- Department of Physiology and Pharmacology, Wake Forest University, School of Medicine, Winston-Salem, NC, USA
Introduction
The surge in development of brain-computer interface (BCI) devices is highly focused on the algorithms, mechanics, and neurophysics of their production (Lebedev and Nicolelis, 2006; Lebedev et al., 2011; Opris, 2013). Here I propose that capitalizing on research findings from the field of social neuroscience can enhance training and effectiveness of BCI devices. BCIs are not just about individual brains but also about brains in interaction with other brains. Learning in a social context is more effective than non-social instruction and countless neurophysiology studies have demonstrated that social interaction actually alters physiology, including changes in neuroplasticity and arousal. Importantly, social interaction also consists of emotional responses that have powerful rewarding qualities and incur reciprocal action. Interdisciplinary cooperation between social neuroscience and BCI innovation has been proposed to promote development of more effective BCIs that demonstrate adaptability during interaction (Mattout, 2012). The challenges of BCI illiteracy, or BCI inefficiency, suggests a vital need to consider all possible contributing factors to decrease the failure rate seen in up to a third of users (Vidaurre and Blankertz, 2010). Additionally, it has been suggested that BCI inefficiency can be reduced by addressing flaws in human training approaches, which have been largely neglected (Lotte et al., 2013). Therefore, the social cues and contexts a patient has when BCIs are integrated and employed should not be overlooked for their potential to improve effectiveness.
Social Intelligence
The human brain, like other primate brains, has evolved to be exquisitely tuned to social interactions (Ghazanfar and Santos, 2004). In fact, sociality might have been the primary force driving the evolution of primate intelligence (Jolly, 1966; Humphrey, 1976; Dunbar and Shultz, 2007). Thus, it is no leap to assume that cognitive abilities would be influenced by social interactions (Ybarra et al., 2008). Importantly, cognitive abilities do not exist as abstract mental activities independent of body and world (Barrett and Henzi, 2005). We are born into an environment that consists of others acting and interacting; successfully navigating this social milieu requires the development of many cognitive skills. These essential capacities include memory, learning, decision-making, and behavioral inhibition as well as more complex abilities such as communicating, problem-solving, teaching, mentalizing, and inferring others' mental states (Gariépy et al., 2014).
Social Cues and Contexts
Social contexts include observing and participating in exchanges between individuals, and responding to cues integral to social behavior. These cues include complex stimuli that require simultaneous processing of multiple sensory inputs, including olfactory, auditory, tactile, and visual cues. The emotional responses of self and others are also social cues. Whom we are interacting with matters, as well as the connection we feel with them along a continuum ranging from perceived social isolation to bonding and attachment (Cacioppo and Cacioppo, 2012). The mere presentation of cues specific to social interaction, such as language or images of the same species, is enough to cause neurophysiological changes in brain and behavior. In primates, many studies take advantage of the fact that pictures of objects, abstract images, or other species are not as rewarding as pictures of conspecifics (Wilson and Goldman-Rakic, 1994). Even the nature of the stimulus is relevant—we have a preference for biological movement over robotic movement for encoding behavior (Tai et al., 2004) and children learn new behaviors better from humans than from robots or androids (robots that look like humans) (Moriguchi et al., 2010a,b). The special effectiveness of social stimuli was recently shown when superimposing familiar face images onto the P300 Speller character matrix for ERP-based BCI performance increased accuracy and speed in healthy individuals (Kaufmann et al., 2011) and patients (Kaufmann et al., 2013).
Social Neurophysiology
Neurophysiological effects of social context and stimuli are distributed throughout the nervous system. Multiple areas of the brain respond to social conditions as assessed by a variety of methods, including transcranial magnetic stimulation, functional magnetic resonance imaging, electrophysiology, and molecular studies. To comprehensively describe all the neurological findings is not possible here so only a few are highlighted (see for reviews: Beer and Ochsner, 2006; Adolphs, 2009). The anterior cingular cortex (ACC) integrates complex stimuli, empathy, and decision making (Lavin et al., 2013). The striatum is a vital area for social interaction and reward (Báez-Mendoza and Schultz, 2013). In the inferior fronto-parietal areas, the mirror neuron system (discussed further below) is essential for social learning (reviewed in Rizzolatti and Sinigaglia, 2010). Differences in genetic expression are seen when zebra finches, a social songbird, sing to females directly rather than singing undirected song (Jarvis et al., 1998). Social behavior induces neurogenesis in the adult hippocampus in rodents (Gheusi et al., 2009; Lieberwirth and Wang, 2014; Peretto and Paredes, 2014). The early findings that social enrichment improves cognitive performance, neuronal growth, and overall brain mass (reviewed by Rosenzweig, 2007) followed by decades of confirmation, has led to the now standard practice that animals in research programs are provided appropriate social interactions (Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals, 2011). Conversely, decades of research have shown social stress during early development results in a number of behavioral and neurochemical deficits in multiple brain areas. For example, social isolation during rearing impairs neurogenesis in the dentate gyrus of the mouse hippocampus as revealed by deficits in spatial memory task (Ibi et al., 2008), and cell proliferation, cell survival, and neuronal differentiation are negatively affected by isolation during adulthood in female prairie voles (Lieberwirth and Wang, 2012).
Cognitive Function
Continuing development of BCIs that treat neurological and psychiatric disorders involving cognitive and emotional impairments suggests the need for the most comprehensive techniques to facilitate success. The research reviewed above clearly shows that cognition is changed by the qualities of social exchanges or social cues. For example, executive function can be increased (Ybarra et al., 2008) or reduced (Richeson et al., 2005) depending on the type of social interaction and even the identity of the actor. Where an individual falls along the continuum of social isolation (e.g., neglect, exclusion) to social connection has important consequences for cognitive abilities. In older adults, correlations between perceived isolation and poor cognitive responses have been shown (Tilvis et al., 2004; Wilson et al., 2007; Dickinson et al., 2011). In fact, brain and behavioral responses differ depending on the specific feeling of isolation/connection to the person with whom one is interacting (Cacioppo and Cacioppo, 2012).
Learning Socially
The social environment can arguably be said to be the richest environment for learning the most complex cognitive skills, pointing out the importance of training methods. A special quality about live social interaction is that it acutely primes the induction of novel responses (Gottlieb, 1991). For example, juvenile sparrows will learn the songs of another species when demonstrated by a live tutor that they do not learn from tape recordings (Baptista and Petrinovich, 1984). The use of social reinforcement has been noted to be particularly useful to improve BCI integration (reviewed in Lotte et al., 2013). Even if social feedback is provided by an android, behavioral change is better than when a computer display provides factual feedback (Ham and Midden, 2014). This suggests that when biofeedback systems are used (e.g., EEG, fMRI, MEG), engaging a person who communicates feedback in addition to computer displays could facilitate acceptance and speed of acquisition during training.
An essential learning substrate lies within the mirror neurons found in the cortical areas in humans; these neurons fire when observing or imitating another's behavior, evoking in the recipient the representational state of the observer's action or emotion (Rizzolatti and Craighero, 2004; Rizzolatti and Sinigaglia, 2010). This allows mere observation of an action to increase motor memory encoding (Stefan et al., 2005; Celnik et al., 2006). Methods in stroke rehabilitation based on the mirror neuron system—action observation, motor imagery, and imitation—take advantage of this opportunity to rebuild motor function (Garrison et al., 2010). Similarly, action observation has been proposed to interact with motor training in neurorehabilitation (Pomeroy et al., 2005), suggesting that cognitive training may also be possible by capitalizing on these mechanisms. For example, in a study with older adults, the combination of physical training and action observation together elicited encoding whereas conditions with only physical training or action observation failed. It is therefore suggested that employing a person who demonstrates the same action as the patient during BCI training would facilitate an increase in encoding more than just using imagery.
Brain-to-brain Coupling
Complex joint behaviors such as communication and social coordination depend on synchronous interactions. Interpersonal synchrony promotes a variety of positive outcomes, including affiliation, liking, rapport, and emotional support satisfaction. It is challenging to measure brain activity simultaneously from two people, but studies that examine inter-person variables such as synchrony have found revealing results using neuroimaging methods (summarized by Konvalinka and Roepstorff, 2012). Inter-personal entrainment of behavior between people occurs when engaged in rhythmic behavior, such as finger-tapping (Konvalinka et al., 2010) or chair-rocking (Richardson et al., 2007) resulting in unintentional coordination. Inducing synchronous activity induces brain-to-brain coupling, which might increase the efficiency of partnerships engaged with BCI use. Indeed, it has been suggested that our joint cognition with other minds increases their efficiency as a unit, particularly when in compromised situations (Wegner et al., 1991). Intriguing results with a multiuser BCI video game based on motor imagery showed improved utility, effectiveness, and engagement (Bonnet et al., 2013), suggesting methods using interacting brains would help reduce BCI illiteracy.
Brain-to-brain Transfer
Perhaps the most outstanding example of social interaction effect is the demonstration of brain-to-brain transfer of information via computer. In a recent study, the neural firing pattern code of a rat performing a memory task was transferred to a recipient rat, who then responded correctly more often than when stimulated with a random code or without stimulation (Deadwyler et al., 2013). Further, a study revealed that sensory information from a rat transmitted via computer included bidirectional reward contingency information that changed behavior of both donor and a naïve recipient rat in a location thousands of miles away (Pais-Vieira et al., 2013). Even human brain to rat brain transfer has been achieved (Yoo et al., 2013). These examples suggest that the day when neural signals can be transmitted between individuals is not out of the realm of possibility.
Recommendations
BCIs used for neurological rehabilitation require progressive practice with feedback and reward (Dobkin, 2007); here I suggest that capitalizing on social factors will result in better outcomes. The social environment provides a context particularly relevant for fostering the development and change of cognition. Given that the presence of social factors facilitates learning suggests that attention should be given to the conditions by which the BCI is integrated. For example, engage a loved one or a trusted facilitator who can supply emotionally rewarding feedback in training and treatment protocols. Use rewards of social stimuli, such as images of people or positive language, to enhance training, and deliver rewards with people instead of only computer displays. Pay attention to the emotional environment to ensure it is conducive for promoting change. Be aware that socially isolated patients will be more sensitive to negative cues and stimuli. Capitalize on the processes of imitation and action observation to stimulate responses of the mirror neuron system; in other words, have the patient and facilitator do the same task. Even if the patient is unable to perform the task, mere observation can stimulate motor neuron responses. Employ synchronization tasks between the facilitator and patient to increase trust and brain coupling. Finally, most revolutionary, use healthy individuals as donors to stimulate the patient via inter-brain coupling with brain-to-brain interfaces. In conclusion, just as it is essential that BCI development and use rely on the accurate use of technical principles, it is vital not to overlook the value of applying findings from social neuroscience in order to maximize the effectiveness of BCI implementation and integration.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
References
Adolphs, R. (2009). The social brain: neural basis of social knowledge. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 60, 693–716. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163514
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Báez-Mendoza, R., and Schultz, W. (2013). The role of the striatum in social behavior. Front. Neurosci. 7:233. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2013.00233
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Baptista, L. F., and Petrinovich, L. (1984). Social interaction, sensitive phases and the song template hypothesis in the white-crowned sparrow. Anim. Behav. 32, 172–181. doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(84)80335-8
Barrett, L., and Henzi, P. (2005). The social nature of primate cognition. Proc. Biol. Sci. 272, 1865–1875. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2005.3200
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Beer, J. S., and Ochsner, K. N. (2006). Social cognition: a multi level analysis. Brain Res. 1079, 98–105. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.01.002
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Bonnet, L., Lotte, F., and Lecuyer, A. (2013). Two brains, one game: design and evaluation of a multiuser BCI video game based on motor imagery. IEEE Trans. Comput. Intell. AI Games 5, 185–198. doi: 10.1109/TCIAIG.2012.2237173
Cacioppo, S., and Cacioppo, J. T. (2012). Decoding the invisible forces of social connections. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 6:51. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2012.00051
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Celnik, P., Stefan, K., Hummel, F., Duque, J., Classen, J., and Cohen, L. G. (2006). Encoding a motor memory in the older adult by action observation. Neuroimage 29, 677–684. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.07.039
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Deadwyler, S. A., Berger, T. W., Sweatt, A. J., Song, D., Chan, R. H. M., Opris, I., et al. (2013). Donor/recipient enhancement of memory in rat hippocampus. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 7:120. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2013.00120
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Dickinson, W. J., Potter, G. G., Hybels, C. F., McQuoid, D. R., and Steffens, D. C. (2011). Change in stress and social support as predictors of cognitive decline in older adults with and without depression. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 26, 1267–1274. doi: 10.1002/gps.2676
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Dobkin, B. H. (2007). Brain-computer interface technology as a tool to augment plasticity and outcomes for neurological rehabilitation. J. Physiol. 579 (Pt 3), 637–642. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2006.123067
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Dunbar, R. I. M., and Shultz, S. (2007). Evolution in the social brain. Science 317, 1344–1347. doi: 10.1126/science.1145463
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Gariépy, J.-F., Watson, K. K., Du, E., Xie, D. L., Erb, J., Amasino, D., et al. (2014). Social learning in humans and other animals. Front. Neurosci. 8:58. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2014.00058
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Garrison, K. A., Winstein, C. J., and Aziz-Zadeh, L. (2010). The mirror neuron system: a neural substrate for methods in stroke rehabilitation. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair 24, 404–412. doi: 10.1177/1545968309354536
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Ghazanfar, A. A., and Santos, L. R. (2004). Primate brains in the wild: the sensory bases for social interactions. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 5, 603–616. doi: 10.1038/nrn1473
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Gheusi, G., Ortega-Perez, I., Murray, K., and Lledo, P.-M. (2009). A niche for adult neurogenesis in social behavior. Behav. Brain Res. 200, 315–322. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2009.02.006
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals (2011). Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals, 8th Edn. The National Academies Press. Available online at: http://www.nap.edu/catalog/12910/guide-for-the-care-and-use-of-laboratory-animals-eighth (Accessed December 30, 2014).
Ham, J., and Midden, C. J. H. (2014). A persuasive robot to stimulate energy conservation: the influence of positive and negative social feedback and task similarity on energy-consumption behavior. Int. J. Soc. Robot. 6, 163–171. doi: 10.1007/s12369-013-0205-z
Humphrey, N. (1976). “The social function of intellect,” in Growing Points in Ethology, eds P. P. G. Bateson and R. A. Hinde. Available online at: http://www.humphrey.org.uk/papers/1976SocialFunction.pdf.
Ibi, D., Takuma, K., Koike, H., Mizoguchi, H., Tsuritani, K., Kuwahara, Y., et al. (2008). Social isolation rearing-induced impairment of the hippocampal neurogenesis is associated with deficits in spatial memory and emotion-related behaviors in juvenile mice. J. Neurochem. 105, 921–932. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2007.05207.x
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Jarvis, E., Scharff, C., Grossman, M., Ramos, J., and Nottebohm, F. (1998). For whom the bird sings: context-dependent gene expression. Neuron 21, 775–788.
Kaufmann, T., Schulz, S. M., Grunzinger, C., and Kubler, A. (2011). Flashing characters with famous faces improves ERP-based brain–computer interface performance. J. Neural Eng. 8, 1–10. doi: 10.1088/1741-2560/8/5/056016
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Kaufmann, T., Schulz, S. M., Köblitz, A., Renner, G., Wessig, C., and Kübler, A. (2013). Face stimuli effectively prevent brain-computer interface inefficiency in patients with neurodegenerative disease. Clin. Neurophysiol. 124, 893–900. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2012.11.006
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Konvalinka, I., and Roepstorff, A. (2012). The two-brain approach: how can mutually interacting brains teach us something about social interaction? Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:215. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00215
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Konvalinka, I., Vuust, P., Roepstorff, A., and Frith, C. D. (2010). Follow you, follow me: continuous mutual prediction and adaptation in joint tapping. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 63, 2220–2230. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2010.497843
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Lavin, C., Melis, C., Mikulan, E., Gelormini, C., Huepe, D., and Ibañez, A. (2013). The anterior cingulate cortex: an integrative hub for human socially-driven interactions. Front. Neurosci. 7:64. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2013.00064
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Lebedev, M. A., and Nicolelis, M. A. L. (2006). Brain–machine interfaces: past, present and future. Trends Neurosci. 29, 536–546. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2006.07.004
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Lebedev, M. A., Tate, A. J., Hanson, T. L., Li, Z., O'Doherty, J. E., Winans, J. A., et al. (2011). Future developments in brain-machine interface research. Clinics 66 Suppl. 1, 25–32. doi: 10.1590/S1807-59322011001300004
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Lieberwirth, C., and Wang, Z. (2012). The social environment and neurogenesis in the adult mammalian brain. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:118. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00118
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Lieberwirth, C., and Wang, Z. (2014). Social bonding: regulation by neuropeptides. Front. Neurosci. 8:171. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2014.00171
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Lotte, F., Larrue, F., and Mühl, C. (2013). Flaws in current human training protocols for spontaneous brain-computer interfaces: lessons learned from instructional design. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:568. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00568
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Mattout, J. (2012). Brain-computer interfaces: a neuroscience paradigm of social interaction? A matter of perspective. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:114. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00114
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Moriguchi, Y., Kanda, T., Ishiguro, H., and Itakura, S. (2010a). Children perseverate to a human's actions but not to a robot's actions. Dev. Sci. 13, 62–68. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00860.x
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Moriguchi, Y., Minato, T., Ishiguro, H., Shinohara, I., and Itakura, S. (2010b). Cues that trigger social transmission of disinhibition in young children. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 107, 181–187. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2010.04.018
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Opris, I. (2013). Inter-laminar microcircuits across neocortex: repair and augmentation. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 7:80. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2013.00080
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Peretto, P., and Paredes, R. G. (2014). “Social cues, adult neurogenesis, and reproductive behavior,” in Neurobiology of Chemical Communication, Chapter 13, ed C. Mucignat-Caretta (Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press). Available online at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK200979/?report=reader
Pais-Vieira, M., Lebedev, M., Kunicki, C., Wang, J., and Nicolelis, M. A. L. (2013). A brain-to-brain interface for real-time sharing of sensorimotor information. Sci. Rep. 3:1319. doi: 10.1038/srep01319
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Pomeroy, V. M., Clark, C. A., Miller, J. S. G., Baron, J.-C., Markus, H. S., and Tallis, R. C. (2005). The potential for utilizing the ‘mirror neurone system’ to enhance recovery of the severely affected upper limb early after stroke: a review and hypothesis. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair 19, 4–13. doi: 10.1177/1545968304274351
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Richardson, M. J., Marsh, K. L., Isenhower, R. W., Goodman, J. R. L., and Schmidt, R. C. (2007). Rocking together: dynamics of intentional and unintentional interpersonal coordination. Hum. Mov. Sci. 26, 867–891. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2007.07.002
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Richeson, J. A., Trawalter, S., and Shelton, J. N. (2005). African Americans' implicit racial attitudes and the depletion of executive function after interracial interactions. Soc. Cogn. 23, 336–352. doi: 10.1521/soco.2005.23.4.336
Rizzolatti, G., and Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 27, 169–192. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144230
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Rizzolatti, G., and Sinigaglia, C. (2010). The functional role of the parieto-frontal mirror circuit: interpretations and misinterpretations. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 264–274. doi: 10.1038/nrn2805
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Rosenzweig, M. R. (2007). “Modification of brain circuits through experience,” in Neural Plasticity and Memory: From Genes To Brain Imaging, ed F. Bermudez-Rattoni (Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press), 67–94. Available online at: http://www.mendeley.com/research/modification-brain-circuits-through-experience/?utm_source=desktop&utm_medium=1.12.4&utm_campaign=open_catalog&userDocumentId={719b8420-1b4f-49b3-85a7-f60344a6bbfe}.
Stefan, K., Cohen, L. G., Duque, J., Mazzocchio, R., Celnik, P., Sawaki, L., et al. (2005). Formation of a motor memory by action observation. J. Neurosci. 25, 9339–9346. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2282-05.2005
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Tai, Y. F., Scherfler, C., Brooks, D. J., Sawamoto, N., and Castiello, U. (2004). The human premotor cortex is ‘mirror’ only for biological actions. Curr. Biol. 14, 117–120. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2004.01.005
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Tilvis, R. S., Kahonen-Vare, M. H., Jolkkonen, J., Valvanne, J., Pitkala, K. H., and Strandberg, T. E. (2004). Predictors of cognitive decline and mortality of aged people over a 10-year period. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 59, M268–M274. doi: 10.1093/gerona/59.3.M268
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Vidaurre, C., and Blankertz, B. (2010). Towards a cure for BCI illiteracy. Brain Topogr. 23, 194–198. doi: 10.1007/s10548-009-0121-6
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Wegner, D. M., Erber, R., and Raymond, P. (1991). Transactive memory in close relationships. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 61, 923–929.
Wilson, F. A. W., and Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1994). Viewing preferences of rhesus monkeys related to memory for complex pictures, colours and faces. Behav. Brain Res. 60, 79–89.
Wilson, R. S., Krueger, K. R., Arnold, S. E., Schneider, J. A., Kelly, J. F., Barnes, L. L., et al. (2007). Loneliness and risk of alzheimer disease. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 64, 234–240. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.2.234
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Ybarra, O., Burnstein, E., Winkielman, P., Keller, M. C., Manis, M., Chan, E., et al. (2008). Mental exercising through simple socializing: social interaction promotes general cognitive functioning. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 34, 248–259. doi: 10.1177/0146167207310454
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Yoo, S.-S., Kim, H., Filandrianos, E., Taghados, S. J., and Park, S. (2013). Non-Invasive Brain-to-Brain Interface (BBI): establishing functional links between two brains. PLoS ONE 8:e60410. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0060410
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Keywords: brain-computer interface (BCI), neurorehabilitatation, social neuroscience, social behavior, augmenting cognition, neural prosthesis
Citation: Sexton CA (2015) The overlooked potential for social factors to improve effectiveness of brain-computer interfaces. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 9:70. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2015.00070
Received: 15 January 2015; Accepted: 17 April 2015;
Published: 06 May 2015.
Edited by:
Mikhail Lebedev, Duke University, USAReviewed by:
Femke Nijboer, Leiden University, NetherlandsJérémie Mattout, Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, France
Paul Root Wolpe, Emory University, USA
Copyright © 2015 Sexton. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Cheryl Ann Sexton, csexton@wakehealth.edu