [PVAQ; (32)], which consists of 16 items that are rated on a 6-point scale (0 = never, to 5 = always).

Emotional Distress

Participants were asked to rate their symptoms of emotional distress in the week prior to study participation using the Depression Anxiety Stress Scale-21 (DASS-21) (33). This scale comprises three subscales (with seven items each), providing a dimensional assessment of three components of emotional distress, including anxiety, depression, and stress. Responses were given on a 4-point scale (from 0 = not at all/never, to 3 = most of the time/almost always).

Executive Functioning Tasks

Participants completed three executive functioning tasks that were implemented in the Psychology Experiment Building Language (PEBL) (34).

Corsi Block Tapping Task

The Corsi block tapping task was used to assess visuo-spatial working memory (WM) (35). In the computerized version used in the present study, participants were presented with an array of nine blocks (squares) and in each trial a sequence of squares was highlighted, one after the other. Participants were required to memorize and reproduce the sequence in the same (forward condition) or reversed order (backward condition), by clicking on the squares. The initial sequence included only two squares and was increased by one square every third trial up to a maximum of nine squares. The task ended once two trials of the same sequence length were recalled incorrectly. The main outcome variable for both conditions was the block span, i.e., the longest length at which at least one sequence was correctly recalled.

Flanker Task

A modified version of the Eriksen flanker task by Stins et al. (36) was used to assess interference control and selective attention, i.e., the ability to filter out competing information by concentrating on one stimulus and ignoring the surrounding stimuli (37). In the present task, five horizontally aligned arrows appeared in the middle of a screen. Participants were asked to identify the direction of the target (centre) arrow, which either pointed in the same direction as the flanking arrows (congruent condition) or in the opposite direction (incongruent condition). In total, participants completed 80 trials (40 congruent, 40 incongruent), following a practice run of eight trials. To calculate the flanker effect, the mean reaction time (RT) of congruent trials was subtracted from the mean RT of incongruent trials. Higher scores in the flanker task indicate less efficient interference control and worse selective attention.

Go/NoGo Task

The go/nogo task (38) assesses prepotent response inhibition abilities, i.e., the ability to suppress inappropriate or irrelevant actions (39). The present task was designed after Bezdjian et al. (40) and required participants to respond to the letter “P” with a button press and to withhold their response to the letter “R.” Participants completed two blocks (in the second block the go and nogo stimuli were reversed) containing 160 trials each, following a practice run of 10 trials. Response inhibition was quantified by computing the percentage of commission errors, i.e., presses in response to nogo stimuli. A lower percentage indicates a better response inhibition capacity.

Pain Thresholds

Pain thresholds were measured using a PATHWAY 30 x 30mm ATS thermal stimulator “thermode” (Medoc Ltd.), which was attached to the lower calf of the participant's non-dominant leg. The baseline temperature of the thermode was set to 32°C and increased at a slope of 0.5°C/s to a maximum of 50°C. As soon as the participants perceived the temperature as painful, they were instructed to press a foot switch (placed under the dominant leg), which triggered a rapid return to baseline temperature at a slope of 10°C/s. The temperature at the time of the participant's response was recorded as the pain threshold. The inter-stimulus interval varied between 45 and 50 seconds. In total, pain thresholds were measured 35 times (five times during a baseline phase), 15 times in the low cognitive load condition (LLC), and 15 times in the high cognitive load condition (HLC).

Electrocardiography Data

Heart rate was assessed continuously using a three-lead electrocardiogram (ECG; electrodes were attached to the chest following Einthoven's triangle), recorded at a 1 kHz sampling rate using the MP150 system and AcqKnowledge software 4.4.1 (Biopac Systems Inc.).

VR Paradigm

Participants were equipped with a head-mounted display (HMD; htc VIVE), headphones and an Xbox controller. In the interactive VR conditions, participants played the video game Subnautica (Unknown Worlds Entertainment, Inc.), an underwater exploration game played from a first-person perspective of a scuba diver (note that participants could only see the scuba diving gloves of their avatar). For the present study, several gaming settings were changed enabling unlimited oxygen supply, invincibility, and reduced swimming speed.

Pain thresholds were assessed during the baseline and two interactive conditions with either a low cognitive load (LLC) or a high cognitive load (HLC). During the baseline, participants sat stationary and passively in the virtual environment on an inflatable island surrounded by the sea and sky. This condition served as an opportunity for participants to familiarize themselves with the VR and the pain threshold procedure. The LLC consisted of a simple navigation task: participants had to follow a pipeline leading through the underwater environment. In the HLC, participants had to follow the same underwater route, but in addition, they were asked to memorize a sequence of eight single digit numbers that were displayed at fixed intervals along the pipeline. Note that the HLC differed from the LLC only in terms of working memory demands (and beacons used to present the digits), allowing a direct comparison of the effects of additional cognitive load on VR-related hypoalgesia. Recall of the digit sequence was assessed directly after completing the session and quantified using the Damerau-Levensthein distance (41) that indicates how many operations are needed to transform one sequence into another (e.g., the reported digit sequence into the correct one). Higher values indicate more operations, i.e., a greater distance, and thus lower memory performance.

Data Preparation

Pain Thresholds

We excluded pain thresholds of 37°C and lower [most likely representing an accidental foot press (42)] and of 50°C (i.e., trials without a registered response). This resulted in removing a total of 23 thresholds from 13 participants. We also excluded one participant from all analyses who had an average pain threshold of M = 49.71°C in the baseline condition, and of M = 49.99°C in the LLC and HLC, exceeding 3 SD.

Heart Rate Variability

ECG data were processed and analyzed with the PhysioData Toolbox [Version 0.5.0; (43)]. Two participants had to be excluded completely from the analyses due to anomalies (e.g., premature heart beats). Technical complications (e.g., detached electrodes) led to the rejection of two measurements for the LLC and one for the HLC. We extracted four metrics to assess heart rate variability in the time and frequency domain for the duration of each condition (LLCduration: M ±SD = 677.79 ± 12.49 s, HLCduration: M ±SD = 696.61 ± 48.20 s). We computed the standard deviation of the inter beat intervals of normal sinus beats (detrended SDNN) and the root mean square of successive differences between normal heartbeats (detrended RMSSD) in milliseconds as time domain indices of vagally mediated changes in HRV (44). Low frequency (LF; Hz range: 0.04–0.15 Hz) and high frequency (HF; Hz range: 0.15–0.4 Hz) power (in ms2) were used as frequency domain measures. The Lomb-Scargle method was used to estimate the Power Spectral Density of the interbeat interval time series to compute LF and HF power (43).

Statistical Analyses

Statistical analyses were performed using SPSS 26 (IBM SPSS Statistics). The difference in pain thresholds between the HLC and LLC (i.e., PainLoad) were assessed with a repeated measures ANOVA, and the predictive power of emotional distress, pain-related cognitions and cognitive skills with a regression analysis on (the difference in) pain thresholds. The association between changes in HRV and pain thresholds were assessed using two-tailed Spearman correlations with bias-corrected and accelerated bootstrapping (1,000 samples) as a Spearman correlation does not require that the data is normally distributed and is relatively robust against outliers (45).

Power analysis with G*Power 3.1 (46, 47) for a repeated measures ANOVA with two measurements and an assumed medium effect size of f = 0.15, and an α = 0.05, showed that a total sample size of 90 participants is sufficient to obtain a power of 0.80. For a multiple linear regression with five tested predictors, and an assumed effect size of f2 = 0.15, α = 0.05 and a power of 0.80, a sample size of 92 participants is necessary. A sample size of 67 or 84 participants is sufficient to obtain a power of 0.80 for a one-tailed or two-tailed correlation, respectively, assuming a correlation effect size of ρ = 0.30 for H1 and ρ = 0.0 for H0.

Results

Descriptive Statistics

A total of 101 participants were recruited and tested for the study. The data of 11 participants were excluded from the analyses due to discontinuation of the study due to simulator sickness (n = 4), an average pain threshold close to the limit of 50°C (n = 1; see Data Preparation), technical problems with the thermal stimulator (n = 1), and self-reported acute or chronic pain (n = 5). Thus, the final sample consisted of 90 participants (50% identified as male; 74.4% German speaking; 84.4% students) with a mean age of 23.46 years (SD = 3.28).

More than half of the participants reported to be advanced users of computer equipment (65.56 %), but only 8.89 % stated to have advanced experience using VR and 56.67 % had no prior VR experience at all. Average daily time spent on video games was <1 h (M = 0.96 hours, SD = 1.47), and 43.33 % stated to be current non-players. About half of all participants (46.7%) completed the HLC first, the other half started with the LLC. Descriptive statistics of the psychological and cognitive (performance) measures are given in Table 1. Task-related and HRV measures for the LLC and HLC are reported in Table 2. Tests of normality for all variables of interest can be found in the Supplementary Materials (see Supplementary Table S1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Psychometric characteristics of the sample.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Task-related and HRV measures.

Effect of Cognitive Load on Pain Thresholds

We first explored whether age and gaming skills (GaPS scores) were correlated with the difference in temperature (pain thresholds) between the HLC and LLC (i.e., PainLoad) using two-tailed Spearman correlations with bootstrapping and tested whether PainLoad was significantly different for gender and task order using independent samples t-tests. All correlations were non-significant (p > 0.324). We also found no significant differences for gender (p = 0.392) but a marginally significant effect for task order (p = 0.068).

A repeated measures ANOVA with the within-subject factor cognitive load (LLC vs. HLC), showed no main effect of cognitive load, F (1, 89) = 1.23, p =0.271, np2 =0.014. (Note that the main effect of cognitive load became significant, F (1, 88) = 4.44, p =0.038, np2 =0.048, when adding task order as covariate. However, the overall difference in °C between the HLC and LLC was small, with a mean difference of M = 0.12, SE = 0.10°C). Closer inspection revealed that only 51.1% of the participants experienced a positive effect of cognitive load, i.e., higher pain thresholds in the HLC compared to the LLC (mean difference: M = 0.66, SD = 1.07°C) whereas the remaining 48.9% participants showed the opposite effect of higher pain thresholds in the LLC than in the HLC (mean difference: M = −0.45, SD = 0.39°C).

A chi-square goodness of fit test revealed no significant difference in gender distribution between the participants who benefited more from the LLC (47.83% identified as male) or HLC (52.27% identified as male), p = 0.673. In line with the results reported above, we found a marginally significant effect of task order, X2 (1, 90) = 3.67, p = 0.055, indicating that participants who had completed the LLC first, tended to benefit more often from the LLC than the HLC (63.64%) and participants who had completed the HLC first, tended to benefit more often from the HLC than the LLC (56.52%). We found no significant differences in age (p = 0.901) or GaPS scores (p = 0.590) between groups, as tested with independent samples t-tests.

To assess whether cognitive load significantly modulated pain thresholds in these two subgroups, we ran another repeated measures ANOVA with cognitive load as within-subject factor, and subgroup (HLC>LLC or LLC>HLC) as between-subject factors. This ANOVA revealed no significant main effect of cognitive load [F (1, 88) = 1.42, p = 0.236, np2 = 0.016]. However, we observed a significant interaction between cognitive load and subgroup [F (1, 88) = 41.85, p < 0.001, np2 =0.322]. (Note that adding task order as a covariate to the ANOVA did not change the significance of this interaction, F (1, 87) = 37.76, p < 0.001, np2 = 0.303). Bonferroni-corrected post hoc tests showed that participants in the HLC>LLC group showed a slightly larger increase in pain thresholds from the LLC to the HLC [F (1, 88) = 30.02, p < 0.001, np2 = 0.254] than the other subgroup from the HLC to the LLC [F (1, 88) = 13.62, p < 0.001, np2 = 0.134]. Boxplots for each condition can be found in Figure 2, and bar charts of the average pain thresholds in each condition and for each subgroup in Figure 3.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Pain thresholds. Pain thresholds were assessed during a baseline condition (in a static VR environment) and two interactive conditions with a low cognitive load (LLC) and a high cognitive load (HLC) while participants (male = 45, female = 45) were immersed in VR. M ± SD: Baseline: 44.73 ± 1.81◦C, LLC: 45.47 ± 1.53◦C, HLC: 45.58 ± 1.20◦C. Note that the “x” in the middle of the boxplot denotes the mean whereas the horizontal line denotes the median. The whiskers denote the minimum and maximum values. Outliers (i.e., data points that are 1.5 times larger or smaller than the interquartile range) are represented by dots.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Effect of cognitive load on pain thresholds. Participants could be subdivided into two subgroups, showing distinct responses to the high load task (HLC). About half of all participants (51.1%) showed an increased pain threshold in the HLC relative to the LLC, whereas the remaining 48.9% exhibited a higher pain threshold in the LLC relative to the HLC. Bar charts illustrate the average pain thresholds for both conditions for each subgroup. Error bars represent the standard error of the mean (SEM). **p <0.01, ***p <0.001.

Predictors of the Effect of Cognitive Load

To estimate the likelihood of participants to benefit more from the HLC than the LLC (or vice versa) based on self-reported emotional distress symptoms, pain-related cognitions, and executive functions, we conducted a hierarchical binary logistic regression (computing bias-corrected and accelerated 95% confidence intervals using bootstrapping (1,000 samples), with subgroup (HLC> LLC or LLC>HLC) as dependent variable. (Note that we decided to conduct a logistic regression instead of a multiple linear regression as this facilitates the interpretation of our finding that participants responded differently to the increased cognitive load. A post hoc analysis of the achieved power for the logistic regression is reported below).

To control for potential effects of task order, we entered this variable at stage one of the model. At stage two, we entered the total DASS-21 score and PCS score as measures of emotional distress and pain-related cognitions, and the performance measures of the executive functioning tasks (block span in the Corsi forward condition, flanker effect, percentage of commission errors in the go/nogo task). We did not control for gaming skills here, as adding GaPS scores as a covariate to the ANOVA showed no influence of gaming skills on pain thresholds.

We chose to enter PCS as a pain-related cognition to limit the number of predictors and to reduce multicollinearity. The total PCS score in the present study was highly correlated with the total FPQ-III score (rs = 0.627) and PVAQ score (rs = 0.621) and has been shown in several previous studies to affect the magnitude of distraction from pain (18, 48, 49). All three measures of the executive functioning tasks were uncorrelated with one another (all ps > 0.055).

Screening the five tested predictors for multivariate outliers by computing Mahalanobis distance revealed that one participant was a multivariate outlier, and hence removed from the regression model. We removed one additional participant from the regression model with a Cook's distance of 0.714 (compared to an average distance of 0.084 ± 0.088 SD), who represented an unduly influential data point. As the percentage of commission errors in the go/nogo task could not be retrieved for one participant, the overall regression model was thus based on 87 participants.

Results revealed that emotional distress and the block span in the forward condition of the Corsi block tapping task were significant predictors of the likelihood to benefit more, or less, from the HLC relative to the LLC with an overall classification accuracy of 67.80%, after controlling for the effects of task order. For a one unit increase in the DASS-21 score, the odds of benefiting more from the HLC than the LLC decreased (OR: 0.916). A one unit increase in the forward condition of the Corsi block tapping task, on the other hand, was associated with an increased odds ratio to benefit more from the HLC than the LLC (OR: 1.566); however, bias-corrected and accelerated bootstrap intervals (BCa 95% CI) suggest that performance in the Corsi task is not a robust predictor. See Table 3 for a comparison of both models and Table 4 for the classification accuracy of the model.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Hierarchical binary logistic regression model.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Classification table.

Post hoc power analysis for the logistic regression in G*Power 3.1 indicated an achieved power of 0.84 for the effect of the DASS-21 score (OR for standardized DASS-21: 0.463; assumed PrH0(Y=1|X=1) = 0.5; R2 = 0.14; α = 0.05), and a power of 0.72 for the effect of block span in the Corsi forward condition (OR for standardized block span: 1.843; assumed PrH0(Y=1|X=1) = 0.5; R2 = 0.04; α = 0.05).

Given that the block span in the Corsi forward condition was only weakly correlated with the block span in the backward condition (rs = 0.275), we conducted another exploratory logistic regression replacing the block span in the forward condition with the block span in the backward condition. However, the regression model showed that the block span in the Corsi backward condition (i.e., visuo-spatial working memory) was not predictive of the group affiliation [B = 0.088, SE = 0.063, p = 0.176, 95%CI(−0.024 0.236)].

Heart Rate Variability and Cognitive Load

In a first step, we computed the differences in HRV measures (RMSSD, SDNN, LF, and HF power) between the HLC and LLC (HLC-LLC) and established that the difference scores were not significantly different for gender or task order using independent sample t-tests (all ps > 0.061). Subsequently, we ran two-tailed Spearman rank correlations with bootstrapping (1,000 samples), computing bias-corrected and accelerated 95% confidence intervals (BCa 95% CI) between the difference in HRV measures and PainLoad to assess whether a relatively higher HRV in the HLC relative to the LLC would reflect higher pain thresholds in the HLC compared to the LLC. Results revealed only marginally significant (p < 0.100) positive correlations between the differences in RMSSD, SDNN and PainLoad, i.e., higher pain thresholds in the HLC compared to the LLC were associated with a higher RMSSD [rs = 0.189, p = 0.083, 95%CI(−0.025 0.377)] and SDNN [rs = 0.187, p = 0.086, 95%CI(−0.018 0.393)] in the HLC relative to the LLC.

Heart Rate Variability and Emotional Distress

Given that HRV signals relaxation and sympathetic/parasympathetic balance (21), we explored whether participants who reported to be more distressed, showed a greater degree of relaxation in the LLC than in the HLC, possibly explaining why they benefited more from the LLC than from the HLC. For this purpose, we conducted one-tailed Spearman correlations (using bootstrapping with 1,000 samples, BCa 95% CI). This revealed a significant negative association between the total DASS-21 score and the difference in RMSSD between the HLC and LLC (note however that the 95%CI indicates no robust association). Correlations with the DASS subscales showed that self-reported anxiety symptoms were particularly strongly related to the difference in HRV measures, i.e., participants reporting more anxiety (and stress) tended to show lower RMSSD, SDNN and LF power in the HLC compared to the LLC (see Table 5).

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. One-tailed spearman correlations between distress symptoms and differences in HRV between the HLC and LLC.

Discussion

In the present study, we investigated the effect of an additional cognitive load in a virtual reality game on modulating pain thresholds and the role of individual differences. Prima facie, we found no effect of cognitive load on pain thresholds when assessing the whole sample, even though significant differences in task difficulty ratings, spatial presence and HRV measures suggest that the two conditions were sufficiently distinct to result in differences on the cognitive and psychophysiological level. Closer examination revealed that the lack of a modulatory effect resulted from two different response types to the additional cognitive load in our sample. While about half of all participants showed higher pain thresholds in the more challenging high load condition (sensory immersion plus cognitive memory challenge), the other half benefitted more from the low load condition (sensory immersion only). These two subgroups emerged independently of participant gender or gaming skills but differed significantly in self-reported emotional distress (depression, anxiety, and stress symptoms) and visuospatial short-term memory (block span in the forward condition of the Corsi block tapping task).

Participants reporting more distress had significantly lower odds to benefit from the additional cognitive challenge (HLC), possibly because the memory task in the HLC exceeded the cognitive resources that more distressed participants were willing to invest in completing the memory task (50). More distressed participants may have also perceived the low load condition as more entertaining than the high load condition, a factor that has been shown to modulate the efficacy of VR-related distraction from pain in previous studies (51, 52), but that we did not assess in the present study. This explanation is also in line with research showing that it is the individual's absorption in the distracting task rather than the type of task that is determining the efficacy of distraction (53). Another potential moderator for the differential influence of increased cognitive load on VR-related hypoalgesia could be individual differences in situational anxiety. It seems, e.g., plausible that exploring the underwater environment during the LLC may have led to stress and anxiety relieving effects (54, 55). These effects may have been more pronounced for participants who had higher levels of distress whereas participants low in emotional distress may have not been able to further down-regulate their distress levels. This explanation is partly supported by previous research showing that VR-induced increases in heat pain tolerance are likely driven by the degree of relaxation (21). In agreement with this, exploratory analyses showed that participants who reported more emotional distress, in particular higher levels of anxiety, tended to show higher RMSSD in the LLC than in the HLC in the present study. However, the overall strength of the associations was weak and future studies should directly assess the degree of relaxation and situational anxiety by self-report measures administered before and after the intervention.

This finding could have important implications for clinical settings; patients who seek medical attention for (pain) symptoms or who undergo medical treatment, such as surgeries, are likely to experience high levels of distress (56, 57) and thus, it seems plausible that effect sizes may be even more pronounced in patients. Importantly, a comparison of our data to intensity and unpleasantness ratings of heat stimuli within the same temperature range from other studies in our lab (see Supplementary Table S2) suggests that an increase of 0.5°C in intensity was associated with a 5–13% increase in subjective pain ratings. This indicates that the changes in heat pain thresholds observed in the present study are likely to have clinical relevance.

We also found that participants with a better visuospatial short-term memory were more likely to benefit from the additional cognitive load (although 95% BCa confidence intervals indicate that this association is not very robust, probably partly due to little heterogeneity in cognitive performance in our sample of healthy young adults). Given that the HLC comprised a visual (digit-based) memory task, the observed association is not surprising and emphasizes the need to assess the cognitive components of VR environments/tasks when using VR as a pain intervention tool and to carefully match them to the participant's cognitive abilities, especially in clinical settings where executive functions may be (temporarily) hampered, e.g., due to side effects from medications (58).

Importantly, we quantified the effect of an additional cognitive load not by relying on a single retrospective self-report (5961), but by assessing heat pain thresholds in regular intervals while participants were immersed in VR. This makes the data less prone to be biased by social desirability or the elapsed time between the nociceptive stimulation and its evaluation (62, 63). We also chose tasks that are unlikely to be confounded by previous gaming experience or motor coordination skills (as all tasks were self-paced and required minimal and easy use of the controller).

Taken together, our results clearly suggest that the analgesic effects of VR cannot be fully explained by distraction, i.e., competition for limited shared attentional resources (5), as such an explanation should have entailed a near-systematic reduction in pain thresholds in the high load, relative to the low load, condition. Rather, our study adds to a complex picture, suggesting that VR-related analgesia may involve various underlying mechanisms, such as affect modulation in addition to attentional diversion (4). Moreover, our findings also indicate that individual differences in cognitive and affective state shape how individuals respond to these mechanisms.

Despite these strengths, our study has a few limitations. First, our participants were healthy pain-free young adults (mostly students) with good executive functions, limiting the generalizability of the results to other populations. Although visual comparisons show that pain thresholds in the interactive VR conditions were significantly increased when compared to the passive baseline condition (see Figure 2), our study design does not allow to draw any concrete conclusions about the efficacy of VR in distracting from pain in general. A more complex design as suggested by Colloca et al. (21) would allow to further disentangle the effects of cognitive load on pain thresholds, albeit this would have likely gone beyond the scope of the present study. Nevertheless, we encourage future studies to replicate our findings including a non-VR baseline condition.

In addition, the specific characteristics of our study design may have hampered our ability to assess the role of cognitive inhibition and selective attention abilities in distraction from pain. In particular, the high load condition required participants to shift their attention from one task (e.g., navigation) to the other (e.g., memorizing the digit span) and thus, did not allow to selectively attend to one task. Switching between tasks has also decreased perceived spatial presence (as evidenced by significantly lower self-reported spatial presence in the HLC relative to the LLC). Future studies that investigate the effects of cognitive load in VR should consider using more gamified approaches of implementing the cognitive challenge in the VR environment to avoid potential breaks in presence. However, researchers should at the same time ensure comparable conditions with regard to the required coordination and motor skills. In addition, it may be helpful to explicitly assess the participants' level of fun and perceived mental workload in future studies [e.g., with the NASA Task Load Index; (64)] to explain individual differences in the efficacy of VR to distract from pain.

Overall, our findings suggest that a VR game with an additional cognitive load does not automatically result in reduced pain sensitivity. Rather than using a “one size fits all” approach, our study emphasizes the need to consider individual differences, especially affective factors, when choosing VR as an intervention tool for pain treatment to maximize its therapeutic effects. Assessing the role of individual differences may also prove helpful in accounting for differences in study findings that cannot be explained by VR configuration factors alone.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary Materials, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Ethics Review Panel at the University of Luxembourg. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

The study was designed by EH, LB, and KMR and implemented by KB, EH, LB, and KMR, who were also involved in the data collection (KB contributed the majority to the data collection). KB wrote the first draft of the manuscript that was subsequently revised by the other authors. All authors contributed to the data analyses, discussion of the results, manuscript revision, read, and approved the submitted version.

Funding

The study was funded by the Doctoral School of Humanities and Social Sciences at the University of Luxembourg with a research grant awarded to EH, LB, and KMR.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Dr. Immo Curio for technical help, Ovgun Ses and Lynn Schlund for assisting in setting up the VR game, and Shervin Vencatachellum for helpful comments on the data analysis. Part of this study was submitted by the first author to the University of Luxembourg, Department of Behavioral and Cognitive Sciences, in partial fulfilment of the requirement for the master's degree.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpain.2021.800258/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Hoffman HG, Patterson DR, Magula J, Carrougher GJ, Zeltzer K, Dagadakis S, et al. Water-friendly virtual reality pain control during wound care. J Clin Psychol. (2004) 60:189–95. doi: 10.1002/jclp.10244

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Mallari B, Spaeth EK, Goh H, Boyd BS. Virtual reality as an analgesic for acute and chronic pain in adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Pain Res. (2019) 12:2053–85. doi: 10.2147/JPR.S200498

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Malloy KM, Milling LS. The effectiveness of virtual reality distraction for pain reduction: a systematic review. Clin Psychol Rev. (2010) 30:1011–8. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2010.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Trost Z, France C, Anam M, Shum C. Virtual reality approaches to pain: toward a state of the science. Pain. (2021) 162:325–31. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000002060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Gupta A, Scott K, Dukewich M. Innovative technology using virtual reality in the treatment of pain: does it reduce pain via distraction, or is there more to it? Pain Med. (2018) 19:151–9. doi: 10.1093/pm/pnx109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Buhle J. Wager TD. Performance-dependent inhibition of pain by an executive working memory task. Pain. (2010) 149:19–26. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2009.10.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Paris TA, Misra G, Archer DB, Coombes SA. Effects of a force production task and a working memory task on pain perception. J Pain. (2013) 14:1492–501. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2013.07.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Demeter N, Pud D, Josman N. Cognitive components predict virtual reality-induced analgesia: repeated measures in healthy subjects. Front Robot AI. (2018) 4:70. doi: 10.3389/frobt.2017.00070

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Dumoulin S, Bouchard S, Loranger C, Quintana P, Gougeon V, Lavoie KL. Are cognitive load and focus of attention differentially involved in pain management: an experimental study using a cold pressor test and virtual reality. J Pain Res. (2020) 13:2213. doi: 10.2147/JPR.S238766

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Fairclough SH, Stamp K, Dobbins C, Poole HM. Computer games as distraction from PAIN: Effects of hardware and difficulty on pain tolerance and subjective IMMERSION. Int J Hum-Comput Stud. (2020) 139:102427. doi: 10.1016/j.ijhcs.2020.102427

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Geva N, Pruessner J, Defrin R. Acute psychosocial stress reduces pain modulation capabilities in healthy men. Pain®. (2014) 155:2418–25. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2014.09.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Geva N, Defrin R. Opposite effects of stress on pain modulation depend on the magnitude of individual stress response. J Pain. (2018) 19:360–71. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2017.11.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Nahman-Averbuch H, Nir RR, Sprecher E, Yarnitsky D. Psychological factors and conditioned pain modulation. Clin J Pain. (2016) 32:541–54. doi: 10.1097/AJP.0000000000000296

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Schreiber KL, Campbell C, Martel MO, Greenbaum S, Wasan AD, Borsook D, et al. Distraction analgesia in chronic pain patients: the impact of catastrophizing. Anesthesiology. (2014) 121:1292–301. doi: 10.1097/ALN.0000000000000465

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Verhoeven K, Goubert L, Jaaniste T, Van Ryckeghem DM, Crombez G. Pain catastrophizing influences the use and the effectiveness of distraction in schoolchildren. Eur J Pain. (2012) 16:256–67. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpain.2011.06.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Bjekić J, Živanović M, Purić D, Oosterman JM, Filipović SR. Pain and executive functions: a unique relationship between stroop task and experimentally induced pain. Psychol Res. (2018) 82:580–9. doi: 10.1007/s00426-016-0838-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Oosterman JM, Dijkerman HC, Kessels RPC, Scherder EJA. A unique association between cognitive inhibition and pain sensitivity in healthy participants. Eur J Pain. (2010) 14:1046–50. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpain.2010.04.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Rischer KM, González-Roldán AM, Montoya P, Gigl S, Anton F, Meulen M. Distraction from pain: the role of selective attention and pain catastrophizing. Eur J Pain. (2020) 13:ejp.1634. doi: 10.1002/ejp.1634

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Smith V, Warty RR, Sursas JA, Payne O, Nair A, Krishnan S, et al. The effectiveness of virtual reality in managing acute pain and anxiety for medical inpatients: systematic review. J Med Internet Res. (2020) 22:e17980. doi: 10.2196/17980

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Chirico A, Maiorano P, Indovina P, Milanese C, Giordano GG, Alivernini F, et al. Virtual reality and music therapy as distraction interventions to alleviate anxiety and improve mood states in breast cancer patients during chemotherapy. J Cell Physiol. (2020) 235:5353–62. doi: 10.1002/jcp.29422

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Colloca L, Raghuraman N, Wang Y, Akintola T, Brawn-Cinani B, Colloca G, et al. Virtual reality: physiological and behavioral mechanisms to increase individual pain tolerance limits. Pain. (2020) 161:2010–21. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001900

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Terkelsen A, Andersen OK, Mølgaard H, Hansen J, Jensen T. Mental stress inhibits pain perception and heart rate variability but not a nociceptive withdrawal reflex. Acta Physiol Scand. (2004) 180:405–14. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-201X.2004.01263.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Mukherjee S, Yadav R, Yung I, Zajdel DP, Oken BS. Sensitivity to mental effort and test–retest reliability of heart rate variability measures in healthy seniors. Clin Neurophysiol. (2011) 122:2059–66. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2011.02.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Segerstrom SC, Nes LS. Heart rate variability reflects self-regulatory strength, effort, and fatigue. Psychol Sci. (2007) 18:275–81. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01888.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Bracken CC, Skalski P. Presence and video games: the impact of image quality and skill level. In: Proceedings of the Ninth Annual International Workshop on Presence. Cleveland State University Cleveland, OH (2006). p. 28–9.

Google Scholar

26. Slater. Presence and the sixth sense. Presence Teleoperators Virtual Environ. (2002) 11:435–9. doi: 10.1162/105474602760204327

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Hartmann T, Wirth W, Schramm H, Klimmt C, Vorderer P, Gysbers A, et al. The spatial presence experience scale (SPES). J Media Psychol. (2015) 28:1–15. doi: 10.1027/1864-1105/a000137

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Kennedy RS, Lane NE, Berbaum KS, Lilienthal MG. Simulator sickness questionnaire: an enhanced method for quantifying simulator sickness. Int J Aviat Psychol. (1993) 3:203–20. doi: 10.1207/s15327108ijap0303_3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Leppink J, Paas F, Van der Vleuten CP, Van Gog T, Van Merriënboer JJ. Development of an instrument for measuring different types of cognitive load. Behav Res Methods. (2013) 45:1058–72. doi: 10.3758/s13428-013-0334-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. McNeil DW, Rainwater AJ. Development of the fear of pain questionnaire-III. J Behav Med. (1998) 21:389–410. doi: 10.1023/A:1018782831217

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Sullivan MJ, Bishop SR, Pivik J. The pain catastrophizing scale: development and validation. Psychol Assess. (1995) 7:524. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.7.4.524

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. McCracken LM. “Attention” to pain in persons with chronic pain: a behavioral approach. Behav Ther. (1997) 28:271–84. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7894(97)80047-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Henry JD, Crawford JR. The short-form version of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS-21): construct validity and normative data in a large non-clinical sample. Br J Clin Psychol. (2005) 44:227–39. doi: 10.1348/014466505X29657

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Mueller ST, Piper BJ. The psychology experiment building language (PEBL) and PEBL test battery. J Neurosci Methods. (2014) 222:250–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2013.10.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Kessels RP, Van Zandvoort MJ, Postma A, Kappelle LJ, De Haan EH. The Corsi block-tapping task: standardization and normative data. Appl Neuropsychol. (2000) 7:252–8. doi: 10.1207/S15324826AN0704_8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Stins JF, Polderman JCT, Boomsma DI, de Geus EJC. Conditional accuracy in response interference tasks: evidence from the eriksen flanker task and the spatial conflict task. Adv Cogn Psychol. (2007) 3:409–17. doi: 10.2478/v10053-008-0005-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Friedman NP, Miyake A. The relations among inhibition and interference control functions: a latent-variable analysis. J Exp Psychol Gen. (2004) 133:101. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.133.1.101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Nosek BA, Banaji MR. The go/no-go association task. Soc Cogn. (2001) 19:625–66. doi: 10.1521/soco.19.6.625.20886

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Mostofsky SH, Simmonds DJ. Response inhibition and response selection: two sides of the same coin. J Cogn Neurosci. (2008) 20:751–61. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20500

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Bezdjian S, Baker LA, Lozano DI, Raine A. Assessing inattention and impulsivity in children during the Go/NoGo task. Br J Dev Psychol. (2009) 27:365–83. doi: 10.1348/026151008X314919

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Damerau FJ. A technique for computer detection and correction of spelling errors. Commun ACM. (1964) 7:171–6. doi: 10.1145/363958.363994

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Wasner GL, Brock JA. Determinants of thermal pain thresholds in normal subjects. Clin Neurophysiol. (2008) 119:2389–95. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2008.07.223

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Sjak-Shie EE,. PhysioData Toolbox. (2019). Available online at: https://PhysioDataToolbox.leidenuniv.nl (accessed April 8, 2021).

Google Scholar

44. Shaffer F, Ginsberg J. An overview of heart rate variability metrics and norms. Front Public Health. (2017) 5:258. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2017.00258

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. de Winter JC, Gosling SD, Potter J. Comparing the pearson and spearman correlation coefficients across distributions and sample sizes: a tutorial using simulations and empirical data. Psychol Methods. (2016) 21:273. doi: 10.1037/met0000079

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang AG, Buchner A. G* Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods. (2007) 39:175–91. doi: 10.3758/BF03193146

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Buchner A, Lang AG. Statistical power analyses using G* Power 3. 1: tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav Res Methods. (2009) 41:1149–60. doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Campbell CM, Witmer K, Simango M, Carteret A, Loggia ML, Campbell JN, et al. Catastrophizing delays the analgesic effect of distraction. Pain. (2010) 149:202–7. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2009.11.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Prins B, Decuypere A, Van Damme S. Effects of mindfulness and distraction on pain depend upon individual differences in pain catastrophizing: An experimental study: effects of mindfulness versus distraction on pain. Eur J Pain. (2014) 18:1307–15. doi: 10.1002/j.1532-2149.2014.491.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Kurzban R, Duckworth A, Kable JW, Myers J. An opportunity cost model of subjective effort and task performance. Behav Brain Sci. (2013) 36:661–79. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X12003196

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Sharar SR, Alamdari A, Hoffer C, Hoffman HG, Jensen MP, Patterson DR. Circumplex model of affect: a measure of pleasure and arousal during virtual reality distraction analgesia. Games Health J. (2016) 5:197–202. doi: 10.1089/g4h.2015.0046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Triberti S, Repetto C, Riva G. Psychological factors influencing the effectiveness of virtual reality–based analgesia: a systematic review. Cyberpsychology Behav Soc Netw. (2014) 17:335–45. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2014.0054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Pence LE, Thorn BE, Davis AM. Cognitive coping strategies in pain management. In: Ebert MH, Kerns RD, editors. Behavioral and Psychopharmacologic Pain Management. 1st ed. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press (2011). p. 219.

Google Scholar

54. Wang X, Shi Y, Zhang B, Chiang Y. The influence of forest resting environments on stress using virtual reality. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2019) 16:3263. doi: 10.3390/ijerph16183263

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Scates D, Dickinson JI, Sullivan K, Cline H, Balaraman R. Using nature-inspired virtual reality as a distraction to reduce stress and pain among cancer patients. Environ Behav. (2020) 52:895–918. doi: 10.1177/0013916520916259

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Meggiolaro E, Berardi MA, Andritsch E, Nanni MG, Sirgo A, Samorì E, et al. Cancer patients' emotional distress, coping styles and perception of doctor-patient interaction in European cancer settings. Palliat Support Care. (2016) 14:204. doi: 10.1017/S1478951515000760

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. O'Keeffe N, Ranjith G. Depression, demoralisation or adjustment disorder? Understanding emotional distress in the severely medically ill. Clin Med. (2007) 7:478. doi: 10.7861/clinmedicine.7-5-478

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Allegri N, Mennuni S, Rulli E, Vanacore N, Corli O, Floriani I, et al. Systematic review and meta-analysis on neuropsychological effects of long-term use of opioids in patients with chronic noncancer pain. Pain Pract. (2019) 19:328–43. doi: 10.1111/papr.12741

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Demeter N, Josman N, Eisenberg E, Pud D. Who can benefit from virtual reality to reduce experimental pain? A crossover study in healthy subjects. Eur J Pain. (2015) 19:1467–75. doi: 10.1002/ejp.678

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Karaman D, Erol F, Yilmaz D, Dikmen Y. Investigation of the effect of the virtual reality application on experimental pain severity in healthy. Rev Assoc Médica Bras. (2019) 65:446–51. doi: 10.1590/1806-9282.65.3.446

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Loreto-Quijada D, Gutiérrez-Maldonado J, Nieto R, Gutiérrez-Martínez O, Ferrer-García M, Saldaña C, et al. Differential effects of two virtual reality interventions: distraction versus pain control. Cyberpsychology Behav Soc Netw. (2014) 17:353–8. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2014.0057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Marino J, Gwynn MI, Spanos NP. Cognitive mediators in the reduction of pain: the role of expectancy, strategy use, and self-presentation. J Abnorm Psychol. (1989) 98:256–62. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.98.3.256

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Rainville P, Doucet JC, Fortin MC, Duncan GH. Rapid deterioration of pain sensory-discriminative information in short-term memory. Pain. (2004) 110:605–15. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2004.04.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Hart SG. NASA-task load index (NASA-TLX); 20 years later. In: Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting. Los Angeles, CA; Sage publications Sage CA (2006). p. 904–8.

Google Scholar

Keywords: virtual reality, distraction from pain, cognitive load, emotional distress, executive functions

Citation: Barcatta K, Holl E, Battistutta L, van der Meulen M and Rischer KM (2022) When Less Is More: Investigating Factors Influencing the Distraction Effect of Virtual Reality From Pain. Front. Pain Res. 2:800258. doi: 10.3389/fpain.2021.800258

Received: 22 October 2021; Accepted: 13 December 2021;
Published: 05 January 2022.

Edited by:

Xin Tong, Simon Fraser University, Canada

Reviewed by:

Yang Wang, University of Maryland, United States
Joe Kossowsky, Harvard University, United States

Copyright © 2022 Barcatta, Holl, Battistutta, van der Meulen and Rischer. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Katharina M. Rischer, katharina.rischer@uni.lu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.