Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Integr. Neurosci., 05 October 2018

Dopaminergic Modulation of Goal-Directed Behavior in a Rodent Model of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder

  • 1Center for Molecular and Behavioral Neuroscience, Rutgers University, Newark, NJ, United States
  • 2Kessler Foundation, East Hanover, NJ, United States
  • 3Palestinian Neuroscience Initiative, Al-Quds University, East Jerusalem, Palestine
  • 4Children’s Specialized Hospital Research Center, New Brunswick, NJ, United States
  • 5Department of Psychology, Rutgers University, Newark, NJ, United States

Aside from its clinical symptoms of inattention, impulsivity and hyperactivity, patients with Attention/Deficit-Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) display reward and motivational impairments. These impairments may reflect a deficit in action control, that is, an inability to flexibly adapt behavior to changing consequences. We previously showed that spontaneously hypertensive rats (SHR), an inbred rodent model of ADHD, show impairments in goal-directed action control, and instead are predominated by habits. In this study, we examined the effects of specific dopamine receptor sub-type (D1 and D2) agonists and antagonists on goal-directed behavior in SHR and the normotensive inbred control strain Wistar-Kyoto (WKY) rats. Rats acquired an instrumental response for different-flavored food rewards. A selective-satiety outcome devaluation procedure followed by a choice test in extinction revealed outcome-insensitive habitual behavior in SHR rats. Outcome-sensitive goal-directed behavior was restored in SHR rats following injection prior to the choice test of the dopamine D2 receptor agonist Quinpirole or dopamine D1 receptor antagonist SCH23390, whereas WKY rats showed habitual responding following exposure to these drugs. This novel finding indicates that the core behavioral deficit in ADHD might not be a consequence of dopamine hypofunction, but rather is due to a misbalance between activation of dopamine D1 and D2 receptor pathways that govern action control.

Introduction

Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) is one of the most prevalent psychiatric disorders, characterized by developmentally inappropriate symptoms of inattention, hyperactivity and impulsivity (Castellanos and Tannock, 2002; Barkley, 2005). Along with these symptoms, individuals with ADHD show motivational impairments (Carlson and Tamm, 2000; Konrad et al., 2000; Slusarek et al., 2001; Castellanos and Tannock, 2002; McInerney and Kerns, 2003; Tripp and Wickens, 2008; Luman et al., 2010). For example, children with ADHD show reduced sensitivity to positive reinforcement compared to healthy children, they fail to adapt appropriately to changing rates of reinforcement, and they require larger incentives to adjust their actions (Tripp and Wickens, 2008; Volkow et al., 2011). Despite this evidence, the nature of reward processing deficits in ADHD has not been fully characterized. For example, reduced task motivation in ADHD could reflect blunted reward sensitivity, a failure to encode action-reward contingencies, or impaired action control. The latter process, action control, refers to the way in which voluntary actions are selected and executed based on prior reinforcement learning. Behavioral, computational and neural evidence identifies two parallel, interactive systems that underlie action control: a goal-directed system for deliberative actions, and a habitual system for reflexive actions (Tolman and Gleitman, 1949; Dickinson, 1985; Balleine and Dickinson, 1998; Dayan and Balleine, 2002; Balleine and O’Doherty, 2010). Importantly, goal-directed and habitual systems place different demands on attentional resources (Hitchcott et al., 2007; Le Pelley et al., 2013; Savalia et al., 2016; Luque et al., 2017), and as a consequence individuals with ADHD may be biased toward habitual action control.

Brain regions responsible for action control (e.g., cortico-basal ganglia circuitry), as well as dopamine signaling within these regions, show abnormalities in ADHD (Castellanos et al., 2006; Volkow et al., 2011; Furukawa et al., 2014; Hauser et al., 2014; von Rhein et al., 2015). Dopamine plays a critical role in action selection and initiation; therefore, a deficit in action control in ADHD may arise, in part, from misbalanced dopamine signaling within the basal ganglia (Yin et al., 2008; Tripp and Wickens, 2009). In our previous study (Natsheh and Shiflett, 2015), we found that spontaneously hypertensive rats (SHR), a rat model of ADHD, show a deficiency in goal-directed behavior that is restored by methylphenidate, which increases dopamine signaling. Studies in healthy rats have shown that exposure to amphetamine (Nelson and Killcross, 2006; Nordquist et al., 2007), alcohol (Corbit et al., 2012), stress (Dias-Ferreira et al., 2009) or binge-like consumption of a palatable food (Furlong et al., 2014) can accelerate habitual control. Interestingly, systemic or intra-striatal administration of D1 receptor antagonists (SCH23390) restored goal-directed behavior in these animals, whereas D2 receptor antagonists (Eticlopride) enhanced habitual behavior (Nelson and Killcross, 2013; Furlong et al., 2014). Taken together, these findings suggest normal patterns of goal-directed behavior rely on optimal activity levels of D1 and D2 dopamine receptors (D1R, D2R). Over-activation of D1R or under-activation of D2R could disrupt goal-directed behavior and/or promote habitual behavior.

Here, we hypothesize that impaired goal-directed behavior in SHR rats results from a misbalance in D1R and D2R activation. We assessed the effects of the D1R agonist SKF38393, and antagonist SCH23390, and D2R agonist Quinpirole and antagonist Raclopride on goal-directed behavior in SHR rats and their control strain, Wistar Kyoto (WKY) rats. To assess action control, we used instrumental procedures in which rats made responses to gain access to rewarding food pellets. We then used a selective-satiety procedure to examine whether rats used outcome value to guide behavior, i.e., whether their behavior was goal-directed. We predict that the D1R antagonist SCH23390 and the D2R agonist Quinpirole will restore goal-directed behavior in SHR rats and impair this behavior in control rats. We expect the opposite set of agonist/antagonist pairings (SKF38393/Raclopride) will further impair performance in SHR rats as well as in control rats (Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Theoretical framework for D1R/D2R balance in Spontaneously Hypertensive Rats (SHR) and Wistar-Kyoto (WKY) rats following normal saline, SCH23390 (SCH), Quinpirole (Quin), SKF38393 (SKF) and Raclopride (Rac) injections. If D1R and D2R are in balance we expect that rats will show goal-directed behavior. If D1R and D2R are not in balance, we expect that rats will show habitual response.

Materials and Methods

Subjects and Apparatus

Seventy-two male adult (P49–P80) rats were used in this study; 36 of which were SHR (ADHD rat model) from Charles River Laboratories (Wilmington, MA, USA), and 36 were WKY rats, the normotensive control strain, from Envigo (Indianapolis, IN, USA). The choice of strains from different vendors is based on previous studies showing that the WKY strain from Envigo (formerly, Harlan) is most similar genetically to Charles River SHR rats (Sagvolden and Johansen, 2012). The choice of age is based on the notion that SHR rats can start to develop symptoms of hypertension between the ages of 4–10 weeks which can result in neurological and behavioral deficits (Marcil et al., 1997; Christiansen et al., 2002; Ueno et al., 2002). Therefore, the age that has been widely used across SHR studies and that offers the fewest complications with hypertension is younger adult rats to serve as a model for ADHD (Sagvolden et al., 1992, 1993, 1998; Sagvolden, 2000). Rats weighed approximately 110–175 g at the time of testing.

Rats were housed in pairs in 47.6 × 20.3 × 26 cm (w × h × d) polycarbonate containers with Alpha Chip bedding material (Northeastern Products Corp., Warrensburg, NY, USA) and had free access to water. One week after arrival, all rats were placed on a restricted food diet of approximately 15 g of standard rat pellets (Purina, St. Louis, MO, USA) per day. Rats were fed after their daily behavioral training session. Food restriction continued for the duration of the experiment. All procedures were approved and carried out in accordance with the recommendations of the Rutgers University Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee.

Behavioral training and testing took place in 12 identical rat operant conditioning chambers (Med Associates, St. Albans, VT, USA). Each operant conditioning chamber measured 30.5 × 24.1 × 21 cm (w × h × d) and was constructed of stainless steel and clear plastic walls and a stainless-steel grid floor. A food cup with infrared detectors was centered on one wall of the operant conditioning chamber. Retractable levers were situated to the left and right of the food cup. Responses on these levers delivered one food pellet from a pellet dispenser mounted outside the operant conditioning chamber. Two types of pellets were used in the experimental procedures: 45-mg grain-based pellets and chocolate-flavored purified pellets (Bio-serv, Frenchtown, NJ, USA). Each operant conditioning chamber was housed in a sound attenuating shell and equipped with a ventilation fan that was activated during behavioral procedures. Control over the operant conditioning chambers was enabled by a personal computer operating through an interface. Operant conditioning chamber operation and data collection were carried out with Med Associates proprietary software (Med-PC).

Behavioral Procedures

General Procedures

A description/timeline of behavioral procedures is depicted in Figure 2. Behavioral procedures commenced after 1 week of food restriction. After instrumental training, rats were divided into two groups. One group received injections of saline, SCH23390 and Quinpirole. The second group received injections of saline, SKF38393 and Raclopride.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. A timeline for behavioral experiments.

Instrumental Conditioning

Rats underwent two training sessions per day. For each training session, one lever was inserted into the chamber and responses the rats made on the lever delivered a single 45-mg food pellet (Bio-serv, Flemington, NJ, USA). Responses on one lever delivered chocolate-flavored pellets, and the opposite lever delivered grain-based pellets. Rats were trained daily on each lever in separate sessions with a 30-min interval between sessions. The session terminated when rats earned 20 pellets or 25 min had elapsed. Training lasted for 10 days (see Figure 2); on days 1–3, each response on the lever resulted in pellet delivery (continuous reinforcement). On days 4–5, pellets were delivered according to a variable-ratio (VR) 5 schedule, which required, on average, five responses to earn a pellet reward. On days 6–8, pellets were delivered according to a VR-15 schedule. On days 9–10, pellets were delivered according to a VR-20 schedule.

Outcome Devaluation Test and Drug Injection

Rats were placed in individual cages identical to their home cage and provided with 25 g of one of the instrumental outcomes (either grain or chocolate-flavored pellets). After 40 min, rats were given an intraperitoneal injection of normal saline or one of the experimental drug treatments. Rats were returned to the cages containing food pellets for an additional 15 min. They were then placed in the operant conditioning chamber and both levers were inserted. Rats had the opportunity to respond on either lever for 5 min. No outcomes were presented in this session. The following day, rats underwent selective satiety devaluation of the opposite outcome from the previous day, followed by injection of the same drug as was used the previous day, and a choice test. Overall, the devaluation test was repeated six times. Under each drug treatment condition, rats underwent chocolate and grain pellet devaluation to control for pellet preference. Rats received reminder instrumental training sessions between consecutive devaluation sessions.

Locomotor Activity Assay

Rats were individually placed in an activity-monitoring arena equipped with an automated locomotor activity detection system (Accuscan, Columbus OH, USA). Rats were placed in the arena for a 30-min habituation session. Immediately after habituation, rats were injected with normal saline and returned to the arena for 30 min. This was followed by an injection of one of the experimental drugs, after which the rats were returned for a final 30-min session. Locomotor activity was estimated based on the number of photobeam breaks that occurred as animals moved through the arena.

Drug Treatment

All drugs were purchased from Sigma Aldrich (St. Louis, MO, USA) and dissolved in 0.8% normal saline. For the outcome devaluation tests we used a dose of 0.0025 mg/kg for SCH23390, 0.001 mg/kg for Quinpirole, 3.0 mg/kg for SKF38393 and 0.1 mg/kg for Raclopride (D2R-antagonist).

Statistics and Data Analysis

For instrumental conditioning tests, the rate of response was calculated as the number of lever presses per min during each session. Reinforcer type (chocolate or grain pellet) was collapsed across instrumental training sessions, as no effect of reinforcer type was observed on measures of response rate. For the devaluation test, responses were categorized as “devalued” if the rat made a response on the lever associated with the sated outcome, and “valued” if the rat made a response on the lever associated with the non-sated outcome. Data were normalized by dividing responses on the valued or devalued lever by total (valued plus devalued) responses. Normalization was carried out because of strain differences in overall response rates during the tests. Drug and saline injections were intermixed for all experiments; therefore, there was no injection-order effect to influence outcome devaluation responding. Similarly, chocolate and grain devaluation was counterbalanced across devaluation sessions to control for devaluation flavor-order effects.

Data analysis was conducted using SPSS v20. The normality of data distribution was checked using Kolmogorov–Smirnov tests. All data were normally distributed (p > 0.1). To analyze instrumental performance, we used a 2-factor analyses of variance (ANOVA) and planned comparisons using two-tailed t-tests. The level of significance was set at α = 0.05 throughout our analyses.

To analyze outcome devaluation data, we used mixed-model ANOVAs and planned comparisons using two-tailed t-tests. For rats that received SCH 23390 and Quinpirole injections, eight SHR and nine WKY rats received only one devaluation session under each drug condition. The results were compared to animals that received two devaluation sessions (10 SHR and 10 WKY), and the same pattern of results were observed; therefore, we combined the two datasets.

Results

Instrumental Training Data

All rats acquired an instrumental response; however, SHR rats exhibited greater response rates across training sessions compared to WKY rats. Figure 3A represents the lever-pressing rate in SHR and WKY rats. A mixed-model ANOVA confirmed: (1) a significant effect of training block (F(1,69) = 788.85, p < 0.001); (2) a significant effect of strain (F(1,69) = 30.137, p < 0.001); and (3) a significant block * strain interaction (F(1,69) = 25.87, p < 0.001). Independent-samples t-test showed that SHR’s response rate was significantly higher than WKY’s response rate over the first (p = 0.003) and the last (p < 0.001) training blocks.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Effects of dopamine receptor agonists and antagonists on extinction test responding following outcome devaluation in SHR and WKY rats. (A) SHR displayed a significantly greater response rate during training. Block-1 represents the average of the first three training sessions, and Block-2 represents the average of the last three training sessions showed as the mean number of presses per min on each block of training in SHR (N = 35) and WKY (N = 36) rats. (B–G) Dopamine receptor subtype agonists and antagonists modulate goal-directed behavior in a strain-dependent manner. The percentage of responses on the valued and devalued levers in SHR and WKY under (B) Saline; (C) SCH23390; (D) Quinpirole; (E) Saline; (F) SKF38393; (G) Raclopride (n = 5–9 per group; error bars = ±SEM; *significant at p < 0.05, paired t-tests).

Outcome Devaluation Results

Effects of SCH23390 and Quinpirole on Choice Following Outcome Devaluation (Figures 3B–D)

We injected rats prior to the choice tests with either saline, Quinpirole (0.01 mg/kg) or SCH23390 (0.0025 mg/kg). Responses on the valued and devalued levers were normalized as a percentage of total responses during the test. We carried out separate 3-factor ANOVAs on responses following SCH23390 and Quinpirole treatment using outcome value as the within-subjects factor and strain and type of injection (saline vs. drug) as between-subject factors. These analyses showed significant effects of outcome value for both drugs (SCH23390: F(1,45) = 16.63, p < 0.001, Quinpirole: F(1,48) = 31.67, p < 0.001). We also found a significant outcome value * strain * injection interaction for both SCH23390 (F(1,45) = 6.1, p = 0.017) and Quinpirole (F(1,48) = 6.63, p = 0.013). These data indicate that the drug effects on responding during devaluation differed depending on the rat strain.

Follow up t-tests revealed that following saline injections, WKY rats showed goal-directed behavior by responding at a significantly higher rate on the valued vs. the devalued lever. In contrast, SCH23390 and Quinpirole disrupted goal-directed behavior in these rats as their responses were not significantly different between valued and devalued levers following drug injections. We observed a different pattern in SHR rats. Following saline injections, SHR rats showed an impairment in goal-directed behavior, responding equally on the valued and devalued levers. Both SCH23390 and Quinpirole restored goal-directed behavior in these rats, as shown by significantly greater responding on the valued lever compared to the devalued lever (paired samples t-tests, see Figure 3 for significance levels).

Effects of SKF38393 and Raclopride on Choice Following Outcome Devaluation (Figures 3E–G)

We injected rats prior to the choice tests with either saline, SKF38393 (3.0 mg/kg) or Raclopride (0.1 mg/kg). We carried out separate 3-factor ANOVAs for SKF38393 and Raclopride using outcome value as the within-subjects factor and strain and type of injection (saline vs. drug) as between-subject factors. These analyses showed significant effects of outcome value for both drugs: SKF38393: F(1,30) = 4.28, p = 0.047, β = 0.52, η2 = 0.13, Raclopride: F(1,28) = 5.45, p = 0.027, β = 0.62, η2 = 0.16; however, we observed no significant interactions. Follow up t-tests revealed that following saline injections, WKY rats showed goal-directed behavior by responding at a significantly higher rate on the valued vs. the devalued lever. In contrast, following SKF38393 and Raclopride injections we observed responses that were not significantly different between valued and devalued levers in WKY rats. SHR rats showed no significant difference on valued vs. devalued responding following saline, SKF38393 or Raclopride. These data suggest, unlike the effects of Quinpirole and SCH23390, SKF38393 and Raclopride do not improve goal-directed behavior in SHR rats.

The effects of the various drugs on responding during the test following devaluation are not due to effects on food consumption or response rates more generally. We found no effect of drug type on the amount of food consumed during the satiety test (see “Food Consumption” section). Likewise, the effects on goal-directed behavior cannot be explained by an inability to produce a motor response, as all animals included in the test responded on the lever (see “Locomotor Activity Test” section).

Goal-Directed Score

For this analysis, we calculated a Goal-directed Score (GDS) for each animal using the following formula: [(% of valued responses −% of devalued responses)/(% of valued responses +% of devalued responses)]. Figure 4 shows the average GDS in SHR and WKY rats under Figure 4A SCH23390, Figure 4B Quinpirole, Figure 4C SKF38393 and Figure 4D Raclopride, as compared to normal saline. Mixed-model ANOVAs were conducted for each drug using type of injection (drug vs. normal saline) as within-subject factor and strain as between-subject factor. These analyses revealed significant drug * strain interaction for SCH23390 (F(1,12) = 4.93, p = 0.046) and Quinpirole (F(1,14) = 8.04, p = 0.013) and significant drug effect for SKF38393 (F(1,10) = 7.7, p = 0.02). Overall, these data indicate that the effects of SCH23390 and Quinpirole on goal-directed responding differ by strain: in SHR rats these drugs tend to increase goal-directed responding, whereas in WKY rats they tend to decrease goal-directed responding.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Effects of dopamine receptor agonists and antagonists on Goal-directed Score (GDS) following outcome devaluation in SHR and WKY rats. GDS was calculated using the formula: [% of valued responses − % of devalued responses)/(% of valued responses + % of devalued responses)]. Dopamine receptor subtype agonists and antagonists modulate GDS in a strain-dependent manner. The GDS under injections with normal saline or: (A) SCH23390; (B) Quinpirole; (C) SKF38393; (D) Raclopride; (n = 4–9 per group; error bars = ±SEM; significant drug × strain interactions were observed under SCH23390 (1) and Quinpirole (B), paired t-tests).

Total Response Rate Under Different Drug Status During the Devaluation Test

Effects of SCH23390 and Quinpirole on Response Rate Following Outcome Devaluation

Figure 5A shows the effect of Quinpirole and SCH23390 compared to normal saline injections on total responses per minute (the average of responses on both levers per minute) for both rat strains during the devaluation test. A 2 × 3 multifactorial ANOVA with total responses per minute as dependent variable and strain and medication status as fixed factors showed no significant effects of drug (F(2,61) = 1.76, p = 0.18) or strain * drug interaction (F(2,61) = 0.65, p = 0.52). However, there was a significant effect of strain (F(1,61) = 34, p < 0.001). Overall, these results indicate that drug injections did not affect total response rate during the devaluation test.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Total responses per minute during devaluation test under injections with (A) normal saline, Quinpirole, or SCH23390 in SHR (Quinpirole N = 8, saline N = 16, SCH N = 9) and WKY (Quinpirole N = 10, Saline N = 18, SCH N = 6) rats and (B): normal saline, Raclopride, or SKF38393 in SHR (Raclopride N = 9, saline N = 9, SKF N = 8) and WKY (Raclopride N = 5, Saline N = 9, SKF N = 5) rats (error bars = ±SEM; *significant at p < 0.05).

Effects of SKF38393 and Raclopride on Response Rate Following Outcome Devaluation

Figure 5B shows the effect of SKF38393 and Raclopride compared to normal saline injections on the total responses per minute during the devaluation test. A 2 × 3 multifactorial ANOVA with total responses per minute as dependent variable and strain and medication status as fixed factors showed significant effects of drug (F(2,42) = 25.65, p < 0.001) and strain* drug interaction (F(2,42) = 4.08, p = 0.024) with an effect of strain approaching significance (F(1,42) = 4.04, p = 0.051). SHR rats showed a significantly greater response rate compared to WKY rats under normal saline (t(16) = 2.4, p = 0.029), and SKF38393 (t(14) = 2.85, p = 0.013). Compared to saline, SKF38393 and Raclopride significantly reduced response rates in SHR rats (SKF38393: t(10.93) = 5.07, p < 0.001; Raclopride: t(9.82) = 6.41, p < 0.001). In WKY rats, SKF38393 significantly reduced response rates compared to saline (t(8.4) = 3.41, p = 0.009). Overall, these results indicate that drug injections affected total response rate during the devaluation test in the two strains as compared to responses under normal saline injections. However, in this experiment the first two devaluation sessions were carried out under normal saline, while drug injections were carried out during the last four devaluation sessions. Therefore, since the decrease in response rate was observed under injections of both drugs (SKF38393 [D1-agonist] and Raclopride [D2-antagonist]), lower sensitivity to the devaluation test, which can ensue with repetitive devaluation sessions, could account for these results. In line with this, locomotor activity test results showed that both drug injections did not affect locomotion at doses of 3.0 mg/kg SKF38393 or 0.1 mg/kg Raclopride (see “Locomotor Activity Test Results” section).

Exclusion Criteria

Exclusion criteria for analysis included: (1) Outcome preference: we excluded rats that had a preference to chocolate or grain pellets during the last block of instrumental training with a response rate of >1.5× responses per minute on the lever that is associated with the preferred outcome. Out of 72 rats, 8 SHR rats and 5 WKY rats were excluded from the outcome devaluation test. (2) Lever preference: we excluded rats that had a preference to the right or left lever during the extinction test with >2 SD from the mean percentage valued or devalued. We excluded two devaluation sessions of SHR and four devaluation sessions of WKY rats. (3) Low response rate: we excluded rats that had a low response rate during extinction with a total response of <1 per minute on both levers. We excluded 13 devaluation sessions of WKY rats and one devaluation session of SHR rats. See Table 1 for reference.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Number of rats included across strain and medication status.

Food Consumption

To determine whether drug or rat strain influenced food consumption during the devaluation procedure, we examined the amount of food rats consumed during the first 40 min of the satiety procedure prior to injections as well as in the 20 min after injections. The majority of food consumption (~90%) occurred during the first 40 min prior to injections (Table 2). In this interval we observed no strain differences in the amount of food consumed. Food consumption following drug injection did not differ from saline except for SKF38393 in SHR rats (t(33.3) = 2.64, p = 0.012). Since the majority of food consumption occurred during the first 40 min prior to injections (92%), only 8% of food consumption (average of 1.1 g) occurred under the effect of drugs, which implicates that even though there was a significant effect of drugs on food consumption, it was minimal, with a low effect size (η = 0.047).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Amount of food pellets consumed during satiety-induced devaluation after 40 min, and in the 20 min following injections with normal saline, SCH23390, Quinpirole, Raclopride and SKF38393.

Locomotor Activity Test

We recorded locomotor activity during 30-min sessions of habituation, following saline injection, and following drug injection. The following doses were used: SCH23390 (0.0025 mg/kg); Quinpirole (0.001 mg/kg or 0.01 mg/kg); SKF38393 (1.0 mg/kg or 3.0 mg/kg); Raclopride (0.05 mg/kg or 0.1 mg/kg). Horizontal activity was averaged across 5-min blocks for each session (blocks 1–6).

Effects of SCH23390 and Quinpirole on Locomotor Activity (Figures 6A–D)

SHR rats traveled a greater distance as measured by horizontal activity during habituation and under saline and drug injections. We analyzed locomotion under each drug separately using two-factor ANOVA’s, with strain and phase (habituation, saline, or drug) as factors. Under both SCH23390 and Quinpirole, we observed a significant effect of strain (SCH23390: F(1,10) = 82.31, p = 0.025; Quinpirole: (F(1,10) = 21.72, p = 0.001), indicating SHR rats were traveling a greater distance. Under virtually every condition tested SHR rats made significantly greater movement compared to WKY rats (independent-samples t-test: habituation (t(22) = 3.36, p = 0.003); saline (t(15.72) = 4.81, p < 0.001), SCH23390 0.0025 mg/kg (t(10) = 4.31, p = 0.002), Quinpirole 0.001 mg/kg (t(10) = 2.33, p = 0.042); Quinpirole 0.01 mg/kg (t(6.2) = 7.92, p < 0.001).

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Effects of dopamine receptor agonists and antagonists on locomotor activity. Locomotor activity in WKY (left column) and SHR (right column) is shown in 5-min blocks during habituation, saline injections and drug injections (A–B) SCH23390, 0.0025 mg/kg; (C–D) Quinpirole, 0.001 mg/kg and 0.01 mg/kg; (E–F) SKF38393, 1.0 mg/kg, 3.0 mg/kg (G–H) Raclopride, 0.05 mg/kg, 0.1 mg/kg (error bars = ±SEM; n = 6–12 per group; paired t-tests; HACTV = average horizontal activity).

We observed a significant effect of phase for both drugs (SCH23390 F(2,20) = 21.22, p < 0.001; Quinpirole (F(3,30) = 56.76, p < 0.001). Comparisons to saline revealed marginally reduced locomotion in rats exposed to 0.0025 mg/kg SCH23390 in both WKY (paired-samples t-test: t(5) = 2.24, p = 0.076) and SHR (t(5) = 2.24, p = 0.075). Under 0.001 mg/kg Quinpirole, we observed no overall effect on locomotion in WKY (t(5) = 0.17, p = 0.83), and a marginally decreased locomotion in SHR rats (t(5) = 2.18, p = 0.08). However, a dose of 0.01 mg/kg did significantly decrease locomotion in both strains as compared to saline injections (WKY: t(5) = 5.99, p = 0.002, SHR: t(5) = 2.61, p = 0.047).

Effects of SKF38393 and Raclopride on Locomotor Activity (Figures 6E–H)

We again observed greater distance traveled in SHR rats. Under SKF38393 and Raclopride we observed a significant effect of strain (ANOVA: SKF38393 (F(1,10) = 11.33, p = 0.007; Raclopride (F(1,10) = 5.79, p = 0.037). Locomotion was significantly greater in SHR rats as compared to WKY rats during virtually every condition (independent-samples t-tests: habituation (t(10) = 2.28, p = 0.046); saline (t(10) = 2.94, p = 0.015), SKF38393 3.0 mg/kg (t(10) = 3.89, p = 0.003); Raclopride 0.1 mg/kg (t(10) = 2.3, p < 0.045) injections.

We observed a significant effect of phase for both drugs (SKF38393 F(3,30) = 10.18, p < 0.001; Raclopride F(3,30) = 10.84, p < 0.001). Within-subjects comparisons revealed no significant differences from saline in animals treated with SKF38393 (all t-tests n.s.). Among rats treated with raclopride, no significant differences from saline in locomotor activity were observed except in SHR rats at a dose of 0.05 mg/kg, which significantly decreased locomotion in these animals (t(5) = 2.66, p = 0.045).

Discussion

Our results reveal novel roles for dopamine receptor sub-types on goal-directed behavior in rats, and suggest how dysregulation of activity across these sub-types may give rise to behavioral impairments in SHR rats. SHR rats exhibit impaired goal-directed behavior when tested using an outcome devaluation paradigm, replicating our previous findings (Natsheh and Shiflett, 2015). Here, we report that stimulation of D2 receptors or inhibition of D1 receptors restored goal-directed behavior in SHR rats. Conversely, we found that stimulation of D1 receptors or inhibition of D2 receptors did not improve goal-directed behavior in SHR rats. In WKY rats (the normotensive strain most often used as a control group for SHR) we observed goal-directed behavior under control conditions. However, in contrast to SHR rats, treatment with quinpirole and SCH23390 impaired goal-directed behavior. The divergence in response to these drugs on measures of goal-directed behavior between strains suggests fundamental differences in how dopamine signaling is engaged during learning in SHR rats.

Our findings support the notion that balanced activation of D1 and D2 receptors is essential to display goal-directed behavior. Our results are in agreement with models of striatal dopamine function in action selection and initiation (Keeler et al., 2014). Viewed through the lens of dopamine function, some features of ADHD, such as impaired motivation, may be attributed to misbalanced activity between these receptor sub-types. That is, over-activation of D1 receptors at the expense of D2 receptors might account for SHR’s tendency towards reward proximal behavior that is under greater control of the habitual system (Figure 1). Using selective D2 receptor agonists and D1 antagonists restored the balance of activation between dopamine receptor sub-types and hence remediated the deficit in goal-directed behavior in SHR rats, whereas these same drugs caused a misbalance of activation in WKY rats and impaired action control.

We hypothesize that dopamine receptor agonists and antagonists restore goal-directed behavior in SHR rats through their stimulation of “direct” striato-nigral and “indirect” striato-pallidal pathways. Dopamine D1 and D2 receptors are almost exclusively expressed in the direct and indirect pathways, respectively (Albin et al., 1989; Gerfen et al., 1990; Wichmann and DeLong, 1996; Burke et al., 2017). Although these pathways are traditionally associated with motor activity, there is increasing evidence that they are also essential for many aspects of learning (Seger and Cincotta, 2006; Pennartz et al., 2009; Keeler et al., 2014). Phasic and tonic dopamine neuron activity differentially activates the direct and indirect pathway based on the unique binding properties of dopamine receptor subtypes: D1 receptors are stimulated during phasic DA release and exhibit low-affinity binding of DA, while tonic release is responsible for modulating high-affinity D2 receptors. Phasic firing is functionally involved in reward-based behavior (Mirenowicz and Schultz, 1994, 1996; Grace et al., 2007; Hikida et al., 2010; Kravitz et al., 2010, 2012; Yawata et al., 2012; Morita and Hikida, 2015). The indirect pathway has been involved in behavioral flexibility through inhibiting actions in reward learning paradigms, leading to flexibly switching between behaviors (Hikida et al., 2010; Kravitz et al., 2012; Yawata et al., 2012). Reduced inhibition of the indirect pathway, through low stimulation of D2R, can lead to loss of inhibitory control, resulting in behavioral deficits such as compulsivity, impulsivity, or excessive habit formation (Yin et al., 2004; Johnson and Kenny, 2010; Seger and Spiering, 2011; Bock et al., 2013). SHR rats have a high density of striatal D1 dopamine receptors and atypical D2 dopamine receptor activity (Lim et al., 1990a,b; Yu et al., 1990; Linthorst et al., 1993; Russell et al., 1995; Carey et al., 1998). Although it remains to be tested, our findings suggest that misbalanced activation exists between the direct and indirect pathway in SHR rats as a consequence of this differential distribution of D1 and D2 receptors.

The acquisition and deployment of goal-directed behavior has been shown to depend on a balanced activity in direct and indirect pathway neurons that may be weighted toward the indirect pathway and the inhibition of its functional output (Macpherson et al., 2014; Shan et al., 2014). Accordingly, normal corticostriatal function should represent a balanced activation/inactivation in the direct and the indirect pathways. Dopamine depletion in the striatum, such as in Parkinson’s disease, results in hypo-activation/hypo-inhibition of the direct/indirect pathways, respectively that account for motor dysfunction (Galvan and Wichmann, 2008; Magrinelli et al., 2016), as well as cognitive deficits (Frank et al., 2004; Redgrave et al., 2010; de Wit et al., 2011). Some studies have proposed that the motor hyperactivity in ADHD may reflect a “reverse Parkinsonism” that is characterized by either overstimulation of dopaminergic activity in the direct pathway, or excessive dopaminergic inhibition of the indirect pathway (Castellanos, 1997). Here, we argue that specific dopamine receptor modulation of the direct and indirect pathways might explain motivational impairments in ADHD, in addition to motor symptoms. Evidence suggests that naturally occurring polymorphisms of the D1R and D2R genes are implicated in patients with ADHD; however, data on the functional significance of specific polymorphisms of these two genes are still inconclusive (Rowe et al., 1999; Bobb et al., 2005; Serý et al., 2006; Luca et al., 2007; Ribasés et al., 2012).

The effects of dopamine modulation on rats’ performance in the devaluation paradigm are not likely a consequence of memory modulation. It has been previously shown that instrumental incentive learning is not dopamine dependent (Dickinson et al., 2000; Wassum et al., 2011). Thus, animals should have intact memory of the incentive properties of the instrumental outcomes during the choice test. It could be argued that the lack of sensitivity to outcome value in SHR rats may reflect either a performance deficit or a learning/memory deficit. However, SHR rats that received D2R agonist or D1R antagonist injections following outcome devaluation prior to the choice test show value-sensitive responding. Therefore, these animals did encode action-outcome associations during instrumental learning; however, they were only able to use these associations to guide behavior when tested under the effects of D2R stimulation or D1R inhibition. Thus, the deficits we observed in tests of goal-directed behavior in non-medicated SHR rats likely reflect a deficit in performance and not learning of goal-directed actions. Similarly, we previously found that methylphenidate was effective in remediating goal-directed behavior in SHR (Natsheh and Shiflett, 2015). Our current results suggest that increased activation of D2R in response to methylphenidate may have been responsible for this effect. Methylphenidate is known to increase dopamine availability in the striatum (Volkow et al., 2002; Wilens, 2008). Given D2R’s higher affinity to dopamine (Marcellino et al., 2012), we expect that methylphenidate exerts its behavioral effects by increasing D2R activation (inhibiting the indirect pathway), rather than D1R activation (activating the direct pathway). D2 receptor expression in the striatum of ADHD patients is correlated with trait motivation, further supporting the relationship between D2 activity and motivational impairments in ADHD (Volkow et al., 2011).

Although the striatum is clearly involved in action control, many studies have shown that other brain regions might also be implicated in this behavior. Lesions in the prefrontal cortex significantly decreased sensitivity to outcome devaluation in healthy animals (Hitchcott et al., 2007). Dopamine D1 and D2 signaling was shown to influence prefrontal circuits that guide goal-directed behavior. Specifically, a balanced activation of prefrontal D1R and D2R seems to be essential for optimal temporal expectations and cognitive flexibility during action control processes (St. Onge et al., 2011; Parker et al., 2013, 2015). Further, altering neural excitation of the thalamostriatal pathway produced a deficit in goal-directed behavior (Bradfield et al., 2013; Okada et al., 2014; Aoki et al., 2015). Another study revealed that altering the connection between the basolateral amygdala and the nucleus accumbens impairs sensitivity to outcome value during instrumental responding (Shiflett and Balleine, 2010). Thus, future studies are essential to characterize region-specific influence of D1R and D2R activation in SHR rats during instrumental performance.

Although SHR is the most commonly used and most widely accepted rat model of ADHD (Sagvolden et al., 1992, 1993), utilizing an animal model to study neurobehavioral aspects of diseases has many limitations. It is impossible for an animal model to completely portray all characteristics of a disorder. Further, given that the pathophysiological mechanisms of ADHD are still unclear, it is especially difficult to find an animal model that will fully match its neurobehavioral correlates. Another limitation of our study was examining medication modulation in SHR and WKY rats under acute administration (one dose). Many studies have reported significant variations in the therapeutic effects of acute vs. chronic treatment with dopaminergic medications. For example, the effects of quinpirole at low doses may primarily activate pre-synaptic D2 autoreceptors, causing an increase in DA release, whereas at higher doses quinpirole may primarily affect post-synaptic D2 receptors. Future studies with different treatment time points are required to address this issue.

In conclusion, we show for the first time that the dominant habitual response in SHR rats might be due to an over-activation of D1R (over-activation of the direct pathway) and/or under-activation of D2R (hypo-inhibition of the indirect pathway). Modulating dopamine receptor activity has a clear effect on mediating action control behavior in SHR and WKY rats. Thus, unraveling action control mechanisms in ADHD can broaden our understanding of the neural circuits underlying cognitive symptoms of this disorder. Further studies using pharmacological and neural imaging techniques in patients with ADHD are imperative to delineate behavioral and neural action control mechanisms as well as novel treatment options for this disorder.

Author Contributions

JN and MS developed the research hypothesis tested in this study. JN carried out the experiments and wrote the manuscript with supervision, feedback and critical revisions from MS.

Funding

This research was supported by (1) the Chancellor’s Seed Grant Program awarded by Rutgers University to Drs. Elizabeth Tricomi (PI), MS (Co-PI), Tracy Tran (Co-PI) and JN (Co-PI), and (2) the Rita Levi-Montalcini Fellowship (Grant No. 801335/435222) awarded by the International Brain Research Organization (IBRO) to Dr. JN (PI).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Victor Lopez, Delia Duran, Sydney B. Pacelli, Dania Rohani and Samanthika Devalaraju for their assistance with data collection.

References

Albin, R. L., Young, A. B., and Penney, J. B. (1989). The functional anatomy of basal ganglia disorders. Trends Neurosci. 12, 366–375. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(89)90074-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aoki, S., Liu, A. W., Zucca, A., Zucca, S., and Wickens, J. R. (2015). Role of striatal cholinergic interneurons in set-shifting in the rat. J. Neurosci. 35, 9424–9431. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0490-15.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balleine, B. W., and Dickinson, A. (1998). Goal-directed instrumental action: contingency and incentive learning and their cortical substrates. Neuropharmacology 37, 407–419. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3908(98)00033-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balleine, B. W., and O’Doherty, J. P. (2010). Human and rodent homologies in action control: corticostriatal determinants of goal-directed and habitual action. Neuropsychopharmacology 35, 48–69. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.131

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barkley, R. A. (2005). Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder, Third Edition: A Handbook for Diagnosis and Treatment. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Bobb, A. J., Addington, A. M., Sidransky, E., Gornick, M. C., Lerch, J. P., Greenstein, D. K., et al. (2005). Support for association between ADHD and two candidate genes: NET1 and DRD1. Am. J. Med. Genet. B Neuropsychiatr. Genet. 134B, 67–72. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30142

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bock, R., Shin, J. H., Kaplan, A. R., Dobi, A., Markey, E., Kramer, P. F., et al. (2013). Strengthening the accumbal indirect pathway promotes resilience to compulsive cocaine use. Nat. Neurosci. 16, 632–638. doi: 10.1038/nn.3369

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bradfield, L. A., Bertran-Gonzalez, J., Chieng, B., and Balleine, B. W. (2013). The thalamostriatal pathway and cholinergic control of goal-directed action: interlacing new with existing learning in the striatum. Neuron 79, 153–166. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.04.039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burke, D. A., Rotstein, H. G., and Alvarez, V. A. (2017). Striatal local circuitry: a new framework for lateral inhibition. Neuron 96, 267–284. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.09.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carey, M. P., Diewald, L. M., Esposito, F. J., Pellicano, M. P., Gironi Carnevale, U. A., Sergeant, J. A., et al. (1998). Differential distribution, affinity and plasticity of dopamine D-1 and D-2 receptors in the target sites of the mesolimbic system in an animal model of ADHD. Behav. Brain Res. 94, 173–185. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(97)00178-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carlson, C. L., and Tamm, L. (2000). Responsiveness of children with attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder to reward and response cost: differential impact on performance and motivation. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 68, 73–83. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.68.1.73

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Castellanos, F. X., Giedd, J. N., Eckburg, P., Marsh, W. L., Vaituzis, A. C., Kaysen, D., et al. (2006). Quantitative morphology of the caudate nucleus in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 151, 1791–1796. doi: 10.1176/ajp.151.12.1791

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Castellanos, F. X. (1997). Toward a pathophysiology of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Clin. Pediatr. 36, 381–393. doi: 10.1177/000992289703600702

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Castellanos, F. X., and Tannock, R. (2002). Neuroscience of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: the search for endophenotypes. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 617–628. doi: 10.1038/nrn896

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Christiansen, R. E., Roald, A. B., Tenstad, O., and Iversen, B. M. (2002). Renal hemodynamics during development of hypertension in young spontaneously hypertensive rats. Kidney Blood Press. Res. 25, 322–328. doi: 10.1159/000066792

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Corbit, L. H., Nie, H., and Janak, P. H. (2012). Habitual alcohol seeking: time course and the contribution of subregions of the dorsal striatum. Biol. Psychiatry 72, 389–395. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.02.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dayan, P., and Balleine, B. W. (2002). Reward, motivation and reinforcement learning. Neuron 36, 285–298. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00963-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Wit, S., Barker, R. A., Dickinson, A. D., and Cools, R. (2011). Habitual versus goal-directed action control in Parkinson disease. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 1218–1229. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2010.21514

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dias-Ferreira, E., Sousa, J. C., Melo, I., Morgado, P., Mesquita, A. R., Cerqueira, J. J., et al. (2009). Chronic stress causes frontostriatal reorganization and affects decision-making. Science 325, 621–625. doi: 10.1126/science.1171203

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dickinson, A. (1985). Actions and habits: the development of behavioural autonomy. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 308, 67–78. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1985.0010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dickinson, A., Smith, J., and Mirenowicz, J. (2000). Dissociation of Pavlovian and instrumental incentive learning under dopamine antagonists. Behav. Neurosci. 114, 468–483. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.114.3.468

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frank, M. J., Seeberger, L. C., and O’Reilly, R. C. (2004). By carrot or by stick: cognitive reinforcement learning in parkinsonism. Science 306, 1940–1943. doi: 10.1126/science.1102941

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Furlong, T. M., Jayaweera, H. K., Balleine, B. W., and Corbit, L. H. (2014). Binge-like consumption of a palatable food accelerates habitual control of behavior and is dependent on activation of the dorsolateral striatum. J. Neurosci. 34, 5012–5022. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3707-13.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Furukawa, E., Bado, P., Tripp, G., Mattos, P., Wickens, J. R., Bramati, I. E., et al. (2014). Abnormal striatal BOLD responses to reward anticipation and reward delivery in ADHD. PLoS One 9:e89129. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0089129

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Galvan, A., and Wichmann, T. (2008). Pathophysiology of parkinsonism. Clin. Neurophysiol. 119, 1459–1474. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2008.03.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gerfen, C. R., Engber, T. M., Mahan, L. C., Susel, Z., Chase, T. N., Monsma, F. J. Jr., et al. (1990). D1 and D2 dopamine receptor-regulated gene expression of striatonigral and striatopallidal neurons. Science 250, 1429–1432. doi: 10.1126/science.2147780

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grace, A. A., Floresco, S. B., Goto, Y., and Lodge, D. J. (2007). Regulation of firing of dopaminergic neurons and control of goal-directed behaviors. Trends Neurosci. 30, 220–227. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2007.03.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hauser, T. U., Iannaccone, R., Ball, J., Mathys, C., Brandeis, D., Walitza, S., et al. (2014). Role of the medial prefrontal cortex in impaired decision making in juvenile attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. JAMA Psychiatry 71, 1165–1173. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.1093

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hikida, T., Kimura, K., Wada, N., Funabiki, K., and Nakanishi, S. (2010). Distinct roles of synaptic transmission in direct and indirect striatal pathways to reward and aversive behavior. Neuron 66, 896–907. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.05.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hitchcott, P. K., Quinn, J. J., and Taylor, J. R. (2007). Bidirectional modulation of goal-directed actions by prefrontal cortical dopamine. Cereb. Cortex 17, 2820–2827. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, P. M., and Kenny, P. J. (2010). Dopamine D2 receptors in addiction-like reward dysfunction and compulsive eating in obese rats. Nat. Neurosci. 13, 635–641. doi: 10.1038/nn.2519

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keeler, J. F., Pretsell, D. O., and Robbins, T. W. (2014). Functional implications of dopamine D1 vs. D2 receptors: a ‘prepare and select’ model of the striatal direct vs. indirect pathways. Neuroscience 282, 156–175. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.07.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Konrad, K., Gauggel, S., Manz, A., and Scholl, M. (2000). Inhibitory control in children with traumatic brain injury (TBI) and children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Brain Inj. 14, 859–875. doi: 10.1080/026990500445691

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kravitz, A. V., Freeze, B. S., Parker, P. R., Kay, K., Thwin, M. T., Deisseroth, K., et al. (2010). Regulation of parkinsonian motor behaviours by optogenetic control of basal ganglia circuitry. Nature 466, 622–626. doi: 10.1038/nature09159

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kravitz, A. V., Tye, L. D., and Kreitzer, A. C. (2012). Distinct roles for direct and indirect pathway striatal neurons in reinforcement. Nat. Neurosci. 15, 816–818. doi: 10.1038/nn.3100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Le Pelley, M. E., Mitchell, C. J., and Johnson, A. M. (2013). Outcome value influences attentional biases in human associative learning: dissociable effects of training and instruction. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 39, 39–55. doi: 10.1037/a0031230

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lim, D. K., Yu, Z. J., Hoskins, B., Rockhold, R. W., and Ho, I. K. (1990a). Effects of acute and subacute cocaine administration on the CNS dopaminergic system in Wistar-Kyoto and spontaneously hypertensive rats: II. Dopamine receptors. Neurochem. Res. 15, 621–627. doi: 10.1007/bf00973753

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lim, D. K., Yu, Z. J., Hoskins, B., Rockhold, R. W., and Ho, I. K. (1990b). Effects of acute and subacute cocaine administration on the CNS dopaminergic system in Wistar-Kyoto and spontaneously hypertensive rats: III. Dopamine uptake. Neurochem. Res. 15, 629–634. doi: 10.1007/bf00973754

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Linthorst, A. C., De Jong, W., De Boer, T., and Versteeg, D. H. (1993). Dopamine D1 and D2 receptors in the caudate nucleus of spontaneously hypertensive rats and normotensive Wistar-Kyoto rats. Brain Res. 602, 119–125. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(93)90250-q

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luca, P., Laurin, N., Misener, V. L., Wigg, K. G., Anderson, B., Cate-Carter, T., et al. (2007). Association of the dopamine receptor D1 gene, DRD1, with inattention symptoms in families selected for reading problems. Mol. Psychiatry 12, 776–785. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001972

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luman, M., Tripp, G., and Scheres, A. (2010). Identifying the neurobiology of altered reinforcement sensitivity in ADHD: a review and research agenda. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 34, 744–754. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.11.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luque, D., Beesley, T., Morris, R. W., Jack, B. N., Griffiths, O., Whitford, T. J., et al. (2017). Goal-directed and habit-like modulations of stimulus processing during reinforcement learning. J. Neurosci. 37, 3009–3017. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3205-16.2017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Macpherson, T., Morita, M., and Hikida, T. (2014). Striatal direct and indirect pathways control decision-making behavior. Front. Psychol. 5:1301. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01301

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Magrinelli, F., Picelli, A., Tocco, P., Federico, A., Roncari, L., Smania, N., et al. (2016). Pathophysiology of motor dysfunction in Parkinson’s disease as the rationale for drug treatment and rehabilitation. Parkinsons Dis. 2016:9832839. doi: 10.1155/2016/9832839

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marcellino, D., Kehr, J., Agnati, L. F., and Fuxe, K. (2012). Increased affinity of dopamine for D2 -like versus D1 -like receptors. Synapse 66, 196–203. doi: 10.1002/syn.21501

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marcil, J., Thibault, C., and Anand-Srivastava, M. B. (1997). Enhanced expression of Gi-protein precedes the development of blood pressure in spontaneously hypertensive rats. J. Mol. Cell. Cardiol. 29, 1009–1022. doi: 10.1006/jmcc.1996.0343

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McInerney, R. J., and Kerns, K. A. (2003). Time reproduction in children with ADHD: motivation matters. Child Neuropsychol. 9, 91–108. doi: 10.1076/chin.9.2.91.14506

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mirenowicz, J., and Schultz, W. (1994). Importance of unpredictability for reward responses in primate dopamine neurons. J. Neurophysiol. 72, 1024–1027. doi: 10.1152/jn.1994.72.2.1024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mirenowicz, J., and Schultz, W. (1996). Preferential activation of midbrain dopamine neurons by appetitive rather than aversive stimuli. Nature 379, 449–451. doi: 10.1038/379449a0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morita, M., and Hikida, T. (2015). [Distinct roles of the direct and indirect pathways in the basal ganglia circuit mechanism]. Nihon Shinkei Seishin Yakurigaku Zasshi 35, 107–111.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Natsheh, J. Y., and Shiflett, M. W. (2015). The effects of methylphenidate on goal-directed behavior in a rat model of ADHD. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 9:326. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00326

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nelson, A., and Killcross, S. (2006). Amphetamine exposure enhances habit formation. J. Neurosci. 26, 3805–3812. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4305-05.2006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nelson, A. J., and Killcross, S. (2013). Accelerated habit formation following amphetamine exposure is reversed by D1, but enhanced by D2, receptor antagonists. Front. Neurosci. 7:76. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2013.00076

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nordquist, R. E., Voorn, P., de Mooij-van Malsen, J. G., Joosten, R. N., Pennartz, C. M., and Vanderschuren, L. J. (2007). Augmented reinforcer value and accelerated habit formation after repeated amphetamine treatment. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 17, 532–540. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2006.12.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Okada, K., Nishizawa, K., Fukabori, R., Kai, N., Shiota, A., Ueda, M., et al. (2014). Enhanced flexibility of place discrimination learning by targeting striatal cholinergic interneurons. Nat. Commun. 5:3778. doi: 10.1038/ncomms4778

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parker, K. L., Alberico, S. L., Miller, A. D., and Narayanan, N. S. (2013). Prefrontal D1 dopamine signaling is necessary for temporal expectation during reaction time performance. Neuroscience 255, 246–254. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.09.057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parker, K. L., Ruggiero, R. N., and Narayanan, N. S. (2015). Infusion of D1 dopamine receptor agonist into medial frontal cortex disrupts neural correlates of interval timing. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 9:294. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00294

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pennartz, C. M., Berke, J. D., Graybiel, A. M., Ito, R., Lansink, C. S., van der Meer, M., et al. (2009). Corticostriatal interactions during learning, memory processing and decision making. J. Neurosci. 29, 12831–12838. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3177-09.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Redgrave, P., Rodriguez, M., Smith, Y., Rodriguez-Oroz, M. C., Lehericy, S., Bergman, H., et al. (2010). Goal-directed and habitual control in the basal ganglia: implications for Parkinson’s disease. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 760–772. doi: 10.1038/nrn2915

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ribasés, M., Ramos-Quiroga, J. A., Hervás, A., Sánchez-Mora, C., Bosch, R., Bielsa, A., et al. (2012). Candidate system analysis in ADHD: evaluation of nine genes involved in dopaminergic neurotransmission identifies association with DRD1. World J. Biol. Psychiatry 13, 281–292. doi: 10.3109/15622975.2011.584905

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rowe, D. C., Van den Oord, E. J., Stever, C., Giedinghagen, L. N., Gard, J. M., Cleveland, H. H., et al. (1999). The DRD2 TaqI polymorphism and symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Mol. Psychiatry 4, 580–586. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4000567

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Russell, V., de Villiers, A., Sagvolden, T., Lamm, M., and Taljaard, J. (1995). Altered dopaminergic function in the prefrontal cortex, nucleus accumbens and caudate-putamen of an animal model of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder—the spontaneously hypertensive rat. Brain Res. 676, 343–351. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(95)00135-d

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sagvolden, T. (2000). Behavioral validation of the spontaneously hypertensive rat (SHR) as an animal model of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (AD/HD). Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 24, 31–39. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(99)00058-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sagvolden, T., Aase, H., Zeiner, P., and Berger, D. (1998). Altered reinforcement mechanisms in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Behav. Brain Res. 94, 61–71. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(97)00170-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sagvolden, T., and Johansen, E. B. (2012). Rat models of ADHD. Curr. Top. Behav. Neurosci. 9, 301–315. doi: 10.1007/7854_2011_126

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sagvolden, T., Metzger, M. A., Schiorbeck, H. K., Rugland, A. L., Spinnangr, I., and Sagvolden, G. (1992). The spontaneously hypertensive rat (SHR) as an animal model of childhood hyperactivity (ADHD): changed reactivity to reinforcers and to psychomotor stimulants. Behav. Neural Biol. 58, 103–112. doi: 10.1016/0163-1047(92)90315-u

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sagvolden, T., Pettersen, M. B., and Larsen, M. C. (1993). Spontaneously hypertensive rats (SHR) as a putative animal model of childhood hyperkinesis: SHR behavior compared to four other rat strains. Physiol. Behav. 54, 1047–1055. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(93)90323-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Savalia, T., Shukla, A., and Bapi, R. S. (2016). A unified theoretical framework for cognitive sequencing. Front. Psychol. 7:1821. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01821

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seger, C. A., and Cincotta, C. M. (2006). Dynamics of frontal, striatal and hippocampal systems during rule learning. Cereb. Cortex 16, 1546–1555. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhj092

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seger, C. A., and Spiering, B. J. (2011). A critical review of habit learning and the basal ganglia. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 5:66. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2011.00066

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Serý, O., Drtilkova, I., Theiner, P., Pitelova, R., Staif, R., Znojil, V., et al. (2006). Polymorphism of DRD2 gene and ADHD. Neuro Endocrinol. Lett. 27, 236–240. Avaliable online at: http://nel.edu/polymorphism-of-drd2-gene-and-adhd-1850/

Google Scholar

Shan, Q., Ge, M., Christie, M. J., and Balleine, B. W. (2014). The acquisition of goal-directed actions generates opposing plasticity in direct and indirect pathways in dorsomedial striatum. J. Neurosci. 34, 9196–9201. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.0313-14.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shiflett, M. W., and Balleine, B. W. (2010). At the limbic-motor interface: disconnection of basolateral amygdala from nucleus accumbens core and shell reveals dissociable components of incentive motivation. Eur. J. Neurosci. 32, 1735–1743. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07439.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Slusarek, M., Velling, S., Bunk, D., and Eggers, C. (2001). Motivational effects on inhibitory control in children with ADHD. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 40, 355–363. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200103000-00016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

St. Onge, J. R., Abhari, H., and Floresco, S. B. (2011). Dissociable contributions by prefrontal D1 and D2 receptors to risk-based decision making. J. Neurosci. 31, 8625–8633. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.1020-11.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tolman, E. C., and Gleitman, H. (1949). Studies in spatial learning; place and response learning under different degrees of motivation. J. Exp. Psychol. 39, 653–659. doi: 10.1037/h0059317

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tripp, G., and Wickens, J. R. (2008). Research review: dopamine transfer deficit: a neurobiological theory of altered reinforcement mechanisms in ADHD. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 49, 691–704. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01851.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tripp, G., and Wickens, J. R. (2009). Neurobiology of ADHD. Neuropharmacology 57, 579–589. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2009.07.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ueno, K. I., Togashi, H., Mori, K., Matsumoto, M., Ohashi, S., Hoshino, A., et al. (2002). Behavioural and pharmacological relevance of stroke-prone spontaneously hypertensive rats as an animal model of a developmental disorder. Behav. Pharmacol. 13, 1–13. doi: 10.1097/00008877-200202000-00001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Volkow, N. D., Fowler, J. S., Wang, G., Ding, Y., and Gatley, S. J. (2002). Mechanism of action of methylphenidate: insights from PET imaging studies. J. Atten. Disord. 6, S31–S43. doi: 10.1177/070674370200601s05

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

von Rhein, D., Cools, R., Zwiers, M. P., van der Schaaf, M., Franke, B., Luman, M., et al. (2015). Increased neural responses to reward in adolescents and young adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and their unaffected siblings. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 54, 394–402. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2015.02.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Volkow, N. D., Wang, G.-J., Newcorn, J. H., Kollins, S. H., Wigal, T. L., Telang, F., et al. (2011). Motivation deficit in ADHD is associated with dysfunction of the dopamine reward pathway. Mol. Psychiatry 16, 1147–1154. doi: 10.1038/mp.2010.97

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wassum, K. M., Ostlund, S. B., Balleine, B. W., and Maidment, N. T. (2011). Differential dependence of Pavlovian incentive motivation and instrumental incentive learning processes on dopamine signaling. Learn. Mem. 18, 475–483. doi: 10.1101/lm.2229311

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wichmann, T., and DeLong, M. R. (1996). Functional and pathophysiological models of the basal ganglia. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 6, 751–758. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(96)80024-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilens, T. E. (2008). Effects of methylphenidate on the catecholaminergic system in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 28, S46–S53. doi: 10.1097/jcp.0b013e318173312f

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yawata, S., Yamaguchi, T., Danjo, T., Hikida, T., and Nakanishi, S. (2012). Pathway-specific control of reward learning and its flexibility via selective dopamine receptors in the nucleus accumbens. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 109, 12764–12769. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1210797109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yin, H. H., Knowlton, B. J., and Balleine, B. W. (2004). Lesions of dorsolateral striatum preserve outcome expectancy but disrupt habit formation in instrumental learning. Eur. J. Neurosci. 19, 181–189. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03095.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yin, H. H., Ostlund, S. B., and Balleine, B. W. (2008). Reward-guided learning beyond dopamine in the nucleus accumbens: the integrative functions of cortico-basal ganglia networks. Eur. J. Neurosci. 28, 1437–1448. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06422.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yu, Z. J., Lim, D. K., Hoskins, B., Rockhold, R. W., and Ho, I. K. (1990). Effects of acute and subacute cocaine administration on the CNS dopaminergic system in Wistar-Kyoto and spontaneously hypertensive rats: I. Levels of dopamine and metabolites. Neurochem. Res. 15, 613–619. doi: 10.1007/bf00973752

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, spontaneous hypertensive rats, Wistar-Kyoto rats, goal-directed behavior, habitual behavior, action control, dopamine

Citation: Natsheh JY and Shiflett MW (2018) Dopaminergic Modulation of Goal-Directed Behavior in a Rodent Model of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 12:45. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2018.00045

Received: 16 June 2018; Accepted: 13 September 2018;
Published: 05 October 2018.

Edited by:

Martín Cammarota, Federal University of Rio Grande do Norte, Brazil

Reviewed by:

Pedro Bekinschtein, Institute of Cognitive and Translational Neuroscience (INCYT), Argentina
Nandakumar Narayanan, University of Iowa, United States

Copyright © 2018 Natsheh and Shiflett. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Joman Y. Natsheh, jnatsheh@kesslerfoundation.org

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.