- 1Department of Psychology, University of Nevada, Las Vegas, NV, USA
- 2Department of Newborn Medicine, Brigham and Women’s Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
- 3Department of Pediatrics, Mass General Hospital for Children, Boston, MA, USA
The musical brain is built over time through experience with a multitude of sounds in the auditory environment. However, learning the melodies, timbres, and rhythms unique to the music and language of one’s culture begins already within the mother’s womb during the third trimester of human development. We review evidence that the intrauterine auditory environment plays a key role in shaping later auditory development and musical preferences. We describe evidence that externally and internally generated sounds influence the developing fetus, and argue that such prenatal auditory experience may set the trajectory for the development of the musical mind.
Linking Prenatal Experience to the Emerging Musical Mind
Early experience lays the foundations for the developing musical mind. Hearing emerges prior to birth (Sansavini, 1997) and the acoustic environment in utero begins to shape the auditory system much earlier than sensory systems that are not exposed to input until after birth, such as vision. Twenty-five week old fetuses are equipped with structural components of the ear that allow them to hear (Cheour-Luhtanen et al., 1996). Hydrophone recordings reveal that many sounds are measurable in the intrauterine environment, such as the maternal heartbeat, breathing, digestion, and the maternal voice (Dunham, 1990). Even non-maternal speech and song is potentially audible, at or above 60 dB sound pressure level (SPL) and attenuated above 250–500 Hz (Busnel et al., 1992; Gerhardt and Abrams, 1996). Fetal heart rate evidence suggests that during the third trimester fetuses discriminate the speech of their mother from that of a stranger, speech of their native language from a non-native language (Kisilevsky et al., 2003; Kisilevsky and Hains, 2009), and they respond differentially to music and speech (Kisilevsky et al., 2004; Granier-Deferre et al., 2011). Thus, a wide range of maternal and non-maternal sounds are available and potentially audible to the fetus during late pregnancy.
The effects of prenatal auditory experience can be observed among newborns within only a few hours or days after birth. Soon after birth, infants show a strong preference for their mother’s voice over the voice of another female (DeCasper and Fifer, 1980; Cooper and Aslin, 1989; Kisilevsky et al., 2003), their mother’s language over a foreign language (Moon et al., 1993, 2012), and specific passages of speech (DeCasper and Spence, 1986) or music (Hepper, 1991) presented during the final weeks of pregnancy. Thus, even prior to birth human listeners may begin to acquire rudimentary auditory representations that may be considered as the earliest building blocks of the musical mind. Here, we examine the nature of the prenatal auditory input and its effects on auditory preferences, perceptual capacities, and musical skills.
Fetal Experience of Externally Generated Sounds
Prenatal exposure to environmental sounds originating outside the uterus can engender subsequent listening preferences for familiar linguistic and musical stimuli. Newborns prefer to listen to music heard prenatally than to unfamiliar music (Hepper, 1991). Interestingly, this preference diminishes by 21 days after birth, suggesting that prenatal memory for specific sounds may be relatively short-lived. However, other evidence suggests that newborn preferences can generalize to novel sounds that share structural features with sounds heard in utero, such as novel speakers uttering familiar passages. Mothers who read a specific nursery rhyme daily during the third trimester had newborns who preferred that nursery rhyme to a novel rhyme; this preference was observed whether the familiar nursery rhyme was read by the infant’s mother or by a female stranger (DeCasper and Spence, 1986). These findings suggest that newborns remember more than the unique characteristics of the mother’s voice, responding to something more general about the familiar passage, such as its prosodic features. The importance of prosody is also implicated by the finding that newborns differentiate between novel, low-pass filtered samples of two languages belonging to contrasting rhythmic classes and they confuse contrasting languages that belong to the same rhythmic class (Nazzi et al., 1998; Ramus et al., 1999). English-learning infants respond similarly to novel utterances of low-pass filtered English and Dutch speech. It is not until 4 or 5 months of age that infants begin to discriminate recordings of their own language from a rhythmically similar foreign language (i.e., English versus Dutch). This evidence suggests that newborns perceive rhythmic features of speech at the earliest stages of development. These early auditory preferences may have important implications for later learning and social development—by 5 months, infants gaze longer at (i.e., show a social preference for) people who speak their native language compared to people who speak a different language or people who speak their native language with a foreign accent (Kinzler et al., 2007).
An important question is whether abnormal experience during this crucial developmental window—for example because of premature birth—influences later language and cognitive development (for review see, McMahon et al., 2012). The later part of gestation is characterized by rapid changes in the neurochemical and structural components of the brain, so infants born prematurely often have diminished brain growth (Guihard-Costa and Larroche, 1990) and medical complications (for review see, Loftin et al., 2010), making it difficult to rigorously compare preterm and full-term infants. However, a longitudinal study found minimal differences in the development of low-level auditory brainstem responses of premature and full-term infants through age six (Jiang, 1995), perhaps because low-level auditory pathways are developed prior to the onset of hearing (Barkat et al., 2011). When perceptual deficits are observed among preterm infants they probably occur at the level of auditory cortex, where abnormal experience with auditory input inappropriate for gestational age would presumably impact the synaptic blooming beginning around 3 months prior to birth (Huttenlocher and Dabholkar, 1997; Fellman et al., 2004; Sanes and Bao, 2009).
Preterm infants show delayed brain responses to features of their native language. For example, greater induced gamma band activity (indicative of selective attention) to the native than to a non-native language was observed among full term 6-month-olds but not among preterm infants until 9 months of age (Pena et al., 2010). This suggests that language differentiation begins 6 months after an infants’ due date rather than 6 months after the onset of exposure to language sounds outside the womb.
Preterm infants also exhibit delayed culture-specific narrowing of phonetic discrimination (i.e., better discrimination of native than non-native speech sounds). While full-term infants exhibit perceptual narrowing by 12 months, preterm infants retain sensitivity to both native and non-native speech contrasts at 12 months of age, as indicated by the presence of an event-related potential component called the mismatched negativity (MMN; Jansson-Verkasalo et al., 2010). Despite no measurable difference in behavioral language outcomes between pre- and full-term infants at 12 months of age, preterm infants show significant deficits in language later in development. For instance, at two years of age, toddlers who were born prematurely lagged behind their peers on measures such as the number of produced words, their development of morphology, and mean length of longest utterances (Jansson-Verkasalo et al., 2010). Further, children’s MMNs at 12 months were significantly correlated with their language abilities at two years, with greater amplitude for non-native speech sounds being associated with greater language deficits. Findings from Pena et al. (2012) suggest preterm infants do not exhibit perceptual narrowing until 15 months of age despite having tested normally for their gestational age on several health (i.e., APGAR scores, cranial size, brain ultrasonography) and auditory (i.e., auditory brainstem response and otoacoustic emissions) measures throughout the course of development, possibly suggesting physical health deficits related to prematurity are unlikely to account for all observed delays. Perceptual learning may begin as soon as hearing is functional, a possibility that is consistent with recent evidence that perceptual narrowing for native-language vowel sounds is already under way prior to birth (Moon et al., 2012). Perceptual narrowing also occurs for musical rhythm and pitch processing (Lynch et al., 1990; Hannon and Trainor, 2007), and there is minimal evidence that additional postnatal experience accelerates culture-specific narrowing for musical pitch processing (Lynch et al., 1995). Auditory experiences during the final trimester may influence the type of input infants attend to based on the preferred or familiar stimuli in the environment (Cooper and Aslin, 1989). These early preferences may bootstrap the acquisition of key features within both the language and the music domains. Although further research is needed, the above findings implicate a maturational process of auditory development that begins in utero and is dependent on the acoustic environment and the listening experience, both pre- and postnatally (Werker and Yeung, 2005).
Fetal Experience of Internally Generated Sounds
Internally generated sounds—in particular the continuous, rhythmic sound of the maternal heartbeat—are undoubtedly the most prominent and frequently heard stimuli in utero. The prominence of the maternal heartbeat has been implied through preferential sucking paradigms, in which the presentation of a reinforcing sound is contingent on newborns’ increased or decreased sucking rates relative to baseline (Salk, 1962; Spence and DeCasper, 1987; Moon and Fifer, 2000). The effectiveness of the low-pass filtered sound of the maternal heartbeat in reinforcing infant sucking attests to its salience in the intrauterine environment (DeCasper and Sigafoos, 1983). Recordings of the maternal heartbeat more effectively reinforce sucking than do recordings of male speech (Panneton and DeCasper, 1984) or even unfamiliar female speech (DeCasper and Prescott, 2009).
The maternal heartbeat is the fetus’ first metronome. A single beat of the heart is composed of several distinct waves, starting with an initial P wave (in lay terminology, the “lub” of the heart’s rhythmic “lub-dub”) followed by a brief pause, which leads up to the subsequent QRS complex (or the “dub”). Importantly, the P wave and the QRS complex occur in a fixed 1:1 ratio. The distance between any two analogous points on two consecutive heartbeats (for instance, the R-R interval) is also at a constant 1:1 ratio (Benbadis et al., 2007). Because the heartbeat is the first regular and periodic stimulus a fetus hears, it could, in principle, influence subsequent preferences for other periodic auditory stimuli. Nevertheless, no studies have systematically examined whether specific maternal heart rates or beat ratios predict newborn listening preferences. Instead, studies examining the role of listening to the maternal heartbeat sounds have focused on the clinical outcomes of such interventions in neonates, typically in addition to presenting the infants with other sounds such as maternal voice and music.
One theory proposes that fetuses become “imprinted” with the isochronous rhythms of the maternal heartbeat leading to a reduction in their overall arousal (Salk, 1960, 1961, 1962). However, evidence of the arousal-modulating effects of the maternal heartbeat is mixed, with some studies revealing no effect on arousal (Tulloch et al., 1964). In an attempt to replicate (Salk, 1960, 1961, 1962), Tulloch and colleagues found no change in weight gain, activity, or formula intake when presented with simulated heartbeat sounds. In this study, the heartbeat sound was audible (as recorded by infants’ startle response at the sound’s offset), but not as loud as the stimuli used in the original study (45 dB versus 85 dB, respectively). Thus, Tulloch et al. postulated that the considerably louder heartbeat sounds used in the Salk studies may have drowned out all background noises, including the sudden noises of airplanes in close proximity to the hospital. On the other hand, if the sounds of the heartbeat, and, specifically, the sounds of the maternal heartbeat are important, then not only should heartbeat sounds act to diminish arousal indicating directed attention, but sounds of their own mother’s heartbeat should have greater arousal reduction effects than other heartbeat sounds. Smith and Steinschneider (1975) attempted to test this hypothesis by measuring the maternal heart rate during the last 4 to 6 weeks prior to each infant’s due date. After delivery, newborns were divided into high and low maternal heart rate groups (based on the average heart rate of each infant’s mother). Newborns were coaxed into a state of high arousal (i.e., crying), and were exposed to recordings of high (105 bpm) and low (75 bpm) heart rate samples at 75 dB SPL, as well as silence. Results showed that heart rate sounds—irrespective of whether they were played at tempi consistent with the mothers’ heart rate—resulted in greater proportions of time sleeping and less time crying compared to silence (Smith and Steinschneider, 1975). However, when arousal was measured by the time elapsed until the infant fell asleep, no arousal reduction was found. These studies suggest that benefits of the heartbeat on arousal are perhaps driven by the regular, constant nature of these sounds above the noise floor, providing a salient stimulus that facilitates the filtering out of loud background noises.
A number of studies have shown that multiple maternal sounds, not just the heartbeat, can have soothing effects and short-term clinical benefits for infants, especially in the preterm population. For example, Doheny et al. (2012a,b) reported improved cardiorespiratory stability and reduced episodes of breathing cessation in preterm infants exposed to audio recordings of their mother’s voice and heartbeat, compared to the presentation of regular hospital noise. The combined effects of the mother’s voice and heartbeat are also observed during Kangaroo Care, which encourages parents to hold the newborn close to the chest and maximize skin-to-skin contact, and has been associated with normalizing their body temperature, breathing, heart rate (Ludington-Hoe et al., 2006) and body weight (Charpak et al., 2005). Despite relatively short-term intervention durations, exposure to recordings of the mother’s voice has been associated with increased oxygen saturation levels (Standley and Moore, 1995), improved growth velocity (Zimmerman et al., 2013), fewer episodes of feeding intolerance (Krueger et al., 2010), and reduced hospital stay (Cevasco, 2008). Live interventions such as parental play and song directed toward their preterm infants in the neonatal intensive care unit (NICU) have been particularly potent, yielding reduced heart rate and deeper sleep for 30 minutes after sessions (Arnon et al., 2006), as well as improved breathing and sucking regulation (Loewy et al., 2013). For recorded voices, however, there appears to be no benefit of maternal voice over a stranger’s voice (Segall, 1972), suggesting that voice familiarity is key. Maternal sounds may therefore help optimize pre-term infants’ arousal levels, improve their capacity to maintain a normal breathing pattern, and promote overall health and well-being (Doheny et al., 2012a,b). The extent to which these benefits extend to instrumental music is unclear. Several studies using sung lullabies or simple melodies have shown positive short-term clinical effects for preterm infants (for meta-analysis, see Standley, 2002), however the effects of instrumental music and maternal sounds have not been directly compared, and it is difficult to infer answers from prior work due to the diversity of stimuli (i.e., mother’s or father’s voice, recorded singing combined with uterine sounds, multimodal stimulation) as well as dependent measures (i.e., motor development, weight gain, contingent sucking, or oxygen regulation) (see Chapman, 1978; Malloy, 1979; Standley and Moore, 1995).
Prenatal Experience as a Foundation for Musical Preference
Prenatal exposure to the sounds of language influences infants’ preferences for language, as described above, but this exposure might also generalize to preferences for music. Both vocal and instrumental music have been shown to reflect the characteristic rhythmic patterns of the composer’s native language (Wenk, 1987). Growing evidence suggests that the prosodic features of the native language can influence how adult users of those languages perceive non-linguistic/musical sound patterns such as temporal groups (Iversen et al., 2008), rhythm, or duration (Patel and Daniele, 2003; Hannon, 2009; Marie et al., 2012) and melody (Pfordresher and Brown, 2009; Bidelman et al., 2011). Some of these effects arise during infancy (Yoshida et al., 2010), but others have been observed in newborns. For example, the intonation of newborn cries appears to mimic their native language intonation contours, with French newborns producing rising cries and German newborns producing falling cries (Mampe et al., 2009). Although crying may or may not be a precursor to singing, given the observed parallels between the melodies of song and those of speech (Patel, 2006), it is possible that newborns might also prefer musical melodies that echo the intonation patterns of their native spoken language. Future research on this question is needed.
Although no studies directly examine whether exposure to internal sounds such as the heartbeat can give rise to newborn musical preferences, some evidence is consistent with this possibility. Musical tempo, defined as the number of beats per minute (BPM), is used by composers to manipulate the subjective speed and mood of a piece of music. Tempo can influence adults’ and children’s musical preferences and emotional responses to music, often taking precedence over other musical elements such as instrumentation, vocal vibrato, and genre (LeBlanc, 1982; LeBlanc and McCrary, 1983). Listeners generally prefer a tempo of 100 bpm (Fraisse, 1957, 1967; Drake and Botte, 1993), and this 600 ms interval is often termed the “indifference interval” that is neither “too slow” nor “too fast” (Wundt, 1910; Fraisse, 1963). Listeners have difficulty processing tempi faster than 600 bpm (Friberg and Sundström, 2002; London, 2004) or slower than 24 bpm (Fraisse, 1982; McAuley et al., 2006). Although systematic tempo preferences are not seen in 4-month-old infants (Baruch et al., 2004), infants do show better discrimination of periodic tone sequences that fall in the optimal (i.e., 600 ms) than suboptimal tempo range (Baruch and Drake, 1997).
There is striking individual variation in tempo preferences over the course of the lifespan, with preferred tempo generally decreasing from ~ 200 bpm in young children (Drake et al., 2000; Provasi and Bobin-Bègue, 2003) to 85 bpm in older adults (Vanneste et al., 2001; McAuley et al., 2006). Interestingly, some studies suggest that the natural, spontaneously occurring tempo in adults closely mirrors the adult resting heart rate (Temperley, 1963; Gerstenblith et al., 1977). The decrease in preferred tempo with age is particularly relevant given that the resting heart rate is also known to decrease with age (for review see, Tanaka et al., 2001). Further, when asked to control the tempo of pure tones (Iwanaga, 1995a) or musical pieces (Iwanaga, 1995b), adults pick a preferred tempo closest to their own heartbeat. While this evidence is suggestive of a potential link between heart rate and musical tempo preference, it does not establish causality, and no studies have systematically examined musical tempo preferences among newborns as a function of the maternal or neonatal heartbeat.
Preferences for musical regularity or isochrony (characterized by a 1:1 inter-beat ratio) might also be linked to the very regular, isochronous heartbeat. Adult listeners appear to be strongly drawn towards temporal isochrony in both perception and production. For example, when asked to spontaneously invent or repeat rhythmic patterns, Western adults produce either isochronous intervals (e.g., 1:1) or simple long-short rhythms with a 2:1 ratio (Fraisse, 1982; Essens and Povel, 1985; Povel and Essens, 1985; Essens, 1986; Semjen and Ivry, 2001; Repp et al., 2002; Snyder et al., 2006), a bias that is observed in musicians and non-musicians (Collier and Wright, 1995; Repp et al., 2005). Brain activity also varies depending on whether the participants are producing simple or complex interval ratios, with greater prefrontal activation, suggesting greater reliance on memory resources, observed during production of complex ratios (Sakai et al., 1999).
Preferences and biases towards isochrony are at least partly experience-driven, because isochrony of musical beat and meter is by no means a cultural universal. Music from the Balkans, India, and many other regions contains beat patterns with more complex ratios, such as 3:2 (Merriam, 1981; London, 1995; Clayton, 2000), and listeners from these cultures do not show the same biases towards isochrony (Hannon and Trehub, 2005a; Hannon et al., 2012). Culture-specific biases towards musical isochrony emerge between 6 and 12 months of age and can be altered through exposure to non-isochronous music (Hannon and Trehub, 2005a,b). However, postnatal listening experience cannot account for all aspects of musical temporal processing during infancy. For example, even before 7 months of age, which is prior to culture-specific narrowing of musical rhythm perception, infants exhibit listening preferences and processing advantages for regular over highly irregular sequences (Nakata and Mitani, 2005; Hannon et al., 2011). Despite the difference in listening environments, Turkish and American infants prefer to listen to isochronous (2:1) or moderately non-isochronous (3:2) melodies over highly complex non-isochronous melodies (Soley and Hannon, 2010). Even long-term exposure to non-isochronous meters does not give Turkish adults an advantage over American adults in detecting changes to highly complex meters (Hannon et al., 2012).
The above evidence reveals that humans have musical preferences for patterns that resemble the regularity and tempo of the human heartbeat. It is unclear, however, whether or not these preferences arise because of exposure to the heartbeat in utero or due to experience with periodic structures heard after birth. Early auditory experience is crucial for perceiving and producing rhythmic structures, as evidenced by individuals with congenital beat deafness (Phillips-Silver et al., 2011). Pre- and post-natally, infants are exposed to periodic biological phenomena, such as breathing and locomotion. One possibility is that periodic movement such as locomotion may be the primary driver of tempo preferences, a proposal that is consistent with robust connections between auditory and motor processes in rhythmic behavior, and the finding that preferred tempi decrease with age and body size (Drake et al., 2000; Trainor, 2007). However, tempo biases and beat induction are evident during infancy even prior to locomotion (Baruch and Drake, 1997; Winkler et al., 2009), suggesting that maternal movement may be more influential than the infant’s own movements, at least during infancy (Phillips-Silver and Trainor, 2005, 2007). To provide more compelling evidence of a link between prenatal exposure and musical preferences, it would be necessary to measure maternal heart rate or walking speed during the last trimester and compare it with infants’ preferred tempo and regularity at birth. Moreover, it would be ideal to directly manipulate the prenatal auditory environment through exercise manipulations, instructions to the mother, and so forth as prior studies have done for language stimuli (DeCasper and Spence, 1986).
The musical brain is shaped by its auditory environment during development. Auditory experience does not commence at birth, but during the months prior to birth. Thus, the unique intrauterine auditory environment, dominated by the isochronous maternal heartbeat as well as filtered but structured linguistic and musical input from the external environment, must be considered for its role in influencing perception of and preferences for musical patterns during infancy that feed into later development. Given the large corpus of evidence that prenatal auditory experience plays a critical role in language development, further research should investigate its role in the development of musical preferences. Moreover, the importance of auditory experience during the last trimester of pregnancy has critical implications for designing neonatal intensive care units for preterm infants, who are deprived of a typical prenatal auditory environment.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
The authors would like to thank Erin McMahon for her helpful comments during the writing of this review.
References
Arnon, S., Shapsa, A., Forman, L., Regev, R., Bauer, S., Litmanovitz, I., et al. (2006). Live music is beneficial to preterm infants in the neonatal intensive care unit environment. Birth 33, 131–136. doi: 10.1111/j.0730-7659.2006.00090.x
Barkat, T. R., Polley, D. B., and Hensch, T. K. (2011). A critical period for auditory thalamocortical connectivity. Nat. Neurosci. 14, 1189–1194. doi: 10.1038/nn.2882
Baruch, C., and Drake, C. (1997). Tempo discrimination in infants. Infant Behav. Dev. 20, 573–577. doi: 10.1016/s0163-6383(97)90049-7
Baruch, C., Panissal-Vieu, N., and Drake, C. (2004). Preferred perceptual tempo for sound sequences: comparison of adults, children, and infants. Percept. Mot. Skills 98, 325–339. doi: 10.2466/pms.98.1.325-339
Benbadis, S., Husain, A., Kaplan, P., and Tatum, W. (2007). Handbook of EEG Interpretation. New York, NY: Demos Medical Publishing.
Bidelman, G. M., Gandour, J. T., and Krishnan, A. (2011). Cross-domain effects of music and language experience on the representation of pitch in the human auditory brainstem. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 424–434. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21362
Busnel, M. C., Granier-Deferre, C., and Lecanuet, J. P. (1992). Fetal audition. Ann. N Y Acad. Sci. 662, 118–194. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1992.tb22857.x
Cevasco, A. M. (2008). The effects of mothers’ singing on full-term and preterm infants and maternal emotional responses. J. Music Ther. 45, 273–306.
Chapman, J. S. (1978). The relationship between auditory stimulation and gross motor activity of short-gestation infants. Res. Nurs. Health 1, 29–36. doi: 10.1002/nur.4770010107
Charpak, N., Ruiz, J., Zupan, J., Cattaneo, A., Figueroa, Z., Tessier, R., et al. (2005). Kangaroo mother care: 25 years after. Acta Paediatr. 94, 514–522. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2005.tb01930.x
Cheour-Luhtanen, M., Alho, K., Sainio, K., Rinne, T., Reinikainen, K., Pohjavuori, M., et al. (1996). The ontogenetically earliest discriminative response of the human brain. Psychophysiology 33, 478–481. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1996.tb01074.x
Collier, G., and Wright, C. (1995). Temporal rescaling of simple and complex ratios in rhythmic tapping. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 21, 602–627. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.21.3.602
Cooper, R., and Aslin, R. (1989). The language environment of the young infant: implications for early perceptual development. Can. J. Psychol. 43, 247–265. doi: 10.1037/h0084216
DeCasper, A. J., and Fifer, W. (1980). Of human bonding: newborns prefer their mothers’ voices. Science 208, 1174–1176. doi: 10.1126/science.7375928
DeCasper, A. J., and Prescott, P. (2009). Lateralized processes constrain auditory reinforcement in human newborns. Hear. Res. 255, 135–141. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2009.06.012
DeCasper, A. J., and Sigafoos, D. (1983). The intrauterine heartbeat: a potent reinforce for newborns. Infant Behav. Dev. 6, 19–25. doi: 10.1016/s0163-6383(83)80004-6
DeCasper, A. J., and Spence, M. (1986). Prenatal maternal speech influences newborns’ perception of speech sounds. Infant Behav. Dev. 9, 133–150. doi: 10.1016/0163-6383(86)90025-1
Doheny, L., Hurwitz, S., Insoft, R., Ringer, S., and Lahav, A. (2012a). Exposure to biological maternal sounds improves cardiorespiratory regulation in extremely preterm infants. J. Matern. Fetal Neonatal Med. 25, 1591–1594. doi: 10.3109/14767058.2011.648237
Doheny, L., Morey, J. A., Ringer, S., and Lahav, A. (2012b). Reduced frequency of apnea and bradycardia episodes caused by exposure to biological maternal sounds. Pediatr. Int. 54, e1–e3. doi: 10.1111/j.1442-200x.2012.03575.x
Drake, C., and Botte, M. (1993). Tempo sensitivity in auditory sequences: evidence for a multiple-look model. Percept. Psychophys. 54, 277–286. doi: 10.3758/bf03205262
Drake, C., Jones, M., and Baruch, C. (2000). The development of rhythmic attending in auditory sequences: attunement, referent period, focal attending. Cognition 77, 251–288. doi: 10.1016/s0010-0277(00)00106-2
Dunham, P. (1990). “Temporal structure of stimulation maintains infant attention,” in The Development of Attention: Research and Theory, ed J. Enns (North Holland: Elsevier), 67–85.
Essens, P. (1986). Hierarchical organization of temporal patterns. Percept. Psychophys. 40, 69–73. doi: 10.3758/bf03208185
Essens, P., and Povel, D. (1985). Metrical and nonmetrical representations of temporal patterns. Percept. Psychophys. 37, 1–7. doi: 10.3758/bf03207132
Fellman, V., Kushnerenko, E., Mikkola, K., Ceponoene, R., Leipala, J., and Naatanen, R. (2004). Atypical auditory event-related potentials in preterm infants during the first year of life: a possible sign of cognitive dysfunction. Pediatr. Res. 56, 291–297. doi: 10.1203/01.PDR.0000132750.97066.B9
Fraisse, P. (1957). La Psychologie du Temps (English Translation). Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.
Fraisse, P. (1967). Le seuil differéntiel de durée dans une suite régulière d’intervalles (english translation). Année Psychol. lxvii, 43–49.
Fraisse, P. (1982). “Rhythm and tempo,” in The Psychology of Music, ed D. Deutsch (New York: Academic Press), 149–180.
Friberg, A., and Sundström, A. (2002). Swing ratios and ensemble timing in jazz performance: evidence for a common rhythmic pattern. Music Percept. 19, 333–349. doi: 10.1525/mp.2002.19.3.333
Gerhardt, K. J., and Abrams, R. M. (1996). Fetal hearing: characterization of the stimulus and response. Semin. Perinatol. 20, 11–20. doi: 10.1016/s0146-0005(96)80053-x
Gerstenblith, G., Frederiksen, J., Yin, F., Fortuin, N., Lakatta, E., and Weisfeldt, M. (1977). Echocardiographic assessment of a normal adult aging population. Circulation 56, 273–278. doi: 10.1161/01.cir.56.2.273
Granier-Deferre, C., Ribeiro, A., Jacquet, A. Y., and Bassereau, S. (2011). Near–term fetuses process temporal features of speech. Dev. Sci. 14, 336–352. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00978.x
Guihard-Costa, A. M., and Larroche, J. C. (1990). Differential growth between the fetal brain and its infratentorial part. Early Hum. Dev. 23, 27–40. doi: 10.1016/0378-3782(90)90126-4
Hannon, E. E. (2009). Perceiving speech rhythm in music: listeners categorize instrumental songs according to language of origin. Cognition 111, 404–410. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.03.003
Hannon, E. E., and Trainor, L. J. (2007). Music acquisition: effects of enculturation and formal training on development. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 466–472. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.08.008
Hannon, E. E., and Trehub, S. (2005a). Metrical categories in infancy and adulthood. Psychol. Sci. 16, 48–55. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.00779.x
Hannon, E. E., and Trehub, S. (2005b). Tuning in to musical rhythms: infants learn more readily than adults. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 102, 12639–12643. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0504254102
Hannon, E. E., Soley, G., and Levine, R. (2011). Constraints on infants’ musical rhythm perception: effects of interval ratio complexity and enculturation. Dev. Sci. 14, 1–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01036.x
Hannon, E. E., Soley, G., and Ullal, S. (2012). Familiarity overrides simplicity in rhythmic pattern perception: a cross-cultural examination of American and Turkish listeners. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 38, 543–548. doi: 10.1037/a0027225
Hepper, P. G. (1991). An examination of fetal learning before and after birth. Ir. J. Psychol. 12, 95–107. doi: 10.1080/03033910.1991.10557830
Huttenlocher, P. R., and Dabholkar, A. S. (1997). Regional differences in synaptogenesis in human cerebral cortex. J. Comp. Neurol. 387, 167–178. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(19971020)387:2<167::AID-CNE1>3.0.CO;2-Z
Iversen, J. R., Patel, A. D., and Ohgushi, K. (2008). Perception of rhythmic grouping depends on auditory experience. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 124, 2263–2271. doi: 10.1121/1.2973189
Iwanaga, I. (1995a). Relationship between heart rate and preference for tempo of music. Percept. Mot. Skills 81, 435–440. doi: 10.2466/pms.1995.81.2.435
Iwanaga, I. (1995b). Harmonic relationship between preferred tempi and heart rate. Percept. Mot. Skills 81, 67–71. doi: 10.2466/pms.1995.81.1.67
Jansson-Verkasalo, E., Ruusuvirta, T., Huotilainen, M., Alku, P., Kushnerenko, E., Suominen, K., et al. (2010). Atypical perceptual narrowing in prematurely born infants is associated with compromised language acquisition at 2 years of age. BMC Neurosci. 11:88. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-11-88
Jiang, Z. D. (1995). Maturation of auditory brainstem in low risk preterm infants: a comparison with age-matched full term infants up to 6 years. Early Hum. Dev. 42, 49–65. doi: 10.1016/0378-3782(95)01639-k
Kinzler, K. D., Dupoux, E., and Spelke, E. S. (2007). The native language of social cognition. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 104, 12577–12580. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0705345104
Kisilevsky, B. S., and Hains, S. M. (2009). Onset and maturation of fetal heart rate response to the mother’s voice over late gestation. Dev. Sci. 14, 214–223.
Kisilevsky, B. S., Hains, S. M., Jacquet, A.-Y., Granier-Deferre, C., and Lecanuet, J. P. (2004). Maturation of fetal responses to music. Dev. Sci. 7, 550–559. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00379.x
Kisilevsky, B. S., Hains, S. M., Lee, K., Xie, X., Huang, H., Ye, H. H., et al. (2003). Effects of experience on fetal voice recognition. Psychol. Sci. 14, 220–224. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.02435
Krueger, C., Parker, L., Chiu, S-H., and Theriaque, D. (2010). Maternal voice and short-term outcomes in preterm infants. Dev. Psychobiol. 52, 205–212. doi: 10.1002/dev.20426
LeBlanc, A., and McCrary, J. (1983). Effect of tempo on children’s music preference. J. Res. Music Educ. 31, 283–294. doi: 10.2307/3344631
Loewy, J., Stewart, K., Dassler, A., Telsey, A., and Homel, P. (2013). The effects of music therapy on vital signs, feeding, and sleep in premature infants. Pediatrics 131, 902–918. doi: 10.1542/peds.2012-1367
Loftin, R. W., Habli, M., Snyder, C. C., Cormier, C. M., Lewis, D. F., and Defranco, E. A. (2010). Late preterm birth. Rev. Obstet. Gynecol. 3, 10–19.
London, J. (1995). Some examples of complex meters and their implications for models of meter perception. Music Percept. 13, 59–77. doi: 10.2307/40285685
Ludington-Hoe, S., Lewis, T., Morgan, K., Cong, X., Anderson, L., and Reese, S. (2006). Breast and infant temperatures with twins during shared kangaroo care. J. Ostet. Gynecol. Neonatal Nurs. 35, 223–231.
Lynch, M. P., Eilers, R. E., Oller, D. K., and Urbano, R. C. (1990). Innateness, experience, and music perception. Psychol. Sci. 4, 272–276. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1990.tb00213.x
Lynch, M. P., Short, L. B., and Chua, R. (1995). Contributions of experience to the development of musical processing in infancy. Dev. Psychobiol. 28, 377–398.
Malloy, G. B. (1979). The relationship between maternal and musical auditory stimulation and the developmental behavior of premature infants. Birth Defects Orig. Artic. Ser. 15, 81–89.
Mampe, B., Friederici, A. D., Christophe, A., and Wermke, K. (2009). Newborns’ cry melody is shaped by their native language. Curr. Biol. 19, 1994–1997. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.09.064
Marie, C., Kujala, T., and Besson, M. (2012). Musical and linguistic expertise influence pre-attentive and attentive processing of non–speech sounds. Cortex 48, 447–457. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2010.11.006
McAuley, J., Jones, M., Holub, S., Johnston, H., and Miller, N. (2006). The time of our lives: lifespan development of timing and event tracking. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 135, 348–367. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.135.3.348
McMahon, E., Wintermark, P., and Lahav, A. (2012). Auditory brain development in premature infants: the importance of early experience. Ann. N Y Acad. Sci. 1252, 17–24. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2012.06445.x
Merriam, A. (1981). “African musical rhythms and concepts of time-reckoning,” in Music East and West: Essays in Honor of Walter Kaufman, ed T. Noblitt (New York: Pendragon Press).
Moon, C., and Fifer, W. (2000). Evidence of trans natal auditory learning. J. Perinatol. 20, S37–S44. doi: 10.1038/sj.jp.7200448
Moon, C., Cooper, R. P., and Fifer, W. P. (1993). Two-day-olds prefer their native language. Infant Behav. Dev. 169, 495–500. doi: 10.1016/0163-6383(93)80007-u
Moon, C., Lagercrantz, H., and Kuhl, P. K. (2012). Language experienced in utero affects vowel perception after birth: a two-country study. Acta Pediatr. 156–160. doi: 10.1111/apa.12098
Nakata, T., and Mitani, C. (2005). Influences of temporal function on infant attention. Music Percept. 22, 401–409. doi: 10.1525/mp.2005.22.3.401
Nazzi, T., Bertoncini, J., and Mehler, J. (1998). Language discrimination by newborns: toward an understanding of the role of rhythm. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 24, 756–766. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.24.3.756
Panneton, R. K., and DeCasper, A. J. (1984). Newborns prefer intrauterine heartbeat sounds to male voices. Poster presented at the 4th International Conference of Infant Studies, New York.
Patel, A. D. (2006). Musical rhythm, linguistic rhythm, and human evolution. Music Percept. 24, 99–104. doi: 10.1525/mp.2006.24.1.99
Patel, A. D., and Daniele, J. R. (2003). An empirical comparison of rhythm in language and music. Cognition 87, B35–B45. doi: 10.1016/s0010-0277(02)00187-7
Pena, M., Pittaluga, E., and Mehler, J. (2010). Language acquisition in premature and full-term infants. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 107, 3823–3828. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0914326107
Pena, M., Werker, J. F., and DeHaene-Lambertz, G. (2012). Earlier speech exposure does not accelerate speech acquisition. J. Neurosci. 32, 11159–11163. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.6516-11.2012
Pfordresher, P. Q., and Brown, S. (2009). Enhanced production and perception of musical pitch in tone language speakers. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 71, 1385–1398. doi: 10.3758/app.71.6.1385
Phillips-Silver, J., Toivainen, P., Gosselin, N., Piche, O., Nozaradan, S., Palmer, C., et al. (2011). Born to dance but beat deaf: a new form of congenital amusia. Neuropsychologia 49, 961–969. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.02.002
Phillips-Silver, J., and Trainor, L. J. (2005). Feeling the beat: movement influences infants’ rhythm perception. Science 308, 1430. doi: 10.1126/science.1110922
Phillips-Silver, J., and Trainor, L. J. (2007). Hearing what the body feels: auditory encoding of rhythmic movement. Cognition 105, 533–546. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2006.11.006
Povel, D., and Essens, P. (1985). Perception of temporal patterns. Music Percept. 2, 411–440. doi: 10.2307/40285311
Provasi, J., and Bobin-Bègue, A. (2003). Spontaneous motor tempo and rhythmical synchronization in 2 1/2 and 4 year-old children. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 27, 220–231. doi: 10.1080/01650250244000290
Ramus, F., Nespor, M., and Mehler, J. (1999). Correlates of linguistic rhythm in the speech signal. Cognition 73, 265–292. doi: 10.1016/s0010-0277(99)00058-x
Repp, B., London, J., and Keller, P. (2005). Production and synchronization of uneven rhythms at fast tempi. Music Percept. 27, 61–78. doi: 10.1525/mp.2005.23.1.61
Repp, B., Windsor, L., and Desain, P. (2002). Effects of tempo on the timing of simple musical rhythms. Music Percept. 19, 565–593. doi: 10.1525/mp.2002.19.4.565
Sakai, K., Hikosaka, O., Miyauchi, S., Takino, R., Tamada, T., Iwata, N., et al. (1999). Neural representations of a rhythm depends on its interval ratio. J. Neurosci. 15, 10074–10081.
Salk, L. (1960). The effects of the normal heartbeat sound on the behavior of the newborn infant: implications for mental health. World Ment. Health 12, 168–175.
Salk, L. (1961). “The importance of the heartbeat rhythm to human nature: theoretical, clinical, and experimental observations,” in Proceedings of the Third World Congress of Psychiatry (Toronto: University of Toronto Press).
Salk, L. (1962). Mothers’ heartbeat as an imprinting stimulus. Trans. N Y Acad. Sci. 31, 753–763. doi: 10.1111/j.2164-0947.1962.tb01441.x
Sanes, D., and Bao, S. (2009). Tuning up the developing auditory CNS. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 19, 188–199. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2009.05.014
Sansavini, A. (1997). Neonatal perception of the rhythmical structure of speech: the role of stress patterns. Infant Child Dev. 6, 3–13. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1099-0917(199703)6:1<3::aid-edp140>3.0.co;2-7
Segall, M. (1972). Cardiac responsivity to auditory stimulation in premature infants. Nurs. Res. 21, 15–19.
Semjen, A., and Ivry, R. (2001). The coupled oscillator model of between–hand coordination in alternate-hand tapping: a reappraisal. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 27, 251–265. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.27.2.251
Smith, C., and Steinschneider, A. (1975). Differential effects of prenatal rhythmic stimulation on neonatal arousal states. Child Dev. 46, 547–578.
Snyder, J., Hannon, E., Large, E., and Christiansen, M. (2006). Synchronization and continuation tapping to complex meters. Music Percept. 24, 135–146. doi: 10.1525/mp.2006.24.2.135
Soley, G., and Hannon, E. (2010). Infants prefer the musical meter of their own culture: a cross-cultural comparison. Dev. Psychol. 46, 286–292. doi: 10.1037/a0017555
Spence, M., and DeCasper, A. (1987). Prenatal experience with low-frequency maternal-voice sounds influence neonatal perception of maternal voice samples. Infant Behav. Dev. 16, 133–142. doi: 10.1016/0163-6383(87)90028-2
Standley, J. M. (2002). A meta-analysis of the efficacy of music therapy for premature infants. J. Pediatr. Nurs. 17, 107–113. doi: 10.1053/jpdn.2002.124128
Standley, J. M., and Moore, R. S. (1995). Therapeutic effects of music and mother’s voice on premature infants. Pediatr. Nurs. 21, 509–512.
Tanaka, H., Monahan, K. D., and Seals, D. R. (2001). Age redicted maximal heart rate revisited. J. Am. Coll. Cardiol. 37, 153–156. doi: 10.1016/s0735-1097(00)01054-8
Temperley, N. (1963). Personal tempo and subject attenuation. J. Gen. Psychol. 63, 267–287. doi: 10.1080/00221309.1963.9920534
Trainor, L. J. (2007). Do preferred beat rate and entrainment to the beat have a common origin in movement? Empiri. Musicol. Rev. 2, 17–20.
Tulloch, J. D., Brown, B. S., Jacobs, H. L., Prugh, D. G., and Greene, W. A. (1964). Normal heartbeat sound and the behavior of newborn infants: a replication study. Psychosom. Med. 26, 661–670.
Vanneste, S., Pouthas, V., and Wearden, J. (2001). Temporal control of rhythmic performance: a comparison. Exp. Aging Res. 27, 83–102. doi: 10.1080/03610730125798
Wenk, B. J. (1987). Just in time: on speech rhythms in music. Linguistics 25, 969–982. doi: 10.1515/ling.1987.25.5.969
Werker, J. F., and Yeung, H. H. (2005). Infant speech perception bootstraps word learning. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 519–527. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.09.003
Winkler, I., Haden, G. P., Ladinig, O., Sziller, I., and Honing, H. (2009). Newborn infants detect the beat in music. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 106, 2468–2471. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0809035106
Yoshida, K. A., Iversen, J. R., Patel, A. D., Mazuka, R., Nito, H., Gervain, J., et al. (2010). The development of perceptual grouping biases in infancy: a Japanese–English crosslinguistic study. Cognition 115, 356–361. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2010.01.005
Keywords: language, music, maternal heartbeat, auditory perception, preterm, development, tempo, isochronous
Citation: Ullal-Gupta S, Vanden Bosch der Nederlanden CM, Tichko P, Lahav A, and Hannon EE (2013) Linking prenatal experience to the emerging musical mind. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 7:48. doi:10.3389/fnsys.2013.00048
Received: 12 June 2013; Accepted: 16 August 2013;
Published online: 03 September 2013.
Edited by:
Isabelle Peretz, Université de Montréal, CanadaReviewed by:
Minna Huotilainen, University of Helsinki, FinlandSandra E. Trehub, University of Toronto, Canada
Copyright © 2013 Ullal-Gupta, Vanden Bosch der Nederlanden, Tichko, Lahav and Hannon. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Amir Lahav, Department of Newborn Medicine, Brigham and Women’s Hospital, Harvard Medical School, 75 Francis Street, Boston, MA 02115, USA e-mail: Amir_Lahav@hms.harvard.edu;
Dr. Erin E. Hannon, Department of Psychology, University of Nevada, 4505 Maryland Parkway #455030, Las Vegas, NV 89154, USA e-mail: erin.hannon@unlv.edu