Skip to main content

PERSPECTIVE article

Front. Syst. Neurosci., 03 November 2015
This article is part of the Research Topic From theory to method to understanding: connectivity analysis of electrophysiological data to understand the function of the dynamic brain View all 8 articles

Methods, caveats and the future of large-scale microelectrode recordings in the non-human primate

  • 1Department of Cell Biology and Neuroscience, Montana State University, Bozeman, MT, USA
  • 2Faculty of Medicine, Department of Basic Neurosciences, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland

Cognitive processes play out on massive brain-wide networks, which produce widely distributed patterns of activity. Capturing these activity patterns requires tools that are able to simultaneously measure activity from many distributed sites with high spatiotemporal resolution. Unfortunately, current techniques with adequate coverage do not provide the requisite spatiotemporal resolution. Large-scale microelectrode recording devices, with dozens to hundreds of microelectrodes capable of simultaneously recording from nearly as many cortical and subcortical areas, provide a potential way to minimize these tradeoffs. However, placing hundreds of microelectrodes into a behaving animal is a highly risky and technically challenging endeavor that has only been pursued by a few groups. Recording activity from multiple electrodes simultaneously also introduces several statistical and conceptual dilemmas, such as the multiple comparisons problem and the uncontrolled stimulus response problem. In this perspective article, we discuss some of the techniques that we, and others, have developed for collecting and analyzing large-scale data sets, and address the future of this emerging field.

Introduction

Decades of microelectrode recordings have laid the foundation for our current understanding of cognitive processes at the single neuron level, while functional imaging and other non-invasive techniques have demonstrated that cognitive processes engage widespread cortical and subcortical areas (Fuster and Alexander, 1971; Andersen et al., 1985; Moran and Desimone, 1985; Funahashi et al., 1989; Chelazzi et al., 1993; Desimone and Duncan, 1995; Courtney et al., 1997; Munk et al., 2002; Constantinidis and Procyk, 2004; Postle, 2006; Harrison and Tong, 2009; Roux and Uhlhaas, 2014). These studies, along with much anatomical and theoretical work, have lead to a common agreement that cognitive processes must involve the dynamic, self-organized coordination of multiple cortical and subcortical areas (Mesulam, 1990; Gray, 1994; Bressler, 1995; Friston, 1997; Bressler and Kelso, 2001; Varela et al., 2001; Fries, 2005; Buzsáki, 2010; Chialvo, 2010; Siegel et al., 2012; Singer, 2013; Tognoli and Kelso, 2014). Understanding how populations of neurons distributed across the brain coordinate their activity in relation to behavior has become a central question in cognitive and systems neuroscience. As the title of this research topic suggests, studying the functional connectivity patterns that arise during cognitive processes may be the key to answering this question.

In order to properly study large-scale brain dynamics, recording technologies with the requisite coverage, and spatiotemporal resolution must be developed. Current techniques make tradeoffs between coverage, spatial resolution, and temporal resolution. For instance, functional MRI (fMRI) indirectly measures neural activity on the time scale of seconds, but can sample signals from small contiguous volumes spanning the entire brain. Standard single microelectrode recordings on the other hand, provide unparalleled spatiotemporal resolution, but poor coverage. The signals recorded with other technologies, such as Electroencephalography (EEG), Magnetoencephalography (MEG) and Electrocorticography (ECoG), also vary substantially in their coverage and spatial resolution, with clear limitations in one or more variables. These severe methodological constraints have prompted several groups, including our own, to develop large-scale microelectrode recording devices, which have high coverage and both high spatial and high temporal resolution. What follows, is a discussion of the state of the art in large-scale microelectrode recordings in the non-human primate.

Large-Scale Recording Systems in the Non-Human Primate

Recording technologies for primates and other mammals allow for recording from entire networks, localized circuits, or a combination of the two (Maynard et al., 1997; Buzsáki, 2004; Kipke et al., 2008; Chang, 2015; Lewis et al., 2015). We restrict our discussion to techniques that use microelectrodes or microwires to chronically or semi-chronically record local field potentials (LFP) and/or unit activity from multiple distributed sites simultaneously. Unfortunately, many interesting studies and methods are left out solely for the purpose of brevity or because they are outside our narrow focus (for example, see Bosman et al., 2012; Yanagawa et al., 2013; Mendoza-Halliday et al., 2014; Bastos et al., 2015; Siegel et al., 2015).

Early examples of large-scale recordings in the non-human primate include the work from Bressler et al. (1993), Nicolelis et al. (1998, 2003) and Hoffman and McNaughton (2002). Using chronically implanted surface-to-depth bipolar electrodes, Bressler et al. (1993) recorded transcortical LFP from up to 15 broadly distributed sites in macaque monkeys performing a visual pattern discrimination task. Electrodes were implanted in frontal, motor, somatosensory, superior and inferior temporal, parietal, pre-striate and striate areas (Bressler et al., 1993). This study revealed broadband coherence between distinct combinations of widely separated sites that occurs in a task-dependent manner. Also using chronic recording methods, Nicolelis et al. (1998) implanted three microwire arrays in three different cortical areas (area 3b, area S2, and area 2) in owl monkeys to identify the tactile information contained in neural populations. Each array, arranged in a 2 by 8 matrix, sampled an area of 2 mm2 using 16 stainless steel, teflon-coated wires. This was later scaled up to over 700 microwires in 10 cortical locations over both hemispheres using stacks of microwire arrays directly attached to connectors (Nicolelis et al., 2003). Animals in this study were implanted for up to 18 months, with early recording sessions boasting up to 247 single neurons.

While these landmark studies made important scientific and technical advances, the recording techniques still had several drawbacks. An ideal large-scale recording device would have independently moveable electrodes and the ability to record from both cortical and sub-cortical areas. Hoffman and McNaughton (2002) overcame the first limitation by chronically implanting arrays with 144 independently moveable electrodes in four different cortical regions (dorsal prefrontal cortex, somatosensory cortex, posterior parietal cortex, and motor cortex) in a macaque monkey performing a sequential reaching task. Electrodes were densely spaced at 650 micron intervals in a 12 by 12 matrix. Using these high-density distributed recordings they provided evidence for coordinated reactivation occurring within and between all of the recorded regions except for dorsal prefrontal cortex. Similarly, Schwarz et al. (2014) modified their earlier design (Nicolelis et al., 2003) to allow for their microwire arrays to be repositioned, along with increasing the channel count and incorporating a telemetry system.

Sub-cortical recordings are also crucial for an accurate understanding of large-scale brain dynamics. Even though the previously mentioned techniques have made major advances in one way or another, they all lacked the ability to make sub-cortical recordings. In part, this is probably because that was not the objective of the experiments. Recognizing the importance of sub-cortical recordings, Feingold et al. (2012) developed a system capable of both cortical and sub-cortical recordings using independently moveable microelectrodes. Using this device, they were able to make simultaneous recordings from 14 different brain regions using up to 127 microelectrodes. This study provides an excellent example of a system with immense potential for studying large-scale brain dynamics.

Over the past decade, we have been developing large-scale recording devices as well. Our objective has been to develop a technique capable of both cortical and sub-cortical exploration, along with the ability to move electrodes independently and scale the devices as needed. Devices developed in the lab include semi-chronic microdrives with 32 or 60 independently moveable microelectrodes capable of simultaneously recording the activity from multiple cortical areas (Gray and Goodell, 2011), to devices with over 250 microelectrodes capable of recording from an entire cerebral hemisphere. The 32-channel microdrive was developed for targeting several nearby cortical areas at a variety of depths. Electrodes are arranged in a 6 by 6 grid (one of the rows is only partially filled) and horizontally spaced at 1.5 mm. Each electrode is independently moveable, with up to 16 mm of travel (for more details, see Gray and Goodell, 2011; Markowitz et al., 2011; Salazar et al., 2012). Figure 1A shows design drawings of two of these devices integrated into an MRI reconstruction of a macaque skull and brain. The recording devices are semi-chronically implanted, meaning they may be removed at the end of a study or replaced with new ones. We used these devices to simultaneously record from a total of 12 different cortical areas in prefrontal and posterior parietal cortex in an animal performing a visual working memory task (Figure 1B; Salazar et al., 2012; Dotson et al., 2014). Using this recording technique, we were able to identify a bimodal patterning of the relative phase relationships among cortical signals (LFPs 8–25 Hz) within the frontoparietal network (Dotson et al., 2014). Figures 1C,D shows an example of the magnitude and relative phase relationships within and between prefrontal and posterior parietal signals during visual working memory from a single recording session (for more details, see Dotson et al., 2014). The relative phase angle was estimated using cross-correlation analysis on pairs of signals bandpass filtered at 8–25 Hz.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Illustration of the hardware for simultaneous frontoparietal recordings and an example of the relative phase relationships in the frontoparietal network. (A) MRI based skull and brain model incorporating 3D design drawings of two 32 channel semi-chronic microdrives and recording chambers. (B) Illustration of the recording chamber locations. (C,D) Magnitude and phase angle within and between prefrontal and posterior parietal cortex during the delay period of a visual working memory task. The magnitude of correlation is indicated by line thickness, and the relative phase angle (absolute value) is indicated by the line color. The relative phase angles were calculated using cross-correlation on bandpassed signals (8–25 Hz). (C) Intra-prefrontal and intra-parietal relative phase angles. Note the clear anti-phase relationship between signals on opposite sides of the intraparietal sulcus. (D) Inter-areal relative phase angles. In this example, a clear anti-phase relationship exists between prefrontal area 8B and the parietal areas it is significantly correlated with (see Dotson et al., 2014 for more details).

While the use of multiple 32-channel microdrives aided us in making parallel recordings in the frontoparietal network, our ultimate objective has been to simultaneously record from the entire working memory network, including the dorsal and ventral visual streams, the prefrontal cortex, and subcortical areas, such as the mediodorsal nucleus of the thalamus. Using two macaque monkeys trained to perform a visually guided delayed match to sample task, we implanted recording devices with over 250 independently moveable microelectrodes spanning an entire hemisphere. Figure 2A shows a side view of the fully loaded device (second generation, 252 electrodes), with electrodes protruding. The electrodes are spaced at 2.4 mm intervals and have a travel of 40 mm on the most lateral three columns and 30 mm on the remaining columns. Prior to implantation, the electrodes are retracted, and the whole device is hermetically sealed using silicone grease followed by a silicone sealant. The implant is done in three steps over a period of several months. First, a chamber is mounted on the skull using cranial screws and bone cement. Obtaining a tight fit between the chamber and the skull is accomplished by customizing the chamber to the animal’s skull using a pre-operative MRI, and by applying bone cement to the interface between the chamber and the skull. Next, a craniotomy is made within the confines of the chamber, and finally the sealed 252 channel microdrive is placed into the chamber. Importantly, the recording device is designed to conform to the surface of the dura, essentially replacing the bone, which assures very little movement of the brain. Figure 2B shows exploded design drawings of the microdrive without actuator components (note: craniotomy is not shown). Figure 2C shows the device, as it would appear fully assembled and implanted. Using these devices, we have recorded high quality broadband signals (LFP and single/multi-unit activity) from months to nearly a year, with the ability to target dozens of cortical and subcortical areas simultaneously. Figure 2D shows an example of the broadband data recorded on a single trial from 127 electrodes. From top to bottom the signals go from anterior to posterior, respectively, and span prefrontal cortex to primary visual cortex. Note the differences in spectral composition and task dependence.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Hemisphere-wide recording technique. (A) Semi-chronic 252-channel microdrive system with electrodes advanced. Prior to implantation, the electrodes are fully retracted and the bottom surface of the device is made impermeable. (B) Exploded views of the design drawing for the microdrive system (the actuator mechanism is excluded). (C) Design drawings of the system as it would appear when implanted on the animal. (D) Example of the broadband signals (local field potentials (LFP) and single/multi-unit activity) simultaneously recorded from 127 electrodes on a single trial of a visual working memory task. From top to bottom the signals go from anterior to posterior, respectively. The vertical (V) and horizontal (H) eye positions are included at the bottom of the plot. The first two vertical black lines indicate the onset and offset of the sample. The third vertical black line indicates the onset of the match.

Caveats with Large-Scale Recordings

There are many hurdles to overcome when running large-scale microelectrode experiments in non-human primates, on top of the already difficult task of training, implanting, and recording from an alert animal. For instance, the data sets are several orders of magnitude larger than ones collected using single electrodes and subsequently require the use of large servers. Computational demands also increase by many orders of magnitude, especially when performing pairwise or multivariate measures. This means more resources, monetary and human, are necessary to complete these projects. Researchers must also become savvy programmers in order to utilize high performance computers and to write computationally efficient programs. Here, we mention a few of the issues that arise during the analysis phase. Specifically, the multiple comparisons problem and what may be referred to as the uncontrolled stimulus response problem.

Analyzing data from large-scale recordings often requires performing multiple parallel statistical tests, leading to the multiple comparisons problem (Shaffer, 1995; Benjamini, 2010). Generally speaking, this issue arises because the probability of making a Type 1 error (false positive) goes up as the number of tests goes up. While the concept is relatively easy to grasp, many interesting debates have arisen around it. For instance, where does one draw the border for a family of tests (Rothman, 1990; Perneger, 1998)? Perneger (1998) takes this issue to an extreme in a discussion of the Bonferroni correction by asking if corrections should be made for all the tests a researcher performs in their career, or if all the tests in a particular journal should be corrected. Misinterpretation, or exclusion, of findings due to high Type 2 error rates (false negative) are also a concern, leading some researchers to advocate against correction procedures altogether (Rothman, 1990). With that said, a commonly accepted technique that balances both type 1 errors (false positive) and type 2 errors (false negative) is to control the false discovery rate (FDR). FDR procedures, like the Benjamini and Hochberg (1995) procedure, ensure that only a certain percentage of Type 1 errors occur (e.g., 5%) and rely on the assumption that a small proportion of Type 1 errors will not alter the conclusions of the study. In fMRI research, FDR procedures, initially introduced by Genovese et al. (2002), have been widely used. Indeed, we can learn a lot from fMRI researchers, who often make 1,000 s of parallel tests, and have been hashing these issues out for many years (Logan and Rowe, 2004; Bennett et al., 2009; Chumbley and Friston, 2009; Nichols, 2012). EEG and MEG researchers have also developed techniques to deal with the multiple comparisons problem, including nonparametric and cluster-based nonparametric tests (Maris and Oostenveld, 2007). Large-scale microelectrode recordings will inevitably be another hot bed for the multiple comparisons problem. Subsequently, it will be important that researchers take the time to understand how results should be interpreted based on different correction procedures, and to avoid focusing too closely on the arbitrary designation of “significance”. Tukey (1991) puts this wisely: “We must pay some chance of error to extract knowledge—or belief—from data. A crucial task is to expend this chance wisely, and to see that how it was spent, as well as how much was spent, is clearly recognizable”. It should also be stressed that since scientific progress relies on large bodies of work there is an inherent control for the multiple comparisons problem: reproducibility (Tukey, 1991; Perneger, 1998).

Methodological issues in study design and interpretation of results also arise when moving from the analysis of single neurons to analyses that bridge mesoscopic and macroscopic levels. Typical microelectrode experiments involve placing a single electrode in a predefined area of the brain, identifying a neuron with desirable response properties, and then tailoring the experimental paradigm for that specific neuron. This provides the ideal setup for understanding the response properties of single neurons in single areas to highly controlled changes in stimulus and task properties, and it has provided us with the foundation of cognitive and systems neuroscience. However, gaining a better understanding of cognitive processes will rely on both an understanding of the response properties of single neurons, and by determining how many widely distributed cortical and subcortical areas cooperate in a dynamic circuit. The brain is after all a complex system, and should be studied as such (Chialvo, 2010; Gazzaniga, 2010). Unfortunately, when the functions of large-scale circuits are of interest, it becomes impractical to search for recording sites in many locations with overlapping response properties (Herrington and Assad, 2010). Moreover, tailoring experiments to only one or two recording sites out of 10’s to 100’s of recordings sites would also be impractical. How then should simultaneous recordings from two or more areas be conducted? The approach that we (Salazar et al., 2012; Dotson et al., 2014) and other researchers (Buschman and Miller, 2007; Pesaran et al., 2008; Siegel et al., 2015) have taken is to determine the response properties of neurons post hoc, rather than selecting neurons with specific response properties. However, these differences in study design can place researchers in a position where their results and interpretations are not able to entirely overlap with previous work in the field. Indeed, significant debate has arisen along these lines (Miller and Buschman, 2007; Schall et al., 2007). From this, it may be clearly seen that in order to record the distributed patterns of activity involved in perception, action, and everything in between, we must adjust our analysis of single neuron response properties. This is especially true if we hope to understand behavior in naturalistic or real world settings. We foresee that as more data is collected, new avenues of investigation that do not rely as closely on single neuron properties will open up, but until then, understanding the tradeoffs and how this affects study design and interpretation of results will be crucial to the success of these types of studies.

The Future of Large-Scale Recordings in the Non-Human Primate

Large-scale microelectrode recordings in the non-human primate are beginning to be paired with common manipulation techniques, such as electrical stimulation and pharmacological injections (Feingold et al., 2012). Future work could include injecting agonists or antagonists for neurotransmitters like dopamine and acetylcholine, which have been shown to play a central role in attentional processes in prefrontal and visual cortex, respectively (Herrero et al., 2008; Noudoost and Moore, 2011a,b). By combining pharmacological injections with simultaneous recordings from the injection sites and other cortical and subcortical areas distributed across the brain, the contribution of these neurotransmitters to cognitive processes could be further understood. Similarly, large-scale microelectrode recording devices could incorporate emerging technologies, such as optogenetic tools developed specifically for non-human primates (Han et al., 2009; Diester et al., 2011; Han, 2012; Gerits and Vanduffel, 2013; Dai et al., 2014). Combining optogenetics with large-scale microelectrode recordings will enable researchers to selectively manipulate and essentially dissect any circuit of interest (Gerits et al., 2012). Chemogenetic tools, such as designer receptors exclusively activated by designer drugs (DREADDs; Sternson and Roth, 2014; Vardy et al., 2015), which are in the early stages of development for non-human primates, provide another promising tool to use in conjunction with large-scale recordings.

Finally, the future of large-scale microelectrode recordings will also rely on the utilization of both functional connectivity measures (this research topic, see Bastos et al., 2015) and machine learning tools (for a recent review, see Jordan and Mitchell, 2015). Machine learning tools, like multi-voxel/variate pattern analysis (MVPA), have already converted much of the functional imaging community from focusing on functional localization to focusing on patterns of activity (Harrison and Tong, 2009; Pereira et al., 2009; Poldrack, 2012; Rissman and Wagner, 2012). The multi-variate data produced by large-scale microelectrode recordings combined with machine learning tools will help electrophysiologists make the same transition. Ultimately, better understanding of the large-scale activity patterns underlying behavior will be achieved by combining several of these emerging technologies and analytical techniques with large-scale microelectrode recordings.

Author Contributions

The ideas and content within this article were put together through a collaborative effort by all authors (NMD, BG, RFS, SJH and CMG).

Funding

This work was supported by National Institute of Mental Health Grant MH081162 (CMG), National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke Grant NS059312 (CMG), a McKnight Memory and Cognitive Disorders award (CMG), the Swiss National Science Foundation (RFS), and the Kopriva Foundation (NMD).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Andersen, R. A., Essick, G. K., and Siegel, R. M. (1985). Encoding of spatial location by posterior parietal neurons. Science 230, 456–458. doi: 10.1126/science.4048942

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bastos, A. M., Vezoli, J., Bosman, C. A., Schoffelen, J. M., Oostenveld, R., Dowdall, J. R., et al. (2015). Visual areas exert feedforward and feedback influences through distinct frequency channels. Neuron 85, 390–401. doi: 10.1101/004804

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benjamini, Y. (2010). Simultaneous and selective inference: current successes and future challenges. Biom. J. 52, 708–721. doi: 10.1002/bimj.200900299

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benjamini, Y., and Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J. R. Stat. Soc. Series B Stat. Methodol. 57, 289–300.

Google Scholar

Bennett, C. M., Wolford, G. L., and Miller, M. B. (2009). The principled control of false positives in neuroimaging. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 4, 417–422. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsp053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bosman, C. A., Schoffelen, J. M., Brunet, N., Oostenveld, R., Bastos, A. M., Womelsdorf, T., et al. (2012). Attentional stimulus selection through selective synchronization between monkey visual areas. Neuron 75, 875–888. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.06.037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bressler, S. L. (1995). Large-scale cortical networks and cognition. Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 20, 288–304. doi: 10.1016/0165-0173(94)00016-i

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bressler, S. L., and Kelso, J. A. (2001). Cortical coordination dynamics and cognition. Trends Cogn. Sci. 5, 26–36. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01564-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bressler, S. L., Coppola, R., and Nakamura, R. (1993). Episodic multiregional cortical coherence at multiple frequencies during visual task performance. Nature 366, 153–156. doi: 10.1038/366153a0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buschman, T. J., and Miller, E. K. (2007). Top-down versus bottom-up control of attention in the prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices. Science 315, 1860–1862. doi: 10.1126/science.1138071

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buzsáki, G. (2004). Large-scale recording of neuronal ensembles. Nat. Neurosci. 7, 446–451. doi: 10.1038/nn1233

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buzsáki, G. (2010). Neural syntax: cell assemblies, synapsembles and readers. Neuron 68, 362–385. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.09.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, E. F. (2015). Towards large-scale, human-based, mesoscopic neurotechnologies. Neuron 86, 68–78. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.03.037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chelazzi, L., Miller, E. K., Duncan, J., and Desimone, R. (1993). A neural basis for visual search in inferior temporal cortex. Nature 363, 345–347.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Chialvo, D. R. (2010). Emergent complex neural dynamics. Nat. Phys. 6, 744–750. doi: 10.1038/nphys1803

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chumbley, J. R., and Friston, K. J. (2009). False discovery rate revisited: FDR and topological inference using Gaussian random fields. Neuroimage 44, 62–70. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.05.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Constantinidis, C., and Procyk, E. (2004). The primate working memory networks. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 4, 444–465. doi: 10.3758/cabn.4.4.444

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Courtney, S. M., Ungerleider, L. G., Keil, K., and Haxby, J. V. (1997). Transient and sustained activity in a distributed neural system for human working memory. Nature 386, 608–611. doi: 10.1038/386608a0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dai, J., Brooks, D. I., and Sheinberg, D. L. (2014). Optogenetic and electrical microstimulation systematically bias visuospatial choice in primates. Curr. Biol. 24, 63–69. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.11.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Desimone, R., and Duncan, J. (1995). Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 18, 193–222. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.18.1.193

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diester, I., Kaufman, M. T., Mogri, M., Pashaie, R., Goo, W., Yizhar, O., et al. (2011). An optogenetic toolbox designed for primates. Nat. Neurosci. 14, 387–397. doi: 10.1038/nn.2749

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dotson, N. M., Salazar, R. F., and Gray, C. M. (2014). Frontoparietal correlation dynamics reveal interplay between integration and segregation during visual working memory. J. Neurosci. 34, 13600–13613. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.1961-14.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feingold, J., Desrochers, T. M., Fujii, N., Harlan, R., Tierney, P. L., Shimazu, H., et al. (2012). A system for recording neural activity chronically and simultaneously from multiple cortical and subcortical regions in nonhuman primates. J. Neurophysiol. 107, 1979–1995.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Fries, P. (2005). A mechanism for cognitive dynamics: neuronal communication through neuronal coherence. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 474–480. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.08.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friston, K. J. (1997). Another neural code? Neuroimage 5, 213–220. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1997.0260

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Funahashi, S., Bruce, C. J., and Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1989). Mnemonic coding of visual space in the monkey’s dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 61, 331–349.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Fuster, J. M., and Alexander, G. E. (1971). Neuron activity related to short-term memory. Science 173, 652–654. doi: 10.1126/science.173.3997.652

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gazzaniga, M. S. (2010). Neuroscience and the correct level of explanation for understanding mind. Trends Cogn. Sci. 14, 291–292. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.04.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Genovese, C. R., Lazar, N. A., and Nichols, T. (2002). Thresholding of statistical maps in functional neuroimaging using the false discovery rate. Neuroimage 15, 870–878. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.1037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gerits, A., and Vanduffel, W. (2013). Optogenetics in primates: a shining future? Trends Genet. 29, 403–411. doi: 10.1016/j.tig.2013.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gerits, A., Farivar, R., Rosen, B. R., Wald, L. L., Boyden, E. S., and Vanduffel, W. (2012). Optogenetically induced behavioral and functional network changes in primates. Curr. Biol. 22, 1722–1726. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.07.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gray, C. M. (1994). Synchronous oscillations in neuronal systems: mechanisms and functions. J. Comput. Neurosci. 1, 11–38. doi: 10.1007/bf00962716

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gray, C. M., and Goodell, B. (2011). “Spatiotemporal dynamics of synchronous activity across multiple areas of the visual cortex in the alert monkey, in “The Dynamic Brain: An Exploration of Neuronal Variability and Its Functional Significance” eds M. Ding and D. Glanzman (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 233–254.

Google Scholar

Han, X. (2012). Optogenetics in the nonhuman primate. Prog. Brain Res. 196, 215–233.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Han, X., Qian, X., Bernstein, J. G., Zhou, H. H., Franzesi, G. T., Stern, P., et al. (2009). Millisecond-timescale optical control of neural dynamics in the nonhuman primate brain. Neuron 62, 191–198. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.03.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harrison, S. A., and Tong, F. (2009). Decoding reveals the contents of visual working memory in early visual areas. Nature 458, 632–635. doi: 10.1038/nature07832

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herrero, J. L., Roberts, M. J., Delicato, L. S., Gieselmann, M. A., Dayan, P., and Thiele, A. (2008). Acetylcholine contributes through muscarinic receptors to attentional modulation in V1. Nature 454, 1110–1114. doi: 10.1038/nature07141

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herrington, T. M., and Assad, J. A. (2010). Temporal sequence of attentional modulation in the lateral intraparietal area and middle temporal area during rapid covert shifts of attention. J. Neurosci. 30, 3287–3296. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.6025-09.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoffman, K. L., and McNaughton, B. L. (2002). Coordinated reactivation of distributed memory traces in primate neocortex. Science 297, 2070–2073. doi: 10.1126/science.1073538

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jordan, M. I., and Mitchell, T. M. (2015). Machine learning: trends, perspectives and prospects. Science 349, 255–260. doi: 10.1126/science.aaa8415

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kipke, D. R., Shain, W., Buzsáki, G., Fetz, E., Henderson, J. M., Hetke, J. F., et al. (2008). Advanced neurotechnologies for chronic neural interfaces: new horizons and clinical opportunities. J. Neurosci. 28, 11830–11838. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.3879-08.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lewis, C. M., Bosman, C. A., and Fries, P. (2015). Recording of brain activity across spatial scales. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 32, 68–77. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2014.12.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Logan, B. R., and Rowe, D. B. (2004). An evaluation of thresholding techniques in fMRI analysis. Neuroimage 22, 95–108. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.12.047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maris, E., and Oostenveld, R. (2007). Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG-and MEG-data. J. Neurosci. Methods 164, 177–190. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2007.03.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Markowitz, D. A., Wong, Y. T., Gray, C. M., and Pesaran, B. (2011). Optimizing the decoding of movement goals from local field potentials in macaque cortex. J. Neurosci. 31, 18412–18422.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Maynard, E. M., Nordhausen, C. T., and Normann, R. A. (1997). The Utah intracortical electrode array: a recording structure for potential brain-computer interfaces. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 102, 228–239. doi: 10.1016/s0013-4694(96)95176-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mendoza-Halliday, D., Torres, S., and Martinez-Trujillo, J. (2014). Sharp emergence of working memories along the primate dorsal visual pathway. J. Vis. 14, 168–168. doi: 10.1167/14.10.168

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mesulam, M. M. (1990). Large-scale neurocognitive networks and distributed processing for attention, language and memory. Ann. Neurol. 28, 597–613. doi: 10.1002/ana.410280502

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, E. K., and Buschman, T. J. (2007). Response to comment on “top-down versus bottom-up control of attention in the prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices”. Science 318, 44–44. doi: 10.1126/science.1145017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moran, J., and Desimone, R. (1985). Selective attention gates visual processing in the extrastriate cortex. Science 229, 782–784. doi: 10.1126/science.4023713

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Munk, M. H., Linden, D. E., Muckli, L., Lanfermann, H., Zanella, F. E., Singer, W., et al. (2002). Distributed cortical systems in visual short-term memory revealed by event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging. Cereb. Cortex 12, 866–876. doi: 10.1093/cercor/12.8.866

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nichols, T. E. (2012). Multiple testing corrections, nonparametric methods and random field theory. Neuroimage 62, 811–815. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.04.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nicolelis, M. A., Dimitrov, D., Carmena, J. M., Crist, R., Lehew, G., Kralik, J. D., et al. (2003). Chronic, multisite, multielectrode recordings in macaque monkeys. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 100, 11041–11046. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1934665100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nicolelis, M. A., Ghazanfar, A. A., Stambaugh, C. R., Oliveira, L. M., Laubach, M., Chapin, J. K., et al. (1998). Simultaneous encoding of tactile information by three primate cortical areas. Nat. Neurosci. 1, 621–630. doi: 10.1038/2855

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Noudoost, B., and Moore, T. (2011a). Control of visual cortical signals by prefrontal dopamine. Nature 474, 372–375. doi: 10.1038/nature09995

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Noudoost, B., and Moore, T. (2011b). The role of neuromodulators in selective attention. Trends Cogn. Sci. 15, 585–591. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.10.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pereira, F., Mitchell, T., and Botvinick, M. (2009). Machine learning classifiers and fMRI: a tutorial overview. Neuroimage 45, S199–S209. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.11.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perneger, T. V. (1998). What’s wrong with Bonferroni adjustments. BMJ 316, 1236–1238. doi: 10.4324/9781315072609

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pesaran, B., Nelson, M. J., and Andersen, R. A. (2008). Free choice activates a decision circuit between frontal and parietal cortex. Nature 453, 406–409. doi: 10.1038/nature06849

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Poldrack, R. A. (2012). The future of fMRI in cognitive neuroscience. Neuroimage 62, 1216–1220. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.08.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Postle, B. R. (2006). Working memory as an emergent property of the mind and brain. Neuroscience 139, 23–38. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.06.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rissman, J., and Wagner, A. D. (2012). Distributed representations in memory: insights from functional brain imaging. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 63, 101–128. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100344

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rothman, K. J. (1990). No adjustments are needed for multiple comparisons. Epidemiology 1, 43–46. doi: 10.1097/00001648-199001000-00010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roux, F., and Uhlhaas, P. J. (2014). Working memory and neural oscillations: alpha-gamma versus theta-gamma codes for distinct WM information? Trends Cogn. Sci. 18, 16–25. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.10.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salazar, R. F., Dotson, N. M., Bressler, S. L., and Gray, C. M. (2012). Content-specific fronto-parietal synchronization during visual working memory. Science 338, 1097–1100. doi: 10.1126/science.1224000

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schall, J. D., Paré, M., and Woodman, G. F. (2007). Comment on “top-down Versus bottom-up control of attention in the prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices”. Science 318, 44–44. doi: 10.1126/science.1144865

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwarz, D. A., Lebedev, M. A., Hanson, T. L., Dimitrov, D. F., Lehew, G., Meloy, J., et al. (2014). Chronic, wireless recordings of large-scale brain activity in freely moving rhesus monkeys. Nat. Methods 11, 670–676. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.2936

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shaffer, J. P. (1995). Multiple hypothesis testing. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 46, 561–584. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ps.46.020195.003021

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siegel, M., Buschman, T. J., and Miller, E. K. (2015). Cortical information flow during flexible sensorimotor decisions. Science 348, 1352–1355. doi: 10.1126/science.aab0551

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siegel, M., Donner, T. H., and Engel, A. K. (2012). Spectral fingerprints of large-scale neuronal interactions. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 121–134. doi: 10.1038/nrn3137

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Singer, W. (2013). Cortical dynamics revisited. Trends Cogn. Sci. 17, 616–626. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sternson, S. M., and Roth, B. L. (2014). Chemogenetic tools to interrogate brain functions. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 37, 387–407. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-071013-014048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tognoli, E., and Kelso, J. A. (2014). The metastable brain. Neuron 81, 35–48. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.12.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tukey, J. W. (1991). The philosophy of multiple comparisons. Statist. Sci. 6, 100–116. doi: 10.1214/ss/1177011945

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vardy, E., Robinson, J. E., Li, C., Olsen, R. H., DiBerto, J. F., Giguere, P. M., et al. (2015). A new DREADD facilitates the multiplexed chemogenetic interrogation of behavior. Neuron 86, 936–9s46. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.03.065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Varela, F., Lachaux, J. P., Rodriguez, E., and Martinerie, J. (2001). The brainweb: phase synchronization and large-scale integration. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2, 229–239. doi: 10.1038/35067550

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yanagawa, T., Chao, Z. C., Hasegawa, N., and Fujii, N. (2013). Large-scale information flow in conscious and unconscious states: an ECoG study in monkeys. PLoS One 8:e80845. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0080845

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: electrophysiology, microelectrode, non-human primate, methods, spatiotemporal activity, large-scale recordings

Citation: Dotson NM, Goodell B, Salazar RF, Hoffman SJ and Gray CM (2015) Methods, caveats and the future of large-scale microelectrode recordings in the non-human primate. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 9:149. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2015.00149

Received: 02 September 2015; Accepted: 19 October 2015;
Published: 03 November 2015.

Edited by:

Andre M. Bastos, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, USA

Reviewed by:

Christopher I. Petkov, Newcastle University, UK
Conrado Arturo Bosman, University of Amsterdam, Netherlands

Copyright © 2015 Dotson, Goodell, Salazar, Hoffman and Gray. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution and reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Nicholas M. Dotson, bmljaG9sYXMuZG90c29uQGxpdmUuY29t

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.