Skip to main content

OPINION article

Front. Neuroergonomics, 31 May 2021
Sec. Augmented and Synthetic Neuroergonomics
This article is part of the Research Topic Understanding Brain Mechanisms Underpinning Physical Movement and Exercise View all 9 articles

Single Case Experimental Design: A New Approach for Non-invasive Brain Stimulation Research?

  • Graduate School of Health, Discipline of Physiotherapy, University of Technology Sydney, Sydney, NSW, Australia

Introduction

Due to the large proportion of people living with chronic neurological impairments, non-invasive brain stimulation (NIBS) was developed as a potential adjuvant to enhance neurological rehabilitation. NIBS techniques such as transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) and repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) have been proposed to temporarily modulate neural excitability in a given direction based on the type of stimulation used. For example, seminal studies in tDCS reported that anodal tDCS over the motor cortex (M1) increased corticomotor excitability, whereas M1 cathodal tDCS decreased corticomotor excitability, for up to an hour (Nitsche and Paulus, 2001; Nitsche et al., 2003). The polarity dependent changes in corticomotor excitability were suggested to represent up-regulation or down-regulation of neural plasticity (Nitsche and Paulus, 2001; Nitsche et al., 2003) and depend on several stimulation parameters (Woods et al., 2016). When delivered before or during rehabilitation NIBS may increase the beneficial neuroplastic effects of rehabilitation alone.

Subsequently, the field of NIBS grew rapidly. Many researchers applied tDCS or rTMS to a wide range of clinical populations and explored many variations in experimental parameters. Most commonly, researchers opted to use a randomized controlled trial (RCT) study design, allocating participants to either; real vs. sham stimulation, facilitatory vs. inhibitory stimulation (e.g., anodal vs. cathodal tDCS), stimulation alone vs. stimulation with therapy, or some combination of the above in a multi-arm trial. In addition, NIBS trials have typically used functional and/or neurophysiological outcomes (such as motor learning tasks or measures of corticomotor excitability after M1 stimulation) to determine the after-effects of stimulation, often followed up at multiple time-points to determine the duration of effects. The ease and reported effectiveness of sham stimulation (e.g., fade in-fade out protocol for tDCS, or sham coil rTMS) (Gandiga et al., 2006; Mennemeier et al., 2009) allows robust methodological study designs such as double-blinded cross-over RCTs frequently used in NIBS research.

However, despite the rapid rise of NIBS research over the past 20 years, current evidence has largely not supported translation into clinical practice, with only rTMS as a treatment for drug-resistant depression adopted clinically so far (O'Connell et al., 2018; Elsner et al., 2020). One of the major criticisms of NIBS that likely acts as a barrier to translation is the substantial between-subject variability observed in response to stimulation (López-Alonso et al., 2014; Wiethoff et al., 2014; McCambridge et al., 2015). With some causal studies of healthy participants showing that only half of participants respond to NIBS as expected (López-Alonso et al., 2014; Wiethoff et al., 2014). Therefore, when investigating group-level changes in outcomes the results are unlikely to show statistical or clinically meaningful differences and may mask the positive benefits experienced by some participants. Yet, it is not surprising that substantial between-subject variability exists given the lack of precision in current NIBS studies that typically adopt a “one-size-fits-all” approach to delivery of the intervention.

In the literature, several determinants of NIBS are known to influence stimulation response, both interventional (e.g., type and location of stimulation) and biological factors (e.g., skull thickness, brain morphology and neurochemistry) (Filmer et al., 2019; Hordacre et al., 2021) that should be taken into consideration when delivering NIBS. Researchers have discussed the need for individualized or tailored stimulation protocols, using known determinants of NIBS to guide stimulation protocols to help address wide-scale variability in responses (Di Pino et al., 2014; McCambridge et al., 2018; Hordacre et al., 2021). For example in tDCS research, individualized stimulation could tailor the stimulation protocol based on clinical characteristics of the patient (e.g., lesion location, structural integrity of neural pathways) (Di Pino et al., 2014) or individually modeled electric fields (Antonenko et al., 2019) or a combination of many factors including methodological (e.g., dosage and biological factors) (Hordacre et al., 2021) (Figure 1). Patient-tailored brain stimulation would be a sensible approach, though the feasibility of conducting a large-scale RCT with highly-precise individualized stimulation protocols for each participant would be difficult, particularly in a clinical population. Therefore, in this opinion article, we consider if an alternate methodological approach to investigating the effectiveness of NIBS in lab-based or clinical settings would be of interest to the field.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. (A) Example of individualized transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) that could be tested using SCED methodology. (B) Hypothetical data for an A-B-C-B-C-A design where five measurements are taken in each of the six phases so that three phase changes between B and C can be reported on (e.g., B → C, C → B, and B → C again).

Single-Case Experimental Designs

To our knowledge, a study design that has not yet been employed in NIBS research is a single case experimental design (SCED). SCEDs are clinical trials of an individual (or several individuals each studied as a single case), in contrast to trials of groups of participants (Tate and Perdices, 2019). They have also been called single-system designs, single-subject designs, single-case research designs, single participant designs, interrupted time-series designs, and N-of-1 trials. As with many other study designs, the results of SCEDs can be included in systematic reviews (Tanious and Onghena, 2020). To establish a causal relationship between a target behavior and an intervention, SCEDs have four essential elements: (1) the participant is their own control, (2) a priori methods are used, (3) the target behavior is repeatedly measured before, during and after an intervention, and (4) the intervention is systematically manipulated (Tate and Perdices, 2019). Inferences about the intervention are then drawn from the repeated demonstration of the intervention effect on the outcome. Table 1 shows the four classifications of SCED designs: (1) withdrawal/reversal designs, (2) multiple-baselines designs, (3) alternating-treatments designs, and (4) changing-criterion designs. A range of modifications and combinations of these four approaches have been used to date (Shadish and Sullivan, 2011), with some designs better suited to particular type of research questions (see Table 1). One key appeal of a SCED is the ability to individualize an intervention to a given participant, as would be done in many clinical practice settings. For health research, SCEDs are considered Level 1 evidence (OCEBM Levels of Evidence Working Group, 2011) and the methodological quality of SCEDs can be assessed and guided by the recently developed Single-Case Reporting guideline In BEhavioural interventions (SCRIBE) 2016 checklist (Tate et al., 2016a,b).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Classification of SCED designs adapted from Chapter 1 of Tate and Perdices (2019).

Example Protocol for Non-Invasive Brain Stimulation

Planning

To further elaborate on SCEDs and the potential utility for NIBS research, we propose an example SCED protocol to answer the research question “Is motor cortex anodal tDCS more effective than sham tDCS at increasing corticomotor excitability in chronic stroke participants?” To answer this question, a withdrawal multiple treatment design is appropriate (see Table 1). In this example, an A-B-C-B-C-A design could be used where A = baseline or follow-up with no stimulation, B = sham tDCS, and C = real tDCS. This design provides three opportunities to examine the experimental effect between B and C. To establish the absolute effectiveness of B or C compared to baseline, the design would require at least three phase changes involving A and C, and three involving A and B. This is a very cumbersome design [e.g., A-B-C-B-C-A-B-A-C-A (Tate and Perdices, 2019)]. One ethical and practical consideration here is whether the design should end on an active intervention phase so that the study ends on a potentially beneficial treatment for the patient. The appropriate duration of each phase is dependent on the “wash-out” period needed after stimulation, estimated to be 1–1.5 h for anodal tDCS (Nitsche and Paulus, 2001) thereby minimizing possible confounding “alternation effects” (Tate and Perdices, 2019). Within each phase, at least five measurements of the outcome measure should be taken (see Figure 1B for hypothetical data) to account for possible variability (Kratochwill et al., 2013). In this example, 10 TMS-pulses could be delivered every 4–5 s at each measurement to provide a total of five average MEP amplitudes per phase.

Randomization and blinding are important for the internal validity of a SCED. Randomization can be achieved in some SCED classifications by randomizing the phases, or randomizing the onset in a multiple-baseline study. In our A-B-C-B-C-A example, the sequence of phases is not randomized. However, blinding could be achieved by concealing the stimulation type (real, sham) to the participants and the outcome assessors and not revealing the order of phases or having a separate researcher administer the intervention that is not involved in data collection or analysis. For example, sham tDCS protocols set up the electrodes the same as in real stimulation except the current is ramped up to provide sensory stimulation on the scalp and then ramped down again to no stimulation. Some tDCS machines can also be pre-programmed to do the “fade in-fade out” protocol without the experimenter's knowledge or use a separate experimenter deliver the stimulation. In addition, blinding during the analysis could be achieved by not disclosing the phase (baseline, real, sham, follow up) to the statistician analyzing the data until all analyses are complete.

Considerations

In terms of reporting on a SCED study, it is important to thoroughly report the selection criteria, participant characteristics, setting, outcome measures, equipment, intervention details, and the procedural fidelity of the intervention. A thorough description is required for replication and to maximize the external validity of the study, for example, researchers may want to repeat the trial on three or more participants that represent the clinical population (i.e., young and old, male and female, different settings etc.).

Detailed descriptions of all phases of the intervention are required (i.e., baseline, real, sham) in SCED studies. If the study uses patient-tailored brain stimulation then a comprehensive description should be given about how stimulation is individualized (i.e., based on what parameters) so that replication of the intervention application and how it was individualized could be achieved in subsequent trials. Reporting how and when the intervention was delivered as well as the intervention dosage (e.g., current strength, duration of stimulation, electrode size and placement etc.) is of particular importance. Similar detail should be given to describing the sham intervention, for example the current strength and duration of the tDCS fade in-fade out protocol (Ambrus et al., 2012).

Analysis

To begin planning a SCED data analysis, it is important to first determine if changes in level, trend, and/or variability of the outcome measure are indicative of a treatment effect. For example, if the outcome measure was corticomotor excitability assessed with TMS–induced motor evoked potentials (MEP), then a change in the level of corticomotor excitability (i.e., amplitude of the MEPs) with real NIBS compared to sham and baseline would indicate stimulation was effective. Similarly, if the outcome was a motor learning task you may hypothesize a change in the trend (i.e., rate of improvement) with real NIBS would indicate that the treatment was effective. Visual and statistical analysis supplement each other in SCED studies and need to be utilized in conjunction (Tate and Perdices, 2019), though controversy about analysis procedures exist.

The approach selected for visual and statistical analysis is essential to consider. Kratochwill et al. (2013) includes four steps in visually evaluating the phases of a SCED study which can be summarized as: (1) “Is the data stable at baseline?” (2) “Are there any trends or variability?” (3) “How long until there is an effect?” and (4) “Did the control work?” Various techniques exist to visually analyse data in a systematic and objective manner, including descriptive statistics, level changes, split-middle trend lines, variability and evaluating trends in each phase (Parsonson et al., 1992; Lane and Gast, 2014; Barton et al., 2018). For statistical analysis methods, detailed guides are available (Manolov and Solanas Pérez, 2018) because a range of issues may influence the validity of statistical tests (Parker et al., 2011; Velicer and Molenaar, 2013; Harrington and Velicer, 2015). Statistical analyses are valuable (1) when variability appears large upon visual inspection, (2) when effects of interventions are not yet well-understood, (3) when small but important changes in target behaviors cannot be detected by visual analysis, or (4) to enhance replication studies (Kazdin and Tuma, 1982).

Advantages and Challenges

SCEDs are a useful study design to further explore NIBS, particularly for patient-tailored NIBS protocols. Similar to a RCT, SCEDs are considered Level 1 evidence for health interventions (OCEBM Levels of Evidence Working Group, 2011). One advantage of a SCED over a large-scale RCT is that it is more cost effective, because it requires fewer participants and resources. Not requiring large sample sizes to achieve statistical power may be of particular importance for low-incidence patient populations that often suffer from being under-powered. In addition, because SCEDs use an individual as their own control, outcome measures can be interpreted as absolute values relative to the participant's baseline. Interpretation of absolute values would avoid non-validated normalization procedures used in some neurophysiological research (e.g., pre-post, pre-post/post, pre-post/pre+post). Although tailored-interventions can be used in RCTs, SCEDs are also suitable for patient-tailored treatments that may involve clinical judgements that are commonplace in clinical practice. In a SCED study, individual modifications to the protocol can be extensively explained for each participant so that replication is possible. Furthermore, exploring patient-tailored NIBS with a SCED design can also inform how a tailored intervention could be further tested in a large-scale RCT.

However, there are some important challenges that should be considered. A current challenge to SCED research is the generalizability of findings, particularly for very heterogenous patient populations (e.g., stroke, dystonia). Replication is therefore required, though guidance on how many participants are needed for acceptable replication is lacking. SCED's can also be burdensome to participants because they require extensive data collection. Another challenge is that SCED's are vulnerable to plausible rival hypotheses that may explain the outcomes such as, maturation, regression to the mean and external factors (Caneiro et al., 2019). Further, there are currently no agreed upon statistical analysis procedures. Criticism of the subjective nature of visual inspection, and hence acceptability of SCEDs as Level 1 evidence by the field, could also be a barrier to translation.

Summary

In summary, the fundamental aspects of NIBS appear to be suitable for studies using SCED methodology. This approach may be a useful avenue to further investigate inter-individual variability and more advanced individualized stimulation protocols. In addition, due to the relative ease and safety of modulating the independent variable in NIBS research, NIBS may also be a candidate field to improve and develop SCED methodology.

Author Contributions

AM: conception of the idea. AM and JP: manuscript preparation and approval of final paper. Both authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Ambrus, G. G., Al-Moyed, H., Chaieb, L., Sarp, L., Antal, A., and Paulus, W. (2012). The fade-in—short stimulation—fade out approach to sham tDCS—reliable at 1 mA for naïve and experienced subjects, but not investigators. Brain Stimul. 5, 499–504. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2011.12.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Antonenko, D., Thielscher, A., Saturnino, G. B., Aydin, S., Ittermann, B., Grittner, U., et al. (2019). Towards precise brain stimulation: is electric field simulation related to neuromodulation? Brain Stimul. 12, 1159–1168. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2019.03.072

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barton, E. E., Lloyd, B. P., Spriggs, A. D., and Gast, D. L. (2018). “Visual analysis of graphic data,” in Single Case Research Methodology: Applications in Special Education and Behavioral Sciences, eds J. R. Ledford and D. L. Gast (New York, NY: Routledge), 179–214.

Google Scholar

Caneiro, J. P., Smith, A., Linton, S. J., Moseley, G. L., and O'Sullivan, P. (2019). How does change unfold? an evaluation of the process of change in four people with chronic low back pain and high pain-related fear managed with Cognitive Functional Therapy: a replicated single-case experimental design study. Behav. Res. Ther. 117, 28–39. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2019.02.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Di Pino, G., Pellegrino, G., Assenza, G., Capone, F., Ferreri, F., Formica, D., et al. (2014). Modulation of brain plasticity in stroke: a novel model for neurorehabilitation. Nat. Rev. Neurol. 10, 597–608. doi: 10.1038/nrneurol.2014.162

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elsner, B., Kugler, J., Pohl, M., and Mehrholz, J. (2020). Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) for improving activities of daily living, and physical and cognitive functioning, in people after stroke. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 3:CD009645. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD009645.pub4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Filmer, H. L., Ehrhardt, S. E., Bollmann, S., Mattingley, J. B., and Dux, P. E. (2019). Accounting for individual differences in the response to tDCS with baseline levels of neurochemical excitability. Cortex 115, 324–334. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2019.02.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gandiga, P. C., Hummel, F. C., and Cohen, L. G. (2006). Transcranial DC stimulation (tDCS): a tool for double-blind sham-controlled clinical studies in brain stimulation. Clin. Neurophysiol. 117, 845–850. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2005.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harrington, M., and Velicer, W. F. (2015). Comparing visual and statistical analysis in single-case studies using published studies. Multivar. Behav. Res. 50, 162–183. doi: 10.1080/00273171.2014.973989

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hordacre, B., McCambridge, A. B., Ridding, M. C., and Bradnam, L. V. (2021). Can transcranial direct current stimulation enhance poststroke motor recovery? development of a theoretical patient-tailored model. Neurology doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000012187. [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kazdin, A. E., and Tuma, A. H. (1982). Single-Case Research Designs. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Kratochwill, T. R., Hitchcock, J. H., Horner, R. H., Levin, J. R., Odom, S. L., Rindskopf, D. M., et al. (2013). Single-case intervention research design standards. Remedial Spec. Educ. 34, 26–38. doi: 10.1177/0741932512452794

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lane, J. D., and Gast, D. L. (2014). Visual analysis in single case experimental design studies: brief review and guidelines. Neuropsychol. Rehabil. 24, 445–463. doi: 10.1080/09602011.2013.815636

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

López-Alonso, V., Cheeran, B., Río-Rodríguez, D., and Fernández-Del-Olmo, M. (2014). Inter-individual variability in response to non-invasive brain stimulation paradigms. Brain Stimul. 7, 372–380. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2014.02.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manolov, R., and Solanas Pérez, A. (2018). Analytical options for single-case experimental designs: review and application to brain impairment. Brain Impair. 19, 18–32. doi: 10.1017/BrImp.2017.17

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCambridge, A. B., Stinear, J. W., and Byblow, W. D. (2015). “I-wave” recruitment determines response to tDCS in the upper limb, but only so far. Brain Stimul. 8, 1124–1129. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2015.07.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCambridge, A. B., Stinear, J. W., and Byblow, W. D. (2018). Revisiting interhemispheric imbalance in chronic stroke: a tDCS study. Clin. Neurophysiol. 129, 42–50. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2017.10.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mennemeier, M., Triggs, W., Chelette, K., Woods, A., Kimbrell, T., and Dornhoffer, J. (2009). Sham transcranial magnetic stimulation using electrical stimulation of the scalp. Brain Stimul. 2, 168–173. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2009.02.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nitsche, M. A., Nitsche, M. S., Klein, C. C., Tergau, F., Rothwell, J. C., and Paulus, W. (2003). Level of action of cathodal DC polarisation induced inhibition of the human motor cortex. Clin. Neurophysiol. 114, 600–604. doi: 10.1016/S1388-2457(02)00412-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nitsche, M. A., and Paulus, W. (2001). Sustained excitability elevations induced by transcranial DC motor cortex stimulation in humans. Neurology 57, 1899–1901. doi: 10.1212/WNL.57.10.1899

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

OCEBM Levels of Evidence Working Group (2011). The Oxford Levels of Evidence 2. Oxford: Centre for Evidence-Based Medicine.

Google Scholar

O'Connell, N. E., Marston, L., Spencer, S., DeSouza, L. H., and Wand, B. M. (2018). Non-invasive brain stimulation techniques for chronic pain. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 3:CD008208. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD008208.pub5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parker, R. I., Vannest, K. J., and Davis, J. L. (2011). Effect size in single-case research: a review of nine nonoverlap techniques. Behav. Modif. 35, 303–322. doi: 10.1177/0145445511399147

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parsonson, B. S., Baer, D. M., Kratochwill, T., and Levin, J. (1992). “The visual analysis of data, and current research into the stimuli controlling it,” in Single-Case Research Design and Analysis: New Directions for Psychology and Education, eds T. R. Kratochwill and J. R. Levin (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.), 15–40.

Google Scholar

Shadish, W. R., and Sullivan, K. J. (2011). Characteristics of single-case designs used to assess intervention effects in 2008. Behav. Res. Methods 43, 971–980. doi: 10.3758/s13428-011-0111-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tanious, R., and Onghena, P. (2020). A systematic review of applied single-case research published between 2016 and 2018: study designs, randomization, data aspects, data analysis. Behav. Res. Methods doi: 10.3758/s13428-020-01502-4. [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tate, R., and Perdices, M. (2019). Single-Case Experimental Designs for Clinical Research and Neurorehabilitation Settings: Planning, Conduct, Analysis, and Reporting. London: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Tate, R. L., Perdices, M., Rosenkoetter, U., McDonald, S., Togher, L., Shadish, W., et al. (2016b). The Single-Case Reporting Guideline In BEhavioural Interventions (SCRIBE) 2016: explanation and elaboration. Arch. Sci. Psychol. 4, 10–31. doi: 10.1037/arc0000027

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tate, R. L., Perdices, M., Rosenkoetter, U., Shadish, W., Vohra, S., Barlow, D. H., et al. (2016a). The Single-Case Reporting Guideline In BEhavioural Interventions (SCRIBE) 2016 statement. Phys. Ther. 96, e1–e10. doi: 10.2522/ptj.2016.96.7.e1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Velicer, W., and Molenaar, P. (2013). “T ime series analysis: research methods in psychology,” in Volume 2 of Handbook of Psychology, eds J. Schinka and W. F. Velicer (New York, NY: John Wiley and Sons), 628–660.

Google Scholar

Wiethoff, S., Hamada, M., and Rothwell, J. C. (2014). Variability in response to transcranial direct current stimulation of the motor cortex. Brain Stimul. 7, 468–475. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2014.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woods, A. J., Antal, A., Bikson, M., Boggio, P. S., Brunoni, A. R., Celnik, P., et al. (2016). A technical guide to tDCS, and related non-invasive brain stimulation tools. Clin. Neurophysiol. 127, 1031–1048. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2015.11.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: single case experimental design, non-invasive brain stimulation, repeated transcranial magnetic stimulation, transcranial direct current stimulation, patient-tailored

Citation: Pate JW and McCambridge AB (2021) Single Case Experimental Design: A New Approach for Non-invasive Brain Stimulation Research? Front. Neuroergon. 2:678579. doi: 10.3389/fnrgo.2021.678579

Received: 09 March 2021; Accepted: 10 May 2021;
Published: 31 May 2021.

Edited by:

Wei-Peng Teo, Nanyang Technological University, Singapore

Reviewed by:

Onno van der Groen, Edith Cowan University, Australia

Copyright © 2021 Pate and McCambridge. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Alana B. McCambridge, alana.mccambridge@uts.edu.au

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.