Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurosci., 05 August 2024
Sec. Perception Science

The dissociating effects of fear and disgust on multisensory integration in autism: evidence from evoked potentials

  • 1Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychotherapy, and Psychosomatics, Medical Center, University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany
  • 2Department of Neuroimaging, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King’s College London, London, United Kingdom
  • 3Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of California, Santa Barbara, Santa Barbara, CA, United States
  • 4Department of Psychology, Bangor University, Bangor, United Kingdom
  • 5Second Department of Psychiatry, National and Kapodistrian University of Athens, Medical School, University General Hospital “Attikon”, Athens, Greece
  • 6Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Sensorimotor Control, University Mental Health, Neurosciences and Precision Medicine Research Institute “COSTAS STEFANIS”, Athens, Greece
  • 7Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Medical Faculty, University of Cologne, Cologne, Germany

Background: Deficits in Multisensory Integration (MSI) in ASD have been reported repeatedly and have been suggested to be caused by altered long-range connectivity. Here we investigate behavioral and ERP correlates of MSI in ASD using ecologically valid videos of emotional expressions.

Methods: In the present study, we set out to investigate the electrophysiological correlates of audiovisual MSI in young autistic and neurotypical adolescents. We employed dynamic stimuli of high ecological validity (500 ms clips produced by actors) that depicted fear or disgust in unimodal (visual and auditory), and bimodal (audiovisual) conditions.

Results: We report robust MSI effects at both the behavioral and electrophysiological levels and pronounced differences between autistic and neurotypical participants. Specifically, neurotypical controls showed robust behavioral MSI for both emotions as seen through a significant speed-up of bimodal response time (RT), confirmed by Miller’s Race Model Inequality (RMI), with greater MSI effects for fear than disgust. Adolescents with ASD, by contrast, showed behavioral MSI only for fear. At the electrophysiological level, the bimodal condition as compared to the unimodal conditions reduced the amplitudes of the visual P100 and auditory P200 and increased the amplitude of the visual N170 regardless of group. Furthermore, a cluster-based analysis across all electrodes revealed that adolescents with ASD showed an overall delayed and spatially constrained MSI effect compared to controls.

Conclusion: Given that the variables we measured reflect attention, our findings suggest that MSI can be modulated by the differential effects on attention that fear and disgust produce. We also argue that the MSI deficits seen in autistic individuals can be compensated for at later processing stages by (a) the attention-orienting effects of fear, at the behavioral level, and (b) at the electrophysiological level via increased attentional effort.

Highlights

• MSI is key for everyday interactions and has been implicated in the social deficits reported in ASD.

• Previous research has reported MSI deficits in autistic individuals through various paradigms. However, findings have been inconsistent, especially regarding to whether MSI is facilitated independently of attention.

• Most studies investigating MSI in ASD have either done so with non-social stimuli or have used stimuli of low-ecological validity.

• To our knowledge, we are the first study to combine highly ecologically valid stimuli with EEG where we report MSI deficits in autistic adolescents, for which they compensate via attentional mechanisms.

• In this context, a corroboration of these findings from future research could lead to the development of techniques to improve MSI and thus higher-level deficits.

1 Introduction

Deficits in social interaction and communication are among the key characteristics of Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) and have been the topic of a wealth of ASD research. In addition, sensory abnormalities—often reported by clinicians to characterize the experiences of autistic individuals—have become increasingly acknowledged as a central dysfunction of the disorder (Klein et al., 2022) and are now featured in the DSM-5 as one of the “B criteria” (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). And indeed, various studies have reported altered sensory processing in a broad range of sensory domains (Gomot et al., 2008; Kujala et al., 2013). While the subjective sensory experience of autistic individuals is often characterized by hyper-sensitivity to auditory, visual or tactile stimuli (Klein et al., 2022), objective measures of abnormal sensory experience have pointed to problems in integrating information from different sensory channels, such as Multisensory Integration (MSI; Iarocci et al., 2010; Brandwein et al., 2013; Collignon et al., 2013; De Boer-Schellekens et al., 2013; Woynaroski et al., 2013; Katagiri et al., 2014; Stevenson et al., 2014). MSI refers to the integration of information conveyed simultaneously through different sensory channels (e.g., seeing and hearing another person speaking). MSI processes begin very early in sensory information processing (Stefanou et al., 2019). They are considered automatic and unconscious (Meredith and Stein, 1983, 1986; Stein et al., 2014) and lead to more accurate and speeded recognition of the incoming information (Giard and Peronnet, 1999; Brandwein et al., 2011), in particular if sensory information is noisy, for example during a cocktail party (Kayser and Logothetis, 2007). Regarding humans, the efficient integration of information conveyed by the face and voice (moving lips and spoken words) is key to efficient communication (Gervais et al., 2004; Magnée et al., 2011). The well-known McGurk effect (McGurk and MacDonald, 1976) underlines this argument and suggests that MSI processes are automatic and not under voluntary control. However, results on the potential role of attention on MSI still remain contradictory (Hartcher-O’Brien et al., 2017; but see also Koelewijn et al., 2010 for the effects of attention and the stages of MSI).

With regards to the effects of attention more specifically, some studies suggest that MSI occurs automatically (regardless of attention), while others suggest that top-down attention influences successful MSI (Fernández et al., 2015). The complexity of the MSI—attention interplay can be seen by studies suggesting that any attention effects on MSI differ depending on the stage of sensory processing, that is, at the early MSI stage we integrate information pre-attentively whereas at the later MSI stage attention is necessary for successful MSI to be facilitated (Koelewijn et al., 2010; Stefanou et al., 2019). Such findings have left attention’s role on MSI not well understood and a matter of debate (Hartcher-O’Brien et al., 2017).

Impaired MSI in ASD has been demonstrated using various paradigms, including simple RT tasks (Brandwein et al., 2013), multisensory speech paradigms (Foxe et al., 2015), cross-modal temporal order judgment tasks (Kwakye et al., 2011), multisensory illusion tasks (Mongillo et al., 2008; Foss-Feig et al., 2010; Woynaroski et al., 2013; Bebko et al., 2014; Stevenson et al., 2014) and visual search tasks (Collignon et al., 2008). These paradigms have conjointly demonstrated the multiple facets of the MSI deficit in ASD. Among these paradigms are tasks like the one developed by Charbonneau et al. (2013) that trigger MSI processes for the recognition of emotions using ecologically valid video-recorded emotional exclamations of the six basic emotions by professional actors (Simon et al., 2008). More specifically, Charbonneau et al. (2013) demonstrated the behavioral effects of MSI employing emotional exclamations of disgust and fear, which were selected due to their biological significance. The results showed that autistic participants exhibited decreased emotion recognition across visual and auditory modalities and reduced or absent behavioral MSI. Notably, from an evolutionary perspective fear enhances attention to alert of a potentially threatening situation while disgust diverts it to reduce exposure (Susskind et al., 2008; Krusemark and Li, 2011) while in previous studies of our lab we have confirmed that the dissociating effects of fear and disgust on attention can extent to MSI and facilitate it or diminish it (Stefanou et al., 2019).

As mentioned before, MSI occurs very early in sensory processing and seems to be triggered automatically (but see also Koelewijn et al., 2010; Stefanou et al., 2019, 2020 for the effects of attention in later processing stages) as shown in the superior colliculus of anaesthetized cats (Meredith and Stein, 1983). Likewise, Event-Related Potential (ERP) studies, taking advantage of the excellent temporal resolution of the EEG technology, have supported that cortical MSI effects begin early in information processing. Electro-cortical MSI effects are expressed as a distinct activation pattern when comparing the amplitude of the bimodal condition and the sum of the unimodal ones; this distinct activation can be seen either as increased or decreased activity that cannot be explained by the sum of the unimodal conditions [AV – (A + V)], called super-additivity and sub-additivity, respectively. Typically, these MSI effects are found as early as 40 and 90 ms after stimulus onset over various scalp areas, both in adults and in children (Giard and Peronnet, 1999; Molholm et al., 2002; Brett-green et al., 2008; Brandwein et al., 2011; Stefanou et al., 2020). MSI effects are also seen through increased amplitudes of visual components such the P100 and N170 (Stefanou et al., 2019, 2020) when comparing the bimodal with unimodal conditions and compared to the sum during these components’ time-windows (McCracken et al., 2019). In terms of auditory components, studies also typically report reduced amplitudes in the bimodal conditions for the N100 (Magnée et al., 2008; Jessen and Kotz, 2011) and increased amplitude for the P200 (Jessen and Kotz, 2011; but see also Magnée et al., 2011 that reported smaller P200 amplitudes). With regards to latency-specific effects, studies report faster latencies for the auditory N1 and P2 components (Magnée et al., 2009) and visual N100 latency (Stefanou et al., 2020).

In autistic individuals, evidence of altered MSI stems from differing neurophysiological findings. Firstly, analysis of MSI effects across various scalp areas have been reported to be delayed compared to controls (Russo et al., 2010; Stefanou et al., 2020; Dwyer et al., 2022). Secondly, in terms of amplitudes, autistic individuals show overall reduced MSI effects compared to controls over central areas (Dwyer et al., 2022). Finally, symptom severity has been found to correlate significantly with amplitude-related MSI effects in autistic individuals with these effects being decreased for individuals with severe symptoms compared to controls, and individuals with moderate symptoms lying in between of these two groups (Brandwein et al., 2015).

Another important aspect of studying MSI is the ecological validity of the stimuli given that MSI is crucial for perceiving social situations and emotions (Young and Bruce, 2011; Skuk and Schweinberger, 2013; Thye et al., 2018). The role of verbal and facial expression of affect is one domain of communication that plays a pivotal role in functioning social interaction. The perception and recognition of all basic emotions (Darwin, 1872, 2009) is considered inborn (Walker-Andrews, 1998) and entails the efficient integration of face and voice. That emotion perception is indeed multimodal, has been found in various studies (Young and Bruce, 2011; Skuk and Schweinberger, 2013). MSI is thus an essential process of our everyday life, necessary for successful social interaction (Magnée et al., 2011). And yet, the majority of the studies investigating the MSI of emotions have neglected this in one or the other way. Specifically, studies investigating MSI and emotion perception in ASD have typically used stimuli with low ecological validity such as black and white still pictures superimposed with sound (for example, Magnée et al., 2008). Ecological validity should be a requirement for both behavioral and neurophysiological studies but so far studies using stimuli with high ecological validity are mainly behavioral studies (for example, Charbonneau et al., 2013) or limited to healthy adults (for example, Jessen and Kotz, 2011; Skuk and Schweinberger, 2013; Stefanou et al., 2019).

Our study aimed to explore MSI in young adolescents with ASD using ecologically valid, dynamic emotional stimuli. We aimed to replicate the behavioral results of Charbonneau et al. (2013) and to explore the neurophysiological bases of MSI with the use of ERPs. We expected that children with ASD would show a decreased benefit from the bimodal (audiovisual) compared to the unimodal (auditory vs. visual) presentation of emotions, compared to neurotypical controls. We also expected the bimodal condition to lead to an increase in the amplitude of the visual components, an amplitude decrease specific to the auditory components, and for these effects to be stronger for controls than adolescents with ASD. We also aimed to investigate the spatio-temporal evolution of MSI and expected robust effects starting early on after stimulus presentation for neurotypical controls with a delayed MSI effect and limited scalp distribution for adolescents with ASD.

2 Methods

The following experimental protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Albert Ludwigs-University of Freiburg (ethics vote no. 238/15) and all participants’ information was treated according to the declaration of Helsinki. Participants and their parents/legal guardians provided informed written consent, after receiving a verbal and written description of the study.

2.1 Participants

A total of 50 children (21 with ASD; 29 controls) between the ages of 11 and 14 years were recruited through the database of the Clinic for Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychotherapy, and Psychosomatics of the University of Freiburg as part of a larger study (Stefanou et al., 2020).

Participants with autism were selected based on already having received the F84.0, F84.1 or F84.5 diagnoses according to the International Classification of Diseases (ICD-10; World Health Organisation, 1992), as given by an experienced psychiatrist/psychologist of the Clinic. Diagnoses were based on anamnestic interviews with parents and children, the administration of the German version of Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedules (Rühl et al., 2004) and the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (Bölte et al., 2005). ADOS-2 (Poustka et al., 2015) was used with four participants.

All participants with autism except two were medication-free. One participant showing ADHD symptoms but not fulfilling the ADHD diagnostic criteria was given methylphenidate to reduce irritability and inattention in social situations. This participant was medication-free during the testing (paused treatment 24 h prior to the testing sessions). Another autistic participant was receiving antipsychotics (Abilify) due to a comorbid diagnosis of obsessive-compulsive disorder.

Exclusion criteria for both groups were a first language other than German, comorbid diagnoses such as motor ticks, epilepsy, ADHD, or an IQ score below 70 (as assessed with the Cultural Fair Intelligence Test 20-R, CFT 20-R; Weiß and Weiß, 2006). Furthermore, participants were excluded from data analysis if EEG data were heavily contaminated by artifacts such as muscle or movement artifacts, or if they failed or refused to complete at least five blocks (~83%) out of the six blocks of the task. For healthy children, scores outside the Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS; Constantino and Gruber, 2005) normal range was also an exclusion criterion.

After the application of the exclusion criteria, the final sample consisted of 15 children with ASD (10 male, 14 right-handed) and 25 healthy children (13 male, 22 right-handed; see Table 1). Participants were compensated for their time with one cinema or book voucher (worth 7.50 €) per hour.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Group characteristics and scores.

2.2 Stimuli and procedure

The current experiment was part of a larger study and was completed over three sessions (see Stefanou et al., 2020), with the first two sessions being dedicated for the completion of two MSI tasks with simultaneous EEG recording and the third one for the administration of the CFT 20-R (Weiß and Weiß, 2006). We here present results from children completing a two-forced-choice emotion recognition task presented using Psychophysics Toolbox extensions 3.0.12 (Brainard, 1997; Pelli, 1997; Kleiner et al., 2007) on MATLAB software (R2015a; The MathWorks, Inc., Natick, Massachusetts, United States). The two presented emotions were fear and disgust, as in Charbonneau et al. (2013) and were presented by four actors (2 female, 2 male) in an auditory (vocalizations), visual (video without sound) and bimodal (video with sound) condition. Each trial contained a pre-stimulus interval of 2,000–3,000 ms (pooled from an exponential distribution with a mean of ~2,400 ms), followed by a 500 ms stimulus presentation tuned to the screen’s refresh rate, and a response period of 2,000 ms. A white fixation cross was present during the pre-stimulus interval, the response period and the auditory condition. The task comprised of six blocks with each block containing 120 trials (720 trials in total; 120 per emotion per condition). All conditions were interspersed within each block in a pseudorandom order. Stimuli were dynamic videos adapted from Simon et al. (2008) and were processed with Adobe Premiere Elements (Adobe Systems, Inc.). To match the original stimuli by Charbonneau et al. (2013), and as described in Stefanou et al. (2019), video stimuli were segmented to sequences of 500 ms (15 frames) and the audio clips were exported based on these sequences. Subsequently, we ensured that all stimuli started with a neutral expression lasting for 1 frame and evolved into full expression afterwards. Given that visual dominance (that is, more efficient visual than auditory processing) was evidenced in previous studies with such stimuli (Collignon et al., 2008; Stefanou et al., 2019) we reduced the reliability of stimuli in order to account for modality dominance. For this, we parsed the six blocks of trials into two sets of three. In one set, we degraded stimuli by adding uniform white noise to the visual channel of visual unimodal and bimodal conditions, at each frame, pixel and the three color dimensions (RGB). In the other set, we added noise to the auditory channels of all auditory unimodal and bimodal conditions; audio files were first normalized to the range [−1 1] while uniform noise was added to the matrix of the raw audio wave and presented at 75 dB SPL. This ensured that any differences between the unimodal and bimodal conditions were due to MSI effects, and not due to the white noise being presented only in the bimodal condition. Each modality of noise was calibrated using a threshold task to establish individually tailored accuracy levels of 80%. This calibration was achieved by degrading the unimodal stimuli in eight levels of noise (60%–95% vs. 40%–75% for visual noise and 50%–85% vs. 40%–75% for auditory noise for controls and patients, respectively, and in increments of 5%).

These individually tailored noise levels were subsequently used for the main task (visual noise for controls: 76.12 ± 8.5%, for ASD: 67.73 ± 17.43%; controls vs. ASD: p = 0.048; auditory noise for controls: 71.92 ± 12.47%; for ASD: 64.8 ± 20.79%; controls vs. ASD: p = 0.182). Consistent with previous studies using a similar approach (Charbonneau et al., 2013) controls showed a significantly higher noise threshold than patients only for the visual modality [F(1,38) = 4.183, p = 0.048].

Brain Vision Recorder (Brain Products, Gilching), two BrainAmps DC amplifiers and a 64-channel actiCap (Brain Products, Gilching) were used for the acquisition of EEG according to the International 10–10System (American Electroencephalographic Society, 1991). The EEG was recorded with a 500 Hz sampling rate, with impedances kept below 5kΩ. FCz and AFz electrodes served as reference and ground, respectively. Finally, two infraorbital channels were placed vertically under each eye, and an additional electrode was positioned at the Nasion.

2.3 Behavioral data analysis

Responses were defined as the first response that accurately identified the presented emotion within 150 and 1,800 ms relative to stimulus onset. Median reaction times (RT) and the standard variability of responses (SDRT) were calculated for correct responses. Median RT, SDRT, and accuracy were submitted to two separate 2*2*2 mixed model ANOVAs, one for trials degraded with visual noise, and a second one for trials degraded with auditory noise. Both ANOVAs included CONDITION [unimodal (visual/auditory), bimodal (audiovisual)] and EMOTION (fear, disgust) as within-subjects factors and GROUP (ASD, controls) as the between-subjects factor.

In order to evaluate whether any decrease in RT was due to MSI, we applied Miller’s Race Model Inequality (RMI; Miller, 1982) using Matlab (as described in Ulrich et al., 2007). The procedure used in this study was identical to the one described in Stefanou et al. (2019). Miller’s RMI was calculated at every 5th percentile for each participant using the RT distributions of the auditory, visual and bimodal conditions. The “bound,” that is, the bimodal RT distribution hypothesized under Miller’s Race Model, was also calculated at every 5th percentile of the distribution (5th–100th percentile) across emotions, as well as for each emotion separately.

2.4 EEG processing and analyses

EEG data were processed offline using Brain Vision Analyzer (Version 2.0, Brain Products, Gilching). Data were down-sampled to 100 Hz and filtered with a 0.1–40 Hz bandpass filter. During a first data inspection, any data sections with a voltage of ≤0.5 μV or ≥ 1,500 μV for a duration of 200 ms or more were marked as artifact-contaminated and were excluded from further analysis. Data were segmented into 1,900 ms epochs (−200 ms to 1,700 ms relative to stimulus onset). Segments were then submitted to an Infomax Independent Component Analysis (ICA) and components representing eye blinks, saccades, and muscle activity were manually identified and removed by not back-projecting them to the electrode space through a semi-automatic ICA Inverse. A second data inspection was performed and segments with activity ≤0.5 or ≥ 200 μV for a duration of ≥200 ms were again excluded. After re-referencing to the common average reference, data were concatenated across sessions, re-grouped according to condition, emotion and whether trials were degraded with visual or auditory noise. The baseline was then normalized to the period of 200 ms pre-stimulus onset, and segments containing only correct responses were averaged for each participant. This resulted in retaining a minimum of 67% (M = 82; SD = 7%) of the trials of interest for controls and a minimum of 58% (M = 76%; SD = 12%) for patients.

2.4.1 Sensory and perceptual event-related potentials

The auditory N100 and P200, the visual P100 and N170 and the late positive component (LPC) were identified through visual inspection of both the grand and the individual averages. The peaks for each component were determined separately for each subject, emotion and condition, as the maximum peak in a defined electrode channel and a defined interval. The auditory N100 component was identified at central electrodes C1, Cz, C2 between 70 and 150 ms after stimulus-onset, and the auditory P200 again at electrodes C1, Cz, C2 between 170 and 250 ms. The visual P100 component was identified at occipital electrodes (O1, Iz, O2) between 80 and 180 ms, while the visual N170 was identified at parietal and temporal–parietal electrodes between 170 and 260 ms (P2, TP8, P8). Given the large deflection of the P100, the ongoing negativity of the N170 remained of positive amplitude at the expected occipital-parietal (e.g., Batty and Taylor, 2003) and temporal-occipital electrodes. Given that several studies investigate the maxima of N170 in lateral posterior electrodes (see, Eimer and Holmes, 2003; Almeida et al., 2016), we chose the aforementioned temporal–parietal electrodes where N170 was most prominent. LPC was identified at electrodes Pz and POz between 350 and 850 ms for all three conditions.

Peak detection for the auditory N100 and P200, and the visual P100 and N170, was performed in a semi-automatic mode in order to visually inspect the peaks and detect possible variance between participants, and the mean amplitude in a ± 10 ms window around the peak of each component was exported. For the LPC, due to the broad nature of this component, we exported the mean area activity between the 350 and 850 ms time window. In order to investigate MSI interactions, the sum of the unimodal conditions [auditory + visual] and difference waves (bimodal – [auditory + visual]) were also calculated.

All subsequent ANOVAs included GROUP (ASD vs. controls) as the between-subjects factor. Amplitudes and latencies of the visual P100 were submitted to a 2*2*2*3 mixed-model ANOVA with CONDITION (visual, bimodal), EMOTION (fear, disgust) and ELECTRODE (O1, Iz, O2) as within-subjects factors. Likewise, N170 amplitudes and latencies were submitted to a 2*2*2*3 mixed-model ANOVA with CONDITION (visual, bimodal), EMOTION (fear, disgust) and ELECTRODE (P2, TP8, P8) as within-subjects factors. Two separate 2*2*2*3 mixed-model ANOVAs were performed for the auditory N100 and P200 amplitudes and latencies, with CONDITION (auditory, bimodal), EMOTION (fear, disgust) and ELECTRODE (C2, Cz, C1) as within-subjects factors. LPC area activity was submitted to two separate 2*2*2 ANOVAs, one for trials degraded with visual noise and one for trials degraded with auditory noise, with CONDITION (bimodal, unimodal), EMOTION (fear, disgust) and ELECTRODE (Pz, POz) as within-subjects factors. Due to the broad peak of this component, latencies were not analyzed. Any significant interactions revealed from the ANOVAs described in this section were submitted to post-hoc analyses (see corresponding results sections).

2.4.2 Evolution of multisensory integration

The sum of the two unimodal conditions cannot be equalized with the bimodal condition since it is missing the pure MSI effect (for a review, see Stein and Stanford, 2008). For this reason, we assessed the spatio-temporal course of MSI by contrasting the bimodal and sum conditions through a cluster-based permutation test using the Monte Carlo method, a non-parametric analysis implemented in the Fieldtrip toolbox (Maris and Oostenveld, 2007; Maris, 2012; please see Stefanou et al., 2020 for more details). The analysis was also performed for the two levels of the EMOTION factor.

2.5 Analysis of covariance

Given that patients and controls differed significantly in IQ, we performed additional ANCOVAs using IQ as a covariate for both the behavioral and the ERP data. Results were robust regarding IQ unless mentioned otherwise in the corresponding results section.

3 Results

3.1 Behavioral results

3.1.1 Median RT

3.1.1.1 Bimodal vs. auditory

Fear produced overall faster responses than disgust [EMOTION: F(1,38) = 7.97, p = 0.008, ηp2 = 0.173]. The bimodal condition also produced a significant speed-up of RT compared to the auditory [CONDITION: F(1,38) = 90.34, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.703] suggesting an MSI-related speed-up. This MSI-related speed-up seen in the bimodal compared to the auditory condition, was greater for trials presenting disgust [−215.58 ms; t(39) = −8.734, p < 0.001] than trials presenting fear [−165.94 ms; t(39) = −9.510, p < 0.001; CONDITION*EMOTION: F(1,38) = 6.500, p = 0.015, ηp2 = 0.146].

3.1.1.2 Bimodal vs. visual

There was again a main effect of fear producing overall faster responses than disgust [EMOTION: F(1,38) = 27.958, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.424] and a main effect of condition with the bimodal condition producing faster RT compared to the visual condition [CONDITION: F(1,38) = 61.507, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.618]. When comparing the bimodal and the visual condition, the MSI-related speed-up was further increased for trials of fear [−84.82 ms; t(39) = −8.411, p < 0.001] than for disgust [−57.263 ms; t(39) = −6.064, p < 0.001; CONDITION*EMOTION: F(1,38) = 8.398, p = 0.006, ηp2 = 0.181].

3.1.2 Variability of responses

Participants responded significantly less variably (measured by SDRT) during the bimodal compared to the auditory [CONDITION in Bimodal vs. Auditory: F(1,38) = 56.427, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.598] and visual condition [CONDITION in Bimodal vs. Visual: F(1,38) = 9.67, p = 0.003, ηp2 = 0.205]. No other effects, interactions or group differences were found.

3.1.3 Accuracy

3.1.3.1 Bimodal vs. auditory

Participants responded overall more accurately during the bimodal compared to the auditory condition [CONDITION: F(1,38) = 6.063, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.612] suggesting improved recognition due to MSI. This MSI-related improved recognition seen in the bimodal compared to the auditory condition was amplified for trials of disgust [20.4%; t(39) = −6.456, p < 0.001] rather than fear [12.1%; t(39) = 5.369, p < 0.001; CONDITION*EMOTION: F(1,38) = 4.869, p = 0.033, ηp2 = 0.114].

3.1.3.2 Bimodal vs. visual

There was again a main effect of emotion and of condition where participants responded more accurately during fear than disgust and during the bimodal compared to the visual condition [EMOTION: F(1,38) = 7.292, p = 0.010, ηp2 = 0.161; CONDITION: F(1,38) = 34.787, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.478]. On the contrary, and similarly to RTs, when comparing the bimodal and the visual condition, accuracy improvement was significantly increased for the emotion of fear [8.2%0.121; t(39) = 5.899, p < 0.001] than for disgust [3.9%; t(39) = 3.219, p = 0.001; CONDITION*EMOTION: F(1,38) = 6.462, p = 0.015, ηp2 = 0.145]. No significant effect of GROUP or interaction with the factor GROUP was found in any of the above contrasts.

Overall, the key behavioral results show that all participants showed faster RTs, increased accuracy and smaller ISVs (see Table 2; Supplementary Table 1), for the bimodal compared to the unimodal conditions. However, the behavioral improvement during the bimodal condition was amplified by fear when compared to the visual condition contrasts and by disgust when compared to the auditory condition.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Descriptive statistics of behavioral responses.

3.1.4 Miller’s Race Model Inequality

Notably, controls presented a robust MSI that was significant from the 5th to the 45th percentile where patients failed to show any significant MSI. When split by emotions, controls showed again a robust MSI for both emotions (significant violation of the model from the 5th to the 50th percentile for fear and from the 5th to the 45th percentiles for disgust; see Figure 1). Conversely, patients showed a violation of the model only for the emotion of fear, and only from the 15th to the 35th percentile (see Figure 1). These findings confirm the reported MSI deficits in autistic individuals, for which they can potentially compensate through the attentional mechanisms associated with fear.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Miller’s Race Model Inequality (RMI): for controls (top row) and patients (bottom row) for the emotions of fear and disgust. Colored circles represent the coactivation for individual participants across each percentile. The mean is represented by the horizontal bars in black if there was no violation of the model, and in red where the violation was statistically significant. The horizontal gray line represents the bound of the model.

3.2 EEG results

3.2.1 Visual P100

In all subjects, the bimodal condition produced a smaller visual P100 amplitude compared to the visual condition [F(1,38) = 10.589, p = 0.002, ηp2 = 0.218; Figure 2]. There was a significant EMOTION*ELECTRODE interaction [F(1,76) = 3.594, p = 0.032, ηp2 = 0.086] with the post-hoc paired samples t-test revealed that the amplitude of P100 was larger for electrode O2 than for Iz and for trials of fear compared to disgust [t(39) = 3.308, p = 0.002].

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Visual P100 & N170. Panel (A) shows the average waveform (O1, Iz, O2) and topographies of the visual P100 component at the bimodal and visual conditions, for controls and ASD. The dashed box highlights the amplitude suppression and speeded latency of the component during the bimodal compared to the visual. The bottom row of the left panel shows a significant GROUP x ELECTRODE interaction with patients presenting faster latency than controls at electrode O2. Panel (B) shows the average waveform (T8, TP8, P2) and topographies of the visual N170 component at the bimodal and visual conditions, for controls and ASD. The bottom row of the panel shows the overall emotion effect with fear producing an overall increased amplitude. The Y-axis of the waveform represents activity in μV and the X-axis time in ms. The color bar represents the range of activity in μV for interpretation of the topographical maps.

The latency of P100 was shorter following the bimodal compared to the visual stimuli [CONDITION: F(1,38) = 5.60, p = 0.023, ηp2 = 0.128] and for trials depicting fear compared to disgust [EMOTION: F(1,38) = 5.537, p = 0.024, ηp2 = 0.13]. The P100 was of shorter latency at electrode O2 compared to O1 [F(2,76) = 5.949, p = 0.004, ηp2 = 0.135], and, according to post-hoc paired samples t-test this difference was amplified for the emotion of fear compared to disgust [t(39) = −2.335, p = 0.025; EMOTION*ELECTRODE: F(2,76) = 3.622, p = 0.026, ηp2 = 0.092]. Finally, the faster latency of the P100 as seen on electrode O2 was larger for patients than controls [F(1,38) = 4.522, p = 0.040; ELECTRODE*GROUP: F(2,76) = 5.673, p = 0.005, ηp2 = 0.13; see Figure 2]. This interaction revealed a laterality effect for the P100 latency that was, according to post-hoc paired samples t-test performed separately for each group, present only in patients and not in controls [ASD: t(14) = −2.576, p = 0.022; Controls: t(24) = 0.153, p = 0.880].

To summarize the key findings, the bimodal condition produced a speed-up of the visual P100 but contrary to our expectations, the P100 amplitude decreased during the bimodal condition.

3.2.2 Visual N170

The bimodal condition produced a larger N170 amplitude than the visual condition [F(1,38) = 4.991, p = 0.031, ηp2 = 0.116]. This difference was increased for trials of fear compared to disgust, with this EMOTION effect being greater for electrode TP8 compared to P2 [t(39) = −3.255, p = 0.002] and P8 [t(39) = −5.664, p < 0.001; ELECTRODE: F(2,76) = 6.132, p = 0.004, ηp2 = 0.14; CONDITION*ELECTRODE: F(1.863,70.776) = 21.645, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.36; CONDITION*EMOTION*ELECTRODE: F(2,76) = 8.031, p = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.17]. For the N170, however, IQ was associated with this effect on its amplitude [F(1,38) = 4.488, p = 0.041]. After controlling for IQ, controls presented increased amplitude compared to patients, which was significant only for trials depicting fear during the visual condition and, according to post-hoc t-tests, was specific to electrode TP8 [t(38) = −2.171, p = 0.036].

The bimodal condition also produced a shorter N170 latency than the visual condition with this being significant only for the emotion of fear [CONDITION: F(1,38) = 5.2167, p = 0.029, ηp2 = 0.120; EMOTION: F(1,38) = 8.754, p = 0.005, ηp2 = 0.187; CONDITION*EMOTION: F(1,38) = 18.200, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.324]. There was an additional interaction of EMOTION*ELECTRODE (p = 0.004) leading to a CONDITION*EMOTION*ELECTRODE*GROUP (p = 0.040) interaction which will not be further interpreted as it was driven purely by electrodes.

Similar to the P100, the bimodal condition again produced a speed-up of the N170 latency which was further shortened by the emotion of fear. In line with our hypothesis, the bimodal condition also produced an increased N170 amplitude and this was amplified for the emotion of fear; after controlling for IQ this was significantly higher for controls compared to autistic individuals and specific to the emotion of fear.

3.2.3 Auditory N100

The bimodal condition produced an increased N100 amplitude compared to the auditory condition [F(1,38) = 3.980, p = 0.053, ηp2 = 0.095]. A CONDITION*EMOTION*GROUP interaction [F(1,38) = 5.217, p = 0.028, ηp2 = 0.121] further qualified this result. The difference between the bimodal and auditory conditions tended to be increased for trials of disgust rather than fear [EMOTION: F(1,38) = 3.980, p = 0.053, ηp2 = 0.095] and amplified for patients than controls. According to post-hoc ANOVAs (performed for each group separately), neither group presented an effect of CONDITION, EMOTION or a CONDITION*EMOTION interaction (all p = ns) indicating that this interaction was driven by groups presenting opposite CONDITION*EMOTION interactions.

3.2.4 Auditory P200

The P200 showed a decreased amplitude and a delayed latency during the bimodal compared to the auditory condition [CONDITION effects for amplitude: F(1,38) = 12.043, p = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.241; for latency: F(1,38) = 29.364, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.436; see Figure 3]. There was also an electrode effect [ELECTRODE: F(1.769,67.239) = 3.306, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.515]; planned contrasts revealed that, Cz was showing a larger amplitude than C1 (p < 0.001) and C2 (p < 0.001), and electrode C1 compared to C2 (p = 0.028). Controls presented again a trend for overall shorter latencies than patients; according to a one-way post-hoc ANOVA this difference was significant for electrode C2 [GROUP effect for C2: F(1,38)=5.468, p = 0.025; ELECTRODE*GROUP: F(1.681,63.886) = 3.360, p = 0.049, ηp2 = 0.081; see Figure 3].

Figure 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Auditory N100 & P200. Panel (A) shows the average waveform (C1, Cz, C2) of the auditory N100 and P200 components and the P200 topography at the bimodal and auditory conditions, for controls and ASD. The dashed box highlights the components. The P200 amplitude suppression of the component during the bimodal compared to the auditory condition was increased more for the ASD group than for controls who also showed an overall speeded P200 compared to the ASD group at electrode C2 (bottom row of left pane). Panel (B) shows the average waveform (C1, Cz, C2) of the auditory N100 for the CONDITION × EMOTION interaction and the topography at the bimodal and auditory conditions, for controls and ASD. The Y-axis of the waveform represents activity in μV and the X-axis time in ms. The color bar represents the range of activity in μV for interpretation of the topographical maps.

In summary, and contrary to our expectations, the auditory N100 was of higher amplitude during the bimodal condition which was increased for disgust—this was driven by the two groups showing opposing CONDITION*EMOTION interactions. The P200, on the other hand, showed the expected increased amplitude and delayed latency during the bimodal compared to the auditory condition.

3.2.5 Late positive component

There were no significant main effects between the bimodal and visual conditions on the LPC mean area activity. There was, however, a significant crossover interaction with controls showing increased amplitude for fear compared to disgust and patients showing increased amplitude for disgust [EMOTION*GROUP: F(1,38) = 5.154, p = 0.029, ηp2 = 0.119]. This crossover interaction was further amplified for the visual condition [CONDITION* EMOTION*GROUP: F(1,38) = 7.435, p = 0.010, ηp2 = 0.164], and for electrode Poz [EMOTION*GROUP*ELECTRODE: F(1,38) = 5.374, p = 0.026, ηp2 = 0.124; CONDITION*EMOTION*GROUP*ELECTRODE: F(1,38) = 4.987, p = 0.032, ηp2 = 0.116]. According to post-hoc ANOVAs, these interactions were not significant (all p = ns) except for the CONDITION*EMOTION interaction which was present only in patients where the emotion of disgust produced an overall larger LPC deflection and this difference was further amplified for the visual compared to the bimodal condition [F(1,14) = 6.646, p = 0.022, ηp2 = 0.322].

With regards to the auditory condition, the LPC deflection was significantly larger under the bimodal compared to the auditory condition [CONDITION: F(1,38) = 77.166, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.670]. Furthermore, fear also produced a significantly larger amplitude than disgust and this difference was further amplified for patients compared to controls [EMOTION: F(1,38) = 11.761, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.236; EMOTION*GROUP: F(1,38) = 8.467, p = 0.006, ηp2 = 0.182]. According to a post-hoc ANOVA (performed for each group separately), the increase of the LPC deflection for the emotion of fear was significant only for patients [F(1,14) = 15.062, p = 0.002, ηp2 = 0.518] and not for controls (p = 0.669). There was an additional effect of electrode site [F(1,38) = 30.611, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.446] and CONDITION*ELECTRODE interaction [F(1,38) = 27.856, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.423] which will not be further interpreted as it was driven purely by electrodes.

3.3 Spatio-temporal evolution of MSI

Analysis revealed significant differences between the bimodal and sum conditions for the control group, with the bimodal condition showing both super-additivity and sub-additivity (this varied with topography) suggesting that MSI process is not the linear addition of the two signals. These effects started at 80 ms post-stimulus onset, with a central topography evolving to a more centro-parietal topography up to 120 ms. This MSI effect shifted to a more fronto-central and temporal topography up to 160 ms, changing to widespread topographical distribution involving both parieto-occipital and fronto-central areas; this effect lasted until 300 ms post-stimulus onset. Patients also showed some MSI effects, but these began about 50 ms later than controls (i.e., at 130 ms). From 200 ms onwards, this cluster shifted to a more centro-parietal but narrow topographical distribution (see Table 3 for statistics).

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Cluster-based permutation test statistics.

When analysis was performed for each emotion separately, controls showed, for fear, MSI effects starting at 100 ms at frontal right-hemisphere areas, extending to central, frontal and bilateral temporal scalp areas. These effects lasted until 170 ms after stimulus onset and shifted to frontal and parieto-occipital areas until 300 ms post-stimulus. Notably, for the emotion of fear patients did not show any spatio-temporal effects (see Figure 4). For disgust relative to fear, controls showed a somewhat delayed MSI effect, starting at 130 ms at central and right-hemisphere temporal areas. From 160 ms onwards these effects extended to bilateral temporal, frontal and central scalp areas and evolved to occipital areas until 300 ms. Patients, this time, showed a similar but weaker MSI effect in terms of topographical distribution, starting as well at 130 ms at central scalp areas and including frontal electrodes from 160 ms onwards. This effect shifted from 230 ms to a topographically narrower central-parietal topography, reaching 300 ms (see Figure 4; Table 3).

Figure 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Spatio-temporal course of the MSI effect [Bimodal – (Audio + Visual)]. Topographies show the effect from 110 to 290 ms averaged over bins of 30 ms for (A) controls starting as early as 110 ms for fear (top row) and disgust (bottom row) from ~140 ms onwards. (B) Patients show no MSI effects for fear (top row) and weaker effects for disgust (bottom row). White asterisks highlight electrodes within clusters where the differences between the bimodal and sum (i.e., MSI) were significant.

In summary, these results confirm an early onset of MSI for controls with autistic individuals showing a delayed onset of MSI which, when analyzed for each emotion separately it was present only for the emotion of disgust.

4 Discussion

The present study set out to investigate the neural basis of MSI for emotion processing in autistic adolescents using highly ecologically valid stimuli (videos) expressing fear and disgust. We found the following main results. With regards to the behavioral data, we, firstly, observed that the bimodal condition produced speeded and more accurate responses compared to the auditory and visual conditions regardless of group. This facilitation of responses during the bimodal condition compared to the visual condition was amplified in the fear compared to disgust expression, while the speed-up seen in the bimodal compared to the auditory condition was amplified for disgust compared to fear. Secondly, autistic adolescents showed dampened or no MSI compared to controls, according to Miller’s RMI. In other words, although both groups showed a facilitation of RT, only in controls this was due to successful MSI.

With regards to the electrophysiological data, significant MSI effects were seen, firstly, in terms of shorter latencies of the visual P100 and N170 components and a delayed P200 latency and, secondly, in a suppressed visual P100 amplitude and an increased N170 amplitude. More importantly, MSI effects were seen in controls as early as 80 ms at several scalp areas with patients showing a delayed and spatially narrowed MSI effect, which was driven by the emotion of disgust only.

4.1 MSI effects and emotion differentiation

In accordance with previous studies (Collignon et al., 2008; Charbonneau et al., 2013) the bimodal presentation of the dynamic stimuli presenting fear and disgust produced significantly improved performance compared to the degraded unimodal conditions (Collignon et al., 2008; Stefanou et al., 2019). Regardless of group, responses to the bimodal condition were faster, more accurate and with decreased variance (SDRT) compared to the auditory and visual conditions. Additionally, fear also produced speeded and more accurate responses compared to disgust in the bimodal vs. visual contrast. Such a finding is in line with the dissociating effects of fear compared to disgust, with fear heightening attention and disgust dampening attention. In accordance with our previous study (Stefanou et al., 2019), the dissociating effects of fear and disgust extend to MSI with fear augmenting the speed-up of RT in the bimodal compared to the visual condition. This finding confirms that the attention-orienting effect of fear, due to its biological significance, does not occur only during unisensory processing but also during bimodal processing when voice and face convey the same emotion.

However, despite fear producing overall faster RT than disgust, the speed-up of responses in the bimodal condition, was amplified by the emotion of disgust. Although somewhat unexpected, such a finding could be related to the fact that in this case, the degraded channel in the bimodal condition was the auditory one, and not the visual one which is also considered to be the dominant channel (Colavita, 1974). This would be in accordance with the inverse effectiveness rule (Meredith and Stein, 1986; Stein et al., 1993); according to the inverse effectiveness rule first observed in animal studies, there is a stronger MSI benefit when one of the two signals of the bimodal stimulus is weak, while when one of the signals of the bimodal condition is strong, there is a reduced MSI. Findings following the inverse effectiveness rule have been reported in MSI studies with humans as well (Senkowski et al., 2011; Stefanou et al., 2019). Given that fear produced faster RT regardless of condition, the combination of these two augmenting effects could have possibly created a ceiling effect for the interaction of the redundant signal and fear.

MSI facilitation was further demonstrated by Miller’s RMI. Specifically, controls showed a significant MSI regardless of emotion, with fear facilitating a somewhat stronger MSI than disgust. Patients on the other hand showed MSI only for the emotion of fear, and not for disgust. These findings align with previously reported MSI deficits in autistic individuals, and further suggest that they can potentially compensate through the attentional mechanisms associated with fear. Despite the emotion of fear facilitating behavioral MSI in patients, this was decreased compared to controls. The above findings combined suggest that, regardless of emotion, ASD patients show behavioral MSI deficits replicating findings of previous studies (Brandwein et al., 2013; Charbonneau et al., 2013; Collignon et al., 2013). That patients show a behavioral MSI effect only for fear, according to Miller’s Model leads us to presume that any speed-up they showed in the conventional median RT analysis was caused by a race of the two signals in the bimodal condition and not by the integration of the audiovisual signals. Indeed, conventional RT analysis cannot definitively argue whether MSI has occurred or if the speed is a result of statistical facilitation, whereas Miller’s RMI provides an indication of an underlying coactivation mechanism (Colonius and Diederich, 2006), indicating thus more reliably whether MSI occurred or not. These findings also validate the dissociating effects that fear and disgust have on attention and by consequence to MSI, with the enhancing effects of fear on MSI being observed in the autistic group as well and suggesting it can facilitate MSI even in individuals with MSI deficits. Such a conclusion is reinforced by studies reporting that attention improves MSI (Magnée et al., 2011; Fernández et al., 2015) and that ASD patients are capable of MSI if they actively attend to stimuli (Russo et al., 2010) and potentially compensate for MSI deficits via later attentional processes (Stefanou et al., 2020), in this case by the attention orienting effect of fear (Susskind et al., 2008; Krusemark and Li, 2011).

4.2 MSI in electrophysiological responses

In line with previous studies (Brefczynski-Lewis et al., 2009; Jessen and Kotz, 2011), the visual P100 and N170 components peaked significantly earlier during the bimodal compared to the visual condition. Unexpectedly, ASD patients, regardless of condition, presented a speeded appearance of the P100 compared to controls. The speed-up of the bimodal N170 was increased only for fear, potentially due to the aforementioned attentional effects of fear and disgust (Susskind et al., 2008).

In contrast to our previous results and general findings of super-additivity both in EEG and fMRI studies (Pourtois et al., 2005; Brandwein et al., 2011; Stefanou et al., 2019; for a review, see Campanella and Belin, 2007), we found decreased amplitude of the visual P100 in the bimodal compared to the visual condition. Despite the visual P100 being linked to the facilitation of sensory processing for stimuli at attended locations (Luck et al., 1990; Mangun, 1995; Hillyard et al., 1998), this amplitude suppression does not necessarily imply decreased facilitation of sensory processing in the bimodal condition. Given that we compared the visual with the bimodal condition when the visual signal was degraded (see Methods), we cannot be certain how this degradation, in combination with the MSI effect, has manipulated the bimodal P100 amplitude. However, fear once more produced increased amplitude for the P100 compared to disgust corroborating previous statements of the diverging effects of fear and disgust.

We further report an overall MSI-related increase of the N170 amplitude which was greater for fear than disgust, in agreement with our previous results (Stefanou et al., 2019) but see also (Brefczynski-Lewis et al., 2009, reporting sub-additive effects of MSI, i.e., reduced amplitude on the N170). This N170 amplitude increase suggests that the different effects fear and disgust have on attention, with fear capturing attention (Susskind et al., 2008) enhanced the processing of the stimuli facilitating MSI. Although this increase of the N170 amplitude contradicts the decreased amplitude during the bimodal condition found by Brefczynski-Lewis et al. (2009), this could result from the fact that our stimuli depicted emotions of real persons whereas their stimuli were non-emotional portrays of synthetic faces. Additionally, fear, as compared to disgust, increases the negativity of the N170 (Batty and Taylor, 2003; Almeida et al., 2016) since it presumably elicits a unique response compared to other emotions. Thus, the fear-related increase of the N170 amplitude suggests greater facilitation of sensory processing of fear compared to disgust, for both uni- and multisensory processing.

With regards to the auditory components, the bimodal condition produced an increase of the N100 deflection, and the emotion of disgust further amplified this effect. The increase of the N100 amplitude is in contrast with other studies using ecologically valid stimuli (Stekelenburg and Vroomen, 2007; Jessen and Kotz, 2011) reporting a reduced N100 amplitude during bimodal conditions. However, post-hoc analyses revealed no modulation of the N100 amplitude, neither by condition nor emotions and no interaction of the two factors. This amplitude increase is, at least topographically, elicited by disgust in the ASD group and by fear in controls. If these are replicated by future studies, it could suggest that the attention-orienting and attention-diverting effects of fear and disgust operated inversely in patients. Such a finding is in line with reports of altered emotion perception in ASD (Stewart et al., 2013; Uljarevic and Hamilton, 2013; Globerson et al., 2015; Bestelmeyer et al., 2018). This finding further argues this deficit cannot be permeated by biologically significant events, at least not at its neurophysiological substrates. This differentiation between controls and patients in MSI processing can be seen by a delayed N100 latency in the bimodal condition, which is again driven by patients. Given that the auditory N100 is an initial orienting response (O’Connor, 2012) the delayed latencies observed in patients compared to controls point to decreased attention orienting, at least at initial processing stages, and an overall slower sensory processing regardless of any MSI-related effects.

As with our previous findings (Stefanou et al., 2020), we report a delayed P200 latency at the bimodal compared to the auditory condition. Given that improved performance and discriminability (Rif et al., 1991; Lijffijt et al., 2009) are linked to a delayed auditory P200, and since there was an overall P200 delay in ASD patients compared to controls, our results suggest greater attentional effort. This attentional effort of the ASD group, as indexed by the overall delayed P200 and a larger MSI-related P200 delay compared to controls, could possibly point to the recruitment of attentional mechanisms in order to compensate for auditory processing and MSI-related deficits. The bimodal condition also produced a suppression of the auditory P200 which could be a delayed integrative effect not seen in the auditory N100. Further support of a potential increased attentional effort, to compensate for early sensory deficits can be seen from the increased LPC amplitude that patients show compared to controls for the emotion of fear. Indeed, the literature suggests that the LPC is not only related to emotion processing but its amplitude additionally increases to biologically related stimuli such as threat (Hajcak et al., 2012) while previous studies have revealed increased LPC amplitudes for emotional stimuli with it being increased when these stimuli are targets (Schindler and Kissler, 2016).

4.3 Spatio-temporal evolution of MSI

The aforementioned MSI effects were confirmed by the cluster-based permutation tests, which contrasted the bimodal condition with to the sum of the two unimodal conditions. Early MSI effects were readily observable in controls, with a topographically widespread distribution, starting as early as 80 ms and lasting up to 300 ms post-stimulus onset. These findings are in line with our previous results (Stefanou et al., 2020) as well as with other studies (Russo et al., 2010; Brandwein et al., 2013) suggesting that, MSI in healthy individuals occurs before basic sensory processing does (e.g., P100). Furthermore, controls presented similar MSI effects for both the emotions of fear and disgust. Still, the MSI effects for fear started earlier than those for disgust corroborating that fear not only enhances attention due to its biological purpose (Susskind et al., 2008) but that its effects facilitate complex processes such as MSI.

By contrast, patients showed a delayed overall MSI effect compared to controls. This effect started at 130 ms and showed a restrained topographical distribution suggesting that autistic individuals in our study present altered MSI. Furthermore, patients with ASD showed MSI only for the emotion of disgust which presumably elicited the overall MSI effect.

The overall delayed MSI effects seen in patients compared to controls, suggests altered MSI for which they compensate at later processing stages possibly through attentional mechanisms (Koelewijn et al., 2010; Stefanou et al., 2020). The latency where we first observe this MSI effect (130 ms) in patients, along with the MSI-related P200 delay suggests an altered MSI in ASD during early processing stages, which is compensated for at later processing stages.

The present study has limitations that narrow the generalisability of our results. Firstly, the sample size was overall rather small reducing the statistical power of the study. Secondly, we degraded the unimodal conditions to account for unisensory dominance that would reduce any MSI facilitation (see Collignon et al., 2008; Stefanou et al., 2019). However, we cannot be certain how this may have affected the electrophysiological results, given that the components under investigation are exogenous components representing the facilitation of sensory processing. During the preliminary noise threshold task, patients differed compared to controls only for the visual noise threshold which may have also affected the results. However, the main purpose of this degradation was to ensure that all participants started the main experiment with 80% accuracy for the degraded unimodal conditions, suggesting that the reported differences between the bimodal and unimodal conditions are due to MSI differences and not due to noise sensitivity levels of each group. Thirdly, because of previous literature reporting altered MSI in ADHD and individuals with ADHD traits (see Panagiotidi et al., 2017) we did not recruit participants with comorbid ADHD. Therefore, our results would not be representative to the subgroups of autistic individuals with comorbid ADHD. Furthermore, although our age range is rather narrow, evidence from previous research reveals that key differences in MSI facilitation are identified between children before the age of 10 years old, and older than 11 years old (Brandwein et al., 2013). It has been further suggested that MSI reaches maturation levels by the age of 14 years old (Brandwein et al., 2011) suggesting that our age-range should in principle suffice to investigate MSI in the adolescence period.

However, to conclude, the results of the present study point to altered emotion recognition and altered MSI in ASD. Such a finding would be in line with theories of disrupted connectivity (for reviews, see Belmonte et al., 2004; Hughes, 2007; Hornix et al., 2019) as this altered long-range connectivity may underlie MSI deficits due to an insufficient synchronization between the involved areas (Martínez-Sanchis, 2014). We present evidence of altered MSI in ASD which, behaviorally, can be masked by biologically significant events such as fear via attention orienting. Such attention-driven compensatory mechanisms, as seen in patients, are reinforced by the electrophysiological data showing a similar compensatory mechanism in the form of attentional effort and not attentional orienting driven by fear.

Data availability statement

Datasets generated for this manuscript are available from the corresponding author upon request.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by the Ethics Committee of the Albert Ludwigs-University of Freiburg (ethics vote no. 238/15). The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation in this study was provided by the participants and their legal guardians/next of kin.

Author contributions

MES: Writing – review & editing, Investigation, Writing – original draft, Visualization, Software, Formal analysis, Data curation, Conceptualization. ND: Writing – review & editing, Software, Conceptualization. PB: Writing – review & editing. MB: Writing – review & editing. NS: Writing – review & editing, Conceptualization. CK: Writing – review & editing, Supervision, Conceptualization.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. MS acknowledges the support of the graduate scholarship “State Law on Graduate Funding (LGFG)” of Albert-Ludwigs-Universität Freiburg. This study is part of the doctoral dissertation of MS, “Deficits in Social Cognition in Autism Spectrum Disorders and their Electro-Cortical Correlates: A Multisensory Integration perspective.” We also acknowledge support by the Open Access Publication Fund of the University of Freiburg.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The author(s) declared that they were an editorial board member of Frontiers, at the time of submission. This had no impact on the peer review process and the final decision.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnins.2024.1390696/full#supplementary-material

References

Almeida, P. R., Ferreira-santos, F., Chaves, P. L., Paiva, T. O., Barbosa, F., and Marques-teixeira, J. (2016). Perceived arousal of facial expressions of emotion modulates the N170, regardless of emotional category: time domain and time – frequency dynamics. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 99, 48–56. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2015.11.017

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

American Electroencephalographic Society (1991). Guidelines for standard electrode position nomenclature. J. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2, 200–202.

Google Scholar

American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 5th Edn. Waschington, DC: American Psychiatric Assiociation.

Google Scholar

Batty, M., and Taylor, M. J. (2003). Early processing of the six basic facial emotional expressions. Cogn. Brain Res. 17, 613–620. doi: 10.1016/S0926-6410(03)00174-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bebko, J. M., Schroeder, J. H., and Weiss, J. A. (2014). The McGurk effect in children with autism and Asperger syndrome. Autism Res. 7, 50–59. doi: 10.1002/aur.1343

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Belmonte, M. K., Allen, G., Beckel-Mitchener, A., Boulanger, L. M., Carper, R. A., and Webb, S. J. (2004). Autism and abnormal development of brain connectivity. J. Neurosci. 24, 9228–9231. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3340-04.2004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bestelmeyer, P. E. G., Williams, B., Lawton, J. J., Stefanou, M. E., Koldewyn, K., Klein, C., et al. (2018). Adaptation to vocal expressions and phonemes is intact in autism Spectrum disorder. Clin. Psychol. Sci. 6, 372–381. doi: 10.1177/2167702617748401

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bölte, S., Rühl, D., Schmötzer, G., and Poustka, F. (2005). Diagnostisches Interview Für Autismus – Revidiert. Deutsche Fassung Des Autism Diagnostic Interview—Revised von Michael Rutter, Ann Le Couteur Und Catherine Lord. Huber.

Google Scholar

Brainard, D. H. (1997). The psychophysics toolbox. Spat. Vis. 10, 433–436. doi: 10.1163/156856897X00357

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brandwein, A. B., Foxe, J. J., Butler, J. S., Frey, H.-P., Bates, J. C., Shulman, L. H., et al. (2015). Neurophysiological indices of atypical auditory processing and multisensory integration are associated with symptom severity in autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 45, 230–244. doi: 10.1007/s10803-014-2212-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brandwein, A. B., Foxe, J. J., Butler, J. S., Russo, N. N., Altschuler, T. S., Gomes, H., et al. (2013). The development of multisensory integration in high-functioning autism: high-density electrical mapping and psychophysical measures reveal impairments in the processing of audiovisual inputs. Cereb. Cortex 23, 1329–1341. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs109

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brandwein, A. B., Foxe, J. J., Russo, N. N., Altschuler, T. S., Gomes, H., and Molholm, S. (2011). The development of audiovisual multisensory integration across childhood and early adolescence: a high-density electrical mapping study. Cereb. Cortex 21, 1042–1055. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq170

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brefczynski-Lewis, J., Lowitszch, S., Parsons, M., Lemieux, S., and Puce, A. (2009). Audiovisual non-verbal dynamic faces elicit converging FMRI and ERP responses. Brain Topogr. 21, 193–206. doi: 10.1007/s10548-009-0093-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brett-green, B. A., Miller, L. J., Gavin, W. J., and Davies, P. L. (2008). Multisensory integration in children: a preliminary ERP study. Brain Res. 1242, 283–290. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.03.090

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Campanella, S., and Belin, P. (2007). Integrating face and voice in person perception. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 535–543. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.10.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Charbonneau, G., Bertone, A., Lepore, F., Nassim, M., Lassonde, M., Mottron, L., et al. (2013). Multilevel alterations in the processing of audio–visual emotion expressions in autism Spectrum disorders. Neuropsychologia 51, 1002–1010. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.02.009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Colavita, F. B. (1974). Human sensory dominance. Percept. Psychophys. 16, 409–412. doi: 10.3758/BF03203962

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Collignon, O., Charbonneau, G., Peters, F., Nassim, M., Lassonde, M., Lepore, F., et al. (2013). Reduced multisensory facilitation in persons with autism. Cortex 49, 1704–1710. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.06.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Collignon, O., Girard, S., Gosselin, F., Roy, S., Saint-Amour, D., Lassonde, M., et al. (2008). Audio-visual integration of emotion expression. Brain Res. 1242, 126–135. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.04.023

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Colonius, H., and Diederich, A. (2006). The race model inequality: interpreting a geometric measure of the amount of violation. Psychol. Rev. 113, 148–154. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.113.1.148

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Constantino, J. N., and Gruber, C. P. (2005). Social responsiveness scale (SRS): Manual. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.

Google Scholar

Darwin, C. (1872). The expression of the emotions in man and animals. London: John Murray.

Google Scholar

Darwin, C. (2009). The expression of the emotions in man and animals. Cambridge Library Collection - Darwin, Evolution and Genetics. 2nd ed. F. Darwin (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press).

Google Scholar

De Boer-Schellekens, L., Keetels, M., Eussen, M., and Vroomen, J. (2013). No evidence for impaired multisensory integration of low-level audiovisual stimuli in adolescents and Young adults with autism Spectrum disorders. Neuropsychologia 51, 3004–3013. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.10.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dwyer, P., Takarae, Y., Zadeh, I., Rivera, S. M., and Saron, C. D. (2022). Multisensory integration and interactions across vision, hearing, and Somatosensation in autism Spectrum development and typical development. Neuropsychologia 175:108340. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2022.108340

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Eimer, M., and Holmes, A. (2003). The role of spatial attention in the processing of facial expression: an ERP study of rapid brain responses to six basic emotions. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 3, 97–110. doi: 10.3758/CABN.3.2.97

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernández, M., Luis, M. V., Ventura-Campos, N., Ávila, C., and Soto-Faraco, S. (2015). Top-down attention regulates the neural expression of audiovisual integration. Neuroimage 119, 272–285. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.06.052

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Foss-Feig, J. H., Kwakye, L. D., Cascio, C. J., Burnette, C. P., Kadivar, H., Stone, W. L., et al. (2010). An extended multisensory temporal binding window in autism Spectrum disorders. Exp. Brain Res. 203, 381–389. doi: 10.1007/s00221-010-2240-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Foxe, J. J., Molholm, S., Del Bene, V. A., Frey, H. P., Russo, N. N., Blanco, D., et al. (2015). Severe multisensory speech integration deficits in high-functioning school-aged children with autism Spectrum disorder (ASD) and their resolution during early adolescence. Cereb. Cortex 25, 298–312. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht213

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gervais, H., Belin, P., Boddaert, N., Leboyer, M., Coez, A., Sfaello, I., et al. (2004). Abnormal cortical voice processing in autism. Nat. Neurosci. 7, 801–802. doi: 10.1038/nn1291

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Giard, M. H., and Peronnet, F. (1999). Auditory-visual integration during multimodal object recognition in humans: a behavioral and electrophysiological study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 11, 473–490. doi: 10.1162/089892999563544

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Globerson, E., Amir, N., Kishon-Rabin, L., and Golan, O. (2015). Prosody recognition in adults with high-functioning autism Spectrum disorders: from psychoacoustics to cognition. Autism Res. 8, 153–163. doi: 10.1002/aur.1432

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gomot, M., Belmonte, M. K., Bullmore, E. T., Frédéric, A. B., and Simon, B.-C. (2008). Brain hyper-reactivity to auditory novel targets in children with high-functioning autism. Brain 131, 2479–2488. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn172

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hajcak, G., Weinberg, A., MacNamara, A., and Foti, D. (2012). The Oxford handbook of event-related potential components. eds. E. S. Kappenman and S. J. Luck (New York, Oxford: Oxford University Press). 442–472. doi: 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195374148.013.0222

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hartcher-O’Brien, J., Soto-Faraco, S., and Adam, R. (2017). Editorial: a matter of bottom-up or top-down processes: the role of attention in multisensory integration. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 11:5. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2017.00005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hillyard, S. A., Vogel, E. K., and Luck, S. J. (1998). Sensory gain control (amplification) as a mechanism of selective attention: electrophysiological and neuroimaging evidence. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 353, 1257–1270. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1998.0281

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hornix, B. E., Havekes, R., and Kas, M. J. H. (2019). Multisensory cortical processing and dysfunction across the neuropsychiatric Spectrum. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 97, 138–151. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.02.010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hughes, J. R. (2007). Autism: the first firm finding = Underconnectivity? Epilepsy Behav. 11, 20–24. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2007.03.010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Iarocci, G., Rombough, A., Yager, J., Weeks, D. J., and Chua, R. (2010). Visual influences on speech perception in children with autism. Autism 14, 305–320. doi: 10.1177/1362361309353615

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jessen, S., and Kotz, S. A. (2011). The temporal dynamics of processing emotions from vocal, facial, and bodily expressions. Neuroimage 58, 665–674. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.06.035

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Katagiri, M., Miya, K., and Matsui, M. (2014). Difficulty of Crossmodal processing in individuals with autism Spectrum disorders: an audio-visual gap/overlap paradigm study. Res. Autism Spectr. Disord. 8, 424–431. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2014.01.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kayser, C., and Logothetis, N. K. (2007). Do early sensory cortices integrate cross-modal information? Brain Struct. Funct. 212, 121–132. doi: 10.1007/s00429-007-0154-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Klein, C., Miczuga, T., Kost, M. S., Röring, H., Jarczok, T. A., Bast, N., et al. (2022). An German short-version of the ‘sensory perception quotient’ for adults with autism Spectrum disorder. Front. Psych. 13:409. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.781409

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kleiner, M., Brainard, D. H., and Pelli, D. G. (2007). What’s new in Psychtoobox-3? Perception 36, 1–16. doi: 10.1068/v070821

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Koelewijn, T., Bronkhorst, A., and Theeuwes, J. (2010). Attention and the multiple stages of multisensory integration: a review of audiovisual studies. Acta Psychol. 134, 372–384. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.03.010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Krusemark, E. A., and Li, W. (2011). Do all threats work the same way? Divergent effects of fear and disgust on sensory perception and attention. J. Neurosci. 31, 3429–3434. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4394-10.2011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kujala, T., Lepistö, T., and Näätänen, R. (2013). The neural basis of aberrant speech and audition in autism Spectrum disorders. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 37, 697–704. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.01.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kwakye, L. D., Foss-Feig, J. H., Cascio, C. J., Stone, W. L., and Wallace, M. T. (2011). Altered auditory and multisensory temporal processing in autism Spectrum disorders. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 4:129. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2010.00129

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lijffijt, M., Lane, S. D., Meier, S. L., Boutros, N. N., Burroughs, S., Steinberg, J. L., et al. (2009). P50, N100, and P200 sensory gating: relationships with behavioral inhibition, attention, and working memory. Psychophysiology 46, 1059–1068. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2009.00845.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Luck, S. J., Heinze, H. J., Mangun, G. R., and Hillyard, S. A. (1990). Visual event-related potentials index focused attention within bilateral stimulus arrays. II. Functional dissociation of P1 and N1 components. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 75, 528–542. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(90)90139-B

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Magnée, M. J. C. M., De Gelder, B., Van Engeland, H., and Kemner, C. (2008). Audiovisual speech integration in pervasive developmental disorder: evidence from event-related potentials. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry Allied Discip. 49, 995–1000. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01902.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Magnée, M. J. C. M., de Gelder, B., van Engeland, H., and Kemner, C. (2011). Multisensory integration and attention in autism Spectrum disorder: evidence from event-related potentials. PLoS One 6:e24196. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0024196

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Magnée, M. J. C. M., Oranje, B., van Engeland, H., Kahn, R. S., and Kemner, C. (2009). Cross-sensory gating in schizophrenia and autism Spectrum disorder: EEG evidence for impaired brain connectivity? Neuropsychologia 47, 1728–1732. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.02.012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mangun, G. R. (1995). Neural mechanisms of visual selective attention. Psychophysiology 32, 4–18. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1995.tb03400.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Maris, E. (2012). Statistical testing in electrophysiological studies. Psychophysiology 49, 549–565. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2011.01320.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Maris, E., and Oostenveld, R. (2007). Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG-data. J. Neurosci. Methods 164, 177–190. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2007.03.024

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Martínez-Sanchis, S. (2014). Neurobiological foundations of multisensory integration in people with autism Spectrum disorders: the role of the medial prefrontal cortex. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8, 1–6. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00970

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McCracken, H. S., Murphy, B. A., Glazebrook, C. M., Burkitt, J. J., Karellas, A. M., and Yielder, P. C. (2019). Audiovisual multisensory integration and evoked potentials in Young adults with and without attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 13:95. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00095

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McGurk, H., and MacDonald, J. (1976). Hearing lips and seeing voices. Nature 264, 746–748. doi: 10.1038/264746a0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Meredith, M., and Stein, B. (1983). Interactions among converging sensory inputs in the superior colliculus. Science 221, 389–391. doi: 10.1126/science.6867718

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Meredith, M. A., and Stein, B. E. (1986). Visual, auditory, and somatosensory convergence on cells in superior colliculus results in multisensory integration. J. Neurophysiol. 56, 640–662. doi: 10.1152/jn.1986.56.3.640

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, J. (1982). Divided attention: evidence for Coactivation with redundant signals. Cogn. Psychol. 14, 247–279. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(82)90010-X

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Molholm, S., Ritter, W., Murray, M. M., Javitt, D. C., Schroeder, C. E., and Foxe, J. J. (2002). Multisensory auditory-visual interactions during early sensory processing in humans: a high-density electrical mapping study. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 14, 115–128. doi: 10.1016/S0926-6410(02)00066-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mongillo, E. A., Irwin, J. R., Whalen, D. H., Klaiman, C., Carter, A. S., and Schultz, R. T. (2008). Audiovisual processing in children with and without autism Spectrum disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 38, 1349–1358. doi: 10.1007/s10803-007-0521-y

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Connor, K. (2012). Auditory processing in autism Spectrum disorder: a review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 36, 836–854. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.11.008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Panagiotidi, M., Overton, P. G., and Stafford, T. (2017). Multisensory integration and ADHD-like traits: evidence for an abnormal temporal integration window in ADHD. Acta Psychol. 181, 10–17. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2017.10.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pelli, D. G. (1997). The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. Spat. Vis. 10, 437–442. doi: 10.1163/156856897X00366

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pourtois, G., de Gelder, B., Bol, A., and Crommelinck, M. (2005). Perception of facial expressions and voices and of their combination in the human brain. Cortex 41, 49–59. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70177-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Poustka, L., Rühl, D., Feineis-Matthews, S., Poustka, F., Hartung, M., and Bölte, S. (2015). ADOS-2. Diagnostische Beobachtungsskala für Autistische Störungen - 2. Deutschsprachige Fassung der Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule. Mannheim: Huber.

Google Scholar

Rif, J., Hari, R., Hämäläinen, M. S., and Sams, M. (1991). Auditory attention affects two different areas in the human Supratemporal cortex. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 79, 464–472. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(91)90166-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rühl, D., Bölte, S., Feineis-Matthews, S., and Poustka, F. (2004). Diagnostische Beobachtungsskala Für Autistische Störungen (ADOS), vol. 32. Bern: Huber, 45–50.

Google Scholar

Russo, N., Foxe, J. J., Brandwein, A. B., Altschuler, T., Gomes, H., and Molholm, S. (2010). Multisensory processing in children with autism: high-density electrical mapping of auditory-somatosensory integration. Autism Res. 3, 253–267. doi: 10.1002/aur.152

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schindler, S., and Kissler, J. (2016). Selective visual attention to emotional words: early parallel frontal and visual activations followed by interactive effects in visual cortex. Hum. Brain Mapp. 37, 3575–3587. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23261

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Senkowski, D., Saint-Amour, D., Höfle, M., and Foxe, J. J. (2011). Multisensory interactions in early evoked brain activity follow the principle of inverse effectiveness. Neuroimage 56, 2200–2208. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.03.075

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Simon, D., Craig, K. D., Gosselin, F., Belin, P., and Rainville, P. (2008). Recognition and discrimination of prototypical dynamic expressions of pain and emotions. Pain 135, 55–64. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2007.05.008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Skuk, V. G., and Schweinberger, S. R. (2013). Adaptation aftereffects in vocal emotion perception elicited by expressive faces and voices. PLoS One 8:e81691. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0081691

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stefanou, M. E., Dundon, N. M., Bestelmeyer, P. E. G., Ioannou, C., Bender, S., Biscaldi, M., et al. (2020). Late attentional processes potentially compensate for early perceptual multisensory integration deficits in children with autism: evidence from evoked potentials. Sci. Rep. 10, 1–13. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-73022-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stefanou, M. E., Dundon, N., Bestelmeyer, P. E. G., Koldewyn, K., Saville, C. W. N., Fleischhaker, C., et al. (2019). Electro-cortical correlates of multisensory integration using ecologically valid emotional stimuli: differential effects for fear and disgust. Biol. Psychol. 142, 132–139. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2019.01.011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stein, B. E., Meredith, M. A., and Wallace, M. (1993). The visually responsive neuron and beyond: Multisensory integration in cat and monkey. Progress in Brain Research. 95, 79–90.

Google Scholar

Stein, B. E., and Stanford, T. R. (2008). Multisensory integration: current issues from the perspective of the single neuron. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 255–266. doi: 10.1038/nrn2331

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stein, B. E., Stanford, T. R., and Rowland, B. A. (2014). Development of multisensory integration from the perspective of the individual neuron. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 15, 520–535. doi: 10.1038/nrn3742

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stekelenburg, J. J., and Vroomen, J. (2007). Neural correlates of multisensory integration of ecologically valid audiovisual events. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 32:080219115128810. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.91213

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stevenson, R., Siemann, J. K., Woynaroski, T. G., Schneider, B. C., Eberly, H. E., Camarata, S. M., et al. (2014). Evidence for diminished multisensory integration in autism Spectrum disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 44, 3161–3167. doi: 10.1007/s10803-014-2179-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stewart, M. E., Mcadam, C., Ota, M., Peppé, S., and Cleland, J. (2013). Emotional recognition in autism Spectrum conditions from voices and faces. Autism 17, 6–14. doi: 10.1177/1362361311424572

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Susskind, J. M., Lee, D. H., Cusi, A., Feiman, R., Grabski, W., and Anderson, A. K. (2008). Expressing fear enhances sensory acquisition. Nat. Neurosci. 11, 843–850. doi: 10.1038/nn.2138

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Thye, M. D., Bednarz, H. M., Herringshaw, A. J., Sartin, E. B., and Kana, R. K. (2018). Developmental cognitive neuroscience the impact of atypical sensory processing on social impairments in autism Spectrum disorder. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 29, 151–167. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2017.04.010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Uljarevic, M., and Hamilton, A. (2013). Recognition of emotions in autism: a formal Meta-analysis. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 43, 1517–1526. doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1695-5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ulrich, R., Miller, J., and Schröter, H. (2007). Testing the race model inequality: an algorithm and computer programs. Behav. Res. Methods 39, 291–302. doi: 10.3758/BF03193160

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Walker-Andrews, A. S. (1998). Emotions and social development: infants’ recognition of emotions in others. Pediatrics 102, 1268–1271. doi: 10.1542/peds.102.SE1.1268

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Weiß, H. R., and Weiß, B. (2006). CFT 20-R. Grundintelligenztest Skala 2 – Revision. Göttingen: Hogrefe.

Google Scholar

World Health Organisation (1992). International statistical classification of diseases and related health problems, 10th revision (ICD-10). Geneva: WHO.

Google Scholar

Woynaroski, T. G., Kwakye, L. D., Foss-Feig, J. H., Stevenson, R. A., Stone, W. L., and Wallace, M. T. (2013). Multisensory speech perception in children with autism Spectrum disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 43, 2891–2902. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-1836-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Young, A. W., and Bruce, V. (2011). Understanding person perception. Br. J. Psychol. 102, 959–974. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.2011.02045.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: autism spectrum disorders, emotion perception, multisensory integration, Miller’s Race Model, EEG, dynamic stimuli

Citation: Stefanou ME, Dundon NM, Bestelmeyer PEG, Biscaldi M, Smyrnis N and Klein C (2024) The dissociating effects of fear and disgust on multisensory integration in autism: evidence from evoked potentials. Front. Neurosci. 18:1390696. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2024.1390696

Received: 23 February 2024; Accepted: 18 July 2024;
Published: 05 August 2024.

Edited by:

Zheng Wang, University of Florida, United States

Reviewed by:

Lauren Ethridge, University of Oklahoma, United States
Savio W. H. Wong, The Chinese University of Hong Kong, China

Copyright © 2024 Stefanou, Dundon, Bestelmeyer, Biscaldi, Smyrnis and Klein. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Christoph Klein, Y2hyaXN0b3BoLmtsZWluLmtqcEB1bmlrbGluaWstZnJlaWJ1cmcuZGU=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.