Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Syst. Neurosci., 28 July 2020
This article is part of the Research Topic Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience – Editors’ Pick 2021 View all 10 articles

Neural Working Memory Changes During a Spaceflight Analog With Elevated Carbon Dioxide: A Pilot Study

\r\nAna Paula SalazarAna Paula Salazar1Kathleen E. HupfeldKathleen E. Hupfeld1Jessica K. LeeJessica K. Lee2Nichole E. BeltranNichole E. Beltran3Igor S. KofmanIgor S. Kofman3Yiri E. De DiosYiri E. De Dios3Edwin MulderEdwin Mulder2Jacob J. BloombergJacob J. Bloomberg4Ajitkumar P. MulavaraAjitkumar P. Mulavara3Rachael D. Seidler,*Rachael D. Seidler1,5*
  • 1Department of Applied Physiology and Kinesiology, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, United States
  • 2Institute of Aerospace Medicine, German Aerospace Center, Cologne, Germany
  • 3KBR, Houston, TX, United States
  • 4NASA Johnson Space Center, Houston, TX, United States
  • 5Department of Neurology, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, United States

Spaceflight missions to the International Space Station (ISS) expose astronauts to microgravity, radiation, isolation, and elevated carbon dioxide (CO2), among other factors. Head down tilt bed rest (HDBR) is an Earth-based analog for spaceflight used to study body unloading, fluid shifts, and other factors unrelated to gravitational changes. While in space, astronauts need to use mental rotation strategies to facilitate their adaptation to the ISS environment. Therefore, spatial working memory is essential for crewmember performance. Although the effects of HDBR on spatial working memory have recently been studied, the results are still inconclusive. Here, we expand upon past work and examine the effects of HDBR with elevated CO2 (HDBR + CO2) on brain activation patterns during spatial working memory performance. In addition, we compare brain activation between 30 days of HDBR + CO2 and 70 days of HDBR to test the isolated effect of CO2. Eleven subjects (6 males, 5 females; mean age = 34 ± 8 years) underwent six functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) sessions pre-, during, and post-HDBR + CO2. During the HDBR + CO2 intervention, we observed decreasing activation in the right middle frontal gyrus and left regions of the cerebellum, followed by post-intervention recovery. We detected several correlations between brain and behavioral slopes of change with the HDBR + CO2 intervention. For example, greater increases in activation in frontal, temporal and parietal regions were associated with larger spatial working memory improvements. Comparing the HDBR + CO2 group to data from our previous 70-day HDBR study, we found greater decreases in activation in the right hippocampus and left inferior temporal gyrus for the HDBR + CO2 group over the course of the intervention. Together, these findings increase our understanding of the neural mechanisms of HDBR, elevated levels of CO2 and spaceflight-related changes in spatial working memory performance.

GRAPHICAL ABSTRACT
www.frontiersin.org

Graphical Abstract. Top: previous findings regarding spatial working memory performance after bed rest protocols. Citations are presented in the Reference section. Bottom: Summary of findings from the present study showing (A) no differences in spatial working memory behavior performance; (B) decreasing activation in some brain regions suggesting that HDBR + CO2 may reduce subjects’ ability to recruit appropriate working memory networks; (C) positive and negative brain-behavior correlations suggesting adaptive or compensatory brain response to HDBR + CO2.

Introduction

Spaceflight negatively affects human sensorimotor functioning and cognition (De la Torre, 2014). Cognitive performance in astronauts may be impaired by microgravity, radiation, noise, fatigue, and sleep deprivation, among other factors (De la Torre, 2014). Spatial orientation, mental rotation, and recognition are among the most common cognitive processes affected by spaceflight (De la Torre, 2014).

Mental rotation is a type of spatial working memory task in which a person imagines how an object would appear if it was rotated away from the presented orientation (Shepard and Metzler, 1971). Working memory is part of the short-term memory system, which involves a series of interactive processes that comprise the ability to temporarily maintain and manipulate information in the mind (Baddeley, 2017). Spatial working memory has an important role for executive function as well as sequence learning and sensorimotor adaptation (Seidler et al., 2012). Therefore, working memory is essential for successful crewmember performance. For instance, while in space, astronauts use mental rotation strategies to facilitate the recognition of objects and other astronauts’ gestures.

Head down tilt bed rest (HDBR) is a well-established Earth-based analog of spaceflight used to investigate the physiological effects of microgravity on human performance (Moore et al., 2010). HDBR simulates the axial body unloading and fluid shifts toward the head that occur during spaceflight. Both spaceflight and HDBR impact sensorimotor function and are associated with modifications of brain structure and function in healthy individuals (Bock et al., 2010; Koppelmans et al., 2016; Roberts et al., 2017; Lee et al., 2019b). The effects of HDBR specifically on working memory remain unclear. Previous work assessed 20 males that underwent seven days of −6° HDBR. These individuals showed reduced mental rotation ability after three days of HDBR, but recovered after the end of HDBR, suggesting that short-duration HDBR temporarily impacts mental rotation abilities (Wang et al., 2017). Our group previously evaluated 17 males who underwent a 70-day HDBR intervention. We reported improvements in spatial working memory performance after 70 days of HDBR compared to baseline, suggestive of test practice effects (Cassady et al., 2016). Further, we found that working memory performance changes correlated with brain connectivity alterations (Cassady et al., 2016). This suggests that neuroplastic mechanisms may facilitate adaptation to the HDBR environment (Cassady et al., 2016).

In addition to microgravity, chronic exposure to elevated levels of carbon dioxide (CO2) on the International Space Station (ISS) may also contribute to cognitive performance impairments (Manzey and Lorenz, 1998; Allen et al., 2019). Astronauts aboard the ISS often report hypercapnia-related symptoms such as headaches (Law et al., 2014), spatial disorientation, reduced attention and concentration, among other symptoms (Kanas and Manzey, 2008; De la Torre, 2014). Our group recently reported the effects of 30 days HDBR coupled with elevated CO2 on cognitive and sensorimotor performance (Lee et al., 2019a). Individuals in this cohort showed improvements in card rotation performance (i.e., a learning effect and no effect of the intervention) and no changes in cube rotation and working memory (Lee et al., 2019a). Although several recent studies have reported HDBR- and spaceflight-related changes in spatial working memory abilities (Leone et al., 1995; Lipnicki et al., 2009; Chen et al., 2013; Wang et al., 2017), there is still little understanding regarding how HDBR may affect the neural processing of spatial working memory. Further, no previous work has investigated neural spatial working memory changes with a combined HDBR and elevated CO2, which better mimics the elevated CO2 onboard the ISS (Law et al., 2014).

In the present pilot study, we examine the effects of 30 days of HDBR combined with elevated CO2 levels (HDBR + CO2) on the neural correlates of spatial working memory performance in eleven participants. We addressed two primary aims: (1) to investigate the time course of effects of a 30-day HDBR + CO2 intervention on brain activation patterns during spatial working memory task performance; and (2) to determine whether any brain changes correlate with changes in spatial working memory performance. As a secondary aim, to investigate the additive effects of elevated CO2 and long-duration HDBR, we compared the data here with those from our previous HDBR work (Yuan et al., 2016, 2018a; Koppelmans et al., 2017). This comparison was exploratory, given that the two HDBR interventions differed on several dimensions including the exposure duration.

Materials and Methods

Participants and Testing Timeline

HDBR + CO2

This longitudinal study conducted at:envihab in the German Aerospace Center, Cologne, Germany, included eleven participants (6 males, 5 females) with mean age of 34 ± 8 years at the beginning of the study. Participants were tested in six different time points: twice before the protocol started, twice during intervention and twice after the end of the bed rest (Figure 1). During the HDBR + CO2 intervention, subjects maintained 6° head down tilt position while exposed to ambient 0.5% CO2 (3.8 mmHg partial pressure of CO2) (Law et al., 2014) at all times during 30 days. Oxygen and nitrogen levels were 20.9% and 78.6%, respectively. These small changes had no physiological effects neither affected the oxygen saturation. All participants received a controlled diet, had daily 8-h sleep opportunities (10:30 PM–6:30 AM) and were not allowed to use a pillow except when laying on their side.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Testing timelines. Top: testing timeline for the HDBR + CO2 group, who completed 30 days of head down tilt bed rest (HDBR) with 0.5% atmospheric CO2. Bottom: testing timeline for the HDBR group, who completed 70 days of HDBR with normal atmospheric CO2 levels. BDC = baseline data collection; HDBR = head down tilt bed rest; R = recovery. Functional MRI and behavioral data were collected at all time points specified here. Stars indicate the three time points used to create the slope images for between-group comparisons.

Three days prior to bed rest and on the first day after bed rest blood draws were acquired to measure arterial partial pressure of carbon dioxide (PaCO2). This was part of NASA’s standard measures assessments.

All procedures were approved by the University of Florida and NASA Institutional Review Boards as well as by the local ethical commission of the regional medical association (Ärztekammer Nordrhein). All subjects provided written informed consent and received monetary compensation for their participation.

70-day HDBR

Sixteen individuals (all males; mean age = 29 ± 3 years) consented to participate in this study. All procedures were approved by the University of Michigan, University of Texas Medical Branch, and NASA Institutional Review Boards. All participants were admitted to the NASA bed rest facility at the University of Texas Medical Branch, Galveston, TX, United States and completed two baseline data collection sessions in the 2 weeks prior to starting HDBR. Subjects then underwent 70 days of HDBR intervention with normal atmospheric CO2 (∼0.04%; 0.3 mmHg partial pressure of CO2). During this campaign, participants remained lying down with a six-degree head down tilt at all times. They were allowed to use a pillow and to support their head with their hand during each meal (30 min). Subjects stayed at the facility for 14 days after HDBR and completed two recovery data collection sessions during this time (Figure 1).

Spatial Working Memory Behavioral Tasks

Spatial working memory behavioral tasks were acquired at all time points specified in Figure 1. Three different tasks were used to assess spatial working memory performance, as follows:

(1) Spatial working memory task during functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) (Figure 2A): This task was performed in the MRI scanner. Participants viewed a three-target set (three solid circles) for 500 ms. Following the presentation of this target set, participants saw a blank screen for 3000 ms (retention interval). During the retention interval, participants were instructed to mentally “connect the dots” and then mentally rotate the shape. After the retention interval, participants decided whether a subsequently presented probe set of open circles formed the same configuration as the target set they mentally rotated. Participants performed two runs of this task. Each run included 30 trials.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Spatial working memory performance tasks. (A) Top: Spatial working memory task performed in the MRI scanner. Bottom: Spatial working memory control task performed in the MRI scanner. (B) Thurstone’s 2D card rotation test. (C) Cube rotation task.

Participants also performed a control task in the MRI scanner (Figure 2A). The control task involved the presentation of three solid circles for 500 ms, followed by a 200 ms retention interval, then by the presentation of a single circle for 2500 ms. At this point, participants determined whether its spatial location matched that of a previously observed dot. Participants performed one run of this task consisting of 40 trials. This control task included all of the processes of the spatial working memory task, except for the working memory and mental rotation components. Thus, the subtraction of images from the control condition should reveal areas actively involved with spatial working memory maintenance and mental rotation while omitting those involved in visual processing and response button pressing (Reuter-Lorenz et al., 2000; Anguera et al., 2010). For both tasks, we calculated the percentage of correct responses (spatial working memory and spatial working memory control accuracy).

(2) Card rotation (Figure 2B): Participants completed Thurstone’s 2D card rotation test (Ekstrom et al., 1976). During each trial, they were presented with a 2D drawing of a card with an abstract shape. To the right of this card, there were eight drawings of the same card that were either only rotated or both rotated and mirrored. Participants determined which cards matched the initial drawing (S = same, i.e., only 2D rotated) and which cards were different (D = different, i.e., mirrored or flipped from the card at the beginning of the row). Time to complete the test (maximum time allowed is 3 min) and accuracy relative to completed trials were used as indicators of performance (Koppelmans et al., 2013; Cassady et al., 2016).

(3) Cube rotation (Figure 2C): Participants compared a collection of 3D cubes (Shepard and Metzler, 1988). During each trial, a 3D cube assemblage was presented on a computer screen for 3 s, followed by a blank screen for 2 s, and then two cube images. One of the two was a match to the target but was rotated three dimensionally; the other was a new cube assemblage. Participants indicated which cube image matched the target image by pressing a left or right button. Outcome measures for this task included reaction time and accuracy.

For both card and cube rotation assessments, the HDBR + CO2 participants were in head down tilt while subjects from 70-day HDBR performed this task in the supine position (Koppelmans et al., 2013; Cassady et al., 2016).

fMRI Acquisition Parameters

HDBR + CO2

Functional images were acquired on a 3 Tesla Siemens MRI scanner, using a gradient echo T2-weighted echo-planar imaging sequence with the following parameters: TR = 2500 ms, TE = 32 ms, flip angle = 90°, FOV = 192 × 192 mm, matrix = 64 × 64, slice thickness = 3.5 mm, voxel size = 3 × 3 × 3.5 mm3, 37 slices. A T1-weighted gradient-echo pulse sequence was also acquired: TR = 1.9 s, TE = 2.4 ms, flip angle = 9°, FOV = 250 × 250 mm, matrix = 512 × 512, slice thickness = 1.0 mm, voxel size = 0.49 × 0.49 × 1.0 mm3, 192 slices. Participants maintained the head down tilt position in the scanner by lying on a wedge of foam; however, the head was supine in the head coil.

70-day HDBR

For the 70-day HDBR group, fMRI scans were acquired on a 3 Tesla Siemens MRI scanner using a gradient echo T2-weighted echo-planar imaging sequence: Repetition time (TR) = 3.66 s, Echo time (TE) = 39 ms, flip angle = 90°, Field of view (FOV) = 240 × 240 mm, matrix = 94 × 94, slice thickness = 4 mm, slice gap = 1 mm, voxel size = 2.55 × 2.55 × 5.0 mm3, 36 slices. A T1-weighted gradient-echo pulse sequence was also collected with parameters: TR = 1.9 s, TE = 2.49 ms, flip angle = 9°, FOV = 270 × 270 mm, matrix = 288 × 288, slice thickness = 0.90 mm, voxel size = 0.94 × 0.94 × 0.90 mm3, 192 slices. Participants did not maintain the head down tilt position in the scanner.

fMRI Data Processing and Statistical Analyses

We used Statistical Parametric Mapping 12 (SPM12, version 7219) and MATLAB R2018a, version 9.0 for preprocessing and statistical analyses. We used a standard SPM preprocessing pipeline for fMRI. All functional images were slice timing and head motion corrected (realigned and resliced). Following these steps, the Artifact Detection Tool (ART)1 was used as an additional quality check. We removed volumes with motion threshold equal or greater than 3 mm (i.e., approximately the size of one voxel for the HDBR + CO2 group) and global brain signal Z threshold equal or greater than 9. Two individuals had movement outliers; for one of them the first 21 of 76 volumes were excluded, while the first 8 of 76 volumes were excluded for the other participant. We included head motion parameters outputted by ART as covariates in the subject-level analyses to minimize effects of these volumes on group-level analyses.

Next, whole brain fMRI images were normalized to MNI152 space using Advanced Normalization Tools (Avants et al., 2011), in a multi-step procedure. First, the T1 images were skull stripped using ImCalc (SPM12). Then, participant-specific templates were created using ANTs’ AntsMultivariateTemplateConstuction.sh function. Next, these templates were normalized to MNI152 common space using ANTs’ AntsRegistration.sh function. In order to normalize the images, we then created mean fMRI participant-specific templates (using ANTs’ AntsMultivariateTemplateConstuction.sh function) and used these templates to coregister the functional images to the T1-specific templates. Coregistration was performed using AntsRegistration.sh. The resulting warp parameters were applied to the 4D EPI images using ANTs’ AntsApplyTransforms.sh function. Finally, the normalized data were spatially smoothed with an 8 mm full-width half-maximum three-dimensional Gaussian kernel.

In addition to the whole brain normalization, we applied specialized processing using portions of both the CEREbellum Segmentation (CERES) (Romero et al., 2017) pipeline and the Spatially Unbiased Infratentorial Template (SUIT) (Diedrichsen, 2006; Diedrichsen et al., 2009) pipeline. The CERES pipeline was used to segment the cerebellum from each person’s structural T1-weighted image. We then coregistered each subject’s native space segmentation to the SUIT.nii template. Binary gray matter, white matter, and full cerebellar masks were created from the CERES native space output, and we then used the suit_normalize_dartel function to obtain the affine transformation matrix and normalize these images into SUIT space. Due to the small size of cerebellar structures, we applied a 2 mm full-width half-maximum three-dimensional Gaussian smoothing kernel to the normalized functional cerebellar images.

We calculated subject-level statistical analyses twice: once for the whole brain and a second time for the cerebellum. Brain activity was calculated for each participant on a voxel-by-voxel basis for the contrast spatial working memory > spatial working memory control. We set the first level masking threshold to -Infinity and masked out non-brain areas using the SPM intracranial volume mask.

fMRI Group-Level Statistical Analyses

Main Effect of Spatial Working Memory

To verify that our spatial working memory task elicited the expected brain activity, we calculated the main effect across all subjects and all sessions at peak-level of p < 0.001 (uncorrected), extent threshold = 10 voxels. In this model, we controlled for age and sex differences, i.e., these variables were included as covariates of no interest. For all analyses we used the contrast spatial working memory > control.

Time Course of Neural Spatial Working Memory Response to HDBR + CO2

We first tested for brain regions that showed a pattern of cumulative change followed by post-HDBR + CO2 recovery. These hypothesized cumulative change models are presented in Figure 3. For these longitudinal analyses, we used flexible factorial models controlling for age and sex assuming independence between subjects, and assuming equal variances between and within subjects (Gläscher and Gitelman, 2008). To better detect within-subject changes with the longitudinal model used in the present pilot study, the alpha level was set at p < 0.0005 (uncorrected), and the extent threshold was set at 10 voxels for the whole brain and 5 voxels for the cerebellum.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Hypothesized contrast weights. Cumulative changes in neural spatial working memory response to HDBR + CO2. Brain changes would slowly increase over the course of HDBR + CO2, followed by recovery after the end of HDBR + CO2. Solid lines represent the positive version while dotted lines describe the negative version of the contrast.

Brain-Behavioral Correlations

First, we computed the slope of changes in brain activation (Yuan et al., 2016, 2018b; Hupfeld et al., 2020) between the 2nd, 3rd, and 4th time points (Figure 1). These are the time points immediately before HDBR started and during HDBR, respectively. Additionally, we computed the slope of changes in behavioral performance on the spatial working memory, card rotation, and cube rotation tasks across the same time points. We then correlated the slope of brain changes with the slope of changes in spatial working memory performance. For these analyses, we used the Statistical Non-Parametric Mapping (SnPM version 13)2 toolbox to run non-parametric permutation tests with 15,000 permutations, variance smoothing = 8 mm kernel for whole brain analyses and 2 mm kernel for cerebellar analyses, and controlling for age and sex. For these analyses, we used a non-parametric threshold of p < 0.0005 (uncorrected) and a minimum cluster size of 10 voxels for the whole brain and 5 voxels for the cerebellum.

HDBR + CO2 vs. 70-day HDBR Group Comparisons

Given that each of the two bed rest studies followed a different testing timeline, to examine differences in neural response between HDBR with and without elevated CO2, we compared only the slopes of change in brain activation between these two studies. We computed slopes of brain change for the 70-day HDBR group in an identical manner to those for the HDBR + CO2 group. Additionally, for each group, we computed intercept images (i.e., baseline brain activation during spatial working memory). We then normalized the slope images using the formula: (slope image/intercept image); this allows us to compare between-group slope changes while accounting for baseline differences between the two groups.

We performed a two-sample t-test to test between-group differences in the normalized slope images. We used SnPM non-parametric permutation tests with 15,000 permutations, variance smoothing = 8 mm kernel for the whole brain analyses and 2 mm kernel for the cerebellar analyses, and controlling for age and sex. Statistical significance was determined by applying false discovery rate (FDR) p < 0.05 at the cluster-level (Nichols and Hayasaka, 2003).

Statistical Analyses

A paired sample one tailed t-test was performed to verify any increases pre- to post-HDBR + CO2 in the PaCO2 blood levels. Statistical significance was defined as p < 0.05.

We previously reported some statistical analyses of the spatial working memory behavioral data for the HDBR + CO2 cohort (Lee et al., 2019a). Here, we further investigated the spatial working memory score using the following equation: Spatial working memory Score = Spatial working memory control accuracy – Spatial working memory accuracy. We did not have any outliers nor missing data. We conducted a linear mixed model regression analysis on the HDBR + CO2 participants, entering time as a continuous variable to assess the effect of the intervention on spatial working memory score. We used R software version 3.6.0 for this analysis entering time as a continuous variable, and age and sex as covariates. We considered the first time point to be a practice session and thus excluded it from the analysis (Lee et al., 2019a).

Results

We observed a small but significant increase in PaCO2 from pre- (41.4 mmHg) to post- (43.4 mmHg) bed rest (p < 0.05).

Spatial Working Memory Behavioral Results

We did not observe an effect of HDBR + CO2 on spatial working memory accuracy score (β = 0.12; p = 0.39). We previously reported the effects of HDBR + CO2 on spatial working memory (β = −0.03; p = 0.76), spatial working memory control (β = 0.10; p = 0.18), card rotation (time: β = −0.30; p < 0.01; accuracy: β = 0.11; p < 0.05), and cube rotation (time: β = −0.01; p = 0.18 and accuracy: β = −0.15; p = 0.15) (Lee et al., 2019a). We only found effects of time on card rotation time and accuracy, in which subjects showed improvement in both measures across HDBR + CO2 (Lee et al., 2019a).

Main Effect of Spatial Working Memory

The main effect of the spatial working memory task contrasted to the control task resulted in activation in the expected brain regions based on prior studies (Lamp et al., 2016). Specifically, we observed bilateral activation in several frontal, parietal, temporal and cerebellar regions (Table 1 and Figure 4). We also found deactivation in parietal and occipital regions (Table 1 and Figure 4).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Brain regions showing activation or deactivation during spatial working memory.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Main effect of spatial working memory. The spatial working memory task resulted in widespread activation (red) and deactivation (green). Whole brain and cerebellar results are overlaid onto MNI (A) and SUIT (B) standard templates, respectively; p < 0.001, k = 10. Abbreviations: L = Left; R = Right.

Time Course of Neural Working Memory Response to HDBR + CO2

Across HDBR + CO2, we found decreasing activation in the right middle frontal gyrus and left dentate nucleus of the cerebellum, followed by recovery after the HDBR + CO2 intervention (Figure 5 and Table 2). We did not observe any increases in brain activation followed by recovery in response to HDBR + CO2.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Time course of neural spatial working memory response to HDBR + CO2. Whole brain and cerebellar results showing decreases in activation (green) followed by recovery. Whole brain and cerebellar results are overlaid onto MNI (A) and SUIT (B) standard templates, respectively; p < 0.0005, k = 10 for whole brain analyses; k = 5 for cerebellar analyses. Abbreviations: L = Left; R = Right.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Brain regions showing cumulative changes during spatial working memory followed by recovery.

Brain-Behavior Correlations

Spatial Working Memory Task

We identified several regions for which the slope of change in brain activity correlated with the slope of change in spatial working memory performance (Figure 6A and Table 3). For instance, for spatial working memory accuracy, we observed that greater increases in activation of the right angular gyrus were associated with larger improvements in spatial working memory performance. That is, subjects who performed this task with fewer errors presented with greater increases in activation of the right angular gyrus during the HDBR + CO2 intervention. Further, a greater decrease in activation of the inferior frontal gyrus was correlated with less decline in spatial working memory accuracy. For the spatial working memory control task, we found that greater increases in activation of several brain regions, including parietal, temporal and occipital regions, correlated with greater accuracy increases. In addition, greater decrease in activation of the left lingual gyrus was correlated with less decline in the accuracy of the spatial working memory control task (Figure 6B and Table 3). Regarding the spatial working memory scores, we observed that greater increases in activation of the right superior temporal gyrus were correlated with greater increases in scores (Figure 6C and Table 3).

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Brain-behavior correlations (spatial working memory). Slope of changes in brain and behavior results showing positive (red) and negative (green) correlations. (A) Spatial working memory; (B) spatial working memory control; and (C) spatial working memory score. Whole brain results are overlaid onto the MNI standard template; p < 0.0005, k = 10. Right side correlation plots include contrast values extracted from the peak coordinate inside an example cluster (indicated with red or green arrows) graphed against the slope of changes in behavior results. Abbreviations: L = Left; R = Right; SWM = Spatial working memory.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Brain regions showing associations between the slopes of change in brain and behavioral during spatial working memory.

Card Rotation Task

We observed several brain regions for which the slope of change in brain activity correlated with the slope of change in card rotation accuracy (Figure 7 and Table 3). We found that greater increases in activation of the left supramarginal gyrus correlated with larger improvements in card rotation accuracy, while greater decreases in activation of the left superior frontal gyrus, right angular gyrus, and left lingual gyrus correlated with less decline in this measure (Figure 7 and Table 3). We did not observe brain and behavior correlation for the time to perform the card rotation task.

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Brain-behavior correlations (card rotation task). Slope of changes in brain and behavior results showing positive (red) and negative (green) correlations. Top: Whole brain results overlaid onto the MNI standard template; p < 0.0005, k = 10. Bottom: Correlation plots include contrast values extracted from the peak coordinate inside an example cluster (indicated with red or green arrows) graphed against the slope of changes in behavior results. Abbreviations: L = Left; R = Right.

Cube Rotation Task

For the cube rotation task, we observed several regions in which the slope of change in brain activity correlated with the slope of change in cube rotation accuracy. We found that greater increases in activation of several brain regions, including frontal, parietal, temporal and subcortical regions, were correlated with greater accuracy increases. That is, participants who increased activation in these brain regions presented with better accuracy on this task. For the time to perform the cube rotation task, we found that those subjects who showed increases activation of the left postcentral gyrus, left fusiform gyrus, and right middle occipital gyrus required less time to perform the task (Figure 8 and Table 3).

FIGURE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 8. Brain-behavior correlations (cube rotation task). Slope of changes in brain and behavior results showing positive (red) correlations. (A) Cube rotation time; and (B) cube rotation accuracy. Whole brain results are overlaid onto the MNI standard template; p < 0.0005, k = 10. Right side correlation plots include contrast values extracted from the peak coordinate inside an example cluster (indicated with red arrows) graphed against the slope of changes in behavior results. Abbreviations: L = Left; R = Right.

HDBR + CO2 vs. 70-day HDBR Group Comparisons

Between-group normalized slope comparisons for HDBR + CO2 and 70-day HDBR revealed differences in the right hippocampus and left inferior temporal gyrus (Table 4). That is, the HDBR + CO2 group showed greater decreases in activation in both brain regions across the intervention in comparison to HDBR alone. These results were detected at the conservative, corrected statistical threshold of FDR < 0.05 (Nichols and Hayasaka, 2003).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Regions with differences in slope of change in brain activation during bed rest between HDBR + CO2 and HDBR.

Discussion

This is the first study to investigate the effects of 30 days of HDBR combined with elevated CO2 on brain activation during spatial working memory performance. While there were no group-level declines in spatial working memory performance, we observed decreases in brain activation in several cortical and cerebellar regions in response to the HDBR + CO2 intervention, followed by recovery. In addition, we found that, in general, individuals who exhibited greater increases in brain activation also showed less declines in spatial working memory performance. The right superior temporal gyrus showed differential changes between the HDBR + CO2 and 70-day HDBR groups, suggesting that elevated CO2 levels may particularly affect the function of this brain region.

Spatial Working Memory Behavioral Results

Here, we found no differences in spatial working memory accuracy and score (which compares accuracy of the working memory condition to that of the control condition) or cube rotation time and accuracy across 30-days of HDBR + CO2. Likewise, Ishizaki et al. (2009) evaluated the effects of a 16-day HDBR intervention on executive function in young healthy participants and found no intervention-related changes in performance. Seaton et al. (2009) also found no differences in cognition after 60 or 90 days of HDBR. In contrast, Wang et al. (2017) assessed male healthy subjects that underwent 7 days of HDBR and reported poorer mental rotation accuracy in comparison to baseline. Similarly, Lipnicki et al. (2009) studied the effects of 60-day HDBR and found declines in working memory performance. Another study also examined healthy young individuals regarding time-based prospective memory with an ongoing word recall task and identified impaired prospective memory during HDBR compared to baseline (Chen et al., 2013). Although the behavioral effects of HDBR on spatial working memory are mixed, it seems that long-duration HDBR largely does not affect working memory abilities, with only one study showing differences due to 7 days of HDBR.

Time Course of Spatial Working Memory Response to HDBR + CO2

We identified decreasing activation in the right middle frontal gyrus and the cerebellar dentate nucleus, followed by recovery. These brain regions are involved in attention, mental rotation, and reorientation and are commonly activated during spatial working memory tasks (Thürling et al., 2012; Japee et al., 2015). Thus, these longitudinal changes suggest that HDBR + CO2 may have reduced the subjects’ ability to recruit appropriate working memory networks, or alternatively that it increased neural efficiency. We previously reported that the upward shift of the brain with both spaceflight (Koppelmans et al., 2016) and bed rest (Koppelmans et al., 2017) results in apparent reductions in gray matter volume of this region, which could potentially reflect gray matter compression. These structural brain changes may relate to the reduction in activation of this region during spatial working memory performance in the current study.

Brain-Behavior Correlations

We observed multiple brain-behavior correlations for the spatial working memory, 2D card rotation and 3D cube rotation tasks. Thus, although card rotation time and accuracy were the only behavioral metrics that changed with the intervention (Lee et al., 2019a), individual differences in performance changes in all tasks associated with individual differences in brain activity changes. We found that greater increases in activation in parietal, temporal, and occipital brain regions were correlated with larger improvements in spatial working memory accuracy. These associations may represent an adaptive or compensatory brain response to the HDBR + CO2 environment. In the past, our group has demonstrated associations between changes in spatial working memory performance (assessed by cube and card rotation tasks) and changes in brain connectivity between sensorimotor seed regions and brain areas associated with spatial cognition after 70 days of HDBR (Cassady et al., 2016). Those participants who had the greatest improvements in spatial working memory performance showed the greatest changes in connectivity between the seed and target brain areas. Thus, in the present study, it could be that these brain-behavior associations represent an adaptive neural response and are related to HDBR more generally and not specifically to elevated levels of CO2.

HDBR + CO2 vs. 70-day HDBR Group Comparisons

As we did not observe group differences in spatial working memory behavioral performance between the HDBR + CO2 and 70-day HDBR groups (Lee et al., 2019a), we expected to find few between-group differences in brain activation. We found that the HDBR + CO2 group presented a steeper slope of change in brain activity in several brain regions. That is, participants who underwent 30 days of HDBR + CO2 had greater decreases in activation in the right hippocampus and left inferior temporal gyrus than in HDBR alone. This was the only effect we observed that survived correction for multiple comparisons (FDR < 0.05). Previous functional neuroimaging studies have suggested that the inferior temporal gyrus is involved in several cognitive processes such as visual memory storage and cognitive learning (Miyashita, 1993). In the present study, greater increases in activation in the left inferior temporal gyrus was also correlated with larger improvements in cube rotation accuracy, which suggests compensatory network engagement to maintain performance during the intervention.

Similarly, the hippocampus plays an important role in long-term memory and working memory processing (Ni et al., 2017). Toepper et al. (2010) found activation in the right hippocampus when participants engaged in a spatial working memory task (Toepper et al., 2010). A more recent study with polar expeditioners who spent 14 months at the German Neumayer III station in Antarctica–a spaceflight analog model to study the effects of social isolation and environmental deprivation–observed reduced hippocampal volume in several regions following the expedition (Stahn et al., 2019). They also reported that reduced hippocampal volume was not associated with general cognitive performance, but it was correlated with performance on a spatial mental rotation task. Thus, it is possible that the hippocampal activation changes we observed here during the mental rotation working memory task occurred at least partly as a result of our subjects being isolated for 30 days.

Another recent study evaluated the effect of acute exposure to elevated levels of CO2 (0.5%) during HDBR in comparison to HDBR alone on cognitive performance. They reported that subjects exposed to 26.5 h of 12° HDBR + CO2 presented with greater accuracy and lower speed on the Visual Object Learning Task in comparison to HDBR alone (Basner et al., 2018). Based on that finding, Scully et al., speculated that the medial temporal cortex and the hippocampus could be more sensitive to changes in CO2 concentration, with concomitant improvement in memory performance (Scully et al., 2019). Our results are in agreement with their speculation since we observed that the elevated CO2 levels combined with HDBR had a small effect on these same brain regions. However, we did not observe significant improvements in working memory performance; it could be that our task was not sensitive enough to detect subtle CO2-induced changes in working memory performance. In combination, these studies support that medial temporal lobe and hippocampal changes with HDBR + CO2 could be due to some combination of HDBR, CO2, and/or isolation.

CO2 has a vasodilation effect which results in increased brain blood flow (Atkinson et al., 1990; Zhou et al., 2008) and consequently increased intensity of the blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) signal measured by fMRI (Corfield et al., 2001). However, the effects of elevated CO2 on brain perfusion are still inconclusive. In the present study, even though participants presented increases in their PaCO2 levels from pre- to post-HDBR + CO2 we did not see increases in brain activity in comparison to HDBR alone. Conversely, HDBR + CO2 presented greater decreases in activation in comparison to HDBR alone. On the other hand, a recent study from our group (again using the same subjects as in the present study) showed greater increases in activation of several regions during vestibular stimulation for the HDBR + CO2 group in comparison to the 70-day HDBR group (Hupfeld et al., 2020). This result suggests interactive or additive effects of bed rest and elevated CO2 for vestibular changes (Hupfeld et al., 2020), but not for spatial working memory changes. Therefore, again, elevated CO2 effects seem to be task-specific rather than global effects of HDBR or CO2.

Limitations

This study has several limitations. First, we had a small sample size and thus the results should be generalized with caution. Second, the testing timelines differed between the HDBR + CO2 and 70-day HDBR groups; each group was part of a separate bed rest campaign. These data were collected on two different Siemens scanners with two slightly different fMRI sequences. The HDBR + CO2 fMRI sequence included a faster TR than the 70-day HDBR sequence. However, we controlled for these differences as much as possible by using age and sex as covariates and by using slope comparisons to account for timeline differences (Yuan et al., 2016, 2018b; Hupfeld et al., 2020). Third, although the between-group comparison is FDR corrected (i.e., a more conservative statistical threshold), due to the limited pilot sample size, we used uncorrected p-values for the other neuroimaging statistical analyses to better detect within- and between-subject differences (Hupfeld et al., 2020). It is known that there is an upward shift of the brain and fluid redistribution during HDBR (Koppelmans et al., 2017), so it is not clear whether or how those changes interact with the functional brain changes seeing here. The fourth caveat of this study is that subjects on the HDBR + CO2 group underwent stricter bed rest, so it is unclear whether the results found here are due to the effects of the elevated levels of CO2 and/or the absence of a pillow in certain postures. Additionally, subjects in the HDBR campaign were scanned while supine, whereas those in the HDBR + CO2 were maintained at −6°. Finally, it should also be mentioned that HDBR + CO2 mimics only some of the effects of spaceflight, such as high levels of CO2, body unloading and fluid shifts toward the head, so it is difficult to fully generalize these findings to spaceflight. Moreover, lung volumes are reduced in bed rest and microgravity but not with the same extent (West, 2000; Prisk, 2005), then elevated CO2 levels may have larger effects on the ISS than on Earth.

Conclusion

We investigated the longitudinal neural effects of HDBR + CO2 on spatial working memory. We observed decreases in activation in brain regions that are involved in attention, mental rotation and reorientation followed by recovery. This suggests that 30 days of HDBR combined with elevated CO2 levels may reduce the ability to recruit these brain regions. These findings contribute to a better understanding of how the working memory system adapts to a spaceflight analog environment.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the University of Florida Institutional Review Board. NASA Institutional Review Board. Local ethical commission of the regional medical association (Ärztekammer Nordrhein). The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

AS analyzed the spatial working memory fMRI and behavioral data, created the figures and tables, and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. KH assisted with fMRI preprocessing, fMRI statistical analyses, and preparation of the initial manuscript draft. JL and EM collected, analyzed, and managed the data. NB and YD collected and analyzed the data. IK participated in project design and software development. JB, AM, and RS designed the project, secured funding, and led the interpretation and discussion of the results. All authors participated in revision of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by grants from the National Aeronautics and Space Administration (NASA; NNX11AR02G, and 80NSSC17K0021) and the National Space Biomedical Research Institute (NSBRI; SA02802) to RS, AM, and JB. Additionally, we thank support from a National Science Foundation Graduate Research Fellowship under Grant nos. DGE-1315138 and DGE-1842473, as well as training grant NIH T32-NS082128 to KH.

Conflict of Interest

NB, IK, YD, and AM were employed by the company KBR.

The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to acknowledge those at the: envihab facility in Cologne, Germany including the VaPER study staff, and the bed rest participants who volunteered their time, without whom this project would not have been possible.

Footnotes

  1. ^ www.nitrc.org/projects/artifact_detect/
  2. ^ http://warwick.ac.uk/snpm

References

Allen, J. G., MacNaughton, P., Cedeno-Laurent, J. G., Cao, X., Flanigan, S., Vallarino, J., et al. (2019). Airplane pilot flight performance on 21 maneuvers in a flight simulator under varying carbon dioxide concentrations. J. Expo. Sci. Environ. Epidemiol. 29, 457–468. doi: 10.1038/s41370-018-0055-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anguera, J. A., Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., Willingham, D. T., and Seidler, R. D. (2010). Contributions of spatial working memory to visuomotor learning. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 22, 1917–1930. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21351

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Atkinson, J. L. D., Anderson, R. E., and Sundt, T. M. (1990). The effect of carbon dioxide on the diameter of brain capillaries. Brain Res. 517, 333–340. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(90)91046-J

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Avants, B. B., Tustison, N. J., Song, G., Cook, P. A., Klein, A., and Gee, J. C. (2011). A reproducible evaluation of ANTs similarity metric performance in brain image registration. Neuroimage 54, 2033–2044. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.09.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baddeley, A. D. (2017). “Working memory: theories, models, and controversies,” in Exploring Working Memory: Selected Works of Alan Baddeley (London: Routledge Taylor & Francis Group). doi: 10.4324/9781315111261

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Basner, M., Nasrini, J., Hermosillo, E., McGuire, S., Dinges, D. F., Moore, T. M., et al. (2018). Effects of -12° head-down tilt with and without elevated levels of CO2 on cognitive performance: the SPACECOT study. J. Appl. Physiol. 124, 750–760. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00855.2017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bock, O., Weigelt, C., and Bloomberg, J. J. (2010). Cognitive demand of human sensorimotor performance during an extended space mission: a dual-task study. Aviat. Sp. Environ. Med. 81, 819–824. doi: 10.3357/ASEM.2608.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cassady, K., Koppelmans, V., Reuter-Lorenz, P., De Dios, Y., Gadd, N., Wood, S., et al. (2016). Effects of a spaceflight analog environment on brain connectivity and behavior. Neuroimage 141, 18–30. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.07.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, S., Zhou, R., Xiu, L., Chen, S., Chen, X., and Tan, C. (2013). Effects of 45-day -6° head-down bed rest on the time-based prospective memory. Acta Astronaut. 84, 81–87. doi: 10.1016/j.actaastro.2012.10.040

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Corfield, D. R., Murphy, K., Josephs, O., Adams, L., and Turner, R. (2001). Does hypercapnia-induced cerebral vasodilation modulate the hemodynamic response to neural activation? Neuroimage 13(6 Pt 1), 1207–1211. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0760

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De la Torre, G. (2014). Cognitive neuroscience in space. Life 4, 281–294. doi: 10.3390/life4030281

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diedrichsen, J. (2006). A spatially unbiased atlas template of the human cerebellum. Neuroimage 33, 127–138. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.05.056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diedrichsen, J., Balsters, J. H., Flavell, J., Cussans, E., and Ramnani, N. (2009). A probabilistic MR atlas of the human cerebellum. Neuroimage 46, 39–46. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.01.045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ekstrom, R. B., French, J. W., and Harman, H. H. (1976). Manual for Kit of Factor-Referenced Cognitive Tests. Princeton, NJ: Education Testing Service.

Google Scholar

Gläscher, J., and Gitelman, D. (2008). Contrast Weights in Flexible Factorial Design with Multiple Groups of Subjects. London: SPM, 1–12.

Google Scholar

Hupfeld, K. E., Lee, J. K., Gadd, N. E., Kofman, I. S., De Dios, Y. E., Bloomberg, J. J., et al. (2020). Neural correlates of vestibular processing during a spaceflight analog with elevated carbon dioxide (co2): a pilot Study. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 13:80. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2019.00080

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ishizaki, Y., Fukuoka, H., Tanaka, H., Ishizaki, T., Fujii, Y., Hattori-Uchida, Y., et al. (2009). Executive function on the 16-day of bed rest in young healthy men. Acta Astronaut. 64, 864–868. doi: 10.1016/j.actaastro.2008.10.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Japee, S., Holiday, K., Satyshur, M. D., Mukai, I., and Ungerleider, L. G. (2015). A role of right middle frontal gyrus in reorienting of attention: a case study. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 9:23. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2015.00023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kanas, N., and Manzey, D. (2008). Space Psychology and Psychiatry. Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4020-6770-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koppelmans, V., Bloomberg, J. J., De Dios, Y. E., Wood, S. J., Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., Kofman, I. S., et al. (2017). Brain plasticity and sensorimotor deterioration as a function of 70 days head down tilt bed rest. PLoS One 12:e0182236. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0182236

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koppelmans, V., Bloomberg, J. J., Mulavara, A. P., and Seidler, R. D. (2016). Brain structural plasticity with spaceflight. npj Microgravity 2:2. doi: 10.1038/s41526-016-0001-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koppelmans, V., Erdeniz, B., De Dios, Y. E., Wood, S. J., Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., Kofman, I., et al. (2013). Study protocol to examine the effects of spaceflight and a spaceflight analog on neurocognitive performance: extent, longevity, and neural bases. BMC Neurol. 13:205. doi: 10.1186/1471-2377-13-205

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lamp, G., Alexander, B., Laycock, R., Crewther, D. P., and Crewther, S. G. (2016). Mapping of the underlying neural mechanisms of maintenance and manipulation in visuo-spatial working memory using an n-back mental rotation task: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 10:87. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00087

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Law, J., Van Baalen, M., Foy, M., Mason, S. S., Mendez, C., Wear, M. L., et al. (2014). Relationship between carbon dioxide levels and reported headaches on the international space station. J. Occup. Environ. Med. 56, 477–483. doi: 10.1097/JOM.0000000000000158

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, J. K., De Dios, Y., Kofman, I., Mulavara, A. P., Bloomberg, J. J., and Seidler, R. D. (2019a). Head down tilt bed rest plus elevated CO2 as a spaceflight analog: effects on cognitive and sensorimotor performance. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 13:355. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00355

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, J. K., Koppelmans, V., Riascos, R. F., Hasan, K. M., Pasternak, O., Mulavara, A. P., et al. (2019b). Spaceflight-associated brain white matter microstructural changes and intracranial fluid redistribution. JAMA Neurol. 76, 412–419. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2018.4882

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leone, G., Lipshits, M., Gurfinkel, V., and Berthoz, A. (1995). Is there an effect of weightlessness on mental rotation of three-dimensional objects? Cogn. Brain Res. 2, 255–267. doi: 10.1016/0926-6410(95)90017-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lipnicki, D. M., Gunga, H. C., Belav?, D. L., and Felsenberg, D. (2009). Bed rest and cognition: effects on executive functioning and reaction time. Aviat. Space Environ. Med. 80, 1018–1024. doi: 10.3357/ASEM.2581.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manzey, D., and Lorenz, B. (1998). Mental performance during short-term and long-term spaceflight. Brain Res. Rev. 28, 215–221. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0173(98)00041-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miyashita, Y. (1993). Inferior temporal cortex: where visual perception meets memory. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 16, 245–263. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.16.030193.001333

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moore, S. T., MacDougall, H. G., and Paloski, W. H. (2010). Effects of head-down bed rest and artiWcial gravity on spatial orientation. Exp. Brain Res. 204, 617–622. doi: 10.1007/s00221-010-2317-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ni, B., Wu, R., Yu, T., Zhu, H., Li, Y., and Liu, Z. (2017). Role of the hippocampus in distinct memory traces: timing of match and mismatch enhancement revealed by intracranial recording. Neurosci. Bull. 33, 664–674. doi: 10.1007/s12264-017-0172-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nichols, T., and Hayasaka, S. (2003). Controlling the familywise error rate in functional neuroimaging: a comparative review. Stat. Methods Med. Res. 12, 419–446. doi: 10.1191/0962280203sm341ra

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prisk, G. K. (2005). The lung in space. Clin. Chest Med. 26, 415–438. doi: 10.1016/j.ccm.2005.05.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., Jonides, J., Smith, E. E., Hartley, A., Miller, A., Marshuetz, C., et al. (2000). Age differences in the frontal lateralization of verbal and spatial working memory revealed by PET. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 12, 174–187. doi: 10.1162/089892900561814

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roberts, D. R., Albrecht, M. H., Collins, H. R., Asemani, D., Chatterjee, A. R., Spampinato, M. V., et al. (2017). Effects of spaceflight on astronaut brain structure as indicated on MRI. N. Engl. J. Med. 377, 1746–1753. doi: 10.1056/nejmoa1705129

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Romero, J. E., Coupé, P., Giraud, R., Ta, V. T., Fonov, V., Park, M. T. M., et al. (2017). CERES: a new cerebellum lobule segmentation method. Neuroimage 147, 916–924. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.11.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scully, R. R., Basner, M., Nasrini, J., Lam, C. W., Hermosillo, E., Gur, R. C., et al. (2019). Effects of acute exposures to carbon dioxide on decision making and cognition in astronaut-like subjects. npj Microgravity 5:17. doi: 10.1038/s41526-019-0071-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seaton, K. A., Slack, K. J., Sipes, W. A., and Bowie, K. E. (2009). Cognitive functioning in long-duration head-down bed rest. Aviat. Sp. Environ. Med. 80(5 Suppl), A62–A65. doi: 10.3357/ASEM.BR09.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seidler, R. D., Bo, J., and Anguera, J. A. (2012). Neurocognitive contributions to motor skill learning: the role of working memory. J. Mot. Behav. 44, 445–453. doi: 10.1080/00222895.2012.672348

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shepard, R. N., and Metzler, J. (1971). Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. Science 171, 701–703. doi: 10.1126/science.171.3972.701

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shepard, S., and Metzler, D. (1988). Mental rotation: effects of dimensionality of objects and type of task. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 14, 3–11. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.14.1.3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stahn, A. C., Gunga, H. C., Kohlberg, E., Gallinat, J., Dinges, D. F., and Kühn, S. (2019). Brain changes in response to long Antarctic expeditions. N. Engl. J. Med. 381, 2273–2275. doi: 10.1056/NEJMc1904905

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thürling, M., Hautzel, H., Küper, M., Stefanescu, M. R., Maderwald, S., Ladd, M. E., et al. (2012). Involvement of the cerebellar cortex and nuclei in verbal and visuospatial working memory: a 7T fMRI study. Neuroimage 62, 1537–1550. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.05.037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Toepper, M., Markowitsch, H. J., Gebhardt, H., Beblo, T., Thomas, C., Gallhofer, B., et al. (2010). Hippocampal involvement in working memory encoding of changing locations: an fMRI study. Brain Res. 1354, 91–99. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.07.065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, H., Duan, J., Liao, Y., Wang, C., Li, H., and Liu, X. (2017). Objects mental rotation under 7 days simulated weightlessness condition: an ERP study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:553. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00553

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

West, J. B. (2000). Physiology of a microgravity environment historical perspectives: physiology in microgravity. J. Appl. Physiol. 89, 379–384. doi: 10.1152/jappl.2000.89.1.379

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yuan, P., Koppelmans, V., Reuter-Lorenz, P., De Dios, Y., Gadd, N., Riascos, R., et al. (2018a). Change of cortical foot activation following 70 days of head-down bed rest. J. Neurophysiol. 119, 2145–2152. doi: 10.1152/jn.00693.2017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yuan, P., Koppelmans, V., Reuter-Lorenz, P., De Dios, Y., Gadd, N., Wood, S., et al. (2018b). Vestibular brain changes within 70 days of head down bed rest. Hum. Brain Mapp. 39, 2753–2763. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yuan, P., Koppelmans, V., Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., De Dios, Y. E., Gadd, N. E., Wood, S. J., et al. (2016). Increased brain activation for dual tasking with 70-days head-down bed rest. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 10:71. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2016.00071

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhou, H., Saidel, G. M., and Lamanna, J. C. (2008). Cerebral blood flow adaptation to chronic hypoxia. Adv. Exp. Med. Biol. 614, 371–377. doi: 10.1007/978-0-387-74911-2_41

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: cognition, spatial working memory, carbon dioxide, head down tilt bed rest, microgravity

Citation: Salazar AP, Hupfeld KE, Lee JK, Beltran NE, Kofman IS, De Dios YE, Mulder E, Bloomberg JJ, Mulavara AP and Seidler RD (2020) Neural Working Memory Changes During a Spaceflight Analog With Elevated Carbon Dioxide: A Pilot Study. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 14:48. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2020.00048

Received: 17 April 2020; Accepted: 29 June 2020;
Published: 28 July 2020.

Edited by:

Carlos Cepeda, University of California, Los Angeles, United States

Reviewed by:

Araceli Espinosa-Jeffrey, UCLA Department of Physiology, United States
Yaoying Ma, Binghamton University, United States

Copyright © 2020 Salazar, Hupfeld, Lee, Beltran, Kofman, De Dios, Mulder, Bloomberg, Mulavara and Seidler. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Rachael D. Seidler, cmFjaGFlbHNlaWRsZXJAdWZsLmVkdQ==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.