Skip to main content

PERSPECTIVE article

Front. Syst. Neurosci., 20 August 2015
This article is part of the Research Topic The Dentate Gyrus and its Local Circuits View all 12 articles

Adult hippocampal neurogenesis and pattern separation in DG: a role for feedback inhibition in modulating sparseness to govern population-based coding

\r\nKathleen McAvoy&#x;Kathleen McAvoy1†Antoine Besnard&#x;Antoine Besnard1†Amar Sahay,,*Amar Sahay1,2,3*
  • 1Center for Regenerative Medicine, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
  • 2Harvard Stem Cell Institute, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, USA
  • 3Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA

The dentate gyrus (DG) of mammals harbors neural stem cells that generate new dentate granule cells (DGCs) throughout life. Behavioral studies using the contextual fear discrimination paradigm have found that selectively augmenting or blocking adult hippocampal neurogenesis enhances or impairs discrimination under conditions of high, but not low, interference suggestive of a role in pattern separation. Although contextual discrimination engages population-based coding mechanisms underlying pattern separation such as global remapping in the DG and CA3, how adult hippocampal neurogenesis modulates pattern separation in the DG is poorly understood. Here, we propose a role for adult-born DGCs in re-activation coupled modulation of sparseness through feed-back inhibition to govern global remapping in the DG.

Introduction

The dentate gyrus (DG) of all mammals harbors neural stem cells that generate new dentate granule cells (DGCs) throughout life (Altman and Das, 1965; Kaplan and Hinds, 1977; Cameron et al., 1993; Kuhn et al., 1996; Eriksson et al., 1998; Seri et al., 2001; Knoth et al., 2010; Spalding et al., 2013). The differentiation of neural stem cells into mature DGCs is marked by changes in physiological properties and connectivity (Zhao et al., 2008; Ming and Song, 2011; Aimone et al., 2014). Whereas young (4–6 weeks old) adult-born DGCs are generally thought to exhibit low input specificity, heightened synaptic plasticity and excitability, mature 6–8 weeks old adult-born DGCs are more similar to developmentally generated DGCs with high input specificity, lower excitability and synaptic plasticity (Schmidt-Hieber et al., 2004; Espósito et al., 2005; Laplagne et al., 2006, 2007; Ge et al., 2007, 2008; Gu et al., 2012). Investigations into the functions of adult hippocampal neurogenesis have suggested numerous roles including the modulation of interference (Becker, 2005; Wiskott et al., 2006; Becker and Wojtowicz, 2007; Garthe et al., 2009; Deng et al., 2010; Sahay et al., 2011a; Burghardt et al., 2012), memory resolution (Aimone et al., 2011), input specific re-activation (Tashiro et al., 2007; Aimone et al., 2011), memory persistence (Arruda-Carvalho et al., 2011; McAvoy et al., 2014; Wang et al., 2014) and forgetting (Chambers et al., 2004; Deisseroth et al., 2004; Weisz and Argibay, 2012; Akers et al., 2014). Behavioral studies using the contextual fear discrimination paradigm have found that selectively augmenting or blocking adult hippocampal neurogenesis enhances or impairs discrimination under conditions of high, but not low, interference (Sahay et al., 2011b; Kheirbek et al., 2012; Nakashiba et al., 2012; Niibori et al., 2012; Tronel et al., 2012). Studies using a delayed non-match to place paradigm and touch screen based object spacing detection assays have obtained similar results in some, but not all studies (Clelland et al., 2009; Pan et al., 2012; Groves et al., 2013; Swan et al., 2014; Zhang et al., 2014). However, the neural mechanisms by which adult hippocampal neurogenesis modulates interference in these tasks are poorly understood (Yassa and Stark, 2011; Piatti et al., 2013; Lepousez et al., 2015; Wadiche and Overstreet-Wadiche, 2015).

Theoretical and experimental studies have posited a critical role for the DG in modulating interference between similar inputs through pattern separation, a process by which similar inputs are made more distinct during storage (McNaughton and Morris, 1987; O’Reilly and McClelland, 1994; Gilbert et al., 2001; Rolls and Kesner, 2006; Bakker et al., 2008; Treves et al., 2008). At a network level, pattern separation in the DG is supported by encoding of similar inputs by differential firing rates of place cells (rate remapping) or the recruitment of non-overlapping ensembles of neurons (or global remapping; Leutgeb et al., 2007; Neunuebel et al., 2013; Neunuebel and Knierim, 2014). Although discrimination of similar contexts (Niibori et al., 2012; Deng et al., 2013; Czerniawski and Guzowski, 2014) elicits global remapping in the DG and CA3 and blockade of adult hippocampal neurogenesis impairs population-based coding in CA3 (Niibori et al., 2012), evidence for how adult-born DGCs contribute to rate remapping and global remapping in the DG is absent. One feature of the DG that has long been recognized as conducive for pattern separation is sparseness of activity (Treves and Rolls, 1992; Jung and McNaughton, 1993; McClelland and Goddard, 1996; Chawla et al., 2005; Colgin et al., 2008; Pernia-Andrade and Jonas, 2014). A sparse coding scheme is thought to support pattern separation by facilitating the recruitment of non-overlapping populations of neurons to encode similar inputs (Treves and Rolls, 1992; McClelland and Goddard, 1996; Colgin et al., 2008; Faghihi and Moustafa, 2015; Petrantonakis and Poirazi, 2015). We recently proposed that adult-born DGCs modulate sparseness of activity in the DG through recruitment of feed-back inhibition to influence pattern separation (Sahay et al., 2011a). We hypothesized that young adult-born DGCs recruit feed-back inhibition via mossy cells and hilar interneurons to dictate sparseness of activity in the DG and this in turn, influences global remapping. Here, we discuss two recent experimental studies that have begun to address this idea and integrate insights gleaned from these studies to formulate a proposal for how adult-born DGCs influence global remapping in the DG.

Experimental Advances Linking Adult Hippocampal Neurogenesis, Sparseness and Feed-Back Inhibition

As a first step towards addressing how adult hippocampal neurogenesis may facilitate global remapping, we sought to test whether levels of adult hippocampal neurogenesis affect sparseness of activity in the DG (Ikrar et al., 2013). We used voltage sensitive dye imaging (VSDI) in combination with laser photostimulation and electrical stimulation of the granule cell layer (GCL) to visualize depolarization induced spread of the voltage sensitive dye (VSD) signal in ex vivo slices from mice in which either adult hippocampal neurogenesis was blocked by targeted x-irradiation or enhanced by genetic deletion of the pro-apoptotic gene Bax in adult neural stem cells. We found that genetically enhancing the number of adult-born DGCs (8 weeks of age and younger) engendered a reduction in spread of VSD signal and strength of neuronal activation in the DG. Conversely, ablating 14 weeks of age and younger adult-born DGCs produced a trend towards increased excitability of the DG. In these experiments the change in the VSD signal was seen throughout the GCL suggesting a non-cell autonomous role for adult-born DGCs in modulating the activation of mature DGCs. Mossy fibers of DGCs synapse onto hilar interneurons and mossy cells, both of which can modulate the activity of the DG. Activation of hilar interneurons and mossy cells is thought to promote local inhibition and long-range activation and inhibition of DGCs, respectively (Freund and Buzsáki, 1996; Scharfman and Myers, 2013; Hu et al., 2014). Changing levels of adult hippocampal neurogenesis did not appear to affect miniature inhibitory post-synaptic currents in the DG; but increasing the number of 4 weeks old adult-born DGCs increased their connectivity with hilar interneurons (Ikrar et al., 2013). Together, these observations demonstrate that levels of adult hippocampal neurogenesis modulate sparseness of activity in the DG as assessed by changes in excitability and suggest a role for excitatory drive onto hilar interneurons as an underlying circuit mechanism (Ikrar et al., 2013). However, whether expanding or ablating populations of adult-born DGCs of various ages differentially affects excitability of the DG was not addressed. In this regard, it is noteworthy to emphasize that a previous report found that genetic ablation of the population of 8 weeks and younger adult-born DGCs decreased sparseness of activity in the DG in vivo only under conditions of high interference in memory processing (Burghardt et al., 2012). The use of ex vivo slices in our study precluded assessment of the contributions of the commissural-associational pathway that conveys feed-forward excitation and inhibition onto DGCs (Myers and Scharfman, 2009; Jinde et al., 2012; Scharfman and Myers, 2013). Finally, our study did not causally examine whether adult-born DGCs recruit feed-back inhibition to modulate excitability of the DG and whether adult-born DGCs of different ages differentially recruit feed-back inhibition.

Temprana et al. (2015) succeeded in illuminating some of these key unaddressed questions. The authors employed retroviruses to express the light activated channelrhodopsin in adult-born DGCs and examined the impact on activating 4 weeks old or 8 weeks old adult-born DGCs on inhibition of DGCs in slices ex vivo. The authors found that pre-activation of 4 weeks old or 8 weeks old adult-born DGCS caused a reduction in spiking in the GCL, but the reduction after pre-activation of 4 weeks old DGCs was half the magnitude of reduction in spiking after pre-activation of 8 weeks old adult-born DGCs. However, chemogenetic activation of 8 weeks old adult-born DGCs, unlike activation of 4 weeks old adult-born DGCs, activated hilar parvalbumin (PV) interneurons in vivo. Whether activation of 4 weeks old adult-born DGCs activate other populations of hilar interneurons that mediate feed-back inhibition onto the DG was not addressed. These observations suggest that 4 weeks old adult-born DGCs may exert modest feed-back inhibition through a potentially different mechanism, at least ex vivo. As with our study, the use of ex vivo slices precluded the possibility of addressing the potentially important contribution of mossy cells to adult-born DGC dependent recruitment of feed-back inhibition in vivo. Nevertheless, the findings of this study unequivocally demonstrate that 8 weeks old adult-born DGCs exert significantly greater feed-back inhibition onto the DG than 4 weeks old adult-born DGCs ex vivo (Temprana et al., 2015). Whether 8 weeks old adult-born DGCs exert different levels of feed-back inhibition from that recruited by developmentally generated DGCs remains to be addressed. Although specific features of input connectivity and physiology are conserved between 8 weeks old adult-born DGCs and developmentally generated DGCs, not all afferent- and efferent-connectivity of these populations have been systematically characterized. Ontogenic analysis of connectivity of hippocampal neurons has revealed differences in connectivity with interneurons suggesting that 8 weeks old adult-born DGCs and developmentally born DGCs may differ in input specificity and output connectivity (Donato et al., 2015). Importantly, it will be critical to determine whether DGCs of different ages are differentially recruited across stimulation intensities to exert feed-back inhibition and modulate sparseness of activity in the DG in vivo. Guided by their data and previous models (Aimone et al., 2011, 2014; Temprana et al., 2015) developed a computational model proposing how increased recruitment of feed-back inhibition coupled with increased input specificity and decreased excitability during maturation, enables 8 weeks old adult-born DGCs to faithfully respond to familiar inputs.

Model Linking Adult Hippocampal Neurogenesis, Sparseness and Feed-Back Inhibition with Global Remapping in DG

Predictions of how adult hippocampal neurogenesis modulates global remapping in the DG must take into account the extent to which adult-born and developmentally generated DGCs contribute to memory traces or engrams. Cellular and genetic approaches to tag activated neurons during specific epochs of encoding such as during exposure to two similar contexts have found that the vast majority of activated neurons reside in the outer two-thirds of the GCL (Deng et al., 2013; Tronel et al., 2015). Deng and Gage found that DGCs recruited for global remapping during contextual encoding were probably older than 6 weeks of age and largely located where developmentally generated DGCs reside (Altman and Bayer, 1990; Espósito et al., 2005; Laplagne et al., 2006; Mathews et al., 2010; Deng et al., 2013). A second study by Tronel et al. (2015) found that activation of developmentally generated DGCs is more sensitive to changes in context than 14 weeks old adult-born DGCs. Although the contribution of adult-born DGCs at different stages of maturation to global remapping has not been directly examined, post-training ablation and silencing studies suggest that 4 weeks and older adult-born DGCs are part of the contextual memory trace (Arruda-Carvalho et al., 2011; Gu et al., 2012; Wang et al., 2014). Preliminary studies from our laboratory suggest that expanding the population of 5–8 weeks old adult-born DGCs maintains long-term contextual fear memory suggesting a role for this population in maintenance of the contextual memory trace (McAvoy et al., 2014). Together, these observations predict that developmentally generated and mature adult-born DGCs are part of the ensembles recruited in global remapping to generate engrams.

Current evidence from studies in mice support differential engagement of feed-back inhibition (Temprana et al., 2015), modulation of DG excitability and sparseness (Ikrar et al., 2013), differences in input specificity (Neunuebel and Knierim, 2012, 2014) and excitability (Schmidt-Hieber et al., 2004; Espósito et al., 2005; Laplagne et al., 2006, 2007; Ge et al., 2007, 2008; Gu et al., 2012) of 4–6 weeks old adult-born DGCs and mature (>6 weeks old) adult-born DGCs). Based on these observations and two recently developed models (Aimone et al., 2011; Kropff et al., 2015; Temprana et al., 2015), we propose that adult-born DGCs contribute to global remapping in DG by expanding the capacity of the DG to encode new information and ensuring high fidelity of re-activation that is coupled with regulation of sparseness through feed-back inhibition. Our model is predicated on two central assumptions. First, the maturation of excitable, immature (4–6 weeks) adult-born DGCs is accompanied by refinement of their synaptic inputs through hebbian learning and competition for neural representation to generate (>4–6 weeks) mature DGCs with high input specificity. The idea is that increasing the number of encoding units available for encoding will promote competition for synaptic inputs and extraction of distinct features to minimize interference (Aimone et al., 2011, 2014; Neunuebel and Knierim, 2012; Kropff et al., 2015; Temprana et al., 2015). Second, limited experimental evidence suggests that adult-born DGCs once mature (>6 weeks of age) are preferentially re-activated by inputs to which they were exposed to when younger (4–6 weeks of age; Tashiro et al., 2007; Aimone et al., 2011). Because the contextual discrimination task in which mice are challenged to discriminate between a training context (Context A) associated with a foot shock and a safe, similar context (Context B; Figure 1A) has been shown to be sensitive to levels of adult hippocampal neurogenesis and involves global remapping in the DG, we will use it to convey our model for how adult-born DGCs contribute to global remapping in the DG. We lead the reader through how changing levels of adult hippocampal neurogenesis affects the engram of context A and context B to dictate the extent of global remapping in the DG (Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Model illustrating how levels of adult hippocampal neurogenesis dictate population-based coding in DG to minimize interference between engrams of two similar contexts. (A) Context A is made up of features that are unique to context A (A1, A2) and common to contexts A and B (A3, A4). Context B is made up of features that are unique to context B (B1, B2) of which B2 is similar to A1. (B) High levels of neurogenesis: Exposure to context A (time point t1) activates 4–6 weeks adult born DGCs (blue circles). The activation of these 4–6 weeks adult born DGCs recruits feed-back inhibition to increase sparseness in the DG and promotes encoding of features of context A in developmentally born DGCs (brown circles). The engram for context A is made up of adult-born DGCs that have matured (time point t2) and through Hebbian learning and competition for representation have acquired high input specificity for features of context A (green circles with A1 or A2 or A3 or A4) and developmentally born DGCs that encode features of context A (brown circles with A1 or A2 or A3 or A4). (C) High levels of neurogenesis: Exposure to context B re-activates DGCs that have encoded features common to both contexts A and B (Green and brown circles with *A3 or *A4). Re-activation of these DGCs together with activation of 4–6 weeks old adult-born DGCs exerts strong feed-back inhibition onto DG, increases sparseness and promotes encoding of features of context B in individual DGCs. Additionally and importantly, there is lower likelihood that DGCs that have encoded unique features of context A (A1) that are similar to features of context B (B2) are re-activated to encode these features. This results in an engram of context B that has little overlap with engram of context A with the exception of DGCs that encode features common to contexts A and B. (D) Low levels of neurogenesis: Exposure to context A (time point t1) activates a small number of 4–6 weeks adult born DGCs (blue circles). The activation of this reduced number of DGCs recruits less feed-back inhibition, decreases sparseness in the DG and promotes encoding of features of A in many more developmentally born DGCs (brown circles). The engram for context A is made up of adult-born DGCs that have matured (time point t2) and have failed to acquired high input specificity for features of context A (green circles with A1–A2 or A3–A4) and developmentally born DGCs that encode features of context A (brown circles). (E) Exposure to context B re-activates DGCs that have encoded features common to both contexts A and B (Green and brown circles with *A3 or *A4). Furthermore, because of reduced feed-back inhibition recruited by small number of 4–6 weeks old adult-born DGCs and the re-activated mature adult-born DGCs, sparseness in DG is decreased. This, in turn, increases the likelihood that developmentally born DGCs that have encoded features of context A (A1) that are similar to features of context B (B2) are re-activated. Consequently, the engram of context B has significantly more overlap with engram of context A relative to mice with high levels of adult hippocampal neurogenesis.

With high levels of adult hippocampal neurogenesis, features of a novel context (A) are encoded by 4–6 weeks old adult-born DGCs (time point t1). Maturation of this population of 4–6 weeks old adult-born DGCs generates a population of mature adult-born DGCs (>4–6 weeks of age) with high input specificity for features of context A. In addition, feed-back inhibition recruited by 4–6 weeks old adult-born DGCs ensures sparseness of activation in the DG (Figure 1B; time point t2). The engram of context A (time point t2, when 4–6 weeks old adult-born DGCs mature) is made up of adult-born DGCs older than >4–6 weeks of age with high input specificity and developmentally born DGCs. Conversely, with low levels of adult hippocampal neurogenesis, context A is encoded by a smaller pool of 4–6 weeks old adult-born DGCs which mature into DGCs with lower input specificity for features of context A (Figure 1D). Decreased feed-back inhibition (due to a smaller pool of 4–6 weeks old adult-born DGCs) decreases sparseness of the DG and this results in many more developmentally generated DGCs active during encoding of features of context A. Thus, the engrams of context A is made up of adult adult-born DGCs older than >4–6 weeks of age with low input specificity and developmentally born DGCs (Figure 1D, t2).

Upon exposure to a similar context B that shares features with context A (Figure 1A, A3 and A4 are features shared by contexts A and B), developmentally generated and mature (>4–6 weeks of age) adult-born DGCs are re-activated by features shared by contexts A and B. In addition, developmentally generated DGCs that encoded features shared by contexts A and B are also re-activated. With high levels of adult hippocampal neurogenesis there is greater feed-back inhibition recruited by re-activated mature (>4–6 weeks of age) adult-born DGCs and the immature (4–6 weeks old) DGCs that encode novel features of context B than with decreased adult hippocampal neurogenesis (Figures 1C,E). Greater sparseness will promote global remapping since the likelihood of developmentally generated DGCs that have encoded unique features of A to be re-activated by context B will be statistically lower. Thus, levels of adult hippocampal neurogenesis will dictate the overlap in engrams of contexts A and B.

The predictions of this model are readily testable and will necessitate addressing the following questions. First, how does enhancing or silencing populations of adult-born DGCs at distinct stages of maturation or developmentally generated DGCs, impact global remapping in the contextual discrimination paradigm in vivo? Second, do adult-born DGCs at distinct stages of maturation or developmentally generated DGCs differentially modulate sparseness and recruit different levels of feed-back inhibition in vivo? Third, how are mossy cells and specific populations of interneurons recruited by DGCs to modulate sparseness and global remapping? The complexity of addressing this undertaking is underscored by the fact that engagement of hilar interneurons and mossy cells may be biased by different neuromodulatory systems that innervate these cell types and firing patterns of dentate granule neurons (Freund and Buzsáki, 1996; Torborg et al., 2010). Thus, behavioral state or cognitive demand will dictate the extent to which feed-back inhibition is effectively recruited to modulate sparseness and global remapping. Visualizing ensembles of activated DGCs by calcium imaging in vivo, genetic tagging using immediate early genes, cellular compartment analysis of temporal activity by fluorescent in-situ hybridization (catFISH), or place cells recordings in vivo with concomitant assessment of identity will unequivocally enable direct assessment of DGC re-activation. Together, these approaches will permit resolution of how ablating or expanding populations of adult-born DGCs of different ages influences global remapping in the DG.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

AB is supported by 2014 NARSAD Young Investigator Award, Bettencourt-Schueller Foundation and Philippe Foundation Award. AS is supported by US National Institutes of Health Biobehavioral Research Awards for Innovative New Scientists (BRAINS) 1-R01MH104175, and the Ellison Medical Foundation New Scholar in Aging, Whitehall Foundation, Inscopix Decode and HSCI Development grants.

References

Aimone, J. B., Deng, W., and Gage, F. H. (2011). Resolving new memories: a critical look at the dentate gyrus, adult neurogenesis and pattern separation. Neuron 70, 589–596. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.05.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aimone, J. B., Li, Y., Lee, S. W., Clemenson, G. D., Deng, W., and Gage, F. H. (2014). Regulation and function of adult neurogenesis: from genes to cognition. Physiol. Rev. 94, 991–1026. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00004.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Akers, K. G., Martinez-Canabal, A., Restivo, L., Yiu, A. P., De Cristofaro, A., Hsiang, H. L., et al. (2014). Hippocampal neurogenesis regulates forgetting during adulthood and infancy. Science 344, 598–602. doi: 10.1126/science.1248903

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Altman, J., and Bayer, S. A. (1990). Migration and distribution of two populations of hippocampal granule cell precursors during the perinatal and postnatal periods. J. Comp. Neurol. 301, 365–381. doi: 10.1002/cne.903010304

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Altman, J., and Das, G. D. (1965). Autoradiographic and histological evidence of postnatal hippocampal neurogenesis in rats. J. Comp. Neurol. 124, 319–335. doi: 10.1002/cne.901240303

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arruda-Carvalho, M., Sakaguchi, M., Akers, K. G., Josselyn, S. A., and Frankland, P. W. (2011). Posttraining ablation of adult-generated neurons degrades previously acquired memories. J. Neurosci. 31, 15113–15127. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3432-11.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bakker, A., Kirwan, C. B., Miller, M., and Stark, C. E. (2008). Pattern separation in the human hippocampal CA3 and dentate gyrus. Science 319, 1640–1642. doi: 10.1126/science.1152882

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Becker, S. (2005). A computational principle for hippocampal learning and neurogenesis. Hippocampus 15, 722–738. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20095

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Becker, S., and Wojtowicz, J. M. (2007). A model of hippocampal neurogenesis in memory and mood disorders. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 70–76. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.10.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burghardt, N. S., Park, E. H., Hen, R., and Fenton, A. A. (2012). Adult-born hippocampal neurons promote cognitive flexibility in mice. Hippocampus 22, 1795–1808. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cameron, H. A., Woolley, C. S., McEwen, B. S., and Gould, E. (1993). Differentiation of newly born neurons and glia in the dentate gyrus of the adult rat. Neuroscience 56, 337–344. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(93)90335-d

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chambers, R. A., Potenza, M. N., Hoffman, R. E., and Miranker, W. (2004). Simulated apoptosis/neurogenesis regulates learning and memory capabilities of adaptive neural networks. Neuropsychopharmacology 29, 747–758. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300358

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chawla, M. K., Guzowski, J. F., Ramirez-Amaya, V., Lipa, P., Hoffman, K. L., Marriott, L. K., et al. (2005). Sparse, environmentally selective expression of Arc RNA in the upper blade of the rodent fascia dentata by brief spatial experience. Hippocampus 15, 579–586. doi: 10.3410/f.1026564.335793

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clelland, C. D., Choi, M., Romberg, C., Clemenson, G. D. Jr., Fragniere, A., Tyers, P., et al. (2009). A functional role for adult hippocampal neurogenesis in spatial pattern separation. Science 325, 210–213. doi: 10.1126/science.1173215

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Colgin, L. L., Moser, E. I., and Moser, M. B. (2008). Understanding memory through hippocampal remapping. Trends Neurosci. 31, 469–477. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2008.06.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Czerniawski, J., and Guzowski, J. F. (2014). Acute neuroinflammation impairs context discrimination memory and disrupts pattern separation processes in hippocampus. J. Neurosci. 34, 12470–12480. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.0542-14.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deisseroth, K., Singla, S., Toda, H., Monje, M., Palmer, T. D., and Malenka, R. C. (2004). Excitation-neurogenesis coupling in adult neural stem/progenitor cells. Neuron 42, 535–552. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(04)00266-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deng, W., Aimone, J. B., and Gage, F. H. (2010). New neurons and new memories: how does adult hippocampal neurogenesis affect learning and memory? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 339–350. doi: 10.1038/nrn2822

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deng, W., Mayford, M., and Gage, F. H. (2013). Selection of distinct populations of dentate granule cells in response to inputs as a mechanism for pattern separation in mice. Elife 2:e00312. doi: 10.7554/elife.00312

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Donato, F., Chowdhury, A., Lahr, M., and Caroni, P. (2015). Early- and late-born parvalbumin basket cell subpopulations exhibiting distinct regulation and roles in learning. Neuron 85, 770–786. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.01.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eriksson, P. S., Perfilieva, E., Björk-Eriksson, T., Alborn, A. M., Nordborg, C., Peterson, D. A., et al. (1998). Neurogenesis in the adult human hippocampus. Nat. Med. 4, 1313–1317. doi: 10.1038/3305

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Espósito, M. S., Piatti, V. C., Laplagne, D. A., Morgenstern, N. A., Ferrari, C. C., Pitossi, F. J., et al. (2005). Neuronal differentiation in the adult hippocampus recapitulates embryonic development. J. Neurosci. 25, 10074–10086. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.3114-05.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faghihi, F., and Moustafa, A. A. (2015). A computational model of pattern separation efficiency in the dentate gyrus with implications in schizophrenia. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 9:42. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2015.00042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Freund, T. F., and Buzsáki, G. (1996). Interneurons of the hippocampus. Hippocampus 6, 347–470. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1098-1063(1996)6:4<347::aid-hipo1>3.0.co;2-i

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garthe, A., Behr, J., and Kempermann, G. (2009). Adult-generated hippocampal neurons allow the flexible use of spatially precise learning strategies. PLoS One 4:e5464. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0005464

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ge, S., Sailor, K. A., Ming, G. L., and Song, H. (2008). Synaptic integration and plasticity of new neurons in the adult hippocampus. J. Physiol. 586, 3759–3765. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2008.155655

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ge, S., Yang, C. H., Hsu, K. S., Ming, G. L., and Song, H. (2007). A critical period for enhanced synaptic plasticity in newly generated neurons of the adult brain. Neuron 54, 559–566. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.05.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gilbert, P. E., Kesner, R. P., and Lee, I. (2001). Dissociating hippocampal subregions: double dissociation between dentate gyrus and CA1. Hippocampus 11, 626–636. doi: 10.1002/hipo.1077

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Groves, J. O., Leslie, I., Huang, G. J., McHugh, S. B., Taylor, A., Mott, R., et al. (2013). Ablating adult neurogenesis in the rat has no effect on spatial processing: evidence from a novel pharmacogenetic model. PLoS Genet. 9:e1003718. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1003718

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gu, Y., Arruda-Carvalho, M., Wang, J., Janoschka, S. R., Josselyn, S. A., Frankland, P. W., et al. (2012). Optical controlling reveals time-dependent roles for adult-born dentate granule cells. Nat. Neurosci. 15, 1700–1706. doi: 10.1038/nn.3260

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, H., Gan, J., and Jonas, P. (2014). Interneurons. Fast-spiking, parvalbumin(+) GABAergic interneurons: from cellular design to microcircuit function. Science 345:1255263. doi: 10.1126/science.1255263

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ikrar, T., Guo, N., He, K., Besnard, A., Levinson, S., Hill, A., et al. (2013). Adult neurogenesis modifies excitability of the dentate gyrus. Front. Neural Circuits 7:204. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2013.00204

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jinde, S., Zsiros, V., Jiang, Z., Nakao, K., Pickel, J., Kohno, K., et al. (2012). Hilar mossy cell degeneration causes transient dentate granule cell hyperexcitability and impaired pattern separation. Neuron 76, 1189–1200. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.10.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jung, M. W., and McNaughton, B. L. (1993). Spatial selectivity of unit activity in the hippocampal granular layer. Hippocampus 3, 165–182. doi: 10.1002/hipo.450030209

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaplan, M. S., and Hinds, J. W. (1977). Neurogenesis in the adult rat: electron microscopic analysis of light radioautographs. Science 197, 1092–1094. doi: 10.1126/science.887941

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kheirbek, M. A., Tannenholz, L., and Hen, R. (2012). NR2B-dependent plasticity of adult-born granule cells is necessary for context discrimination. J. Neurosci. 32, 8696–8702. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.1692-12.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knoth, R., Singec, I., Ditter, M., Pantazis, G., Capetian, P., Meyer, R. P., et al. (2010). Murine features of neurogenesis in the human hippocampus across the lifespan from 0 to 100 years. PLoS One 5:e8809. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0008809

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kropff, E., Yang, S. M., and Schinder, A. F. (2015). Dynamic role of adult-born dentate granule cells in memory processing. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 35, 21–26. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2015.06.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuhn, H. G., Dickinson-Anson, H., and Gage, F. H. (1996). Neurogenesis in the dentate gyrus of the adult rat: age-related decrease of neuronal progenitor proliferation. J. Neurosci. 16, 2027–2033.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Laplagne, D. A., Espósito, M. S., Piatti, V. C., Morgenstern, N. A., Zhao, C., van Praag, H., et al. (2006). Functional convergence of neurons generated in the developing and adult hippocampus. PLoS Biol. 4:e409. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0040409

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laplagne, D. A., Kamienkowski, J. E., Espósito, M. S., Piatti, V. C., Zhao, C., Gage, F. H., et al. (2007). Similar GABAergic inputs in dentate granule cells born during embryonic and adult neurogenesis. Eur. J. Neurosci. 25, 2973–2981. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05549.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lepousez, G., Nissant, A., and Lledo, P. M. (2015). Adult neurogenesis and the future of the rejuvenating brain circuits. Neuron 86, 387–401. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.01.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leutgeb, J. K., Leutgeb, S., Moser, M. B., and Moser, E. I. (2007). Pattern separation in the dentate gyrus and CA3 of the hippocampus. Science 315, 961–966. doi: 10.1126/science.1135801

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mathews, E. A., Morgenstern, N. A., Piatti, V. C., Zhao, C., Jessberger, S., Schinder, A. F., et al. (2010). A distinctive layering pattern of mouse dentate granule cells is generated by developmental and adult neurogenesis. J. Comp. Neurol. 518, 4479–4490. doi: 10.1002/cne.22489

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McAvoy, K., Scobie, K. N., Berger, F., Guo, N., Vega-Ramirez, H., Miake-Lye, S., et al. (2014). Rejuvenating the dentate gyrus with stage-specific expansion of adult-born neurons to enhance memory precision in adulthood and aging. Soc. Neurosci. Abstracts 847.19/UU15.

McClelland, J. L., and Goddard, N. H. (1996). Considerations arising from a complementary learning systems perspective on hippocampus and neocortex. Hippocampus 6, 654–665. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1098-1063(1996)6:6<654::aid-hipo8>3.3.co;2-d

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McNaughton, B. L., and Morris, R. G. (1987). Hippocampal synaptic enhancement and information storage within a distributed memory system. Trends Neurosci. 10, 408–415. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(87)90011-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ming, G. L., and Song, H. (2011). Adult neurogenesis in the mammalian brain: significant answers and significant questions. Neuron 70, 687–702. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.05.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Myers, C. E., and Scharfman, H. E. (2009). A role for hilar cells in pattern separation in the dentate gyrus: a computational approach. Hippocampus 19, 321–337. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20516

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nakashiba, T., Cushman, J. D., Pelkey, K. A., Renaudineau, S., Buhl, D. L., McHugh, T. J., et al. (2012). Young dentate granule cells mediate pattern separation, whereas old granule cells facilitate pattern completion. Cell 149, 188–201. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2012.01.046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neunuebel, J. P., and Knierim, J. J. (2012). Spatial firing correlates of physiologically distinct cell types of the rat dentate gyrus. J. Neurosci. 32, 3848–3858. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.6038-11.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neunuebel, J. P., and Knierim, J. J. (2014). CA3 retrieves coherent representations from degraded input: direct evidence for CA3 pattern completion and dentate gyrus pattern separation. Neuron 81, 416–427. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.11.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neunuebel, J. P., Yoganarasimha, D., Rao, G., and Knierim, J. J. (2013). Conflicts between local and global spatial frameworks dissociate neural representations of the lateral and medial entorhinal cortex. J. Neurosci. 33, 9246–9258. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.0946-13.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Niibori, Y., Yu, T. S., Epp, J. R., Akers, K. G., Josselyn, S. A., and Frankland, P. W. (2012). Suppression of adult neurogenesis impairs population coding of similar contexts in hippocampal CA3 region. Nat. Commun. 3, 1–7. doi: 10.1038/ncomms2261

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Reilly, R. C., and McClelland, J. L. (1994). Hippocampal conjunctive encoding, storage and recall: avoiding a trade-off. Hippocampus 4, 661–682. doi: 10.1002/hipo.450040605

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pan, Y. W., Chan, G. C., Kuo, C. T., Storm, D. R., and Xia, Z. (2012). Inhibition of adult neurogenesis by inducible and targeted deletion of ERK5 mitogen-activated protein kinase specifically in adult neurogenic regions impairs contextual fear extinction and remote fear memory. J. Neurosci. 32, 6444–6455. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.6076-11.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pernia-Andrade, A. J., and Jonas, P. (2014). Theta-gamma-modulated synaptic currents in hippocampal granule cells in vivo define a mechanism for network oscillations. Neuron 81, 140–152. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.09.046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petrantonakis, P. C., and Poirazi, P. (2015). Dentate gyrus circuitry features improve performance of sparse approximation algorithms. PLoS One 10:e0117023. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0117023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Piatti, V. C., Ewell, L. A., and Leutgeb, J. K. (2013). Neurogenesis in the dentate gyrus: carrying the message or dictating the tone. Front. Neurosci. 7:50. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2013.00050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rolls, E. T., and Kesner, R. P. (2006). A computational theory of hippocampal function and empirical tests of the theory. Prog. Neurobiol. 79, 1–48. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2006.04.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sahay, A., Scobie, K. N., Hill, A. S., O’Carroll, C. M., Kheirbek, M. A., Burghardt, N. S., et al. (2011b). Increasing adult hippocampal neurogenesis is sufficient to improve pattern separation. Nature 472, 466–470. doi: 10.1038/nature09817

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sahay, A., Wilson, D. A., and Hen, R. (2011a). Pattern separation: a common function for new neurons in hippocampus and olfactory bulb. Neuron 70, 582–588. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.05.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scharfman, H. E., and Myers, C. E. (2013). Hilar mossy cells of the dentate gyrus: a historical perspective. Front. Neural Circuits 6:106. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2012.00106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmidt-Hieber, C., Jonas, P., and Bischofberger, J. (2004). Enhanced synaptic plasticity in newly generated granule cells of the adult hippocampus. Nature 429, 184–187. doi: 10.1038/nature02553

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seri, B., Garcia-Verdugo, J. M., McEwen, B. S., and Alvarez-Buylla, A. (2001). Astrocytes give rise to new neurons in the adult mammalian hippocampus. J. Neurosci. 21, 7153–7160.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Spalding, K. L., Bergmann, O., Alkass, K., Bernard, S., Salehpour, M., Huttner, H. B., et al. (2013). Dynamics of hippocampal neurogenesis in adult humans. Cell 153, 1219–1227. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2013.05.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swan, A. A., Clutton, J. E., Chary, P. K., Cook, S. G., Liu, G. G., and Drew, M. R. (2014). Characterization of the role of adult neurogenesis in touch-screen discrimination learning. Hippocampus 24, 1581–1591. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22337

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tashiro, A., Makino, H., and Gage, F. H. (2007). Experience-specific functional modification of the dentate gyrus through adult neurogenesis: a critical period during an immature stage. J. Neurosci. 27, 3252–3259. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.4941-06.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Temprana, S. G., Mongiat, L. A., Yang, S. M., Trinchero, M. F., Alvarez, D. D., Kropff, E., et al. (2015). Delayed coupling to feedback inhibition during a critical period for the integration of adult-born granule cells. Neuron 85, 116–130. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.11.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Torborg, C. L., Nakashiba, T., Tonegawa, S., and McBain, C. J. (2010). Control of CA3 output by feedforward inhibition despite developmental changes in the excitation-inhibition balance. J. Neurosci. 30, 15628–15637. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.3099-10.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Treves, A., and Rolls, E. T. (1992). Computational constraints suggest the need for two distinct input systems to the hippocampal CA3 network. Hippocampus 2, 189–199. doi: 10.1002/hipo.450020209

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Treves, A., Tashiro, A., Witter, M. E., and Moser, E. I. (2008). What is the mammalian dentate gyrus good for? Neuroscience 154, 1155–1172.doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2008.04.073

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tronel, S., Belnoue, L., Grosjean, N., Revest, J. M., Piazza, P. V., Koehl, M., et al. (2012). Adult-born neurons are necessary for extended contextual discrimination. Hippocampus 22, 292–298. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20895

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tronel, S., Lemaire, V., Charrier, V., Montaron, M. F., and Abrous, D. N. (2015). Influence of ontogenetic age on the role of dentate granule neurons. Brain struct. Funct. 220, 645–661. doi: 10.1007/s00429-014-0715-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wadiche, J. I., and Overstreet-Wadiche, L. (2015). New neurons don’t talk back. Neuron 85, 3–5. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.12.047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, W., Pan, Y. W., Zou, J., Li, T., Abel, G. M., Palmiter, R. D., et al. (2014). Genetic activation of ERK5 MAP kinase enhances adult neurogenesis and extends hippocampus-dependent long-term memory. J. Neurosci. 34, 2130–2147. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3324-13.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weisz, V. I., and Argibay, P. F. (2012). Neurogenesis interferes with the retrieval of remote memories: forgetting in neurocomputational terms. Cognition 125, 13–25. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2012.07.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wiskott, L., Rasch, M. J., and Kempermann, G. (2006). A functional hypothesis for adult hippocampal neurogenesis: avoidance of catastrophic interference in the dentate gyrus. Hippocampus 16, 329–343. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20167

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yassa, M. A., and Stark, C. E. (2011). Pattern separation in the hippocampus. Trends Neurosci. 34, 515–525. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2011.06.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, J., Ji, F., Liu, Y., Lei, X., Li, H., Ji, G., et al. (2014). Ezh2 regulates adult hippocampal neurogenesis and memory. J. Neurosci. 34, 5184–5199. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.4129-13.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhao, C., Deng, W., and Gage, F. H. (2008). Mechanisms and functional implications of adult neurogenesis. Cell 132, 645–660. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2008.01.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: dentate gyrus, adult hippocampal neurogenesis, pattern separation, interference,discrimination, generalization, sparseness, feed-back inhibition

Citation: McAvoy K, Besnard A and Sahay A (2015) Adult hippocampal neurogenesis and pattern separation in DG: a role for feedback inhibition in modulating sparseness to govern population-based coding. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 9:120. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2015.00120

Received: 04 May 2015; Accepted: 07 August 2015;
Published: 20 August 2015.

Edited by:

James B. Aimone, Sandia National Laboratories, USA

Reviewed by:

Sue Becker, McMaster University, Canada
Adam Santoro, University of Toronto, Canada

Copyright © 2015 McAvoy, Besnard and Sahay. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution and reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Amar Sahay, Center for Regenerative Medicine, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, CPZN 4242, 185 Cambridge Street, Boston, MA 02114, USA, asahay@mgh.harvard.edu

These authors have contributed equally to this work.

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.