Skip to main content

PERSPECTIVE article

Front. Syst. Neurosci., 26 May 2022
This article is part of the Research Topic Negative Valence Systems View all 6 articles

Adaptation of Threat Responses Within the Negative Valence Framework

  • 1Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, United States
  • 2Staglin Center for Brain and Behavioral Health, Los Angeles, CA, United States
  • 3Department of Neurosurgery, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, United States
  • 4Department of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, United States

External threats are a major source of our experience of negatively valanced emotion. As a threat becomes closer and more real, our specific behavior patterns and our experiences of negative affect change in response to the perceived imminence of threat. Recognizing this, the National Institute of Mental Health’s Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) Negative Valence system is largely based around different levels of threat imminence. This perspective describes the correspondence between the RDoC Negative Valence System and a particular neurobiological/neuroecological model of reactions to threat, the Predatory Imminence Continuum (PIC) Theory. Using the COVID-19 pandemic as an illustration, we describe both adaptive and maladaptive behavior patterns from this perspective to illustrate how behavior in response to a crisis may get shaped. We end with suggestions on how further consideration of the PIC suggests potential modifications of the negative valence systems RDoC.

Introduction

Beginning in late 2019, a common threat unfolded before humanity. Our reactions to COVID-19 provide an all too real illustration of how our actions are organized by the imminence of threat as it moves closer, as illustrated in Figure 1. For those in California, the sequence looked something like this:

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. A comparative illustration between the predatory imminence continuum (PIC) theory and the RDoC’s negative valence system’s representation of changes in defensive action patterns based upon the perceived psychological proximity to the COVID-19 virus. Early in the pandemic, the COVID-19 threat was considered distal and not something to be that concerned about. Social distancing was not part of the typical behavior; instead, people engaged in more preferred social interactions (top row). As coronavirus cases surged and regular reports of hospital admittance and death tolls rose, social interaction was replaced with stockpiling supplies in preparation for the potential and pre-encounter of the coronavirus (middle row). As the threat became more proximal, stay-at-home orders were put into effect as a way to help evade contact and increase survival (bottom row). Some aspects of the evolution of the imminence of COVID-19 from distal to proximal threat also follow patterned stages in the RDoC Negative Valence System that ranged from potential threat, where increased vigilance emerges under conditions of uncertain safety, to acute threat activated when a specific danger is detected.

December 2019-early January 2020: Large gatherings of friends and families pack office parties and homes as the prototypical holiday season comes and goes. We engage in our typical pattern of preferred activity. Still, the world watches with one eye the unfolding of a mysterious coronavirus-related pneumonia spread in a populated city in China. For those in distal regions, thoughts of good will are sent out to those affected, along with internal thoughts of relief, “good thing it’s not here, and it’s not me.”

Late January-early March 2020: The presence of the new disease extends beyond the borders of the point of origin with no signs of containment. Coronavirus cases rise rapidly, and regular reports of death tolls in the most affected areas catch the media’s full attention worldwide. Cases spread in distributed pockets around the world. Hospitals in the highest affected areas are overwhelmed. While as individuals, we have not directly encountered the virus, even pre-encounter, our behaviors change in preparation for the potential arrival of the virus in our community. Households send out one brave member at a time to stock up on survival supplies like toilet paper and anti-bacterial products and clear store shelves of canned and frozen foods. The stockpiling of supplies goes beyond rational and necessity.

March 2020 and Beyond: The World Health Organization declares COVID-19 a pandemic. Our communities have now directly encountered the virus. The post-encounter world shuts down in a rapid crash of dominoes. Stay at home orders affect everyone as the presence of the threat in nearly every community is confirmed. Simple daily tasks like grocery shopping or even leaving the house put one at risk of contracting COVID-19 and potentially dying.

The evolution of the imminence of COVID-19 from distal to proximal threat followed patterned stages that ranged from low to high risk to even interaction with the threat itself. As the spatial, temporal, and psychological distance from the virus narrowed, defensive response strategies shifted in an effort to evade contact and increase survival. The COVID-19 global pandemic tells a directly relatable and unavoidable story of the predatory imminence continuum.

It is important to acknowledge that the COVID-19 global pandemic remains an acute source of loss and trauma for too many. The current perspective relates the predatory imminence continuum as one way to conceptualize the complex pattern of human behavior in response to the pandemic. We acknowledge that many of the subjective emotions experienced in response to and throughout the course of the pandemic and beyond may be explained by other psychological theories, as well.

The Predatory Imminence Continuum Theory

Animals translate environmental threat into specific patterns of behavioral action that have a phylogenetic history of protecting the species (Bolles, 1970). Supportive physiological changes accompany these species-specific defense reactions (SSDRs). Under threat, behavior becomes limited to these SSDRs, and the animal must strategically select the most appropriate SSDR from its repertoire of defensive behaviors. An influential model of SSDR selection applied to both humans and rodents is the Predatory (or Threat) Imminence Continuum (PIC) theory (Fanselow and Lester, 1988; Bouton et al., 2001; Mobbs et al., 2007, 2015). PIC states that qualitatively distinct defensive behaviors are matched to the psychological distance from physical contact with a life-threatening situation. For example, rodents freeze when they detect a predator but show vigorous bursts of activity to contact with the predator (Fanselow and Lester, 1988). Each defense mode across the PIC has unique antecedent conditions, and engages a distinct set of consequent behaviors. The three defense modes, pre-encounter, post-encounter, and circa-strike, map well onto states of anxiety, fear, and panic in mammals’ behavior (Mobbs et al., 2009; Perusini and Fanselow, 2015). Each mode is served by different, but interacting, neural circuits (Fanselow, 1994).

This organization of defensive behavior is highly conserved across many animal species, including humans (Roelofs et al., 2010; Hagenaars et al., 2014; Roelofs, 2017). The application of this framework to human defensive behaviors underscores the use of this model in understanding conditions that select for states of heightened anxiety and even the development of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), phobias, and panic disorder. For example, a response to a minor inconvenience may be amplified and even trigger a panic attack (circa-strike) when in a state of already high anxiety (pre-encounter defense) compared to our ability to brush it off when presented in a neutral or relaxed state (Bouton et al., 2001).

While the PIC theory was developed from the perspective of prey-predator interactions, the organization of the PIC theory can be generally applied to environmental threats. A rat responding to a cat or a human responding to the threat of COVID-19 both have their behavior organized with respect to the spatial, temporal, and probabilistic relationship with the threat. One possibility is that the highly significant threat of being eaten by a predator was the selective force behind the evolution of the PIC. Evolution often borrows existing adaptations in a process referred to as exaptation (Gould and Vrba, 1982). With this defensive brain-behavior mechanism in place, other threats acquired the ability to tap into this system (exaptation) over the course of phylogeny and, perhaps even, ontogenetic experience with other types of threat. This idea of novel threats tapping into the already evolved antipredator defensive system is common and is likely why rats’ reactions to innate threats like predators and novel learned threats like stimuli predicting electric shock are highly overlapping (Bolles, 1970; Lester and Fanselow, 1985).

Pre-Encounter Defense: Risky Foraging, Panic Shopping, and Hoarding

Despite the repeated assurance that food and supplies were not scarce or at risk for running out in our communities, panic shopping was a real phenomenon that plagued grocery and supply stores all over the world (Hao et al., 2020; Tyagi et al., 2020; Valaskova et al., 2021; Prentice et al., 2022). Pre-encounter defense consists of patterns of behavior that include increased vigilance and risk assessment. Additionally, meal pattern reorganization and cautiously leaving the nest area are components of pre-encounter defense. In early experiments simulating a naturalistic environment where foraging occurred under a low probability of threat, rodents lived for extended periods in a “closed economy” ecosystem that included a safe nest, as well as a foraging area that required lever press behavior in order to procure food (Fanselow et al., 1988; Helmstetter and Fanselow, 1993). Predation was modeled by the risk of delivery of footshock while foraging (Fanselow et al., 1988; Helmstetter and Fanselow, 1993). In these studies, the administration of random but rare shocks caused rats to decrease meal frequency but increase meal size such that while energy demands were met, the animals adapted their behavior to reduce the risk of exposure to shock by reducing total time foraging on the potentially dangerous grid. Interestingly, this meal pattern reorganization changed as a function of shock density (Fanselow and Lester, 1988; Fanselow, 1989; Helmstetter and Fanselow, 1993). In other words, as risk increased, the number of foraging trips decreased whereas meal size increased.

Similarly, during the pandemic, as the perceived risk of danger increased, humans adjusted foraging patterns. These shifts included adapting to optimal time windows for grocery shopping, relying on Google Maps data to determine the least busy store hours, implementing senior citizen shopping hours to protect the vulnerable, and a new surge in the reliance on grocery delivery. Based on PIC’s theoretical framework, one study directly tested this pre-encounter defense feature by asking whether the perceived threat of COVID-19 accounted for changes in purchasing behavior (Schmidt et al., 2021). Consistent with meal reorganization outcomes observed in the closed economy rodent experiments, Schmidt et al. found that the perceived threat of COVID-19 was a significant predictor for subjective changes in purchasing behavior, where high perceived threat was associated with increased purchasing quantities and a reduction in purchasing frequency (Schmidt et al., 2021). People faced the potential risk of COVID-19 by adopting shopping strategies similar to rats in the closed economy. Factors associated with the perceived threat of COVID-19 included higher levels of intolerance of uncertainty and high levels of media exposure, also related to increased purchasing quantity, contributing to the complexity of human threat perception during the pandemic.

Consistent with other infectious disease outbreaks and other crises such as natural disasters and extreme weather, COVID-19 led to hoarding or stockpiling behavior (Micalizzi et al., 2020). Stockpiling behavior is thought to stem from a response, either rationally or psychologically, to scarcity and psychological uncertainty (Micalizzi et al., 2020). In one survey study during the COVID-19 pandemic, the most hoarded item was toilet paper (Micalizzi et al., 2020). Additionally, high rates of guns and other weapons were procured. Interestingly, stockpiling behavior was more commonly observed in those that socially distanced less.

Post-Encounter Defense: Freezing and Lockdown

While pre-encounter defense occurs when no specific threat is close, things change dramatically when a threat has been detected in the vicinity. One example is when a foraging rat detects a predator. The dominant response of the rat at this point is to freeze. This response is not simply a lack of movement as the rat will move toward and freeze in corners and next to nearby objects. That is, freezing and thigmotaxis are highly related (Grossen and Kelley, 1972; Fanselow and Lester, 1988). If the prey has not been detected yet, freezing is effective because movement is a visually salient stimulus that will attract attention (Rokszin et al., 2010). However, freezing is also effective when the prey has been detected because movement is often a releasing stimulus for an attack (Suarez and Gallup, 1981). While the formal study of freezing has been predominantly studied in rodents, there have been laboratory examples of freezing in humans (Roelofs et al., 2010).

The threat from COVID-19 dramatically increased when it was detected in our communities. The virus was no longer a distal threat when it was encountered. Rather than shopping trips to hoard items, people refused to leave their houses. A sort of post-encounter freezing at home because the threat was in the immediate vicinity.

A large body of literature on rodents indicates that the amygdala is intimately related to freezing (Kim et al., 1993; Fanselow and LeDoux, 1999; Fendt and Fanselow, 1999; Davis and Whalen, 2001; Fanselow and Gale, 2003). The amygdala becomes more active during freezing, and inhibition of the amygdala dramatically reduces freezing (Helmstetter, 1992; Zelikowsky et al., 2014; Liu et al., 2022). The onset of the COVID-19 pandemic allowed itself a rare opportunity for a real-life natural experiment. As part of a pre-registered study, one research group in Israel compared brain volumetric changes in healthy people who received an MRI before and after the COVID-19 outbreak and lockdown (Salomon et al., 2021). While none of the participants were physically infected with the virus, they found significant impacts of the initial lockdown period on the amygdala. The authors showed prominent bilateral increases in amygdala volume in subjects that were scanned prior to and after lockdown (March-May 2020) compared to those that received two separate scans prior to COVID-19. Interestingly, the amygdala changes showed a time-dependent effect where the biggest increases were related to the time since lockdown and not the time since the baseline scan (Salomon et al., 2021).

The Relationship Between Predatory Imminence Continuum Theory and the Negative Valence Research Domain Criteria

With the Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) initiative, the National Institute of Mental Health of the United States (NIMH) set out to establish a framework that more tightly related mental disorders to neurobiology (“Definitions of the RDoC Domains and Constructs”) than the traditional framework described in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5, American Psychiatric Association [APA], 2013). United States health care professionals use the DSM-5 as a tool to assess clinical symptoms, and clusters of symptoms due to the comorbidity, of many mental disorders. However, a gap exists between clinical research and neurobiological mechanisms that may contribute to psychopathology (Insel et al., 2010; Sanislow et al., 2010). A goal of the RDoC framework was to bridge the gap between clinical research and behavioral neuroscience (Sanislow et al., 2010; Nicholson and Sommer, 2018) to form a more precise, quantifiable understanding of neuropsychiatric diseases (Nicholson and Sommer, 2018; Pacheco et al., 2022). The RDoC may also provide a framework for improving translational relevance from preclinical to clinical research (Insel et al., 2010; Nicholson and Sommer, 2018). Within the Negative Valence System, three of the constructs were specifically related to threat: Potential Threat, Acute Threat, and Sustained Threat (see Table 1). The clear correspondence of the potential threat and acute threat constructs with the predatory imminence modes of the pre-encounter defense and the post-encounter defense emerged from the discussions at the NIMH that generated the negative valence RDoC:1 National Institute of Mental Health [NIMH] (2011).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. The organization of PIC defense modes and relevant RDoC constructs.

Pre-encounter defense emerges when an organism leaves a situation of relative safety to one that has the potential of encountering a threat, making the antecedent causes of these two constructs synonymous (potential threat). A specific threat is not present, but behavior is altered in ways to reduce that potential. Behavioral changes such as increased vigilance emerge, but as we described above, under conditions of uncertain safety, there are more complex changes such as alterations of foraging patterns in both humans and rodents. The Pre-encounter/Potential Threat constructs well to an anxious state.

Similarly, the Acute Threat category was designed to correspond to Post-Encounter Defense. Both are activated when a specific danger is detected, and harm is far more likely than in the Pre-encounter mode. Given that the threat is actual, the state corresponds to fear. It is important to recognize that the difference between acute and potential risk conditions is often probabilistic, the stimulus that is the source of risk may be the same. For example, a rat may engage in pre-encounter defense because there is a possibility of encountering a threat, but a cat has not yet been detected. Post-encounter defense occurs because a cat has been detected. The source of the threat, a cat, is the same in both cases, but the probability of an attack is different. Another example was provided by Helmstetter and Fanselow (1993). They reported that rats that lived in a free foraging environment decreased meal frequency and increased meal size, defending body weight when the probability of shock while foraging was low. When shock probability increased further feeding was suppressed, and the rats lost weight. So, the same stimulus, shock, produced both Pre-encounter and Post-encounter behavior, dependent on the probability of occurrence. Interestingly, the shock probability that suppressed feeding is about the same as the shock probability needed to produce freezing (Fanselow, 1989).

Divergence of Predatory Imminence Continuum Theory and the Research Domain Criteria

There are two points of divergence between the RDoC Schema and PIC Theory. Predatory imminence includes a component called Circa-strike behavior that is not represented in the RDoC. RDoC also considers a category of sustained threat that PIC Theory does not address. We examine these two in turn.

Circa-strike responses occur around the time of actual physical contact with a threat. In rodents, freezing is replaced by vigorous bursts of activity, struggling, and vocalization designed to evade the clutches of a predator. These reactions seem akin to panic. Interestingly, in perhaps another example of exaptation, suffocation leads to similar struggling. Both circa-strike behavior to contact related stimuli and panic reactions to CO2 are mediated by the midbrain periaqueductal gray (Fanselow, 1991; Graeff, 2004; Spiacci et al., 2018). The final stages of predation often include the predator suffocating the prey before consumption (Greene and Burghardt, 1978; Anton and Turner, 1997). In humans, this can be modeled by CO2 inhalation, which causes panic-like reactions, especially in those with panic disorder (Gorman et al., 2001; Wiese and Boutros, 2019). Similarly, rodents show Circa-strike-like panic behavior in response to CO2 (Graeff, 2004; Spiacci et al., 2018). The absence of a panic-like category in the RDoC is notable. Given the correspondence between PIC and the Negative Valence System, this would seem like a valuable addition and should be considered in future revisions of the Negative Valence Systems RDoC.

Sustained Threat is the category in the Negative Valence System that corresponds to exposure to aversive conditions lasting weeks to months with behavioral changes that persist in the absence of threat. Cleary, such sustained reactions are maladaptive. On the other hand, PIC Theory describes adaptive behavior; defending against a threat is the phylogenetically appropriate response in threatening situations. However, if defensive behavior persists beyond the time of threat, it would reduce the amount of time for the important non-aversively motivated behaviors that characterize the preferred activity pattern. Anxiety disorders are considered disorders when they compromise normal adaptive behavior. Perhaps we can conceptualize anxiety disorders as a distortion of the PIC. Recently, we reported that prior exposure to potent stress causes an increase in behaviors across the PIC (Hoffman et al., 2022). Furthermore, these changes in behavior persisted long after the stress, in other words, they were sustained. Thus, the sustained threat construct can be viewed within PIC Theory as a long-term distortion of the continuum.

Conclusion

In this perspective, we develop the idea that much of our negatively valanced emotion, particularly anxiety, fear, and panic, are rooted in the systems that evolved to deal with the threat of predation. Antipredator defensive behavior is organized around a threat imminence continuum anchored on one end by typical activity patterns that occur in the absence of threat and serve biologically important functions other than defense, such as feeding and mating. The other end of the continuum is terminal, the prey is consumed by the predator. The specifics of defensive behavior and their underlying emotions proceed through stages of potential threat, to actual threat, to physical contact with the threat. Constructs within the NIMH Negative Valence system correspond to these different modes of defensive action. While predation may have provided the original selection pressure for this behavior system, over phylogeny other types of threat have tapped into this pre-existing organization through the process of exaptation. We use responses to the COVID-19 pandemic to illustrate this point. The PIC Theory allows adaptive defensive behaviors to match the currently threatening situation. We suggest that some experiences, and perhaps some genetic variations, may result in a distorted PIC that limits the time available for non-threat-related behavior by increasing time in defensive states. Such distortions of the PIC may be the basis of anxiety and stress-related disorders.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author Contributions

NJS made Figure 1. SYM developed Table 1. All authors contributed equally to the writing of this perspective.

Funding

This research was funded by NIH grant RO1-MH62122 (MSF), NARSAD Young Investigator Grant (AH) and the Staglin Center for Brain and Behavioral Health (MSF).

Conflict of Interest

MSF is Director of Research for Neurovation Labs.

The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Footnotes

  1. ^ https://www.nimh.nih.gov/research/research-funded-by-nimh/rdoc/negative-valence-systems-workshop-proceedings

References

American Psychiatric Association [APA] (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-5 (5th edition). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association.

Google Scholar

Anton, M., and Turner, A. (1997). Big Cats and Their Fossil Relatives. [online]. New York: Columbia University Press.

Google Scholar

Bolles, R. C. (1970). Species-specific Defense Reactions and Avoidance learning. Psychol. Rev. 77, 32–48. doi: 10.1037/h0028589

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bouton, M. E., Mineka, S., and Barlow, D. H. (2001). A Modern Learning Theory Perspective on the Etiology of Panic disorder. Psychol. Rev. 108, 4–32. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.108.1.4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, M., and Whalen, P. J. (2001). The amygdala: vigilance and emotion. Mol. Psychiatry 6, 13–34. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4000812

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fanselow, M. S. (1989). “The adaptive function of conditioned defensive behavior: An ecological approach to Pavlovian stimulus substitution theory,” in Ethoexperimental Approaches to the Study of Behavior (NATO ASI Series D: 48: 151-166), eds R. J. Blanchard, P. F. Brain, D. C. Blanchard, and S. Parmigiani (Boston: Kluver Academic Publishers), doi: 10.3758/BF03210947

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fanselow, M. S. (1991). “The Midbrain Periaqueductal Gray as a Coordinator of Action in Response to Fear and Anxiety,” in The Midbrain Periaqueductal Gray Matter: Functional, Anatomical and Immunohistochemical Organization, eds A. Depaulis and R. Bandler (New York: Plenum Publishing Corp), 151–173. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4615-3302-3_10

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fanselow, M. S. (1994). Neural Organization of the Defensive Behavior System Responsible for Fear. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 1, 429–438. doi: 10.3758/bf03210947

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fanselow, M. S., and Lester, L. (1988). “A Functional Behavioristic Approach to Aversively Motivated behavior: Predatory Imminence as a Determinant of the Topography of Defensive behavior,” in Evolution and Learning, eds R. C. Bolles and M. D. Beecher (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erbaum Associate, Inc).

Google Scholar

Fanselow, M. S., and LeDoux, J. E. (1999). Why we think plasticity underlying Pavlovian fear conditioning occurs in the basolateral amygdala. Neuron 23, 229–232.

Google Scholar

Fanselow, M. S., and Gale, G. D. (2003) The Amygdala, Fear, and Memory. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 985, 125–134. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2003.tb07077.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fanselow, M. S., Lester, L. S., and Helmstetter, F. J. (1988). Changes in Feeding and Foraging Patterns as an Antipredator Defensive strategy: a Laboratory Simulation Using Aversive Stimulation in a Closed Economy. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 50, 361–374. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1988.50-361

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fendt, M., and Fanselow, M. S. (1999). The neuroanatomical and neurochemical basis of conditioned fear. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 23, 743–760. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(99)00016-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gorman, J. M., Kent, J., Martinez, J., Browne, S., Coplan, J., and Papp, L. A. (2001). Physiological Changes during Carbon Dioxide Inhalation in Patients with Panic Disorder, Major Depression, and Premenstrual Dysphoric Disorder. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 58:125. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.58.2.125

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gould, S. J., and Vrba, E. S. (1982). Exaptation—a Missing Term in the Science of Form. Paleobiology 8, 4–15. doi: 10.1017/s0094837300004310

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Graeff, F. G. (2004). Serotonin, the Periaqueductal Gray and Panic. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 28, 239–259. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2003.12.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greene, H., and Burghardt, G. (1978). Behavior and phylogeny: constriction in Ancient and Modern Snakes. Science 200, 74–77. doi: 10.1126/science.635575

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grossen, N. E., and Kelley, M. J. (1972). Species-specific behavior and acquisition of avoidance behavior in rats. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 81:307. doi: 10.1037/h0033536

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hagenaars, M. A., Oitzl, M., and Roelofs, K. (2014). Updating freeze: aligning animal and human research. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 47, 165–176. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.07.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hao, N., Wang, H. H., and Zhou, Q. (2020). The impact of online grocery shopping on stockpile behavior in Covid-19. China Agric. Econ. Rev. 12, 459–470 doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2021.779654

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Helmstetter, F. J. (1992). Contribution of the Amygdala to Learning and Performance of Conditional Fear. Physiol. Behav. 51, 1271–1276. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(92)90320-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Helmstetter, F. J., and Fanselow, M. S. (1993). Aversively Motivated Changes in Meal Patterns of Rats in a Closed economy: the Effects of Shock Density. Anim. Learn. Behav. 21, 168–175. doi: 10.3758/bf03213397

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoffman, A. N., Trott, J. M., Makridis, A., and Fanselow, M. S. (2022). Anxiety, fear, panic: an Approach to Assessing the Defensive Behavior System across the Predatory Imminence Continuum. Learn. Behav. [Epub ahead of print]. doi: 10.3758/s13420-021-00509-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Insel, T., Cuthbert, B., Garvey, M., Heinssen, R., Pine, D. S., Quinn, K., et al. (2010). Research domain criteria (RDoC): toward a new classification framework for research on mental disorders. Am. J. Psychiatry 167, 748–751. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09091379

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, J. J., Rison, R. A., and Fanselow, M. S. (1993). Effects of Amygdala, Hippocampus, and Periaqueductal Gray Lesions on Short- and Long-Term Contextual Fear. Behav Neurosci. 107, 1093–1098. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.107.6.1093

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lester, L. S., and Fanselow, M. S. (1985). Exposure to a Cat Produces Opioid Analgesia in rats. Behav. Neurosci. 99, 756–759. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.99.4.756

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, J., Totty, M. S., Melissari, L., Bayer, H., and Maren, S. (2022). Convergent Coding of Recent and Remote Fear Memory in the Basolateral Amygdala. Biol. Psychiatry 91, 832–840 doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2021.12.018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Micalizzi, L., Zambrotta, N. S., and Bernstein, M. H. (2020). Stockpiling in the Time of COVID-19. Br. J. Health Psychol. 26, 535–543. doi: 10.1111/bjhp.12480

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mobbs, D., Hagan, C. C., Dalgleish, T., Silston, B., and Prévost, C. (2015). The ecology of human fear: survival optimization and the nervous system. Front. Neurosci. 9:55. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2015.00055

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mobbs, D., Marchant, J. L., Hassabis, D., Seymour, B., Tan, G., Gray, M., et al. (2009). From Threat to Fear: the Neural Organization of Defensive Fear Systems in Humans. J. Neurosci. 29, 12236–12243. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.2378-09.2009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mobbs, D., Petrovic, P., Marchant, J. L., Hassabis, D., Weiskopf, N., Seymour, B., et al. (2007). When Fear Is Near: threat Imminence Elicits Prefrontal-Periaqueductal Gray Shifts in Humans. Science 317, 1079–1083. doi: 10.1126/science.1144298

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

National Institute of Mental Health [NIMH] (2011). Negative Valence Systems: Workshop Proceedings. Bethesda: National Institute of Mental Health.

Google Scholar

Nicholson, J. R., and Sommer, B. (2018). The research domain criteria framework in drug discovery for neuropsychiatric diseases: focus on negative valence. Brain Neurosci. Adv. 2:2398212818804030. doi: 10.1177/2398212818804030

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pacheco, J., Garvey, M. A., Sarampote, C. S., Cohen, E. D., Murphy, E. R., and Friedman-Hill, S. R. (2022). Annual Research Review: the contributions of the RDoC research framework on understanding the neurodevelopmental origins, progression and treatment of mental illnesses. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry Allied Discip. 63, 360–376. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.13543

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perusini, J. N., and Fanselow, M. S. (2015). Neurobehavioral Perspectives on the Distinction between Fear and Anxiety. Learn. Mem. 22, 417–425. doi: 10.1101/lm.039180.115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prentice, C., Quach, S., and Thaichon, P. (2022). Antecedents and consequences of panic buying: the case of COVID-19. Int. J. Consum. Stud. 46, 132–146. doi: 10.1111/ijcs.12649

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roelofs, K. (2017). Freeze for action: neurobiological mechanisms in animal and human freezing. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 372:20160206. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2016.0206

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roelofs, K., Hagenaars, M. A., and Stins, J. (2010). Facing freeze: social threat induces bodily freeze in humans. Psychol. Sci. 21, 1575–81. doi: 10.1177/0956797610384746

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rokszin, A., Márkus, Z., Braunitzer, G., Berényi, A., Benedek, G., and Nagy, A. (2010). Visual pathways serving motion detection in the mammalian brain. Sensors 10, 3218–3242. doi: 10.3390/s100403218

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salomon, T., Cohen, A., Barazany, D., Ben-Zvi, G., Botvinik-Nezer, R., Gera, R., et al. (2021). Brain Volumetric Changes in the General Population following the COVID-19 Outbreak and Lockdown. NeuroImage 239:118311. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118311

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sanislow, C. A., Pine, D. S., Quinn, K. J., Kozak, M. J., Garvey, M. A., Heinssen, R. K., et al. (2010). Developing constructs for psychopathology research: research domain criteria. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 119, 631–639. doi: 10.1037/a0020909

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmidt, S., Benke, C., and Pané-Farré, C. A. (2021). Purchasing under threat: changes in Shopping Patterns during the COVID-19 Pandemic. PLoS One 16:e0253231. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0253231

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spiacci, A., Vilela-Costa, H. H., Sant’Ana, A. B., Fernandes, G. G., Frias, A. T., da Silva, G. S. F., et al. (2018). Panic-like Escape Response Elicited in Mice by Exposure to CO2, but Not Hypoxia. Prog. Neuro-Psychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 81, 178–186. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2017.10.018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suarez, S. D., and Gallup, G. G. (1981). An Ethological Analysis of open-field Behavior in Rats and Mice. Learn. Motiv. 12, 342–363. doi: 10.1016/0023-9690(81)90013-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tyagi, P., Singh, U., and Sharma, R. (2020). Panic shopping in COVID 19 pandemic: a comparative study of eastern and western culture. Int. J. Creat. Res. Thoughts 8, 44–49.

Google Scholar

Valaskova, K., Durana, P., and Adamko, P. (2021). Changes in consumers’ purchase patterns as a consequence of the COVID-19 pandemic. Mathematics 9:1788. doi: 10.3390/math9151788

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wiese, A. D., and Boutros, N. N. (2019). Diagnostic Utility of Sodium Lactate Infusion and CO2-35% Inhalation for Panic Disorder. Neuropsychobiology 78, 59–69. doi: 10.1159/000499136

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zelikowsky, M., Hersman, S., Chawla, M. K., Barnes, C. A., and Fanselow, M. S. (2014). Neuronal Ensembles in Amygdala, Hippocampus, and Prefrontal Cortex Track Differential Components of Contextual Fear. J. Neurosci. 34, 8462–8466. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3624-13.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: fear, anxiety disorders, defensive behavior, threat imminence, COVID-19, RDoC

Citation: Smith NJ, Markowitz SY, Hoffman AN and Fanselow MS (2022) Adaptation of Threat Responses Within the Negative Valence Framework. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 16:886771. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2022.886771

Received: 28 February 2022; Accepted: 22 April 2022;
Published: 26 May 2022.

Edited by:

Alessandra Porcu, University of California, San Diego, United States

Reviewed by:

Jiban Shrestha, Nepal Agricultural Research Council, Nepal
Maya Opendak, Johns Hopkins University, United States

Copyright © 2022 Smith, Markowitz, Hoffman and Fanselow. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Michael S. Fanselow, fanselow@UCLA.edu

These authors have contributed equally to this work and share first authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.