Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Integr. Neurosci., 30 June 2023
This article is part of the Research Topic Bridging the Gap between Integrative Neuroscience and Translational Neuroscience View all 6 articles

Visuo-spatial imagery in dreams of congenitally and early blind: a systematic review

  • 1Department of Neuroimaging, Sleep and Brain Plasticity Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King’s College London, London, United Kingdom
  • 2BRAIN, Imaging Centre, CNS, King’s College London, London, United Kingdom
  • 3School of Basic and Medical Biosciences, Faculty of Life Sciences and Medicine, King’s College London, London, United Kingdom
  • 4Sleep Disorders Centre, Guy’s and St Thomas’ NHS Foundation Trust, London, United Kingdom
  • 5Department of Basic and Clinical Neuroscience, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King’s College London, London, United Kingdom
  • 6Department of Philosophy, King’s College London, London, United Kingdom
  • 7Department of General and Specialized Medicine, Sleep Disorders Center, University Hospital of Parma, Parma, Italy
  • 8Department of Biomedical Engineering, School of Biomedical Engineering and Imaging Sciences, King’s College London, London, United Kingdom
  • 9NIHR-Wellcome Trust King’s Clinical Research Facility, King’s College London, London, United Kingdom
  • 10Centre for Learning, Teaching and Human Development, School of Education, University of Roehampton, London, United Kingdom

Background: The presence of visual imagery in dreams of congenitally blind people has long been a matter of substantial controversy. We set to systematically review body of published work on the presence and nature of oneiric visuo-spatial impressions in congenitally and early blind subjects across different areas of research, from experimental psychology, functional neuroimaging, sensory substitution, and sleep research.

Methods: Relevant studies were identified using the following databases: EMBASE, MEDLINE and PsychINFO.

Results: Studies using diverse imaging techniques and sensory substitution devices broadly suggest that the “blind” occipital cortex may be able to integrate non-visual sensory inputs, and thus possibly also generate visuo-spatial impressions. Visual impressions have also been reported by blind subjects who had near-death or out-of-body experiences.

Conclusion: Deciphering the mechanistic nature of these visual impression could open new possibility in utilization of neuroplasticity and its potential role for treatment of neurodisability.

1. Introduction

The presence of visual imagery in dreams of congenitally blind people has long been a matter of substantial controversy (Berger et al., 1962; Amadeo and Gomez, 1966; Kirtley, 1975; Kerr et al., 1982; Hurovitz et al., 1999; Holzinger, 2000; Lopes Da Silva, 2003; Staunton and O’Rourke, 2012; Meaidi et al., 2014; Christensen et al., 2019; Andrade, 2021). However, several recent studies appear to support the notion that (oneiric) visuo-spatial imagery, whilst very rare, can occur during dreaming in congenitally blind people (Bértolo et al., 2003, 2017).

Interestingly, more recent research appears to support suggestion of significant neuroplasticity in congenitally blind, with 40% of the congenitally blind population reported to have an absolute musical pitch, compared to 0.01% of neurotypical population (Ockelford, 2021). Similarly, superior verbal memory (Amedi et al., 2003), and an enhanced auditory localisation have been shown for congenitally blind by comparison to normally sighted population (Lessard et al., 1998).

However, it remains unclear to what extent the absence of vision affects the overall development of multi-modal sensory sensitivity, and underlying all this, to what extent it impacts the development of the visual cortex and other brain regions. For instance, it is known that early brain development is dependent on inherent genetic code and in utero environment; neocortical functional realization is coded by genetic and molecular information, and independent on sensory input (O’Leary et al., 2007; Espinosa and Stryker, 2012). In primates and humans, primary visual cortex shows basic region-specific cytoarchitecture, organization of receptive fields and cortical columns, even in the absence of thalamic input (Hubel et al., 1976; Rakic, 1988; Rakic et al., 1991; Miyashita-Lin et al., 1999; Nakagawa et al., 1999). Nonetheless, the final cellular, molecular and synaptic profile does not develop without appropriate thalamic afferents (Rakic et al., 1991).

Thus, it appears that the visual input that triggers response from cortical neurons is necessary for maintenance, rather than development of primary visual cortex in humans (Espinosa and Stryker, 2012). Moreover, whilst total absence of visual experience has been shown to delay functional maturation of the striate cortex (Timney et al., 1978; Blakemore and Price, 1987; Fagiolini et al., 1994; Fernando Maya-Vetencourt and Origlia, 2012), it has been shown that different non-visual components of the environment may protect, and further moderate development of the visual system, even in the absence of the visual input (Fernando Maya-Vetencourt and Origlia, 2012). For example, in transgenic mice, overexpressing brain-derived neurotrophic factor in forebrain and reared in dark, primary visual area neurons responded normally to visual stimuli, suggesting that mice may develop a functional sight despite the lack of visual experience during the critical period (Gianfranceschi et al., 2003).

This is important as it implies that a lack of sensory stimulus in one system could be rescued by increased sensory-motor stimulation or additional stimulation of other sensory systems (environmental enrichment). For instance, in one study, an increased sensory-motor stimulation through exploratory behavior in an enriched environment has been shown to prevent effects of dark rearing on rodent visual cortex development (Bartoletti et al., 2004). This process has been long referred to as cross-modal plasticity (Fernando Maya-Vetencourt and Origlia, 2012).

Cross-modal plasticity is a cortical phenomenon when deprivation of one sensory input during critical period of development strengthens remaining sensory modalities (Fernando Maya-Vetencourt and Origlia, 2012). In keeping, several studies have shown that an enriched environment accelerates structural and functional development of rodent visual system (Cancedda et al., 2004; Fernando Maya-Vetencourt and Origlia, 2012). Similarly, it has been reported that body massage in human preterm infants increases IGF-1 serum levels and accelerates visual system maturation (Guzzetta et al., 2009; Fernando Maya-Vetencourt and Origlia, 2012). Taken together, this data indicates that increased stimulation of other sensory modalities accelerates development of visual cortex, even without visual deprivation. Consequently, this may suggest that any such visual system may contribute to oneiric visual imagery-alike perceptions even in blind.

Arguably, the creation of new connections between the occipital cortex and areas of the brain involved in auditory or haptic processing and/or the unmasking of existing connections, which are normally inhibited in the presence of vision (Bavelier and Neville, 2002; Burton, 2003; Müller et al., 2019), may, in the blind, enable integration of non-visual sensory inputs to generate any such visuo-spatial images. Another interesting possibility is that of human brain functioning as the intrinsically metamodal structure, organized as operators that execute a given function or computation regardless of sensory input modality [for more on this topic please also refer to Pascual-Leone and Hamilton (2001)].

Nonetheless, it remains unclear to what extent the absence of vision may affects the sensory sensitivity for oneiric construction and thus, in order to gain a better insight into possible sleep and neural mechanisms which may underlie this phenomenon, we set to systematically review and to critically analyze a body of published work on the presence and nature of oneiric visuo-spatial impressions in congenitally and early blind subjects across different areas of research, from experimental psychology, functional neuroimaging, sensory substitution, and sleep research. Finally, we hypothesized that (oneiric) mental representation of images may not be entirely dependent on visual input.

2. Materials and methods

2.1. Literature search

This systematic review was conducted following the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines (Page et al., 2021). Relevant studies were identified using the online search databases EMBASE, MEDLINE and PsychINFO. The following keywords were used: {(exp *blindness/) AND [(exp dream/OR exp dreaming/) OR (exp REM sleep/) OR (*vision/OR exp visual hallucination/OR exp visual illusion/)]} in EMBASE, {(exp *Blindness/) AND [(exp Dreams/OR exp Sleep, REM/) OR [(Visual Hallucination* OR Visual Perception).mp]]} in MEDLINE, and {(exp *blind/) AND [(exp dreaming/OR exp dream analysis/) OR (exp rem sleep/OR exp rapid eye movement/OR exp rem dreams/) OR (exp visual hallucinations/OR *visual perception/)]} in PsychINFO. As each database employed different subject terms, different search string was chosen for each of the three databases (please see Table 1). Eligible studies were extracted from January 1960 to March 2021. The references of the selected studies were also examined to retrieve documents missed by the literature search.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. The search strategy and exclusion/inclusion criteria.

2.2. Inclusion and exclusion criteria

All databases were screened following the same protocol. The studies were included if they came from (1) original research articles published by a peer-reviewed scientific journal; (2) were written in English and (3) if they included observational, descriptive, longitudinal, retrospective, cross-sectional, or cohort studies. Included studies were required to investigate presence of visual imagery in congenitally blind participants, or early blind individuals with blindness of peripheral origin. Studies were excluded if they were case reports, or if they included only late blind participants. Two reviewers (NL and RB) independently screened each eligible study, and disagreements were resolved through discussion after retrieving full text to determine whether inclusion and exclusion criteria were met or consulting a third independent investigator (KI) (please also refer to Table 2). The PICOS statement is available in Table 3 and PRISMA 2020 flow diagram of study selection process is shown in Figure 1.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. The exclusion and inclusion criteria.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. The PICOS statement.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. PRISMA 2020 flow diagram of study selection process (Liberati et al., 2009).

2.3. Data extraction

The following data were extracted: type of study, sample size, type of blindness, sex, age and handedness of participants; techniques and measures employed; main findings and study limitations (also please refer to Supplementary Tables 15). Subsequently, the studies were classified into five predetermined categories, based on the investigating techniques employed in the studies (i.e., neurotechnology, psychological investigations, sensory substitution or near-death and out-of-body experiences). A third independent investigator went through all extracted data and tables to reconcile any eventual disagreements.

3. Results

A total of 1,397 studies were identified through database searching (see Figure 1). Two more studies (Holzinger, 2000; Bértolo et al., 2017), cited in the literature but absent in the three databases used, were reviewed and added to the relevant studies, for a total of 1,399. Of remaining studies, only 41 studies were found to fulfill the exclusion and inclusion criteria (Figure 1) and they were consequently classified as belonging to one of the following predefined five categories, based on the techniques employed: (1) studies using neurotechnology to investigate the presence of visuo-spatial imagery in blind subjects during wake (10 studies; please refer for more in-depth analysis in the Supplementary Table 1); (2) psychological investigations of visuo-spatial imagery in blind subjects (11 studies; Supplementary Table 2); (3) studies investigating visuo-spatial imagery as a consequence of sensory substitution in blind subjects (11 studies; Supplementary Table 3); (4) studies investigating visuo-spatial imagery during near-death and out-of-body experiences in blind subjects (1 study); and finally (5) studies investigating the presence of visuo-spatial imagery in blind subjects’ dreams (8 studies; Supplementary Table 5). Of note is that many studies employed a combination of different techniques.

3.1. Studies using neurotechnology to investigate the presence of visuo-spatial imagery in blind subjects during wake

The changes in human brain in subjects that are either blind from birth or very early age (congenitally blind and early blind) and in those with blindness onset later in life (late blind) were investigated by several studies. Number of neuroimaging studies in blind report different patterns of activation in primary visual and associative cortexes, as well as in prefrontal cortex (Supplementary Table 1). Röder et al. (1997) used electroencephalography in congenitally blind and sighted control subjects and detected a marked slow negative potential over the occipital cortex of 15 congenitally blind subjects who were performing mental rotation and comparison of images explored haptically. This slow negative potential was not observed in sighted control subjects who performed the same tasks. In two similar studies by De Volder et al. (2001) and Vanlierde et al. (2003), positron emission tomography was used in early blind and sighted control subjects who were performing a visuo-spatial imagery task. In both subject groups, De Volder et al. (2001) observed activation in lateral occipito-temporal areas; Vanlierde et al. (2003) in the precuneus, superior parietal lobe and occipital gyrus. Early blind group demonstrated stronger occipital activations (Vanlierde et al., 2003), whilst activation of the right prefrontal cortex and left posterior cingulate gyrus was observed only in the sighted control group (De Volder et al., 2001).

Only two studies used functional magnetic resonance imaging to explore brain activity in blind in response to specific concepts (Striem-Amit et al., 2018; Mattioni et al., 2020). Mattioni et al. (2020) found that the ventral occipito-temporal cortex encodes different concepts in a similar way in early blind (to whom concepts were presented acoustically) and in sighted control subjects (to whom concepts were presented visually). However, in the other functional magnetic resonance imaging study (Striem-Amit et al., 2018), a difference was spotted between congenitally blind and sighted controls: while in both groups, abstract concepts (e.g., “freedom”) were correlated with activity in the lateral anterior temporal lobe, and concrete concepts (e.g., “cup”) with activity in medial anterior temporal lobe, concepts such as “red,” which are imperceptible for blind people, were correlated with activity in left dorsal anterior temporal lobe in the congenitally blind group only. In the same study by Striem-Amit et al. (2018), resting-state functional connectivity analysis revealed that, in both congenitally blind and sighted control, dorsal and lateral anterior temporal lobe are connected with areas involved in semantic, non-sensorially derived information (e.g., the inferior frontal lobe), while the medial anterior temporal lobe is connected to multisensory object-related regions in central visual cortex, and in frontal and parietal lobes.

3.2. Psychological investigations of visuo-spatial imagery in blind subjects

In the Supplementary Table 2, 11 studies are reported, in which mental representations of patterns, pathways, objects and spatial relations were compared between groups of congenitally blind, early blind, late blind and sighted control subjects. In three of those studies, blind subjects performed better or as well as sighted controls in visuo-spatial imagery tasks. For instance, after administering the Onomatopoeia and Images (Torrance et al., 1973), Johnson (1980) found that congenitally blind subjects produced more visual images than the sighted controls. Hollins (1985) reports that congenitally blind and sighted control subjects were equally good at identifying objects represented as patterns of squares on 2D matrices or of cubes on 3D matrices. Tinti et al. (2006) reported that both congenitally blind and late blind subjects were better than sighted controls at drawing two pathways they had just learned to walk through; however, when drawings of the two pathways were analyzed together, only late blind performed better than sighted controls.

In five of the eight remaining studies, blind and sighted subjects performed similarly well, although there were some interesting differences. For example, in one study (Kerr, 1983), the congenitally blind performed similarly to the sighted at imagining objects and their properties, although they struggled when they had to imagine concealed objects. Heller et al. (1996) found evidence of foreshortening in sighted control subjects only; however, drawings of a panel at different angular orientations were not significantly different between the early blind, the late blind and the sighted controls. In Vanlierde and Wanet-Defalque (2004), early blind, late blind and sighted control individuals performed similarly in a series of pattern recognition tasks, but they used different strategies: all 27 sighted controls, nine out of ten late blind and one out of ten early blind subjects used a “visual” strategy, while the remaining nine early blind subjects used a “coordinate” strategy, and the last late blind utilized a “mixed” (visual and coordinate) strategy. Noordzij et al. (2007) report that only their late blind–but not the early blind–group performed worse than sighted controls in a visual imagery task, while the early blind performed worse than the sighted in a spatial imagery task. In Cattaneo et al. (2010), both early blind and sighted controls participants remembered more symmetrical than non-symmetrical patterns; however, only in the sighted controls group vertical symmetries were recalled more easily than horizontal ones.

3.3. Studies investigating visuo-spatial imagery as a consequence of sensory substitution in blind subjects

Of the 11 sensory substitution studies selected for this review (Supplementary Table 3), five employed visual-to-tactile sensory substitution devices. Specifically, Miletic et al. (1988) used a modified version of the Optacon (Linvill et al., 1966), Sampaio et al. (2001) and Chebat et al. (2007) used the TDU (Bach-y-Rita et al., 1998), Velázquez and Bazán (2010) the “on-shoe tactile display” (Velázquez et al., 2009), while Nau et al. (2015) used the BrainPort Artificial Vision Device (Wicab Inc., Madison, WI, USA). In the remaining 6 studies, the authors employed a visual-to-auditory sensory substitution devices. Renier et al. (2005), Renier et al. (2006), and Renier and De Volder (2010) used the PSVA (Capelle et al., 1998), while Abboud et al. (2014), Buchs et al. (2015), and Buchs et al. (2019) employed EyeMusic, an sensory substitution devices which uses different musical instruments to convey color information (Abboud et al., 2014).

3.4. Studies investigating visuo-spatial imagery during near-death and out-of-body experiences in blind subjects

Ring and Cooper (1997) interviewed 14 early blind, 11 late blind and 6 visually impaired subjects who had at least one near-death experience or one out-of-body experience, and found that 25 (80%) of their 31 subjects–9 (64%) out of 14 early blind subjects–claimed sight during near-death experiences and/or out-of-body experiences. These visual experiences included seeing one’s own physical body, going through a tunnel or dark space, and seeing a radiant light. Interestingly, when asked to compare near-death experiences and out-of-body experiences with their usual dream content, early blind individuals answered that visual experiences were present only in their near-death experiences/out-of-body experiences.

3.5. Studies investigating the presence of visuo-spatial imagery in blind subjects’ dreams

Several studies have demonstrated that congenitally blind people can experience oneiric visuo-spatial imagery in a way similar to sighted individuals (see Figure 2 and Supplementary Table 5; Bértolo et al., 2003, 2017; Bértolo, 2005). Moreover, some congenitally blind subjects have also been able to represent the visual content of their dreams in drawings which were similarly accurate, if somewhat less detailed, and slightly more symbolic and archetypal, as those of sighted controls (Bértolo et al., 2003). Also, in keeping with positive findings by Bértolo et al. (2003, 2017) and Bértolo (2005), some of the past studies concomitantly report findings at odds with their own negative conclusion that congenitally, and early blind subjects completely lack oneiric visual imagery. For instance, in Hurovitz et al. (1999), one of the early blind participants, totally blind since the age of 4, reported “not metaphorical” visual imagery in his dream reports; in Christensen et al. (2019), visual dream elements were reported by congenitally blind subjects on two occasions; Meaidi et al. (2014) write that “many of the blind participants in our study described an object or a scene verbally in such rich visual terms that the interlocutor began to doubt if these individuals really lacked vision.”

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Representative dreams from the group of congenitally blind subjects, with examples of reported visuo-spatial imagery, adapted from with permission from Kang et al. (2023) (Illustrations by Davor Aslanovski).

4. Discussion

In this systematic review, we analyzed 41 studies on the presence and nature of visuo-spatial imagery in the blind, with the overarching goal of elucidating what happens in the visual cortex of blind people. We set out to investigate how blind people “see,” whether they may recreate visuo-spatial imagery via sensory substitution and, finally, whether they are able to dream in images.

4.1. What happens in the visual cortex of blind people?

Whilst numerous studies in animals have elucidated how the visual cortex is developed, and how it changes in cases of monocular or binocular deprivation during different periods after birth, relative sparsity of human studies and technologic limitations have so far prevented us from gaining similar insight into human development. A complex picture emerges from reported studies: during visuo-spatial imagery tasks, the occipital cortex is activated in the blind more than in the sighted (Röder et al., 1997; Arno et al., 2001; Vanlierde et al., 2003; Collignon et al., 2007; Striem-Amit et al., 2012; Striem-Amit and Amedi, 2014), and possibly in substitution for prefrontal activity, which was sometimes detected in the sighted but not in the blind (Arno et al., 2001). However, in the occipital, temporal and parietal lobes of blind subjects, higher-order areas, which are involved in multisensory integration and supramodal representations, seem to generally preserve their function and connectivity (Arno et al., 2001; Vanlierde et al., 2003; Amedi et al., 2007; Striem-Amit and Amedi, 2014; Striem-Amit et al., 2018; Mattioni et al., 2020). Nonetheless, several distinctive features have been recorded in the blind; for instance, representations of concrete concepts that are perceptible by the sighted but not by the blind have been shown to involve activations in the left dorsal anterior temporal lobe in blind subjects only (Striem-Amit et al., 2018).

4.2. How does a blind brain see?

In blind subjects, the occipital cortex is activated during haptic (Amedi et al., 2010) and auditory (Kujala et al., 1995, 2005; Liotti et al., 1998; Weeks et al., 2000) processing, including pitch memory tasks (Gaab et al., 2006); “visual” cortex activation in the blind has been associated with episodic retrieval (Raz et al., 2005) and with Braille reading (Sadato et al., 1996); transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) used to disrupt occipital activity leads to errors during Braille reading in blind subjects, but not in sighted people (Cohen et al., 1997). Moreover, when used to stimulate occipital areas, TMS induces subjective sensations (qualia) in the fingertips of blind subjects, and then in the tongue after training with the sensory substitution device TDU (Kupers et al., 2006).

This is of note, as the geniculo-striate system is atrophic in the blind, with up to 25% volume decrease in the BA17 cortical region (Ptito et al., 2008). The creation of new corticocortical and/or thalamocortical connections, or the unmasking of pre-existing connections, normally inhibited in the presence of vision, has been suggested as a potential mechanism behind the involvement of the “blind” visual cortex in non-visual sensory processing (Sadato et al., 1996; Cohen et al., 1997; Liotti et al., 1998; Arno et al., 2001; Burton, 2003; Kupers et al., 2006, 2011; Müller et al., 2019). Indeed, in animals, such as for example blind moles, the inferior colliculus projects not only to the auditory medial geniculate nucleus, but also to the visual lateral geniculate nucleus of the thalamus (Doron and Wollberg, 1994). In humans, diffusion tensor imaging revealed microstructural changes within thalamic clusters in blind individuals, even when no gross differences in the thalamocortical network were observed (Reislev et al., 2017). In keeping, Müller et al. (2019) have proposed the existence of a thalamocortical pathway for fast rerouting of haptic information to the blind “visual” cortex, activated only 35 ms after haptic stimulation in congenitally blind subjects, but not in blindfolded sighted people.

Different strategies in perceiving objects by sighted and blind were previously argued by Révész and Wolff (1950) in Psychology and Art of the Blind. He distinguished between “Haptics” and “Optics,” which, in his opinion, lead to different representations of objects, shapes and space in blind and sighted subjects. He argued that an object can be immediately perceived in its entirety through vision, while it can only be gradually “discovered” in a series of subsequent haptic explorations by a blind person. Thus, according to Révész and Wolff (1950), the same object must be represented in qualitatively different ways between the blind and sighted individuals. Indeed, considering diverging findings from analyzed studies, it remains unclear which mechanistic processes may support the presence and nature of visuo-spatial imagery in blind individuals. It is obvious that congenitally blind and early blind subjects may have some mental representation of patterns, shapes and spatial relations. Nonetheless, it is not clear how much these representations are affected by the blind’s main perceptual modality (i.e., haptics), and therefore how much visuo-spatial representations in the blind differ from those of the sighted. For instance, it appears that while congenitally blind and early blind subjects are as good as–or even better than–sighted controls at drawing pathways, imagining objects and remembering 2D patterns, they struggle to imagine concealed objects and reproduce 3D patterns, and they lack foreshortening.

4.3. Can we produce visuo-spatial imagery via sensory substitution in blind subjects?

A century ago, Romains (1920) managed to convey visuo-spatial information through the skin of blindfolded sighted subjects. Later in the 20th century, Starkiewicz and Kuliszewski (1963) developed the “Elektroftalm,” a device which transformed visuo-spatial images into patterns of vibrations, enabling blind subjects to “see.” Another pioneering method of sensory substitution was trialed by Bach-y-Rita et al. (1969), whose research group built an sensory substitution device consisting of a camera mounted on a dental chair. Here, visual information from the camera was converted to vibratory stimulation of the back of blind people sitting on the chair (Bach-y-Rita et al., 1969). Since then, the field of sensory substitution has grown and seen the development of various sensory substitution devices, not only for the blind; see for example Riso (1999) or Tyler et al. (2003).

In this review, we included 11 recent studies in which visual-to-auditory or visual-to-vibrotactile sensory substitution devices were employed to investigate visuo-spatial perception and imagery in congenitally and/or early blind subjects. Overall, it would appear that congenitally blind, early blind and late blind, as well as sighted subjects, can integrate non-visual information (auditory and haptic in this case) to create visuo-spatial mental images. However, there are differences between vision as a result of visual inputs and “blind vision” acquired through sensory substitution. For example, blind subjects using sensory substitution devices were shown insensitive to either Ponzo (Renier et al., 2005) or vertical-horizontal (Renier et al., 2006) illusions, and were similarly shown to have difficulties recognizing 3D shapes and spatial relations (Miletic et al., 1988; Velázquez and Bazán, 2010). Thus, one could argue that whilst visual imagery is possible even in the absence of inputs from the retina, it is still influenced by the specific sensory modality in which information is conveyed to visual association areas.

4.4. Visual experience from the beyond

Anecdotally, visuo-spatial imagery has been reported during near-death and out-of-body experiences by the blind, albeit the mechanistic nature of any such imagery remains poorly understood (Ring and Cooper, 1997). Near-death or out-of-body experience are rare parapsychological phenomena reported to occur during clinical cardio-respiratory arrest, or other drug or accident induced states, when a person seems to be awake and sees his body and the world from a location outside his physical body (Bünning and Blanke, 2005) [for an in-depth overview of the topic, also see Mindsight: Near-Death and Out-of-Body Experiences in the Blind (Ring and Cooper, 1999)].

4.5. Do congenitally blind people dream in images?

The above body of data may be taken to support the notion that (oneiric) mental representation of images may not be dependent on visual input, and that they can therefore be a, (albeit infrequent) part of congenitally blind individuals’ dreams.

Past evidence suggests that congenitally blind people may experience more circadian dysregulation (Lockley et al., 2007) and sleep-related problems (64% of congenitally blind subjects) than late blind or sighted controls (43 and 30%, respectively), as assessed using the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index. They also report suffering with more nightmares [mean frequency of nightmares over total number of dream reports in congenitally blind subjects: (25%) than late blind (7%) or sighted controls (6%) (Meaidi et al., 2014)].

However, the presence of visual imagery in the oneiric mentation in congenitally blind remains widely disputed, and highly debatable, despite some anecdotal, as well as some limited scientific evidence to the contrary (Berger et al., 1962; Amadeo and Gomez, 1966; Kirtley, 1975; Kerr et al., 1982; Hurovitz et al., 1999; Holzinger, 2000; Lopes Da Silva, 2003; Staunton and O’Rourke, 2012; Meaidi et al., 2014; Christensen et al., 2019; Andrade, 2021). It is also possible, that any such reports might have been historically ignored and disregarded (Kerr et al., 1982; Hurovitz et al., 1999; Holzinger, 2000; Staunton and O’Rourke, 2012; Meaidi et al., 2014; Christensen et al., 2019). Instead, it has been widely accepted that auditory, haptic, proprioceptive, somaesthetic, olfactory and gustatory imagery, as well as that of pain and temperature may be more prevalent in dreams of congenitally blind, by comparison to sighted people (Hurovitz et al., 1999; Holzinger, 2000; Staunton and O’Rourke, 2012; Meaidi et al., 2014; Supplementary Table 5).

When analyzing dream contents, most studies failed to record reports of visual impressions in the dreams of congenitally blind individuals (Kerr et al., 1982; Hurovitz et al., 1999; Holzinger, 2000; Staunton and O’Rourke, 2012; Meaidi et al., 2014), with only visual impressions (color, light) reported in the dreams of congenitally blind subjects with residual vision (Kerr et al., 1982; Meaidi et al., 2014). In late blind participants, the average blindness duration index has been shown to negatively correlates with duration, clarity, and color content of visual impressions in dreams (Meaidi et al., 2014). Positive correlations between lack of oneiric visual imagery and (1) congenital or early blindness, (2) total blindness, and (3) a high percentage of the subject’s life spent being blind has been reported by Hurovitz et al. (1999). Interestingly, some late blind subjects reported experiencing oneiric visual imagery (including colors) of people and objects only after the onset of blindness (Kerr et al., 1982; Holzinger, 2000). In that respect, it is of note that Kirtley (1975) proposed that, if the onset of blindness is before the age of five, no visual experience in the dreams should be possible, whilst onset of blindness between the ages five and seven may produce some oneiric visual imagery. It has been thus proposed that visual imagery in dreams will be retained only if onset of blindness happens after age of seven. Conversely, in one study by Hurovitz et al. (1999) it has been suggested that oneiric visual impressions may be retained if sight was lost after the age of four. Moreover, Meaidi et al. (2014) argued that only individuals who were born blind (congenitally blind), or who lost their sight before 2.5 years of age, will completely lack visual imagery in their dreams.

In this background, it is also of note that congenitally blind people have been shown to have significantly reduced, or absent, rapid eye movements (REM) during sleep (Christensen et al., 2019), whilst they appear to retain some of the other EEG features of phasic REM sleep microstructure. The phasic periods of REM sleep are commonly discerned by bursts of eye movements linked to so-called ponto-geniculo-occipital waves, contractions of the middle ear muscles, myoclonic twitches of skeletal muscles, sawtooth waves, as well as irregularities in cardio-respiratory activity (Simor et al., 2020). Some authors have suggested that the phasic REM sleep may play its own specific role in dreaming and emotion regulation processes during sleep, likely relating to reactivation of vivid visuo-spatial (emotionally relevant) mental pictures, or to the reprocessing of emotional memories during sleep (Simor et al., 2020). Thus, arguably, in blind, differential neurocircuitry may be activated during the phasic REM to that in sighted individuals, possibly reflecting use of auditory (or haptic), instead of visual referencing points. However, it remains unclear whether this may also translate in differential functional role of this fundamental physiologic process.

This is of particular importance given that, sleep itself, and more specifically, REM sleep (Marks et al., 1995) is crucial for the neurodevelopment of the visual cortex (Frank et al., 2001; Hobson, 2009), and therefore of mental imagery (Foulkes, 1982, 1999; Hobson, 2009; Siclari et al., 2020).

5. Conclusion, congenitally blind and the phenomenon of cross-modal neuroplasticity

At the neurophysiologic level, it would appear that the existence of visual-alike mental imagery could be argued by the demonstrations of cross-modal neuroplasticity, as evidenced by the neuroimaging (Röder et al., 1997; Arno et al., 2001; Vanlierde et al., 2003) and sensory substitution (Bach-y-Rita et al., 1969; Abboud et al., 2014; Buchs et al., 2019) studies. For instance, as previously discussed, studies using sensory substitution devices demonstrate that congenitally blind and early blind individuals can use auditory or haptic inputs to generate visuo-spatial representations of the world around them. These representations are accompanied by activation of brain areas such as extrastriate body area and lateral-occipital tactile-visual area, as demonstrated by neuroimaging studies. In the blind, activation of these and similar areas during visuo-spatial imagery tasks suggests that visual perception is not necessary for the creation of supramodal representations through multisensory integration. The visual system consists of complex parallel and interacting processing pathways in the brain which process neural information on form, motion, and color (Hasson et al., 2002; Bértolo, 2005). However, even for a neurotypical visual system, it remains ambiguous how separate pathways of visual system are brought together into a single image, and perhaps even more pertinently, whether mental imagery activated all of them (Hasson et al., 2002; Bértolo, 2005; Pulvermuller, 2018).

Arguably, if present, any such phenomenon of the cross-modal plasticity of the blind occipital cortex could be argued to support cortical deference (i.e., the thesis that the function of a specific brain area is determined by the type of input received) over cortical dominance (i.e., the idea that brain areas are pre-determined in their function, independently of sensory inputs) (Hurley and Noë, 2003). Moreover, this notion would then also support the idea that the “visual” cortex of the blind is anything but visual. Nonetheless, there are some valid arguments against this notion. For instance, if this was the case, this would not fully explain why the occipital cortex of congenitally blind people maintains the division between ventral (shape-processing) and dorsal (location-processing) pathways, normally found in the sighted (Striem-Amit et al., 2012). Moreover, several fundamental neurophysiologic processes and structure of sleep remain surprisingly intact in congenitally blind people (Bértolo et al., 2003). In addition, Aubin et al. (2018) report decreased alpha power and a generally higher resting-state metabolic rate over the occipital cortex during wakefulness, as well as REM sleep, in blind subjects. This constant increase of occipital activity in the blind, compared to the sighted individuals, has been linked to multisensory integration and therefore could underlie process of visual imagery in blind. Conversely, it could also represent an involvement of the blind “visual” cortex in higher-order functions–such as episodic memory (Raz et al., 2005) and language processing (Röder et al., 2000)–which are not directly related to the creation of visuo-spatial images.

More recently, it has been also shown that circuitry underlying REM sleep serves to selectively amplify the visual system’s activity periodically throughout the night, possibly preventing unregulated neuroplastic changes and takeover from other sensory inputs (Eagleman and Vaughn, 2021). It would follow that, during REM dreaming in blind, therefore, the impressions generated by different sensory modalities (e.g., auditory, haptic) can be extracted and integrated into a richer visual imagery-alike percept, in part thanks to the eccentric genetic wiring of our early visual cortex (Vetter et al., 2020). In turn, this may then enable development of a typical spatio-temporal organization of early visual areas by eccentricity (Hasson et al., 2002), even in the life-long absence of vision (Eagleman and Vaughn, 2021). Finally, this could also explain ability of some congenitally blind individuals to draw symbolic representations of various visual images (Bértolo et al., 2017) in striking likeness to those drawn by normally sighted.

Author contributions

KI and RB selected and reviewed the studies. KI, NL, and RB assessed all the eligible studies. All authors involved in reviewing and drafting the manuscript and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This research was funded in whole, or in part, by the Wellcome Trust (103952/Z/14/Z). For the purpose of open access, the author IR has applied a CC BY public copyright license to any author accepted manuscript version arising from this submission. This article represents independent research in part funded by the NIHR Maudsley Biomedical Research Centre at South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust and King’s College London.

Acknowledgments

We thank Dr. Kurt Kaczmarek and Mr. Mitchell Tyler for kindly sharing some of their and Dr. Paul Bach-y-Rita’s early work. We also grateful to the librarians at the British Library and at King’s College London for their help in retrieving some of the papers included in this review.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Author disclaimer

The views expressed are those of the author(s) and not necessarily those of the NIHR or the Department of Health and Social Care.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnint.2023.1204129/full#supplementary-material

References

Abboud, S., Hanassy, S., Levy-Tzedek, S., Maidenbaum, S., and Amedi, A. (2014). EyeMusic: Introducing a “visual” colorful experience for the blind using auditory sensory substitution. Restor. Neurol. Neurosci. 32, 247–257. doi: 10.3233/RNN-130338

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Amadeo, M., and Gomez, E. (1966). Eye Movements, Attention and Dreaming in Subjects with Lifelong Blindness. Can. Psychiatric Assoc. J. 11, 501–507. doi: 10.1111/jsr.12866

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Amedi, A., Raz, N., Azulay, H., Malach, R., and Zohary, E. (2010). Cortical activity during tactile exploration of objects in blind and sighted humans. Restor. Neurol. Neurosci. 28, 143–156. doi: 10.3233/RNN-2010-0503

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Amedi, A., Raz, N., Pianka, P., Malach, R., and Zohary, E. (2003). Early “visual” cortex activation correlates with superior verbal memory performance in the blind. Nat. Neurosci. 6, 758–766. doi: 10.1038/nn1072

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Amedi, A., Stern, W. M., Camprodon, J. A., Bermpohl, F., Merabet, L., Rotman, S., et al. (2007). Shape conveyed by visual-to-auditory sensory substitution activates the lateral occipital complex. Nat. Neurosci. 10, 687–689. doi: 10.1038/nn1912

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andrade, M. J. O. (2021). Do congenitally blind people have visual dreams? Sleep Sci. 14, 190–192. doi: 10.5935/1984-0063.20200068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arno, P., De Volder, A. G., Vanlierde, A., Wanet-Defalque, M.-C., Streel, E., Robert, A., et al. (2001). Occipital activation by pattern recognition in the early blind using auditory substitution for vision. NeuroImage 13, 632–645.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Aubin, S., Christensen, J. A. E., Jennum, P., Nielsen, T., Kupers, R., and Ptito, M. (2018). Preserved sleep microstructure in blind individuals. Sleep Med. 42, 21–30. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2017.11.1135

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bach-y-Rita, P., Collins, C. C., Saunders, F. A., White, B., and Scadden, L. (1969). Vision substitution by tactile image projection. Nature 221, 963–964. doi: 10.1038/221963a0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bach-y-Rita, P., Kaczmarek, K. A., Tyler, M. E., and Garcia-Lara, J. (1998). Form perception with a 49-point electrotactile stimulus array on the tongue: a technical note. J. Rehabil. Res. Dev. 35, 427–430.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Bartoletti, A., Medini, P., Berardi, N., and Maffei, L. (2004). Environmental enrichment prevents effects of dark-rearing in the rat visual cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 7, 215–216.

Google Scholar

Bavelier, D., and Neville, H. J. (2002). Cross-modal plasticity: where and how? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 443–452. doi: 10.1038/nrn848

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berger, R. J., Olley, P., and Oswald, I. (1962). The EEG, eye-movements and dreams of the blind. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 14, 183–186. doi: 10.1097/00005053-196509000-00014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bértolo, H. (2005). Visual imagery without visual perception. Psicologica 26, 173–188. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2020.11.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bértolo, H., Mestre, T., Barrio, A., and Antona, B. (2017). Rapid Eye Movements (REMs) and visual dream recall in both congenitally blind and sighted subjects. Paris: SPIE.

Google Scholar

Bértolo, H., Paiva, T., Pessoa, L., Mestre, T., Marques, R., and Santos, R. (2003). Visual dream content, graphical representation and EEG alpha activity in congenitally blind subjects. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 15, 277–284. doi: 10.1016/s0926-6410(02)00199-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blakemore, C., and Price, D. J. (1987). Effects of dark-rearing on the development of area 18 of the cat’s visual cortex. J. Physiol. 384, 293–309. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1987.sp016455

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buchs, G., Heimler, B., and Amedi, A. (2019). The effect of irrelevant environmental noise on the performance of visual-to-auditory sensory substitution devices used by blind adults. Multisens. Res. 32, 87–109. doi: 10.1163/22134808-20181327

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buchs, G., Maidenbaum, S., Levy-Tzedek, S., and Amedi, A. (2015). Integration and binding in rehabilitative sensory substitution: Increasing resolution using a new Zooming-in approach. Restor. Neurol. Neurosci. 34, 97–105. doi: 10.3233/RNN-150592

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bünning, S., and Blanke, O. (2005). The out-of body experience: precipitating factors and neural correlates. Progress Brain Res. 150, 331–606. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(05)50024-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burton, H. (2003). Visual cortex activity in early and late blind people. J. Neurosci. 23:4005. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-10-04005.2003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cancedda, L., Putignano, E., Sale, A., Viegi, A., Berardi, N., and Maffei, L. (2004). Acceleration of visual system development by environmental enrichment. J. Neurosci. 24, 4840–4848. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0845-04.2004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Capelle, C., Trullemans, C., Arno, P., and Veraart, C. (1998). A real-time experimental prototype for enhancement of vision rehabilitation using auditory substitution. IEEE Trans. Biomed. Eng. 45, 1279–1293. doi: 10.1109/10.720206

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cattaneo, Z., Fantino, M., Silvanto, J., Tinti, C., Pascual-Leone, A., and Vecchi, T. (2010). Symmetry perception in the blind. Acta Psychol. 134, 398–402. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chebat, D. R., Rainville, C., Kupers, R., and Ptito, M. (2007). Tactile-“visual” acuity of the tongue in early blind individuals. Neuroreport 18, 1901–1904. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e3282f2a63

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Christensen, J. A. E., Aubin, S., Nielsen, T., Ptito, M., Kupers, R., and Jennum, P. (2019). Rapid eye movements are reduced in blind individuals. J. Sleep Res. 28:e12866.

Google Scholar

Cohen, L. G., Celnik, P., Pascual-Leone, A., Corwell, B., Faiz, L., Dambrosia, J., et al. (1997). Functional relevance of cross-modal plasticity in blind humans. Nature 389, 180–183. doi: 10.1038/38278

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Collignon, O., Lassonde, M., Lepore, F., Bastien, D., and Veraart, C. (2007). Functional cerebral reorganization for auditory spatial processing and auditory substitution of vision in early blind subjects. Cereb. Cortex 17, 457–465. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhj162

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Volder, A. G., Toyama, H., Kimura, Y., Kiyosawa, M., Nakano, H., Vanlierde, A., et al. (2001). Auditory triggered mental imagery of shape involves visual association areas in early blind humans. Neuroimage 14, 129–139. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0782

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Doron, N., and Wollberg, Z. (1994). Cross-modal neuroplasticity in the blind mole rat Spalax ehrenbergi: a WGA-HRP tracing study. Neuroreport 5, 2697–2701. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199412000-00072

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eagleman, D. M., and Vaughn, D. A. (2021). The defensive activation theory: REM sleep as a mechanism to prevent takeover of the visual cortex. Front. Neurosci. 15:632853. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2021.632853

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Espinosa, J. S., and Stryker, M. P. (2012). Development and plasticity of the primary visual cortex. Neuron 75, 230–249. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.06.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fagiolini, M., Pizzorusso, T., Berardi, N., Domenici, L., and Maffei, L. (1994). Functional postnatal development of the rat primary visual cortex and the role of visual experience: dark rearing and monocular deprivation. Vis. Res. 34, 709–720.

Google Scholar

Fernando Maya-Vetencourt, J., and Origlia, N. (2012). Visual cortex plasticity: A complex interplay of genetic and environmental influences. Neural Plasticity 2012:631965. doi: 10.1155/2012/631965

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Foulkes, D. (1982). Children’s dreams: longitudinal studies. New York, NY: Wiley.

Google Scholar

Foulkes, D. (1999). Children’s Dreaming and the Development of Consciousness. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Google Scholar

Frank, M. G., Issa, N. P., and Stryker, M. P. (2001). Sleep Enhances Plasticity in the Developing Visual Cortex. Neuron 30, 275–287. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(01)00279-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gaab, N., Schulze, K., Ozdemir, E., and Schlaug, G. (2006). Neural correlates of absolute pitch differ between blind and sighted musicians. Neuroreport 17, 1853–1857. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e3280107bee

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gianfranceschi, L., Siciliano, R., Walls, J., Morales, B., Kirkwood, A., Huang, Z. J., et al. (2003). Visual cortex is rescued from the effects of dark rearing by overexpression of BDNF. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 100, 12486–12491. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1934836100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guzzetta, A., Baldini, S., Bancale, A., Baroncelli, L., Ciucci, F., Ghirri, P., et al. (2009). Massage accelerates brain development and the maturation of visual function. J. Neurosci. 29, 6042–6051.

Google Scholar

Hasson, U., Levy, I., Behrmann, M., Hendler, T., and Malach, R. (2002). Eccentricity bias as an organizing principle for human high-order object areas. Neuron 34, 479–490.

Google Scholar

Heller, M. A., Calcaterra, J. A., Tyler, L. A., and Burson, L. L. (1996). Production and interpretation of perspective drawings by blind and sighted people. Perception 25, 321–334.

Google Scholar

Hobson, J. A. (2009). REM sleep and dreaming: towards a theory of protoconsciousness. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 803–813. doi: 10.1038/nrn2716

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hollins, M. (1985). Styles of mental imagery in blind adults. Neuropsychologia 23, 561–566. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(85)90009-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holzinger, B. (2000). The Dreams of the Blind: In Consideration of the Congenital and Adventitiously Blind. J. Sleep Res. 9:83.

Google Scholar

Hubel, D., Wiesel, T., and LeVay, S. (1976). Functional architecture of area 17 in normal and monocularly deprived macaque monkeys. Cold Spring Harb. Symp. Quant. Biol. 40, 581–589. doi: 10.1101/sqb.1976.040.01.054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hurley, S., and Noë, A. (2003). Neural Plasticity and Consciousness. Biol. Philos. 18, 131–168.

Google Scholar

Hurovitz, C. S., Dunn, S., Domhoff, G. W., and Fiss, H. (1999). The dreams of blind men and women: A replication and extension of previous findings. Dreaming 9, 183–193.

Google Scholar

Johnson, R. A. (1980). Sensory images in the absence of sight: blind versus sighted adolescents. Percept. Mot. Skills 51, 177–178. doi: 10.2466/pms.1980.51.1.177

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kang, J., Bertani, R., Raheel, K., Soteriou, M., Rosenzweig, J., Valentin, A., et al. (2023). Homoiōma in Dreams of Congenitally Blind. Sci. Rep. doi: 10.21203/rs.3.rs-1614543/v1 [Epub ahead of print].

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kerr, N. H. (1983). The role of vision in “visual imagery” experiments: evidence from the congenitally blind. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 112, 265–277. doi: 10.1037//0096-3445.112.2.265

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kerr, N. H., Foulkes, D., and Schmidt, M. (1982). The structure of laboratory dream reports in blind and sighted subjects. J. Nerv. Mental Dis. 170, 286–294. doi: 10.1097/00005053-198205000-00006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirtley, D. D. (1975). The psychology of blindness. Oxford: Nelson-Hall.

Google Scholar

Kujala, T., Huotilainen, M., Sinkkonen, J., Ahonen, A. I., Alho, K., Hämälä:inen, M. S., et al. (1995). Visual cortex activation in blind humans during sound discrimination. Neurosci. Lett. 183, 143–146.

Google Scholar

Kujala, T., Palva, M. J., Salonen, O., Alku, P., Huotilainen, M., Järvinen, A., et al. (2005). The role of blind humans’ visual cortex in auditory change detection. Neurosci. Lett. 379, 127–131. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2004.12.070

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kupers, R., Fumal, A., de Noordhout, A. M., Gjedde, A., Schoenen, J., and Ptito, M. (2006). Transcranial magnetic stimulation of the visual cortex induces somatotopically organized qualia in blind subjects. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 103:13256. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0602925103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kupers, R., Pietrini, P., Ricciardi, E., and Ptito, M. (2011). The Nature of Consciousness in the Visually Deprived Brain. Front. Psychol. 2:19. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lessard, N., Paré, M., Lepore, F., and Lassonde, M. (1998). Early-blind human subjects localize sound sources better than sighted subjects. Nature 395, 278–280. doi: 10.1038/26228

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liberati, A., Altman, D. G., Tetzlaff, J., Mulrow, C., Gøtzsche, P. C., Ioannidis, J. P. A., et al. (2009). The PRISMA statement for reporting systematic reviews and meta-analyses of studies that evaluate healthcare interventions: explanation and elaboration. BMJ 339, b2700. doi: 10.1136/bmj.b2700

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Linvill, J. G., Bliss, J. C., and direct, A. (1966). translation reading aid for the blind. Proc. IEEE 54, 40–51.

Google Scholar

Liotti, M., Ryder, K., and Woldorff, M. G. (1998). Auditory attention in the congenitally blind: Where, when and what gets reorganized? Neuroreport 9, 1007–1012. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199804200-00010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lockley, S. W., Arendt, J., and Skene, D. J. (2007). Visual impairment and circadian rhythm disorders. Dialogues Clin. Neurosci. 9, 301–314.

Google Scholar

Lopes Da Silva, F. H. (2003). Visual dreams in the congenitally blind? Trends Cogn. Sci. 7, 328–330. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(03)00155-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marks, G. A., Shaffery, J. P., Oksenberg, A., Speciale, S. G., and Roffwarg, H. P. (1995). A functional role for REM sleep in brain maturation. Behav. Brain Res. 69, 1–11. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(95)00018-o

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mattioni, S., Rezk, M., Battal, C., Bottini, R., Mendoza, K. E. C., Oosterhof, N. N., et al. (2020). Categorical representation from sound and sight in the ventral occipito-temporal cortex of sighted and blind. eLife 9:e50732.

Google Scholar

Meaidi, A., Jennum, P., Ptito, M., and Kupers, R. (2014). The sensory construction of dreams and nightmare frequency in congenitally blind and late blind individuals. Sleep Med. 15, 586–595. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2013.12.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miletic, G., Hughes, B., and Bach-y-Rita, P. (1988). Vibrotactile stimulation: An educational program for spatial concept development. J. Vis. Impair. Blind. 82, 366–370.

Google Scholar

Miyashita-Lin, E. M., Hevner, R., Wassarman, K. M., Martinez, S., and Rubenstein, J. L. (1999). Early neocortical regionalization in the absence of thalamic innervation. Science 285, 906–909. doi: 10.1126/science.285.5429.906

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Müller, F., Niso, G., Samiee, S., Ptito, M., Baillet, S., and Kupers, R. (2019). A thalamocortical pathway for fast rerouting of tactile information to occipital cortex in congenital blindness. Nat. Commun. 10:5154. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-13173-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nakagawa, Y., Johnson, J. E., and O’Leary, D. D. (1999). Graded and areal expression patterns of regulatory genes and cadherins in embryonic neocortex independent of thalamocortical input. J. Neurosci. 19, 10877–10885. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-24-10877.1999

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nau, A. C., Pintar, C., Arnoldussen, A., and Fisher, C. (2015). Acquisition of Visual Perception in Blind Adults Using the BrainPort Artificial Vision Device. Am. J. Occup. Ther. 69, 1–8. doi: 10.5014/ajot.2015.011809

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Noordzij, M. L., Zuidhoek, S., and Postma, A. (2007). The influence of visual experience on visual and spatial imagery. Perception 36, 101–112.

Google Scholar

Ockelford, A. (2021). Case studies in neuroplasticity: the musical abilities of some blind and autistic children. London: KCL Neuroscience Society.

Google Scholar

O’Leary, D. D., Chou, S.-J., and Sahara, S. (2007). Area patterning of the mammalian cortex. Neuron 56, 252–269.

Google Scholar

Page, M. J., McKenzie, J. E., Bossuyt, P. M., Boutron, I., Hoffmann, T. C., Mulrow, C. D., et al. (2021). The PRISMA 2020 statement: an updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ 372:n71.

Google Scholar

Pascual-Leone, A., and Hamilton, R. (2001). The metamodal organization of the brain. Progress Brain Res. 134, 427–445.

Google Scholar

Ptito, M., Schneider, F. C. G., Paulson, O. B., and Kupers, R. (2008). Alterations of the visual pathways in congenital blindness. Exp. Brain Res. 187, 41–49.

Google Scholar

Pulvermuller, F. (2018). Neural reuse of action perception circuits for language, concepts and communication. Prog. Neurobiol. 160, 1–44. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2017.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rakic, P. (1988). Specification of cerebral cortical areas. Science 241, 170–176. doi: 10.1126/science.3291116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rakic, P., Suñer, I., and Williams, R. W. (1991). A novel cytoarchitectonic area induced experimentally within the primate visual cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 88, 2083–2087.

Google Scholar

Raz, N., Amedi, A., and Zohary, E. (2005). V1 activation in congenitally blind humans is associated with episodic retrieval. Cereb. Cortex 15, 1459–1468.

Google Scholar

Reislev, N. H., Dyrby, T. B., Siebner, H. R., Lundell, H., Ptito, M., and Kupers, R. (2017). Thalamocortical connectivity and microstructural changes in congenital and late blindness. Neural Plast. 2017:9807512.

Google Scholar

Renier, L., Bruyer, R., and De Volder, A. G. (2006). Vertical-horizontal illusion present for sighted but not early blind humans using auditory substitution of vision. Percept. Psychophys. 68, 535–542.

Google Scholar

Renier, L., and De Volder, A. G. (2010). Vision substitution and depth perception: Early blind subjects experience visual perspective through their ears. Disabil. Rehabil. 5, 175–183.

Google Scholar

Renier, L., Laloyaux, C., Collignon, O., Tranduy, D., Vanlierde, A., Bruyer, R., et al. (2005). The Ponzo illusion with auditory substitution of vision in sighted and early-blind subjects. Perception 34, 857–867.

Google Scholar

Révész, G., and Wolff, H. A. (1950). Psychology and Art of the Blind. Harlow: Longmans.

Google Scholar

Ring, K., and Cooper, S. (1997). Near-death and out-of-body experiences in the blind: A study of apparent eyeless vision. J. Near-Death Stud. 16, 101–147.

Google Scholar

Ring, K., and Cooper, S. (1999). Mindsight: Near-death and out-of-body experiences in the blind. Mindsight: Near-death and out-of-body experiences in the blind. Palo Alto, CA: William James for Center for Consciousness Studies.

Google Scholar

Riso, R. R. (1999). Strategies for providing upper extremity amputees with tactile and hand position feedback–moving closer to the bionic arm. Technol. Health Care 7, 401–409.

Google Scholar

Röder, B., Rösler, F., and Hennighausen, E. (1997). Different cortical activation patterns in blind and sighted humans during encoding and transformation of haptic images. Psychophysiology 34, 292–307.

Google Scholar

Röder, B., Rösler, F., and Neville, H. J. (2000). Event-related potentials during auditory language processing in congenitally blind and sighted people. Neuropsychologia 38, 1482–1502.

Google Scholar

Romains, J. (1920). La Vision extra-rétinienne et le sens paroptique, recherches de psycho-physiologie expérimentale et de physiologie histologique. Paris: Nouvelle revue française.

Google Scholar

Sadato, N., Pascual-Leone, A., Grafman, J., Ibañez, V., Deiber, M. P., Dold, G., et al. (1996). Activation of the primary visual cortex by Braille reading in blind subjects. Nature 380, 526–528.

Google Scholar

Sampaio, E., Maris, S., and Bach-y-Rita, P. (2001). Brain plasticity: ‘visual’ acuity of blind persons via the tongue. Brain Res. 908, 204–207.

Google Scholar

Siclari, F., Valli, K., and Arnulf, I. (2020). Dreams and nightmares in healthy adults and in patients with sleep and neurological disorders. Lancet Neurol. 19, 849–859.

Google Scholar

Simor, P., van der Wijk, G., Nobili, L., and Peigneux, P. (2020). The microstructure of REM sleep: Why phasic and tonic? Sleep Med. Rev. 52:101305.

Google Scholar

Starkiewicz, W., and Kuliszewski, T. (1963). “The 80-channel elektroftalm,” in Proceedings of the lnternational Congress Technology Blindness, New York, NY.

Google Scholar

Staunton, H., and O’Rourke, K. (2012). The creation of a topographical world and its contents in the dreams of the congenitally blind. Dreaming 22, 53–57.

Google Scholar

Striem-Amit, E., and Amedi, A. (2014). Visual cortex extrastriate body-selective area activation in congenitally blind people “Seeing” by using sounds. Curr. Biol. 24, 687–692.

Google Scholar

Striem-Amit, E., Dakwar, O., Reich, L., and Amedi, A. (2012). The large-scale organization of “visual” streams emerges without visual experience. Cereb. Cortex 22, 1698–1709.

Google Scholar

Striem-Amit, E., Wang, X., Bi, Y., and Caramazza, A. (2018). Neural representation of visual concepts in people born blind. Nat. Commun. 9:5250.

Google Scholar

Timney, B., Mitchell, D., and Giffin, F. (1978). The development of vision in cats after extended periods of dark-rearing. Exp. Brain Res. 31, 547–560.

Google Scholar

Tinti, C., Adenzato, M., Tamietto, M., and Cornoldi, C. (2006). Visual experience is not necessary for efficient survey spatial cognition: Evidence from blindness. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 59, 1306–1328.

Google Scholar

Torrance, E. P., Khatena, J., and Cunnington, B. F. (1973). Thinking Creatively with Sounds and Words: Sounds and Images, Onomatopoeia and Images: Directions Manual and Scoring Guide. Lexington, MA: Personnel Press/Ginn.

Google Scholar

Tyler, M., Danilov, Y., and Bach-y-Rita, P. (2003). Closing an open-loop control system: vestibular substitution through the tongue. J. Integr. Neurosci. 2, 159–164.

Google Scholar

Vanlierde, A., De Volder, A. G., Wanet-Defalque, M. C., and Veraart, C. (2003). Occipito-parietal cortex activation during visuo-spatial imagery in early blind humans. Neuroimage 19, 698–709.

Google Scholar

Vanlierde, A., and Wanet-Defalque, M. C. (2004). Abilities and strategies of blind and sighted subjects in visuo-spatial imagery. Acta Psychol. 116, 205–222.

Google Scholar

Velázquez, R., and Bazán, O. (2010). Preliminary evaluation of podotactile feedback in sighted and blind users. Conference proceedings: Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society. IEEE Eng. Med. Biol. Soc. Conf. 2010, 2103–2106.

Google Scholar

Velázquez, R., Bazán, O., and Magaña, M. (2009). “A shoe-integrated tactile display for directional navigation,” in Proceedings of the 2009 IEEE/RSJ International Conference on Intelligent Robots and Systems, New York, NY.

Google Scholar

Vetter, P., Bola, L., Reich, L., Bennett, M., Muckli, L., and Amedi, A. (2020). Decoding natural sounds in early “visual” cortex of congenitally blind individuals. Curr. Biol. 30, 3039–3044.e2.

Google Scholar

Weeks, R., Horwitz, B., Aziz-Sultan, A., Tian, B., Wessinger, C. M., Cohen, L. G., et al. (2000). A positron emission tomographic study of auditory localization in the congenitally blind. J. Neurosci. 20, 2664–2672.

Google Scholar

Keywords: dreams, congenitally blind, cross-modal plasticity, metamodal brain, visuo-spatial imagery

Citation: Ilic K, Bertani R, Lapteva N, Drakatos P, Delogu A, Raheel K, Soteriou M, Mutti C, Steier J, Carmichael DW, Goadsby PJ, Ockelford A and Rosenzweig I (2023) Visuo-spatial imagery in dreams of congenitally and early blind: a systematic review. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 17:1204129. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2023.1204129

Received: 11 April 2023; Accepted: 19 June 2023;
Published: 30 June 2023.

Edited by:

Elias Manjarrez, Meritorious Autonomous University of Puebla, Mexico

Reviewed by:

Walter G. Sannita, University of Genoa, Italy
Marcelo Fernandes Costa, University of São Paulo, Brazil

Copyright © 2023 Ilic, Bertani, Lapteva, Drakatos, Delogu, Raheel, Soteriou, Mutti, Steier, Carmichael, Goadsby, Ockelford and Rosenzweig. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Ivana Rosenzweig, ivana.1.rosenzweig@kcl.ac.uk

These authors share first authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.