Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Syst. Neurosci., 30 March 2021
This article is part of the Research Topic Does Electrical Stimulation Map Brain Function? View all 14 articles

Modulating Inhibitory Control Processes Using Individualized High Definition Theta Transcranial Alternating Current Stimulation (HD θ-tACS) of the Anterior Cingulate and Medial Prefrontal Cortex

  • 1National Institute of Mental Health, Prague, Czechia
  • 2Department of Psychiatry, Third Faculty of Medicine, Charles University, Prague, Czechia

Increased frontal midline theta activity generated by the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) is induced by conflict processing in the medial frontal cortex (MFC). There is evidence that theta band transcranial alternating current stimulation (θ-tACS) modulates ACC function and alters inhibitory control performance during neuromodulation. Multi-electric (256 electrodes) high definition θ-tACS (HD θ-tACS) using computational modeling based on individual MRI allows precise neuromodulation targeting of the ACC via the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), and optimizes the required current density with a minimum impact on the rest of the brain. We therefore tested whether the individualized electrode montage of HD θ-tACS with the current flow targeted to the mPFC-ACC compared with a fixed montage (non-individualized) induces a higher post-modulatory effect on inhibitory control. Twenty healthy subjects were randomly assigned to a sequence of three HD θ-tACS conditions (individualized mPFC-ACC targeting; non-individualized MFC targeting; and a sham) in a double-blind cross-over study. Changes in the Visual Simon Task, Stop Signal Task, CPT III, and Stroop test were assessed before and after each session. Compared with non-individualized θ-tACS, the individualized HD θ-tACS significantly increased the number of interference words and the interference score in the Stroop test. The changes in the non-verbal cognitive tests did not induce a parallel effect. This is the first study to examine the influence of individualized HD θ-tACS targeted to the ACC on inhibitory control performance. The proposed algorithm represents a well-tolerated method that helps to improve the specificity of neuromodulation targeting of the ACC.

Introduction

The anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) plays an important role in the processing of cognition and emotions (Gasquoine, 2013; Onoda et al., 2017). While cognitive processes are attributed to the dorsal ACC (dACC), emotional processes are assigned to the rostral division of the ACC (Bush et al., 2000). According to recent comprehensive theory, the ACC is activated in response to a conflict between incompatible streams of information processing; it represents an essential structure for the inhibitory control process (van Veen and Carter, 2002). ACC disruptions in various neuropsychiatric disorders, such as schizophrenia and obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD), have been documented by functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies (Carter et al., 2001; Ursu et al., 2003; Fitzgerald et al., 2005). In schizophrenia, the reduction of ACC activity results in decreased activity in the processing of conflicting information and inhibition ability (Carter et al., 2001), while in OCD, higher ACC activity increases conflict-processing information and inhibitory activity (van Veen and Carter, 2002; Ursu et al., 2003; Fitzgerald et al., 2005; Gasquoine, 2013).

The medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), also covering the dACC, generates theta oscillatory activity, so-called Frontal Midline Theta (FMT). FMT enhancement over the medial frontal cortex (MFC), recorded during conflict monitoring, error processing, and top-down behavior adjustments (Cohen et al., 2008; Cavanagh and Shackman, 2015; Van Noordt et al., 2016) has also been documented as having behaviorally relevant event-related potentials (ERPs) during resolution of conflict-related tasks (Nigbur et al., 2012; Cohen and Donner, 2013) associated with the procession of inhibitory control. The latter can be evaluated by verbal (Stroop Test) or non-verbal (Stop Signal, Visual Simon, or Flanker Task) tests (Kopp et al., 1996a,b; Gratton et al., 1988; Bush et al., 2000; Taylor et al., 2007; Cohen and Donner, 2013; Spielberg et al., 2015), measuring different parameters, such as commission of mistakes or those related to conflict and error.

Transcranial Alternating Current Stimulation (tACS) enables modulation of endogenous oscillations at the pacing rate (Paulus, 2011; Ali et al., 2013; Antal and Paulus, 2013; Herrmann et al., 2013; Antal and Herrmann, 2016), inducing frequency-specific activity changes (Vossen et al., 2015; Witkowski et al., 2016) and the subsequent induction of synaptic changes via neuronal plasticity, providing a post-modulation effect on brain oscillation activity (Antal et al., 2008; Zaehle et al., 2010; Vossen et al., 2015; Berger et al., 2018; Moliadze et al., 2019). The effect of various tACS frequencies on cognition was tested (Sela et al., 2012; Hoy et al., 2015; van Driel et al., 2015; Vosskuhl et al., 2015; Wischnewski et al., 2016; Fusco et al., 2018; Pahor and Jaušovec, 2018; Lehr et al., 2019; Moliadze et al., 2019). Specific cognitive abilities were significantly modified mostly by the theta frequency tACS (θ-tACS; Sela et al., 2012; van Driel et al., 2015; Vosskuhl et al., 2015; Wischnewski et al., 2016; Fusco et al., 2018, 2020; Pahor and Jaušovec, 2018; Lang et al., 2019; Lehr et al., 2019). Neuroimaging (MEG and fMRI) studies have also shown that FMT performance is influenced by θ-tACS, which modulates dACC network functions (Chander et al., 2016; Onoda et al., 2017). However, previous studies that documented the behavioral and electrophysiological effects of θ-tACS have mostly targeted various non-specific anatomic frontal cortex regions (van Driel et al., 2015; Vosskuhl et al., 2015; Chander et al., 2016; Onoda et al., 2017; Fusco et al., 2018; Pahor and Jaušovec, 2018), including the MFC (van Driel et al., 2015; Vosskuhl et al., 2015; Fusco et al., 2018).

So far, only a few studies have tested the impact of θ-tACS on inhibitory control performance (van Driel et al., 2015; Fusco et al., 2018, 2020; Lehr et al., 2019). For example, Lehr et al.’s (2019) study targeted the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), which together with the ACC belongs to the inhibitory control circuit. Other studies (van Driel et al., 2015; Fusco et al., 2018, 2020) targeted the ACC indirectly via the MFC. With the exception of van Driel et al.’s (2015) study, these experiments monitored the effect of θ-tACS on inhibitory control performance only during the application of θ-tACS; the post-modulatory effect was not tested (Fusco et al., 2018, 2020; Lehr et al., 2019). The studies have yielded inconsistent results, ranging from enhancement (Fusco et al., 2018, 2020) to deterioration of inhibitory control performance (van Driel et al., 2015; Lehr et al., 2019).

It has been suggested that tACS may be used as a tool to investigate diseases with altered EEG activity (Antal et al., 2008; Ahn et al., 2019; Del Felice et al., 2019). Therefore, the effect of θ-tACS of the ACC on inhibitory control could be examined in neuropsychiatric disorders associated with disrupted dACC activity. We assume that θ-tACS targeted directly at the source of FMT via the mPFC-ACC may modify inhibitory control processes with a sustained effect.

However, standard transcranial Electrical Stimulation (tES; Nikolin et al., 2015) technologies (in terms of electrode assembly, number, shape, and size) are insufficient for more specific targeted tES (Lefaucheur et al., 2017); precise targeting of the deeper brain structures cannot therefore be documented. In addition, standard tES directly affects not only the selected area of interest but also the surrounding and interconnected structures.

High definition (HD) tES provides a solution to these methodological limitations. It is guided by structural neuroimaging, which allows a computer model of the electric field intensity distributions to be created. This model is essential for precise neuromodulation targeting with respect to the different conductivity of various brain tissues (Nikolin et al., 2015; Alam et al., 2016). Geodesic Transcranial Electrical Neuromodulation (GTEN) system with 256 electrodes enables Multielectrode Transcranial Electrical Stimulation (MTES), which optimizes the required current density in the targeted brain area. MTES analyses (Dmochowski et al., 2011; Alam et al., 2016; Fernández-Corazza et al., 2016) have shown that the use of a higher electrode density improves the focus, directionality, and stimulation intensity parameters by penetrating the deeper brain structures. MTES enables targeted neuromodulation of selected areas with greater specificity than the standard technologies; therefore, it is possible to modulate the ACC activity via mPFC-ACC stimulation more selectively.

Our study goal was to investigate the post-modulatory effects of θ-tACS in inhibitory control processing. For this purpose, we applied different θ-tACS neuromodulation protocols targeted at the MFC in healthy subjects, and evaluated θ-tACS-induced post-modulatory changes in tasks specific to inhibitory control. We assumed that by strengthening an oscillating current at the theta frequency, θ-tACS would induce a persistent improvement of the inhibitory control performance in the post-modulation assessment. We hypothesized that the individualized HD θ-tACS, targeted according to the computational modeling for optimal electrode arrangement, would induce a post-modulatory effect of θ-tACS on inhibitory control. This effect would be significantly stronger than the effect of a non-individualized θ-tACS targeting the ACC indirectly via the MFC. Moreover, we also compared the effect of individualized HD θ-tACS with non-individualized θ-tACS and with a sham condition.

Materials and Methods

Subjects

Twenty healthy volunteers (mean age 34.4 ± 7.2 years, 10 females) were included in the study. Exclusion criteria were a current diagnosis or history of a psychiatric disorder; substance use disorder (with the exception of nicotine); regular use of any medication that might affect cognitive functions (e.g., antihistamines and benzodiazepines); a history of serious head injury or a neurological disorder; a medical condition that could interfere with the tES administration (Poreisz et al., 2007); pregnancy; breastfeeding; or a sensory or motor impairment. All participants signed an informed consent form in accordance with the latest version of the Declaration of Helsinki; the study protocol was approved by the Independent Ethics Committee of the National Institute of Mental Health, Klecany.

Study Protocol

A double-blind, sham-controlled, three-condition, three-period cross-over study was carried out in random order, with a 1 week minimum wash-out period to avoid carryover effects. Following Williams’ design (Williams, 1949) for the 3 × 3 cross-over trial, participants were randomly assigned to one of six different sequences of three θ-tACS conditions: (1) individualized targeting of the mPFC-ACC according to the individual head model; (2) non-individualized electrodes placed in the FCz and Pz areas according to the skull’s anatomy (Vosskuhl et al., 2015; Fusco et al., 2018); and (3) the sham (Figure 1A). To assure a balanced number of subjects in each sequence, we used non-stratified blocked randomization with a block size of six (computer generated1). Every experimental session for each subject was carried out at the same time of day, on the same weekday. Participants were asked to abstain from caffeine and nicotine for at least 2 h prior to the cognitive assessment and the tACS session and to refrain from alcohol or any medication for 24 h prior to the tACS administration.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Flow chart of the protocol. (A) Schematic overview of the study. (B) Illustration of head modeling with tissue segmentation, co-registration of an individual head model with GPS electrode positioning, and setting of a protocol to select the appropriate electrodes according to the individual cortical current density, followed by neuromodulation protocol settings. (C) Experimental procedure. A double-blind, cross-over study. Three neuromodulation sessions were carried out in random order with a minimum 1 week washout period. The psychometric measurement was tested immediately before and after the end of each neuromodulation session. Types of HD θ-tACS protocols: an individualized HD θ-tACS was targeted with the highest specificity to the mPFC-ACC; a non-individualized θ-tACS was targeted over the MFC with fixed electrode positions in the FCz and Pz areas; a sham tACS was administered with identical electrode montage (as used in the non-individualized protocol). Active θ-tACS sessions comprised a 5 s ramp up, 30 min of 6 Hz θ-tACS, and a 5 s ramp down. Sham condition comprised a 5 s ramp up and 30 s of 6 Hz θ-tACS followed by a 29 min 30 s rest period and a 5 s ramp down. Psychometric measurement was assessed by the Simon Task, Stop Signal Task, Conners’ Continuous Performance Test 3rd edition (CPT III), and the Stroop Test.

The study participants and the researchers evaluating the psychometric measurements remained blind to the stimulation conditions and parameters. Neuropsychological assessment of inhibitory control was performed before and after each tACS application (Figure 1C). To test for the integrity of the blinding procedure, all participants were asked the following question at the end of the study: “During which stimulation session was a placebo applied? Was it during the first, second, or third session?” There were three possible answers: a placebo was applied during the first (011), the second (101), or the third (110).

Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI) and the Geodesic Photogrammetry System (GPS)

Individual magnetic resonance images obtained with a Siemens Magnetom Prisma 3T system (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) were used to create an individual head model for each subject. The subjects were scanned using a T1 Sag MPR 1 mm ISO sequence (repetition time TR of 2,300 ms; echo time TE of 1.69 ms; field of view 288 mm; slice slab 256 mm; voxel size of 1 × 1 × 1 mm and total acquisition time 5 min 12 s).

Subsequently, to obtain accurate positions for the individual electrodes on the scalp, the head of the subject was targeted by a sensor-registration system determining the 3-dimensional position of each of 256 sensors (neuromodulation electrodes). Electrode sensor positions of the 256-channel HydroCel Geodesic Sensor Net (HCGSN) 100 (EGI, Eugene, OR, United States) were digitalized with a GPS (EGI, Eugene, OR, United States) (Luu et al., 2016).

Head Modeling

Figure 1B schematically illustrates the head modeling with tissue segmentation, co-registration of an individual head model with GPS electrode positioning, and the settings of a protocol to select the appropriate electrodes according to the individual cortical current density followed by the neuromodulation protocol settings.

The individual head models were processed by the Modal Image Pipeline, ver. 1.10 (EGI, Eugene, OR, United States). To calculate the accurate head model, the initial segmentation of the white matter, gray matter, and background ratio was processed. Particular attention was given to the manual correction of gray matter smoothing and cortical surface extraction, to avoid digital errors when calculating normal the cortical conductivity vectors (Fernández-Corazza et al., 2016). The MRI images were subsequently segmented into tissue types (scalp, skull, eyeballs, cerebrospinal fluid, gray matter, white matter, and air) and proportionalized to the right and left hemispheres. Following the scalp reconstruction necessary for co-registration with the GPS sensors, the landmarks corresponding to the exact positions of selected electrodes from the individual GPS images were marked on the head model. In the next step, a 3D cortical surface with a 4800 dipoles patch was created to describe the current flow from the scalp to the cortex (Fernández-Corazza et al., 2016; Luu et al., 2016). The head model of the cortical surface was subsequently co-registered in JavaScript with a GPS file containing the photographic images of the individual positions of each of the 256 sensors placed on the subject’s head while images were taken by the GPS camera.

The final head model with a neuromodulatory electrode selection and stimulation protocol setting was designed in the Reciprocity Visualization Environment, ver. 1.1 (EGI, Eugene, OR, United States), which allows the optimal electrode arrangement to be selected and the appropriate amount of current delivered by the current injection through each electrode (Luu et al., 2016) to be determined to achieve the maximum current density at a given target of the cortical surface model.

Neuromodulation Protocols

An optimal electrode arrangement based on computational modeling from individual MRI was used for the individualized protocol with the current flow targeted with the highest specificity at the mPFC-ACC, and to achieve maximum current density in the ACC. A mounting (up to 16 channels) with a maximum of six cathode-anodes and 10 anode-cathodes was employed, because the anatomical variations of the subjects would have a potentially significant impact on the field strength in a given area. The specification of the electrode layouts, including the number of electrodes, their position, and current intensity in each electrode for individualized tACS for each subject is shown in Table 1. The arrangement of the neuromodulatory electrodes for the individual head models was computed with respect to individual cortical geometry, the head shapes of the subjects, and the corresponding current densities of the cortical surface (Figure 2).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. The specification of electrode layouts (256-channel HydroCel Geodesic Sensor Net), including the number of electrodes, their position, and current intensity in each electrode used for individualized HD θ-tACS in subjects.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. The electrode placement used in the individualized and non-individualized HD θ-tACS protocols. (Top) The individualized placement of electrodes for HD θ-tACS of the ACC targeted through the mPFC-ACC was selected on the basis of a reciprocity-optimized selection of current injection electrodes. Six anode-cathodes (red electrodes) and 10 cathode-anodes (blue electrodes) are shown. Note that to select the optimal electrode arrangement and current injection electrode, the individualized flux pattern with the highest specificity of current flow for a given target region (mPFC-ACC) on the cortical surface model was used. This is an example montage. (Bottom) Non-individualized placement of electrodes in the FCz and Pz areas (Vosskuhl et al., 2015; Fusco et al., 2018) adapted for the HCGSN 100 using the 10-channel mount with five anode-cathodes (red electrodes) in the FCz area and five cathode-anodes (blue electrodes) in the Pz area for a MFC and medial parietal cortex electrode layout. (Left) anterior view without marking the current density; (middle left) sagittal view; (middle right) anterior view; and (right) view from above, with cortex coloring representing the calculated current density on the cortical surface from 0.0 μA/mm2 (black) to 1.0 μA/mm2 (yellow).

The non-individualized focusing of the electrode assembly (according to the skull anatomy) used MFC and medial parietal cortex electrode layouts (Vosskuhl et al., 2015; Fusco et al., 2018), specifically in the FCz and Pz areas (according to the international 10/20 EEG electrode system for “standard” anatomically guided positioning), and adapted for HCGSN 100 using the 10-channel mount with five anode-cathodes in the FCz area and five cathode-anodes in the Pz area (Figure 2).

The sham tACS was administered with an identical electrode montage, as used in the non-individualized protocol.

tACS Administration

Electrode positioning of the 256-channel HCGSN 100 was adjusted under the control of the GPS subordinate images prior to each tACS application. An Elefix conductive paste (Nihon Kohden, Tokyo, Japan) was used as the conductive material between the scalp and selected neuromodulatory electrodes. The impedance of the electrodes was monitored immediately before tACS application; it was below 10 KΩ.

Subsequently, a pulsed alternating current was applied with Net Station Acquisition software, ver. 5.4.2 (EGI, Eugene, OR, United States), using a CE-certified Geodesic Transcranial Electrical Neuromodulation (GTEN) 100 system (EGI, Eugene, OR, United States) with the 256-channel HCGSN 100, which is an evenly spaced network of Ag-AgCl electrodes (Luu et al., 2016). The maximum current at any given electrode (1 cm2) was 200 μA, with a total current of tACS 1 mA for each active session, under monitoring by a GTEN 100 Sentinel Circuit®.

The parameter settings for each active θ-tACS session were: a 5 s ramp up, 30 min of θ-tACS with 6 Hz, and a 5 s ramp down. For the sham condition, the application of θ-tACS after a 5 s ramp up was shortened to 30 s and was followed by a rest period of 29 min 30 s, terminated by a 5 s ramp down. A sham condition applied for a duration of 30 s has been previously described as a reliable blinding method for tACS (Zaghi et al., 2010; Pahor and Jaušovec, 2018). This reproduces somatic sensations similar to active stimulation. Tingling or heating up of the scalp occurs mostly at the beginning (ramp up and initial adaptation to θ-tACS) of the neuromodulation and at the end of the protocol (ramp down). During the θ-tACS application, the participants were in a resting condition (Figure 1C).

Psychometric Measurement

Psychometric measurements were performed immediately before and after each neuromodulation session by a rater blind to the treatment condition. The neuropsychological battery consisted of the following non-verbal tests: Visual Simon Task (Simon Task), Stop Signal Task, Conners’ Continuous Performance Test 3rd edition (CPT III), and verbal tests: Stroop Color and Word Test (SCWT or Stroop; Figure 3). The tests focused primarily on specific parameters of the cognitive domains that are related to cognitive control and that have shown minimum practice effect. The Simon Task and the Stop Signal Task were administered using the program Inquisit 3 (Inquisit, 2007).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Flow chart of the psychometric measurement. Flow chart of the psychometric measurement. Illustration of the individual assessments performed for the following periods: Visual Simon Task = 5 min, Stop Signal Task = 9 min, Conners’ Continuous Performance Test 3rd edition (CPT III) = 14 min, Stroop test = 3 min.

The Simon Task is used to measure the stimulus-response compatibility and the difference in reaction time (RT) between trials in which the prepotent association is congruent to the stimulus and the trials in which it is incongruent (Bialystok et al., 2004). Each trial of the task begins with a central fixation cross on the screen (800 ms), followed by a blank interval (250 ms). Then, a red or blue square appears on either the left or the right side of the screen and remains there if there is no response (1,000 ms). Participants are required to press the A key (left) when a blue square is presented or the L key (right) when a red square is presented. Response timing starts with the onset of the stimulus, and the participant’s response terminates the stimulus. The task takes approximately 2 min to complete, and involves 5 min of instruction. Participants have to complete all eight practice trials without a mistake before the experimental trials begin. If participants fail to press the key within the available time, they receive additional practice trials until all eight attempts are completed correctly. The following experiment (28 trials) consists of 14 congruent trials (presenting the square on the same side as the related response key) and 14 incongruent trials (presenting the square on the opposite side), which are presented in random order (Inquisit, 2007).

The Stop Signal Task was selected to assess the ability to stop a planned or ongoing action (van Gaal et al., 2009). It is a go/nogo reaction time task that provides a means of estimating the time it takes to stop executing a response that might already be underway but needs to be halted. This is called the stop signal reaction time. At the beginning of the task, a fixation circle is displayed in which an arrow points to the left or to the right. Participants are required to press the “←” key or “→” key, depending on the direction of the arrow, unless a beep sounds to stop the response before execution. Depending on performance, the delay between the appearance of the arrow and the beep sound is adjusted up or down. Previous successful trials make the delay longer in the new trial and previous unsuccessful trials make it shorter. Participants’ responses are available until the end of the trial. Each task trial has a duration of 2,000 ms. The initial Stop Signal Delay (SSD) is set to 250 ms, and between 50 to 1,150 ms with adjustment steps of 50 ms thereafter. The task takes approximately 9 min to complete. Arrows with different directions (half to the right; half to the left) are presented in random order. The practice block includes 32 trials (8 signal trials and 24 no signal trials). The following three experimental blocks consist of 72 trials each (18 signal trials and 54 no signal trials; Inquisit, 2007).

CPT III, which is designed primarily to assess attention-related changes (Conners, 2014), was used to measure expected changes in commissions (incorrect response to non-targeted), perseverations (number of perseverative responses represented by responses with a reaction time of less than 100 ms); hit reaction time (average speed of correct responses), hit reaction time (HRT) block change (the slope of change in HRT across the six blocks of the administration), and detectability (the ability of a respondent to discriminate non-target from other targets) scores. During the task, participants are required to press the spacebar when any letter appears (with the exception of “X”). The interstimulus interval between each letter is 1, 2, or 4 s. The test takes 14 min to complete and consists of 18 blocks with 20 trials (360 in total).

The classic card version of the Stroop assesses the test parameters associated with inhibitory control, specifically the difficulty in warding off distractors (interference words) in the color-word (CW) condition (Lezak et al., 2004), where successful performance requires the ability to inhibit pre-potent verbal responses and conflict monitoring (Cipolotti et al., 2016), and the Stroop Interference Score (IG), a value obtained by subtracting the “predicted CW score” from the actual CW score; this reflects an ability to inhibit interference. The test consists of three subtests/charts: a word (W) chart, a color (C) chart, and a chart with colored words (CW). Each chart contains 100 items arranged in five columns of 20 items, printed on a white background. The individual charts are administered in the order W, C, and CW. The W chart consists of the words “red,” “green,” and “blue,” printed in black. The words are arranged in random order so that two identical words do not follow each other within the column. C chart consists of the color items “XXX,” which are printed in a corresponding color: red, green, or blue. The color of two consecutive items is always different. The CW chart contains colored words in the same order as the W cart, but no item is printed in a color corresponding to the meaning of the word. The time limit on each chart is 45 s. In total, even with the instruction, the test takes approximately 3 min.

In addition, a qualitative questionnaire of adverse effects, including bodily sensations and subjective mood changes induced by θ-tACS was administered during and after each neuromodulation session.

Statistical Analyses

All analyses were conducted using the software package Stata, ver. 15 (StataCorp, 2017; College Station, TX: StataCorp LLC). The tests were two-sided and p < 0.05 was regarded as being statistically significant. Differences in the effect of tACS conditions (individualized [A1], non-individualized [A2], and sham [S]) on cognitive performance (Visual Simon Task, Stop Signal Task, CPT III, and Stroop Test) were analyzed in a linear mixed-effect model fitted by a restricted maximum likelihood procedure and with a Kenward-Roger degrees of freedom approximation. In the model, sequence (six sequences: A1A2S, A2SA1, SA1A2, A1SA2, A2A1S, and SA2A1), period (three periods: sessions 1, 2, and 3), first-order carryover, and treatment (three tACS conditions: A1, A2, and S) were entered as fixed effects, baseline performance as a continuous covariate, subject within sequence as a random effect, and covariance structure as unstructured. Additionally, post hoc differences in least-square means (and 95% confidence intervals) between treatment conditions were obtained. Pairwise comparisons with Sidak’s multiple testing adjustment were applied.

To assess blinding integrity we compared agreement/disagreement using Cohen’s kappa with an expected chance agreement of 34.3%. To assess the side effects of θ-tACS, including bodily sensations or mood changes, the numbers of participants who experienced them were compared across treatment conditions using a Cochrane Q-test.

Results

Twenty healthy volunteers randomized to treatment sequences completed three treatment conditions. The data obtained from cognitive testing were then analyzed.

The only significant treatment effect was found in the Stroop-CW subtest [F(2, 27.2) = 4.47, p = 0.021] and Stroop-IG interference score [F(2, 28.7) = 4.05, p = 0.028], whereas the effects of sequence [CW: F(5, 19.6) = 0.9, p = 0.5; IG: F(5, 16.4) = 0.6, p = 0.7], period [CW: F(2, 27.5) = 0.3, p = 0.7; IG: F(2, 27.7) = 1.5, p = 0.2], carryover [CW: F(2, 26.2) = 1.4, p = 0.3; IG: F(2, 27.2) = 0.7, p = 0.5], and treatment × period interaction [CW: F(4, 27.1) = 0.1, p = 1.00; IG: F(4, 27.9) = 0.4, p = 0.8] were non-significant. A significantly better performance was achieved after the individualized (rather than the non-individualized) tACS [CW: difference in least-square (LS) means = 3.89, 95%CI 0.30–7.46, t = 2.72, p = 0.03; IG: difference in LS means = 4.22, 95%CI 0.32–8.11, t = 2.71, p = 0.03], but when compared with the sham, neither the individualized nor the non-individualized tACS differed significantly (CW: individualized vs. sham: 0.47, 95%CI −3.19–4.12, t = 0.32, p = 0.9; non-individualized vs. sham: −3.40, 95%CI −0.16–6.98, t = −2.40, p = 0.07; IG: individualized vs. sham: 2.39, 95%CI −1.60–6.47, t = 1.18, p = 0.25; non-individualized vs. sham: −1.82, 95%CI −5.66d–2.02, t = −0.85, p = 0.4 (Table 2 and Figure 4).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Post-modulatory effect of High Definition θ-tACS on cognitive performance.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Post-modulation changes in the Stroop task after HD θ-tACS. Post-pretest change in the Stroop task represented by the black (W) and colored ink (C) congruous conditions, by the incongruous color-word condition (CW), and by the interference score (IG). The data are presented as mean ± SE. p < 0.05 and ∗∗p<0.01 after Sidak’s correction, linear mixed-effects model.

Except for transient sensations of tingling or burning at the beginning of the tACS procedure, induction of phosphenes (visual sensations of a ring or spot) during the active protocols, and transient headache, HD θ-tACS was well-tolerated, with no serious adverse effects reported (Table 3). Interestingly, neuromodulatory conditions differed in emotional and mood effects at debriefing. Frequent irritation was reported after the non-individualized θ-tACS, while the individualized condition was associated more frequently with calm and harmonization (Table 3).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Adverse or side effects, including bodily sensations and mood state changes induced by θ-tACS.

When evaluating blinding integrity, the observed agreement was 50%, with kappa 0.24, SE 0.15, z = 1.58, p = 0.06. The participants did not estimate the order of treatments (a position of placebo) significantly better than by chance (34%), so a major violation of blinding was not detected.

Discussion

The present study is the first to examine the influence of individualized HD θ-tACS targeted at the ACC on inhibitory control performance. We used the optimal electrode arrangement based on computational modeling from individual MRIs to target the current flow with the highest specificity at the mPFC-ACC, with maximum current density in the ACC, to investigate the post-modulatory θ-tACS effect on inhibitory control. Based on the evidence from previous studies that confirmed the alteration of inhibitory control performance during θ-tACS neuromodulation (van Driel et al., 2015; Moliadze et al., 2019), we extended the experiment to include a θ-tACS condition that modulated the ACC indirectly through MFC.

The post-modulatory effects of different θ-tACS protocols on inhibitory control performance were investigated with a novel paradigm of individualized HD θ-tACS, non-individualized θ-tACS targeting the ACC indirectly via the MFC, and a sham condition.

The main finding of our study was the presence of a post-modulatory effect in the improvement of inhibitory control with individualized HD θ-tACS. In contrast with the non-individualized θ-tACS, it demonstrated a post-modulatory improvement of inhibitory control with a verbal component, as documented within 45 min post neuromodulation. Specifically, we found a significant improvement in Stroop-CW and Stroop-IG performances following individualized HD θ-tACS. Unlike the parameters in other selected tasks, these tasks reflect the inhibitory control function with a verbal component. The changes in the non-verbal cognitive tasks (Simon Task, Stop Signal Task, and CPT III) did not induce a parallel effect. Compared with the sham condition, neither of the active θ-tACS protocols (individualized or non-individualized) were able to alter inhibitory control performance.

In general, our findings are in keeping with previous studies that have reported θ-tACS modulation of ACC activity (Chander et al., 2016; Onoda et al., 2017) and inhibitory control performance (van Driel et al., 2015; Moliadze et al., 2019).

Our results confirm those of a previous study in which the effect of alpha band (α) tACS on phonological word decisions was monitored (Moliadze et al., 2019). Although the authors used a different frequency (10 Hz rather than 6 Hz), bilateral prefrontal electrode positioning, and a different task for inhibitory control performance monitoring, their results showed a similar post-modulatory effect of tACS on inhibitory control with a verbal component. Interestingly, Fusco et al.’s (2020) study, in which they monitor the effect of tACS over the MFC and the extrastriate body area (with an electrode arrangement in the FCz and P08 areas) in cognitive conflicts related to bodily stimuli, highlights the causal relationship between function-specific brain areas and category-specific conflict tasks. During θ-tACS, there was an improvement in the Hand-Flanker task (representing bodily stimuli), but not in the Letter-Flanker task.

However, our results did not correspond with the findings of a study that used a non-individualized θ-tACS over the MFC (Fusco et al., 2018), though the authors only tested the immediate effect of a short-lasting (240 s duration) θ-tACS during the activation of the ACC with a specific inhibitory control task. This may interfere directly with cognitive processes and boost the effect of neuromodulation, similar to the effect shown in tDCS studies involving additional on-line cognitive training (Dedoncker et al., 2016; Hill et al., 2016). In contrast, and in keeping with van Driel et al. (2015), we used long-lasting θ-tACS to evaluate changes in the post-session processing of inhibitory control.

The finding that the non-individualized protocol was not effective in altering inhibitory control may be attributed to more complex regulation of inhibitory control than simple dACC activation. It also supports the assumption that the effect of θ-tACS may be affected by the concurrent modulation of surrounding and interconnected structures, by parallel modulation of the DLPFC in the individualized protocol, and by simultaneous modulation of the frontal and parietal cortex, the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) in particular. This would explain why van Lehr et al.’s (2019) study, in which the DLPFC was modulated, and Moliadze et al.’s (2019) study, in which the prefrontal cortex was targeted bilaterally, showed a tACS effect in altering the verbal component of inhibitory control (though the results were inconsistent).

Interestingly, the present study did not show any sustained changes in non-verbal performance. We speculate that the absence of the effect on inhibitory control in non-verbal tasks after θ-tACS may be related to the greater stability of simpler non-verbal inhibition processes that are possibly more difficult to affect. Inhibitory control of verbal performance, where the ACC is also responsible for the semantic coding process in verbal working memory and is considered to be the central neural base of the central executive processes (Kaneda and Osaka, 2008; Reverberi et al., 2015), is a more complex function that may be more sensitive to the changes induced by neuromodulation, including the post-modulatory effect. An alternative explanation may be the use of different cognitive control strategies in the Stroop and Simon Task. Each of these tests bias attention toward a different approach in resolving the conflict. While the Stroop enhances the processing of task-relevant cues (i.e., it enhances the processing of colors and does not inhibit processing of irrelevant cues—namely, the words), the Simon Task inhibits task-irrelevant cues (i.e., it suppresses the position of the button, but does not enhance processing of the relevant cue—namely, the color of the square (Egner and Hirsch, 2005; Egner et al., 2007; Egner, 2008; Soutschek et al., 2013). Although both control strategies employ the same network (Peterson et al., 2002), the first modulates the activity in the parietal cortex, while the response-based conflict resolution modifies premotor cortex activity (Egner et al., 2007). This difference is consistent with Pratte et al. (2010), who found that the Stroop interference effect is smallest in fast responses and increases with longer response times, while the Simon’s effect is strongest in fast response time trials, and decreases as the response time increases. In the present study, the stimulation led to better performance in Stroop-CW (i.e., the Stroop effect was smaller), which meant a shorter RT per CW. This indicates a boost in the processing of task-relevant traits of the stimuli. On the other hand, no Simon effect modulation resulted in the improvement of response-selection processing. A possible explanation for this difference is the fact that partial (unintentional) stimulation of the premotor cortex may disrupt the overall effect in the Simon Task, but not in the Stroop.

A previous θ-tACS study that modulated PFC showed an altered processing in emotion evaluation/appraisal that corresponded to the integration function of the dACC (Onoda et al., 2017). Analogously, we observed unexpected mood changes induced by neuromodulation, where the volunteers reported irritation following the non-individualized protocol, and more often felt calm and harmonized after the individualized session (Table 3). We speculate that irritability after non-individualized θ-tACS may be mediated by the simultaneous modulation of the IFG and parietal region, which are not influenced by the individualized protocol. Therefore, non-individualized θ-tACS of the ACC may alter common brain activity (particularly motor and premotor) in other areas (Veniero et al., 2011; Alfonso et al., 2013), and this may impair the soothing effect of θ-tACS of the ACC. In contrast, a calm mood or mild fatigue induced by the individualized protocol may be attributed to simultaneous synchronization of the activity in the rostral ACC, which is associated with emotional processing (Bush et al., 2000).

For further exploration and verification of these observations, future protocols should include emotional tasks. For example, the emotional Stroop Test (Dresler et al., 2009), unlike the “standard” cognitive Stroop, is related to the rostral ACC activation, which was modulated simultaneously in both active protocols in the present study. It would also be beneficial to use specific scales to quantify emotional responses to neuromodulation.

To calculate individualized HD θ-tACS, we used high current density distribution to the mPFC-ACC along with low current density distribution in other cortical areas, including the parietal and temporal cortex. Electrode placement in the occipital area targeting the ACC via the mPFC also caused simultaneous neuromodulation of the visual cortex and may induce phosphenes, which could also be of a retinal origin (Kanai et al., 2010). Nevertheless, network analyses of inhibitory control-related network connections did not identify the occipital cortex as a region that is essential for inhibitory control (Spielberg et al., 2015). Moreover, the Stroop was measured before and after neuromodulation; therefore, visual performance during the cognitive tests could not be impaired.

In keeping with most previous studies testing the effect of θ-tACS on inhibitory control (van Driel et al., 2015; Lehr et al., 2019), our neuromodulation protocols used a total current of 1 mA. Fusco et al. (2018, 2020) administered a current of 1.5 and 2 mA, respectively. So far, the GTEN 100 used in our experiment has not allowed neuromodulation with a higher intensity of current. Previous findings have confirmed the dependence of tACS’s efficacy on current strength (Vosskuhl et al., 2016). It may be presumed that the upgrading of GTEN technology to higher current intensities will enhance the effect of neuromodulation.

Fusco et al. (2018) protocol used two circular sponge-conductive-rubber electrodes (Sponstim, 25 cm2, Neuroelectrics, Barcelona, Spain) placed over the MFC (in the FCz and Pz areas) for tACS. Our non-individualized protocol used MFC electrode layouts with 10 (five anode-cathodes; five cathode-anodes) circular electrodes (HCGSN 100, 1 cm2, EGI, Eugene, OR, United States), where each group of electrodes (in the FCz and Pz areas) covered 16–25 cm2, depending on the size of the subject’s head. The contact area of 5 cm2 was not changeable. The group of five electrodes was grouped in a square arrangement (four electrodes placed at the vertices of the square and the fifth central electrode in the FCz or Pz areas). Therefore, we cannot rule out the possibility that this methodological difference may lead to different results and outcomes.

The main shortcoming of the present study is the lack of neurophysiological evidence to confirm that the ACC was affected by tACS. Furthermore, individualized HD θ-tACS was applied via the mPFC, a cortical hub functionally connected with many associated brain areas, which might interfere with the effect. Therefore, we cannot rule out the possibility that the effect of individualized HD θ-tACS was influenced by concurrent modulation of the mPFC and its associated areas.

The study is limited by the lack of a control frequency condition. The testing could be enriched, in addition to the sham, by a control frequency condition to show that the detected behavioral changes are causally related to θ-tACS (6 Hz) and do not depend on other factors.

Future studies should also investigate the electrophysiological effects of θ-tACS (Bergmann et al., 2016; Cunillera et al., 2016; Neuling et al., 2017). In particular, it would be useful to monitor θ-tACS-induced changes in ERPs during performance tasks focused on cognitive functions. In the case of action monitoring, the suitable ERPs to study would be error-related negativity/error negativity (ERN; Gehring et al., 1995; Luu et al., 2000), or cognitive inhibition (N2; Yeung et al., 2004; Huster et al., 2013). Both, ERN and N2 are associated with the theta band (Luu et al., 2004; Cavanagh and Frank, 2014). In the latter, the nogo condition elicits a more negative response than the go condition (Lavric et al., 2004). Better performance in cognitive control tasks is associated with smaller N2 amplitudes (Lamm et al., 2006), while in ERN, the enhancement of negative amplitude is related to hyper-functioning error monitoring processes (Ruchsow et al., 2005).

Conclusion

In conclusion, we have demonstrated that, compared with non-individualized θ-tACS, individualized HD θ-tACS applied via the mPFC-ACC significantly improves the post-tACS verbal component of conflict-processing. The proposed algorithm of the individualized HD θ-tACS confirmed better specificity in neuromodulation targeting. Our findings support the role of the ACC as the candidate region for inhibitory control processes and show that HD θ-tACS is a safe and well-tolerated method. Further studies examining the neuromodulation-induced changes in inhibitory control processes are warranted to verify the effect of the procedure and to optimize the neuromodulation parameters, including electrode assembly and current intensity. Future researchers should also investigate whether the individualized HD θ-tACS of the mPFC-ACC can modulate inhibitory control processes impaired by neuropsychiatric disorders associated with ACC disruption.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Independent Ethics Committee of the National Institute of Mental Health, Klecany, Czechia. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study. Written informed consent was obtained from the individual(s) for the publication of any potentially identifiable images or data included in this article.

Author Contributions

MK: conceptualization, methodology, investigation, project administration, visualization, writing – original draft preparation, and supervision. VV: conceptualization, methodology, investigation, project administration, and writing - reviewing and editing. JH: supervision and writing - reviewing and editing. PM: conceptualization and writing - reviewing and editing. JJ: investigation and writing – original draft preparation. DD: conceptualization, methodology, investigation, and writing - reviewing and editing. LK, SB, and OL: investigation and writing - reviewing and editing. DF: validation, data curation, and writing - reviewing and editing. TN: conceptualization, formal analysis, writing – original draft preparation, and writing - reviewing and editing. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

The study was funded by the grants nos. 16-31380A, 17-30833A, NU20-04-00147, NV18-04-00260, and DRO (National Institute of Mental Health–NIMH, IN: 00023752) of the MH, CR, PharmaBrain No. CZ.02.1.01/0.0/0.0/16_0250007444, and grant no. 20-24782S by the Czech Science Foundation.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank Dr. Martin Brunovsky for providing the excellent technical facilities that enabled us to carry out the project.

Footnotes

  1. ^ http://www.randomization.com

References

Ahn, S., Mellin, J. M., Alagapan, S., Alexander, M. L., Gilmore, J. H., Jarskog, L. F., et al. (2019). Targeting reduced neural oscillations in patients with schizophrenia by transcranial alternating current stimulation. Neuroimage 186, 126–136. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.10.056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alam, M., Truong, D. Q., Khadka, N., and Bikson, M. (2016). Spatial and polarity precision of concentric high-definition transcranial direct current stimulation (HD-tDCS). Phys. Med. Biol. 61, 4506–4521. doi: 10.1088/0031-9155/61/12/4506

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alfonso, M. R., Miquel, T. F., Xavier, B., and Blanca, A. S. (2013). Resting parietal electroencephalogram asymmetries and self-reported attentional control. Clin. EEG Neurosci. 44, 188–192. doi: 10.1177/1550059412465871

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ali, M. M., Sellers, K. K., and Fröhlich, F. (2013). Transcranial alternating current stimulation modulates large-scale cortical network activity by network resonance. J. Neurosci. 33, 11262–11275. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.5867-12.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Antal, A., Boros, K., Poreisz, C., Chaieb, L., Terney, D., and Paulus, W. (2008). Comparatively weak after-effects of transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) on cortical excitability in humans. Brain Stimul. 1, 97–105. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2007.10.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Antal, A., and Herrmann, C. S. (2016). Transcranial alternating current and random noise stimulation: possible mechanisms. Neural Plast. 2016:3616807. doi: 10.1155/2016/3616807

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Antal, A., and Paulus, W. (2013). Transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS). Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:317. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00317

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berger, A., Pixa, N. H., Steinberg, F., and Doppelmayr, M. (2018). Brain oscillatory and hemodynamic activity in a bimanual coordination task following transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS): a combined EEG-fNIRS study. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 12:67. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00067

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bergmann, T. O., Karabanov, A., Hartwigsen, G., Thielscher, A., and Siebner, H. R. (2016). Combining non-invasive transcranial brain stimulation with neuroimaging and electrophysiology: current approaches and future perspectives. Neuroimage 140, 4–19. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.02.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I., Klein, R., and Viswanathan, M. (2004). Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: evidence from the Simon task. Psychol. Aging 19:290. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.19.2.290

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bush, G., Luu, P., and Posner, M. I. (2000). Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex. Trends Cogn. Sci. 4, 215–222. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01483-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carter, C. S., MacDonald Iii, A. W., Ross, L. L., and Stenger, V. A. (2001). Anterior cingulate cortex activity and impaired self-monitoring of performance in patients with schizophrenia: an event-related fMRI study. Am. J. Psychiatry 158, 1423–1428. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.9.1423

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cavanagh, J. F., and Frank, M. J. (2014). Frontal theta as a mechanism for cognitive control. Trends Cogn. Sci. 18, 414–421. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2014.04.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cavanagh, J. F., and Shackman, A. J. (2015). Frontal midline theta reflects anxiety and cognitive control: meta-analytic evidence. J. Physiol. Paris 109, 3–15. doi: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2014.04.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chander, B. S., Witkowski, M., Braun, C., Robinson, S. E., Born, J., Cohen, L. G., et al. (2016). tACS phase locking of frontal midline theta oscillations disrupts working memory performance. Front. Cell Neurosci. 10:120. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2016.00120

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cipolotti, L., Spanò, B., Healy, C., Tudor-Sfetea, C., Chan, E., White, M., et al. (2016). Inhibition processes are dissociable and lateralized in human prefrontal cortex. Neuropsychologia 93, 1–12. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.09.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, M. X., and Donner, T. H. (2013). Midfrontal conflict-related theta-band power reflects neural oscillations that predict behavior. J. Neurophysiol. 110, 2752–2763. doi: 10.1152/jn.00479.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, M. X., Ridderinkhof, K. R., Haupt, S., Elger, C. E., and Fell, J. (2008). Medial frontal cortex and response conflict: evidence from human intracranial EEG and medial frontal cortex lesion. Brain Res. 1238, 127–142. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.07.114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Conners, C. (2014). Conners Continuous Performance Test, 3rd Edn. North Tonawanda, NY: Multi-Health Systems Inc.

Google Scholar

Cunillera, T., Brignani, D., Cucurell, D., Fuentemilla, L., and Miniussi, C. (2016). The right inferior frontal cortex in response inhibition: a tDCS–ERP co-registration study. NeuroImage 140, 66–75. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.11.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dedoncker, J., Brunoni, A. R., Baeken, C., and Vanderhasselt, M. A. (2016). A systematic review and meta-analysis of the effects of transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) over the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in healthy and neuropsychiatric samples: influence of stimulation parameters. Brain Stimul. 9, 501–517. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2016.04.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Del Felice, A., Castiglia, L., Formaggio, E., Cattelan, M., Scarpa, B., Manganotti, P., et al. (2019). Personalized transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) and physical therapy to treat motor and cognitive symptoms in Parkinson’s disease: a randomized cross-over trial. NeuroImage Clin. 22:101768. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2019.101768

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dmochowski, J. P., Datta, A., Bikson, M., Su, Y., and Parra, L. C. (2011). Optimized multi-electrode stimulation increases focality and intensity at target. J. Neural Eng. 8:046011. doi: 10.1088/1741-2560/8/4/046011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dresler, T., Mériau, K., Heekeren, H. R., and van der Meer, E. (2009). Emotional Stroop task: effect of word arousal and subject anxiety on emotional interference. Psychol. Res. 73, 364–371. doi: 10.1007/s00426-008-0154-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Egner, T. (2008). Multiple conflict-driven control mechanisms in the human brain. Trends Cogn. Sci. 12, 374–380. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Egner, T., Delano, M., and Hirsch, J. (2007). Separate conflict-specific cognitive control mechanisms in the human brain. NeuroImage 35, 940–948. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.11.061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Egner, T., and Hirsch, J. (2005). Cognitive control mechanisms resolve conflict through cortical amplification of task-relevant information. Nat. Neurosci. 8:1784. doi: 10.1038/nn1594

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernández-Corazza, M., Turovets, S., Luu, P., Anderson, E., and Tucker, D. (2016). Transcranial electrical neuromodulation based on the reciprocity principle. Front. Psychiatry 7:87. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2016.00087

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fitzgerald, K. D., Welsh, R. C., Gehring, W. J., Abelson, J. L., Himle, J. A., Liberzon, I., et al. (2005). Error-related hyperactivity of the anterior cingulate cortex in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 57, 287–294. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.10.038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fusco, G., Fusaro, M., and Aglioti, S. (2020). Midfrontal-occipital θ-tACS modulates cognitive conflicts related to bodily stimuli. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. nsaa125. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsaa125

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fusco, G., Scandola, M., Feurra, M., Pavone, E. F., Rossi, S., and Aglioti, S. M. (2018). Midfrontal theta transcranial alternating current stimulation modulates behavioural adjustment after error execution. Eur. J. Neurosci. 48, 3159–3170. doi: 10.1111/ejn.14174

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gasquoine, P. G. (2013). Localization of function in anterior cingulate cortex: from psychosurgery to functional neuroimaging. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 37, 340–348. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.01.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gehring, W. J., Coles, M. G., Meyer, D. E., and Donchin, E. (1995). A brain potential manifestation of error-related processing. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. Suppl. 44, 261–272.

Google Scholar

Gratton, G., Coles, M. G. H., Sirevaag, E. J., Eriksen, C. W., and Donchin, E. (1988). Pre- and poststimulus activation of response channels: a psychophysiological analysis. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 14, 331–344. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.14.3.331

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herrmann, C., Rach, S., Neuling, T., and Strüber, D. (2013). Transcranial alternating current stimulation: a review of the underlying mechanisms and modulation of cognitive processes. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:279. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00279

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hill, A. T., Fitzgerald, P. B., and Hoy, K. E. (2016). Effects of anodal transcranial direct current stimulation on working memory: a systematic review and meta-analysis of findings from healthy and neuropsychiatric populations. Brain Stimul. 9, 197–208. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2015.10.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoy, K. E., Bailey, N., Arnold, S., Windsor, K., John, J., Daskalakis, Z. J., et al. (2015). The effect of γ-tACS on working memory performance in healthy controls. Brain Cogn. 101, 51–56. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2015.11.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huster, R. J., Enriquez-Geppert, S., Lavallee, C. F., Falkenstein, M., and Herrmann, C. S. (2013). Electroencephalography of response inhibition tasks: functional networks and cognitive contributions. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 87, 217–233. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2012.08.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Inquisit, M. (2007). 3.0 [Computer Software]. Seattle, WA: Millisecond Software.

Google Scholar

Kanai, R., Paulus, W., and Walsh, V. (2010). Transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) modulates cortical excitability as assessed by TMS-induced phosphene thresholds. Clin. Neurophysiol. 121, 1551–1554. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2010.03.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaneda, M., and Osaka, N. (2008). Role of anterior cingulate cortex during semantic coding in verbal working memory. Neurosci. Lett. 436, 57–61. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2008.02.069

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kopp, B., Mattler, U., Goertz, R., and Rist, F. (1996a). N2, P3 and the lateralized readiness potential in a nogo task involving selective response priming. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 99, 19–27. doi: 10.1016/0921-884X(96)95617-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kopp, B., Rist, F., and Mattler, U. (1996b). N200 in the flanker task as a neurobehavioral tool for investigating executive control. Psychophysiology 33, 282–294. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1996.tb00425.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lamm, C., Zelazo, P. D., and Lewis, M. D. (2006). Neural correlates of cognitive control in childhood and adolescence: disentangling the contributions of age and executive function. Neuropsychologia 44, 2139–2148. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.10.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lang, S., Gan, L., Alrazi, T., and Monchi, O. (2019). High definition transcranial alternating current stimulation of the right fusiform cortex improves visual associative memory. Brain Stimul. 12:429. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2018.12.389

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lavric, A., Pizzagalli, D. A., and Forstmeier, S. (2004). When ‘go’and ‘nogo’are equally frequent: ERP components and cortical tomography. Eur. J. Neurosci. 20, 2483–2488. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03683.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lefaucheur, J. P., Antal, A., Ayache, S. S., Benninger, D. H., Brunelin, J., Cogiamanian, F., et al. (2017). Evidence-based guidelines on the therapeutic use of transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS). Clin. Neurophysiol. 128, 56–92. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2016.10.087

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lehr, A., Henneberg, N., Nigam, T., Paulus, W., and Antal, A. (2019). Modulation of conflict processing by theta-range tACS over the Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Neural Plast. 2019:6747049. doi: 10.1155/2019/6747049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lezak, M. D., Howieson, D. B., Loring, D. W., and Fischer, J. S. (2004). Neuropsychological Assessment. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Luu, P., Essaki Arumugam, E. M., Anderson, E., Gunn, A., Rech, D., Turovets, S., et al. (2016). Slow-Frequency pulsed transcranial electrical stimulation for modulation of cortical plasticity based on reciprocity targeting with precision electrical head modeling. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:377. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00377

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luu, P., Flaisch, T., and Tucker, D. M. (2000). Medial frontal cortex in action monitoring. J. Neurosci. 20, 464–469. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-01-00464.2000

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luu, P., Tucker, D. M., and Makeig, S. (2004). Frontal midline theta and the error-related negativity: neurophysiological mechanisms of action regulation. Clin. Neurophysiol. 115, 1821–1835. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2004.03.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moliadze, V., Sierau, L., Lyzhko, E., Stenner, T., Werchowski, M., Siniatchkin, M., et al. (2019). After-effects of 10 Hz tACS over the prefrontal cortex on phonological word decisions. Brain Stimul. 12, 1464–1474. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2019.06.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neuling, T., Ruhnau, P., Weisz, N., Herrmann, C. S., and Demarchi, G. (2017). Faith and oscillations recovered: on analyzing EEG/MEG signals during tACS. NeuroImage 147, 960–963. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.11.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nigbur, R., Cohen, M. X., Ridderinkhof, K. R., and Stürmer, B. (2012). Theta dynamics reveal domain-specific control over stimulus and response conflict. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 24, 1264–1274. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00128

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nikolin, S., Loo, C. K., Bai, S., Dokos, S., and Martin, D. M. (2015). Focalised stimulation using high definition transcranial direct current stimulation (HD-tDCS) to investigate declarative verbal learning and memory functioning. NeuroImage 117, 11–19. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.05.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Onoda, K., Kawagoe, T., Zheng, H., and Yamaguchi, S. (2017). Theta band transcranial alternating current stimulations modulates network behavior of dorsal anterior cingulate cortex. Sci. Rep. 7:3607. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-03859-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pahor, A., and Jaušovec, N. (2018). The effects of theta and gamma tACS on working memory and electrophysiology. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:651. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00651

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paulus, W. (2011). Transcranial electrical stimulation (tES – tDCS; tRNS, tACS) methods. Neuropsychol. Rehabil. 21, 602–617. doi: 10.1080/09602011.2011.557292

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peterson, B. S., Kane, M. J., Alexander, G. M., Lacadie, C., Skudlarski, P., Leung, H. C., et al. (2002). An event-related functional MRI study comparing interference effects in the Simon and Stroop tasks. Cogn. Brain Res. 13, 427–440. doi: 10.1016/S0926-6410(02)00054-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Poreisz, C., Boros, K., Antal, A., and Paulus, W. (2007). Safety aspects of transcranial direct current stimulation concerning healthy subjects and patients. Brain Res. Bull. 72, 208–214. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2007.01.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pratte, M. S., Rouder, J. N., Morey, R. D., and Feng, C. (2010). Exploring the differences in distributional properties between Stroop and Simon effects using delta plots. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 72, 2013–2025. doi: 10.3758/APP.72.7.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reverberi, C., Kuhlen, A., Abutalebi, J., Greulich, R. S., Costa, A., Seyed-Allaei, S., et al. (2015). Language control in bilinguals: intention to speak vs. execution of speech. Brain Lang. 144, 1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2015.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruchsow, M., Grön, G., Reuter, K., Spitzer, M., Hermle, L., and Kiefer, M. (2005). Error-related brain activity in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder and in healthy controls. J. Psychophysiol. 19, 298–304. doi: 10.1027/0269-8803.19.4.298

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sela, T., Kilim, A., and Lavidor, M. (2012). Transcranial alternating current stimulation increases risk-taking behavior in the balloon analog risk task. Front. Neurosci. 6:22. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2012.00022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soutschek, A., Müller, H. J., and Schubert, T. (2013). Conflict-specific effects of accessory stimuli on cognitive control in the Stroop task and the Simon task. Exp. Psychol. 60, 140–148. doi: 10.1027/1618-3169/a000181

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spielberg, J. M., Miller, G. A., Heller, W., and Banich, M. T. (2015). Flexible brain network reconfiguration supporting inhibitory control. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 112, 10020–10025. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1500048112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

StataCorp (2017). Stata Statistical Software: Release 15. College Station, TX: StataCorp LLC.

Google Scholar

Taylor, S. F., Stern, E. R., and Gehring, W. J. (2007). Neural systems for error monitoring: recent findings and theoretical perspectives. Neuroscientist 13, 160–172. doi: 10.1177/1073858406298184

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ursu, S., Stenger, V. A., Shear, M. K., Jones, M. R., and Carter, C. S. (2003). Overactive action monitoring in obsessive-compulsive disorder: evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging. Psychol. Sci. 14, 347–353. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.24411

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Driel, J., Sligte, I. G., Linders, J., Elport, D., and Cohen, M. X. (2015). Frequency band-specific electrical brain stimulation modulates cognitive control processes. PLoS One 10:e0138984. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0138984

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Gaal, S., Ridderinkhof, K. R., van den Wildenberg, W. P., and Lamme, V. A. (2009). Dissociating consciousness from inhibitory control: evidence for unconsciously triggered response inhibition in the stop-signal task. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 35:1129. doi: 10.1037/a0013551

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Noordt, S. J. R., Campopiano, A., and Segalowitz, S. J. (2016). A functional classification of medial frontal negativity ERPs: theta oscillations and single subject effects. Psychophysiology 53, 1317–1334. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12689

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Veen, V., and Carter, C. S. (2002). The anterior cingulate as a conflict monitor: fMRI and ERP studies. Physiol. Behav. 77, 477–482. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(02)00930-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Veniero, D., Brignani, D., Thut, G., and Miniussi, C. (2011). Alpha-generation as basic response-signature to transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) targeting the human resting motor cortex: a TMS/EEG co-registration study. Psychophysiology 48, 1381–1389. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2011.01218.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vossen, A., Gross, J., and Thut, G. (2015). Alpha power increase after transcranial alternating current stimulation at alpha frequency (α-tACS) reflects plastic changes rather than entrainment. Brain Stimul. 8, 499–508. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2014.12.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vosskuhl, J., Huster, R. J., and Herrmann, C. S. (2015). Increase in short-term memory capacity induced by down-regulating individual theta frequency via transcranial alternating current stimulation. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9:257. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00257

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vosskuhl, J., Huster, R. J., and Herrmann, C. S. (2016). BOLD signal effects of transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) in the alpha range: a concurrent tACS–fMRI study. NeuroImage 140, 118–125. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, E. J. (1949). Experimental designs balanced for the estimation of residual effects of treatments. Aust. J. Chem. 2, 149–168. doi: 10.1071/ch9490149

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wischnewski, M., Zerr, P., and Schutter, D. J. (2016). Effects of theta transcranial alternating current stimulation over the frontal cortex on reversal learning. Brain Stimul. 9, 705–711. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2016.04.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Witkowski, M., Garcia-Cossio, E., Chander, B. S., Braun, C., Birbaumer, N., Robinson, S. E., et al. (2016). Mapping entrained brain oscillations during transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS). NeuroImage 140, 89–98. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.10.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yeung, N., Botvinick, M. M., and Cohen, J. D. (2004). The neural basis of error detection: conflict monitoring and the error-related negativity. Psychol. Rev. 111:931. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.111.4.931

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zaehle, T., Rach, S., and Herrmann, C. S. (2010). Transcranial alternating current stimulation enhances individual alpha activity in human EEG. PLoS One 5:e13766. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0013766

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zaghi, S., de Freitas Rezende, L., de Oliveira, L. M., El-Nazer, R., Menning, S., Tadini, L., et al. (2010). Inhibition of motor cortex excitability with 15 Hz transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS). Neurosci. Lett. 479, 211–214. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2010.05.060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: transcranial alternating current stimulation, tACS, ACC, theta frequency, inhibitory control, high definition

Citation: Klírová M, Voráčková V, Horáček J, Mohr P, Jonáš J, Dudysová DU, Kostýlková L, Fayette D, Krejčová L, Baumann S, Laskov O and Novák T (2021) Modulating Inhibitory Control Processes Using Individualized High Definition Theta Transcranial Alternating Current Stimulation (HD θ-tACS) of the Anterior Cingulate and Medial Prefrontal Cortex. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 15:611507. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2021.611507

Received: 29 September 2020; Accepted: 04 March 2021;
Published: 30 March 2021.

Edited by:

Jorge Alvaro Gonzalez-Martinez, University of Pittsburgh, United States

Reviewed by:

Connie Lamm, University of Arkansas, United States
Gabriele Fusco, Sapienza University of Rome, Italy

Copyright © 2021 Klírová, Voráčková, Horáček, Mohr, Jonáš, Dudysová, Kostýlková, Fayette, Krejčová, Baumann, Laskov and Novák. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Monika Klírová, monika.klirova@nudz.cz

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.