Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurosci., 16 July 2024
Sec. Auditory Cognitive Neuroscience

Alpha and theta oscillations on a visual strategic processing task in age-related hearing loss

  • 1Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, Illinois State University, Normal, IL, United States
  • 2Department of Speech and Hearing Science, University of Illinois Urbana-Champaign, Champaign, IL, United States

Introduction: Emerging evidence suggests changes in several cognitive control processes in individuals with age-related hearing loss (ARHL). However, value-directed strategic processing, which involves selectively processing salient information based on high value, has been relatively unexplored in ARHL. Our previous work has shown behavioral changes in strategic processing in individuals with ARHL. The current study examined event-related alpha and theta oscillations linked to a visual, value-directed strategic processing task in 19 individuals with mild untreated ARHL and 17 normal hearing controls of comparable age and education.

Methods: Five unique word lists were presented where words were assigned high- or low-value based on the letter case, and electroencephalography (EEG) data was recorded during task performance.

Results: The main effect of the group was observed in early time periods. Specifically, greater theta synchronization was seen in the ARHL group relative to the control group. Interaction between group and value was observed at later time points, with greater theta synchronization for high- versus low-value information in those with ARHL.

Discussion: Our findings provide evidence for oscillatory changes tied to a visual task of value-directed strategic processing in individuals with mild untreated ARHL. This points towards modality-independent neurophysiological changes in cognitive control in individuals with mild degrees of ARHL and adds to the rapidly growing literature on the cognitive consequences of ARHL.

1 Introduction

Age-related hearing loss (ARHL), or presbycusis, is a common condition involving gradual hearing loss due to aging (National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication, 2023). The increasing prevalence of ARHL globally is resulting in a major public health burden (Haile et al., 2021; World Health Organization, 2021). In the United States alone, a third of adults between 65 to 74 years and half over 75 years have ARHL (National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication, 2023). It is estimated that by 2030, ARHL will have an economic impact of 30 billion dollars (Stucky et al., 2010). Research in the past decade has shown that not only do individuals with ARHL experience typical auditory processing challenges, such as recognition of speech-in-noise (SiN), but they also experience non-auditory cognitive challenges beyond those seen with typical aging (CHABA, 1988; Lin, 2011; Humes, 2013; Powell et al., 2022; Shende and Mudar, 2023). Emerging work, including studies conducted by our group, has shown cognitive alterations even in those with mild ARHL in cognitive control assessed using visual modality (Shende et al., 2021, 2022).

Cognitive control refers to various mental operations that allow prioritization of information to accomplish target goals (Diamond, 2013; Mackie et al., 2013). An important cognitive control process is “value-directed strategic processing,” which refers to the preferential processing of salient information of higher value (Castel, 2007; Nguyen et al., 2019). This process is critical for efficient information processing in all sensory modalities. For instance, we strategically attend to warning signs related to hazardous conditions when driving on a freeway in a snowstorm compared to general information signs. In laboratory settings, value-directed strategic processing has been typically examined using visual word-list learning tasks (Castel, 2007; Castel et al., 2009; Murphy et al., 2024). In these tasks, words are paired with different point values ranging from low- to high-, such as 1–16 (1: low-value; 16: high-value), and participants are asked to recall as many words as they can with the goal of scoring maximum points. Typically, participants who perform well on these tasks recall more words of higher value, suggesting preferential value-based processing of information (Castel et al., 2007; Nguyen et al., 2019, 2020; Schwartz et al., 2023). It is important to note that the pairing of words with point values distinguishes strategic processing tasks from episodic learning tasks that use a list learning paradigm to measure recall irrespective of value as a measure of episodic learning (Castel et al., 2011; Elliott et al., 2020).

Value-directed strategic processing subsumes other attentional processes, including selective attention (Anderson, 2013). In the context of ARHL, selective attention has been long examined (Barr and Giambra, 1990; Craik, 2007; Shinn-Cunningham and Best, 2008; Van Gerven and Guerreiro, 2016; Dai et al., 2018), especially in the context of speech recognition (Humes and Dubno, 2010; Nuesse et al., 2018; Fuglsang et al., 2020; Shechter Shvartzman et al., 2022). On various tasks, such as coordinate response measure (Humes et al., 2006, 2017), attentive matrices (Bonmassar et al., 2022), n- back with distracting stimuli (Guerreiro and Van Gerven, 2017), Stroop (Guerreiro and Van Gerven, 2017; Gillingham et al., 2018; Ren et al., 2019; Huber et al., 2020; Chavant and Kapoula, 2022), and dichotic listening tasks (Shinn-Cunningham and Best, 2008; Torrente et al., 2022), studies have found alterations in selective attention in older adults with ARHL relative to younger and older adults with normal hearing. These findings suggest that changes in individuals with ARHL go beyond typical age-related declines in attention. Considering that these alterations have been found even on visual tasks, these findings suggest a more modality-general change in attentional processing in this population.

In contrast to the large evidence body on selective attention changes, value-directed strategic processing has remained largely unexplored in ARHL, with the exception of a study by our group (Shende et al., 2022). In our study, we examined behavioral changes in value-directed strategic processing in individuals with mild untreated ARHL relative to normal hearing (NH) controls with comparable age and education. We used an in-house developed value-directed list learning task where words were paired with binary values (10: high-value; 1: low-value) differentiated by letter case (Upper vs. Lower Case). See (Nguyen et al., 2019) for details of task development. Our results revealed that our untreated mild ARHL group recalled fewer high-value words relative to the NH controls, suggesting behavioral alterations in value-directed strategic processing beyond typical age-related cognitive control changes. Given that we used a visual task, these behavioral changes in individuals with ARHL offered preliminary evidence for modality-general alterations in value-directed strategic processing. Whether there are underlying neural alterations linked to these behavioral changes as one would expect based on theoretical postulations, such as information degradation (CHABA, 1988; Schneider and Pichora-Fuller, 2000), sensory deprivation (Lindenberger and Baltes, 1994; Baltes and Lindenberger, 1997), and common cause hypotheses (CHABA, 1988; Lindenberger and Baltes, 1994; Baltes and Lindenberger, 1997), remains unknown.

Event-related encephalography (EEG) is a useful technique to capture real-time temporal unfolding of value-directed strategic processing from the neurophysiological standpoint since it taps into neural activity time-locked to specific events (Sur and Sinha, 2009). Particularly measures linked to event-related spectral perturbations (ERSPs) capture both phase-locked and non-phase-locked spectral activity in the EEG signal (Makeig, 1993; Makeig et al., 2004). Using a value-directed strategic processing task, our group examined ERSPs in the theta and alpha bands in both cognitively healthy controls (Nguyen et al., 2019, 2020) and older adults with mild cognitive impairment (Nguyen et al., 2022). Overall, we found oscillatory activity in theta and alpha bands linked to strategic processing. Specifically, we found greater theta synchronization (more positive power relative to baseline) during processing of low-value relative to high-value words in frontal electrodes (Nguyen et al., 2019, 2020, 2022). Extant literature suggests that frontal theta activity is linked to cognitive control in general [(see Cavanagh and Frank, 2014) for a review] and is typically recorded at Fz and FPz electrodes [(see Mitchell et al., 2008)]. In particular, studies have linked frontal theta to general processing (Eschmann et al., 2020), working memory (Itthipuripat et al., 2013), and inhibitory control (Nigbur et al., 2011; Cavanagh and Frank, 2014), all of which are integral to value-directed strategic processing. We also found greater alpha desynchronization (more negative power relative to baseline) during processing of high- relative to low-value words in centroparietal and parietal electrodes in our study on value-directed strategic processing (Nguyen et al., 2019, 2020). Numerous studies have suggested that alpha desynchronization reflects information processing (Benedek et al., 2014; Fumuro et al., 2015) and selective attention (Klimesch et al., 1997, 2007).

In the context of ARHL, there is an emerging body of work on ERSPs (Petersen et al., 2015; Price et al., 2019) in relation to SiN recognition (Price et al., 2019) and auditory working memory tasks (Petersen et al., 2015). Findings suggest changes in alpha band power in those with ARHL relative to controls, specifically increased alpha power with mild and moderate degrees of hearing loss (Petersen et al., 2015). Studies have also found association between listening in challenging/noisy environments and parietal alpha power (Dimitrijevic et al., 2019; Wisniewski et al., 2021). In cochlear implant users, lower alpha desynchronization has been associated with increased listening effort (Dimitrijevic et al., 2019). In comparison to alpha oscillation, examination of theta in ARHL has received very little attention with some exceptions (Griffiths et al., 2020). To the best of our knowledge, none have examined theta and alpha oscillations linked to cognitively demanding tasks, such as value-directed strategic processing tasks, in this population.

The goal of the current study was to examine differences in theta and alpha power in individuals with unaided mild ARHL relative to NH controls of comparable age and education using our visual value-directed strategic processing task. Guided by the literature, including emerging work on neural oscillations in ARHL, and our previous studies on value-directed strategic attention in cognitively healthy and impaired populations, we hypothesized that the mild ARHL group would show lower theta synchronization and alpha desynchronization relative to the control group. To clarify, given the lack of studies on theta ERSPs in ARHL, our hypothesis of lower theta synchronization in the ARHL relative to the control group was guided by findings of our previous study (Nguyen et al., 2022), as well as expectation of poorer cognitive control in ARHL given that studies have shown poorer performance on cognitive control tasks in those with ARHL relative to NH controls (Lin, 2011; Powell et al., 2022; Shende and Mudar, 2023). We also hypothesized greater theta synchronization for low- versus high-value words and greater alpha desynchronization for high- versus low-value words. It is important to mention that we focused on individuals with unaided mild ARHL to understand whether mild alterations in hearing, which typically go untreated, impact neural activity linked to strategic processing. Furthermore, we intentionally chose to use a visual paradigm to minimize any confounds related to unaided hearing loss, and to ascertain whether neural changes, if observed, are modality-general in those with ARHL.

2 Materials and methods

2.1 Participants

19 older adults with untreated bilateral mild ARHL (11 female; mean age: 71.53 ± 8.04 years; mean education: 17.58 ± 3.53 years), and 17 NH controls (12 female; mean age: 67.76 ± 4.92 years; mean education: 18.06 ± 1.56 years) of comparable age and education participated in the study. All were right-handed and native English speakers without any history of communication disorders, learning disabilities, neurological disorders, traumatic brain injury, psychiatric disorders, or uncorrected visual impairment. Those with a history of substance abuse, use of psychoactive medications, other known etiologies of hearing loss such as noise-induced, injury-related, or ototoxicity, or other hearing-related issues such as unilateral and/or bilateral continuous tinnitus, conductive or mixed hearing loss, and those with hearing aid use were excluded. All participants signed a written informed consent in accordance with protocols approved by the Institutional Review Board of the University of Illinois Urbana-Champaign (protocol # 17067) before completing the study. Demographic information for both groups is reported in Table 1.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Participant demographics.

2.2 Tasks and procedures

2.2.1 Baseline

All participants were asked if they had any vision-related difficulties. Those who reported issues despite having corrected vision were excluded since we used a visual task in this study to examine value-directed strategic processing. Additionally, all study participants underwent cognitive and mental health screenings. A global cognitive screener, the Montreal Cognitive Assessment Scale (MoCA) (Nasreddine et al., 2005), was used for cognitive screenings (MoCA scores, ARHL group: 26.95 [2.09]; NH group: 28.24 [1.20]), and the Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS) (Almeida and Almeida, 1999) was used to conduct the mental health screenings (GDS scores, ARHL group: 0.78 [1.03]; NH group: 0.29 [0.58]).

2.2.2 Audiological evaluation

A comprehensive audiological examination was conducted for all participants. Otoscopic evaluation was conducted to visually inspect for external ear and tympanic membrane pathologies/disorders; participants with these issues were excluded. Tympanometry and acoustic reflexes were examined to ensure normal middle ear function and to rule out reflex pathway abnormalities. Pure-tone audiometry was performed to obtain hearing thresholds, with air conduction thresholds determined from 0.25 to 8 kHz in decibel hearing level (dB HL) using the modified Hughson-Westlake method (Carhart and Jerger, 1959). Insert earphones were used as transducers with the Equinox 2.0 audiometer, which was calibrated to the American National Standards Institute S3.6 2010 standards. We calculated pure-tone average (PTA) using an average of air conduction thresholds at 0.5, 1, 2, and 4 kHz in each ear and operationally defined NH as ≤25 dB HL PTA in the better ear and hearing loss as >25 dB HL PTA in the better ear (Lin, 2011).

Speech audiometry was conducted to determine the participant’s acuity to speech sounds. Evaluation included obtaining Speech Reception Thresholds (SRTs) using spondee words and Word Recognition Score (WRS) using Northwestern University list 6 (NU-6) (Tillman and Carhart, 1966) in each ear. Additionally, we examined participants’ ability to recognize SiN using the Quick Speech-in-Noise test (QuickSIN) (Killion et al., 2004) in each ear separately (monoaural condition) and both ears simultaneously (binaural condition). The task required participants to repeat back sentences presented against multi-talker babble at signal-to-noise ratios (SNRs) that varied from +25 dB to 0 dB in 5 dB steps. QuickSiN scores of SNR loss were recorded, with higher scores suggesting worse SiN recognition. Most comfortable and uncomfortable listening levels were obtained to ensure the audibility of speech stimuli for each participant. QuickSiN sentences were presented at 70 dB HL as recommended or at the participant’s most comfortable level if 70 dB HL presentation level was uncomfortable. All audiometric testing was performed in a sound-treated booth in a two-room setup. Group means for all audiological measures are reported in Table 2. Audiograms for NH and ARHL are shown in Figure 1.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Group means for audiological measures.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Audiograms for ARHL and NH groups. Average of air conduction hearing thresholds for both ears across frequencies in ARHL and NH groups. Error bars represent standard errors. ARHL, age-related hearing loss; NH, normal hearing.

2.2.3 Strategic processing

Following the audiological evaluation, participants completed a value-directed strategic processing task, which is a variation of a word list learning task (Nguyen et al., 2019, 2020). Stimuli consisted of 200 single-syllable four-letter nouns controlled for imageability, concreteness, frequency, and familiarity, divided into five lists of 40 words each, with each list consisting of a different set of words. In each list, half of the words (n = 20) were assigned to the “high-value” condition (worth 10 points), and half (n = 20) were assigned to the “low-value” condition (worth 1 point). To differentiate the value of the words, letter case was used, wherein all letters of the word were written in either lowercase (e.g., shoe) or uppercase (e.g., SHOE) letters. The font size was controlled so that lowercase and uppercase letters all appeared the same size on the computer screen. The word order was pseudorandomized for each list. See (Nguyen et al., 2019) for additional details related to the task. We developed four versions of the task, which were counterbalanced for word value and letter case, such that two versions had high-value words presented in lowercase letters and low-value words presented in uppercase letters, and two versions had high-value words presented in uppercase letters and low-value words presented in lowercase letters. Random assignments of the versions were done across participants.

Participants saw the following instructions on a computer screen: “You will see words appear on the screen one at a time. Some words are in uppercase, and some words are in lowercase. The uppercase words [or lowercase words] are worth 10 points each (high-value words). The lowercase words [or uppercase words] are worth 1 point each (low-value words). At the end of the list you will see the word “REMEMBER” on the screen. Your task is to remember as many of the words from the list as possible with the goal of scoring the maximum number of points. This is similar to a game in which words are worth different amounts of money.” Task understanding was ensured, including the point values for the uppercase and lowercase words depending on the assigned version. It is critical to note that no specific instructions were provided to the participants on how to be strategic, for instance, by only focusing on the high-value words.

After the task instructions, the word “Ready” appeared in the center of the computer screen for 3 s, subsequent to which a fixation (+) was presented for 3 s. Then, the 40 words (in each list) were displayed one-at-a-time in the center of the computer screen for a duration of 1900 ms each with an inter-stimulus interval of 100 ms (blank screen). The word “REMEMBER” appeared at the end of each list and remained on the screen for 60 s, during which participants recalled as many words as they could, with their responses recorded on a score sheet by the experimenter (see Figure 2 for task schematic). Each participant received immediate feedback about their recall score after each list and before the presentation of the next list.

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Strategic processing task schematic. Lowercase or uppercase words served as high- or low-value words depending on task version. When the word “REMEMBER” was presented, participants verbally recalled words from that list. Responses were recorded on paper and scored for each of the five lists.

2.2.4 EEG data collection and pre-processing

Continuous EEG was recorded while participants performed the strategic processing task, using a 64-electrode elastic cap (Neuroscan Quikcap) with the Neuroscan SynAmpsRT amplifier and Scan v4.5 software (sampling rate: 1 kHz, bandpass filter: DC-200 Hz). Impedances for all electrodes were kept below 10kΩ. The reference electrode was located in a midline position between Cz and CPz, and the vertical electrooculogram was recorded at sites below and above the left eye. Raw EEG data from all five word lists (obtained during a single recording session) were appended together to have 100 trials per condition (i.e., high-value and low-value words), and data were processed offline. Poorly functioning electrodes were identified by high impedance values as well as visual inspection and were excluded from analyses (average number of electrodes excluded: 0.73 in ARHL group, 1.23 in the NH group; no group differences noted, F [1,34] = 3.33, p = 0.077). Eye blinks were corrected using spatial filtering in Neuroscan. The data were epoched from 500 ms before stimulus onset to 1,500 ms after stimulus offset. Epochs with peak signal amplitudes of ±75 μV were rejected (rejection rates: 17.84% for high-value in ARHL group, 16.58% in NH group, F [1,34] = 0.094, p = 0.761; 18.26% for low-value in ARHL group, 16.58% in NH group, F [1,34] = 0.094, p = 0.761). EEG data were then re-referenced to the average potential over the entire scalp.

2.2.5 ERSP analysis

ERSPs were analyzed from 0 to 1,000 ms (post-stimulus onset) using a non-overlapping baseline of −400 to −100 ms (pre-stimulus onset). EEGLAB toolbox (Version 14.1.1b) (Delorme and Makeig, 2004) running under Matlab 2016b (MathWorks, Natick, MA, United States) was used to analyze the data. Time-frequency decomposition was conducted using short-time Fourier transform with Hanning window tapering as implemented in the EEGLAB function newtimef.m (Delorme and Makeig, 2004). Time-frequency data were obtained using a 256-ms sliding window with a step-size of 10 ms and a pad ratio of 4 resulting in a frequency resolution of approximately 1 Hz. Baseline correction was done in accordance with a gain model (Delorme and Makeig, 2004; Grandchamp and Delorme, 2011) where each time-frequency time point was divided by the average pre-stimulus baseline power from −400 to −100 ms relative to stimulus onset at the same frequency.

2.2.6 ERSP power estimation

We estimated mean power in the theta band (4–8 Hz) at prefrontal (FP1, FPz, FP2) and frontal (F1, Fz, F2) electrode clusters; and in the alpha band (8–12 Hz) at centroparietal (CP1, CPz, CP2) and parietal (P1, Pz, P2) electrode clusters. Figure 3 illustrates electrode clusters used for analysis. The selection of these electrode sites was guided by our previous work, as well as the literature, which shows greater prominence of theta band at frontal sites and alpha band at parietal sites (Pfurtscheller and Lopes da Silva, 1999; Kawasaki et al., 2010; Cavanagh and Frank, 2014; Nguyen et al., 2019, 2020, 2022). Given that this is the first study examining ERSP correlates of value-directed strategic processing in ARHL, we decided to explore additional electrode sites, specifically prefrontal and centroparietal electrodes (Mitchell et al., 2008; Nguyen et al., 2022). Change in power will be described as synchronization or desynchronization, depending on an increase or decrease in the power, respectively, relative to baseline. Mean power was computed for high- and low-value conditions in 100 ms time windows from 0 ms to 1,000 ms, resulting in 10 time windows for analysis.

Figure 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Electrode clusters used for Event-Related Spectral Perturbations (ERSP) power estimation. Solid boxes represent electrode clusters for theta analyses and dotted boxes represent clusters for alpha analyses.

2.3 Statistical analysis

All data were analyzed using IBM SPSS Statistics (Version 26). For the audiological measures, General Linear Models (GLMs) were used, with group (ARHL/NH) as a between-subject variable and the audiological measures as within-subject variables. For the strategic processing task, behavioral data combined across all five lists (total number of words recalled) were examined using a GLM, with group (ARHL/NH) as a between-subject factor and value (high−/low-) as a within-subject factor. We also examined group differences in total points earned.

ERSP data were examined using separate GLMs for theta and alpha mean power for each of the 10 time windows (100 ms time windows between 0 and 1,000 ms post-stimulus). For this, group (ARHL/NH) was used as a between-subject factor, and value (high−/low-) as a within-subject factor. The Bonferroni method was used to correct for multiple comparisons with a threshold of p ≤ 0.05. The reported p-values, where not specified otherwise, are derived from F- and t- statistics.

3 Results

3.1 Task-related behavioral data

Behavioral data showed a main effect of group for the total number of words recalled, F(1,34) = 5.639, p = 0.023, with the ARHL group recalling fewer total words than the NH group (total # words recalled: ARHL group = 29.15 [7.15]; NH group = 35.88 [9.76]). A significant main effect of value was also observed, F(1,34) = 117.288, p < 0.001, with more high-value words recalled (ARHL group = 23.68 [7.79]; NH group = 30.65 [10.71]) than low-value words (ARHL group = 5.47 [4.53]; NH group = 5.24 [4.96]). No other effects were significant when examining the total number of words. Significant group differences were observed for total points recalled, F(1,34) = 5.219, p = 0.029, with the ARHL group scoring lower total points than the NH group (ARHL group = 242.31 [76.13]; NH group = 311.70 [105.21]). See Figure 4.

Figure 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Boxplots depicting behavioral data. The figure on the left depicts boxplots for number of words recalled and figure on the right depicts boxplots for total points earned. Horizontal lines represent median power, with boxes representing the interquartile range and whiskers extending to minimum and maximum values. ARHL, age-related hearing loss, NH, normal hearing. *p ≤ 0.05.

3.2 Theta band (4–8 Hz) mean power

ERSP data showed a main effect of group between 100 and 200 ms post-stimulus onset in the prefrontal electrode cluster, with greater theta synchronization in the ARHL relative to NH group (p = 0.046; Table 3; Supplementary Figure S1). A significant main effect of value was observed at the frontal electrode cluster, between 200–300 ms, and 700–1000 ms post-stimulus onset (p < 0.05), with greater theta synchronization for low- than high-value words (Table 4; Supplementary Figure S2). Significant interaction effects between group and value were observed between 400 and 500 ms post-stimulus onset in the prefrontal electrode cluster (p = 0.011; Table 5; Figure 5). Also see Supplementary Figure S3 for the depiction of individual variability for this significant group by value interaction effect. While the post-hoc analyses did not reveal any significant between-group differences (p > 0.05), within-group differences between high- and low-value words were observed in the ARHL group (p = 0.019) with greater theta synchronization for high- compared to low-value words. No such differences were observed within the NH group. Results for ERSP data, including theta findings, are reported in Tables 35.

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Statistical results for main effects of group for theta and alpha band mean power.

Table 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Statistical results for main effects of value for theta and alpha band mean power.

Table 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Statistical results for the group by value interactions for theta and alpha band mean power.

Figure 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. ERSP comparisons for theta band at prefrontal electrode cluster for interaction effects between group and value. Spectrograms illustrate differences between groups (ARHL/NH) and value (high-low-value) for theta band (4 to 8 Hz) at the prefrontal electrode cluster. The 0 ms time point (solid vertical line) represents stimulus onset. Dashed black rectangles indicate the time windows in which significant interaction effects between group and value were observed (also see Table 5). ARHL, age- related hearing loss; NH, normal hearing.

3.3 Alpha band (8–12 Hz) mean power

A significant main effect of value was observed between 400 and 1,000 ms at the centroparietal electrode cluster (Supplementary Figure S4), and between 200–300 ms and 600–1,000 ms at the parietal cluster, with greater alpha desynchronization for the high- compared to low-value words (Table 4; Supplementary Figure S5). A significant interaction between group and value was observed between 700 and 800 ms post-stimulus onset at the centroparietal electrode cluster (p = 0.040; Table 5; Figure 6; Also see Supplementary Figure S6 for the depiction of individual variability for this significant group by value interaction effect). Post-hoc analyses did not reveal any significant differences between groups, but there were significant within-group differences between high- and low-value words. These differences were seen in both ARHL and NH groups, with greater alpha desynchronization observed for high- compared to low-value words (p < 0.001). Alpha findings are reported in Tables 35. All significant ERSP findings in the time course are depicted in Figure 7.

Figure 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. ERSP comparisons for alpha band at centroparietal electrode cluster for interaction effects between group and value. Spectrograms illustrate differences between groups (ARHL/NH) and value (high−/low-value) for alpha band (8–12 Hz) at the centroparietal electrode cluster. The 0 ms time point (solid vertical line) represents stimulus onset. Dashed black rectangles indicate the time windows in which significant interaction effects between group and value were observed (also see Table 5). ARHL, age-related hearing loss; NH, normal hearing.

Figure 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Schematic of main findings. Top part of the figure represents theta findings; bottom part shows alpha findings. Orange dotted box shows findings of main effect of group; red solid boxes show findings of main effect of value; blue dashed boxes show findings of interaction effect between group and value.

4 Discussion

The current study examined ERSPs corresponding to a visual value-directed strategic processing task in older adults with ARHL relative to NH controls of similar age and education. Behaviorally, the ARHL group recalled fewer total words across all lists combined than the NH group. On EEG measures, two major findings related to group differences emerged: (1) the ARHL group had greater theta synchronization (100–200 ms) relative to the NH group during early processing periods; and (2) furthermore, the ARHL group had differences in theta synchronization (400–500 ms) for high- compared to low-value words, unlike the NH group. There were also some similarities observed in the two groups. Both groups showed greater alpha desynchronization for high- compared to low-value words (700–800 ms) although the magnitude of these differences was slightly more in the NH group. Also, both groups showed value-based differences in theta (200–300 ms; 700–1,000 ms) and alpha (600–1,000 ms).

Task-related behavioral data combined across all lists showed that the mild ARHL group recalled significantly fewer total number of words relative to the NH controls. These results could be attributed to just differences in episodic list learning between the groups. A few others have also found poorer episodic memory/list learning in individuals with ARHL relative to normal hearing controls, including on visual tasks of episodic memory (Rönnberg et al., 2011; Brewster and Rutherford, 2021; Tarawneh et al., 2022). Studies have shown that in ARHL, the brain undergoes functional reorganization (Campbell and Sharma, 2013; Campbell and Sharma, 2014; Lin et al., 2014; Armstrong et al., 2019), which can negatively affect the ability to retain information in memory (Loughrey et al., 2019). This is known as maladaptive plasticity, which has been found even in individuals with milder ARHL (Campbell and Sharma, 2014; Sharma and Glick, 2016; Slade et al., 2020). Changes in strategic processing likely contributed to the difficulties in list learning in individuals with ARHL relative to controls. Significant group difference in total points earned, with the ARHL group scoring lower total points than the NH controls, supports this speculation. To encourage strategic processing and learning of high-value words, participants were instructed to remember as many words as possible at the end of each list, with the goal of scoring a maximum number of points. However, participants were not given explicit instructions to focus on only high-value words since we wanted to examine inherent strategic processing ability. Our findings suggest that older adults with mild ARHL have compromised strategic processing compared to NH controls when point-values earned is used as a metric.

Task-related ERSP data revealed differences in neural processing between ARHL and NH groups in early time periods. Contrary to our hypothesis, we observed greater theta synchronization between 100 and 200 ms post-stimulus onset in the prefrontal electrode cluster in the ARHL group compared to NH controls. Theta synchronization in frontal regions has been linked to proactive and reactive control (Cooper et al., 2015; Messel et al., 2021; Pscherer et al., 2023). Proactive control allows one to adapt thoughts and behavior in anticipation of task goals, including any interference, whereas reactive control is a correction mechanism that involves detection and resolution of interference after its onset (Braver, 2012; Stuphorn and Emeric, 2012; Botvinick and Braver, 2015). Perhaps higher theta synchronization in the ARHL group relative to controls was related to the engagement of additional neural resources to support proactive control of task performance. This compensatory mechanism in the ARHL group to better support overall processing related to the task was still not sufficient to match their behavioral performance to NH controls, since they recalled fewer total number of words and earned lower total points. We have to be cautious in linking our ERSP findings to behavioral data given that behavioral data in our value-directed list learning paradigm includes cognitive processes beyond what is linked to strategic processing captured by the ERSP findings. In general, word recall on list learning tasks involves initial processing, learning/encoding, and subsequent retrieval. While our instructions promoted strategic processing, when EEG data were collected, participants were not asked to provide any overt response after each word in the list was presented. The task was designed this way intentionally to examine real-time strategic processing with minimal confounds from other cognitive processes (e.g., retrieval) or motoric response from a button press. We elicited recall at the end of each list to demonstrate that the behavioral results are consistent with value-directed strategic processing and learning behavioral literature (more high-value words recalled compared to low-value words). While behavioral data in terms of recall at the end of the list provides some insight into strategic processing, given that such recall involves cognitive processes related to word encoding and retrieval, not captured by the ERSPs, these findings need further validation. Future studies should consider how this task could be modified to collect ERSP data that can reveal possible associations between top-down cognitive processes of strategic processing and subsequent behavioral recall of high and low-value words to verify these findings. Higher theta synchronization in the ARHL relative to the control group between 100–200 ms could also point toward alterations in bottom-up sensory processing. Studies examining electrophysiological activity involving visual tasks have associated processing within 200 ms post-stimulus onset to visual awareness (Koivisto and Grassini, 2016) and sensory processing (Loughrey et al., 2023; Luck, 2023). Higher theta synchronization in the ARHL group relative to controls could be related to hyperexcitability due to cross-modal sensory enhancement. This aligns with evidence from electrophysiological studies (Puschmann et al., 2014; Slade et al., 2020; Madashetty et al., 2024), including a recent study that examined neural correlates of visual working memory tasks in older adults with ARHL (Loughrey et al., 2023). Our theta finding in the ARHL group is consistent with a few other studies (Petersen et al., 2015; Griffiths et al., 2020) and furthermore adds to the rapidly emerging body of work showing that sensory and cognitive processing interact more than has been previously acknowledged (Rönnberg et al., 2022a,b).

Group differences in information processing based on value (interaction between group and value) were observed at later time points (400-500 ms). Specifically, the ARHL group had greater theta synchronization for high- compared to low-value words. This was not observed for the NH controls. We did not expect this given that our previous studies have not found similar differences between theta synchronization for high- versus low-value words in cognitively healthy adults (Nguyen et al., 2020) and those with mild cognitive impairment (Nguyen et al., 2022). Whether these findings are specific to the ARHL group needs further validation. With theta synchronization linked to proactive control (as mentioned earlier), it could be that our participants with ARHL proactively engaged their top-down processes to strategically attend to and process high-value over low-value words as part of a compensatory neural mechanism as has been postulated by others (Loughrey et al., 2023). Studies have reported increased neural engagement of higher-order cognitive functions on non-auditory cognitive tasks in individuals with mild degrees of ARHL (Slade et al., 2020; Zhao et al., 2022; Loughrey et al., 2023). Given that theta is also linked to inhibitory control (Nigbur et al., 2011; Cavanagh and Frank, 2014; Cavanagh and Shackman, 2015), alternately, our findings might suggest that the ARHL group did the unexpected, i.e., used more neural resources linked to inhibitory control during the processing of high-value words (in other words, inhibited processing of high-value words), suggestive of maladaptive plasticity. However, our behavioral data does not support these findings, although further research, including modification of the current task, is necessary to truly connect our behavioral and ERSP findings. Regardless, our theta findings point toward potential cross-modal changes in those with mild ARHL, considering that the NH group did not demonstrate theta differentiation for high- versus low-value words. This aligns with emerging studies that have reported cross-modal plasticity in those with mild to moderate ARHL, especially in the visual cortical areas (Campbell and Sharma, 2014; Puschmann et al., 2014; Sharma and Glick, 2016; Ponticorvo et al., 2022). Further examination is needed to better delineate cross-modal reorganization in the context of visual strategic processing in ARHL. In general, our findings show that ARHL influences theta oscillations at earlier time points on a visual cognitive control task. Our findings align with other studies, both human (Heinrichs-Graham et al., 2022; Ryan et al., 2023) and animal (Johne et al., 2022), which have shown that hearing loss modulates oscillatory activity in the theta band. However, adequate comparison of our findings with others is difficult since we used a visual task while most have examined theta activity in relation to auditory tasks, primarily SiN recognition (Doelling et al., 2014; Hyafil et al., 2015; Etard and Reichenbach, 2019).

Some similarities were observed in both groups in strategic processing in the alpha band between 700 and 800 ms at the centroparietal electrode cluster. As hypothesized, both ARHL and NH groups had greater alpha desynchronization for high- than low-value words suggesting similar alpha modulation for processing information of varying values (i.e., high vs. low) in later time points. Alpha desynchronization in parietal regions is considered to reflect the engagement of attentional processes during selective attention (Foxe and Snyder, 2011; Deng et al., 2019; Trajkovic et al., 2023). The effect size of these alpha findings (more alpha desynchronization for high- than low-value words) was marginally larger in size within the NH group (Cohens’ d = 1.2) relative to the ARHL group (Cohen’s d = 1.12), suggesting a more robust modulation in the control group. We also found some similarities in our groups in theta oscillations. Greater frontal theta synchronization was observed for low-value relative to high-value words between 200 and 300 ms, and 700–1,000 ms. This suggests that both ARHL and NH groups strategically allocate more cognitive control resources to inhibit the processing of low-value information to prioritize the processing of high-value information, and these findings are consistent with our hypotheses and with our previous studies that used the same task in cognitively healthy (Nguyen et al., 2019, 2020) and cognitively impaired (Nguyen et al., 2022) populations. Both groups also showed greater alpha desynchronization for high- versus low-value words, between 400 and 1,000 ms in the centroparietal electrode cluster and between 200–300 ms and 600–1,000 ms in the parietal cluster, indicating greater attentional allocation for processing the high-value versus low-value words. The similarities between ARHL and NH groups, especially during the later time windows (700 ms and beyond) is similar to the findings of ERP studies involving P3 and late positive potential (LPP) on visual paradigms (Loughrey et al., 2023; Zhao et al., 2023). Perhaps, by the later stages of processing, those with ARHL are able to “catch up” to their age-matched NH peers by utilizing compensatory neurocognitive resources.

This study has several limitations. We analyzed ERSPs from a subset of electrodes that were defined a priori based on past literature. Future studies that use data-driven approaches, such as principal component analyses, would be useful to corroborate findings and advance knowledge related to neural oscillatory changes in older adults with mild ARHL. Future studies should examine ERSP changes with varying measures (e.g., power, phase coherence), as well as utilize complementary techniques such as source localization with EEG or functional magnetic resonance imaging to further unravel the neural mechanisms that underlie strategic processing in this population. Additionally, our study had a small sample size, and replicability from future studies is critical before the generalization of results. Finally, we did not develop the value-directed strategic processing task to distinguish between the processes of encoding, storage, and retrieval, and their examination is critical to understanding changes in value-directed strategic processing in ARHL.

In summary, the current study examined ERSPs underlying visual value-directed strategic processing in older adults with mild ARHL relative to NH controls of comparable age and education. Behavioral data revealed observable differences in total recall of words and total points earned, which points towards cognitive changes in those with ARHL. Changes in theta band were observed during early time periods in ARHL, but these were not specific to value-based processing, indicating more general changes in proactive control and cross-modal sensory enhancement. During the mid-time points (400–500 ms), the ARHL group showed greater theta modulations for high-value compared to low-value information, while these differences were not significant in controls. This might indicate adaptive changes in those with ARHL during strategic processing. In later time widows (after 700 ms), theta and alpha modulations were similar in both groups, perhaps indicating that the ARHL group does “catch up” to the NH controls. Our study adds to the emerging body of work on neural oscillatory changes underlying a cognitive control task in individuals with ARHL. Given that neural changes typically precede behavioral changes, these ERSPs can be used to assess the benefits of intervention (e.g., hearing aids, auditory and cognitive intervention programs) before behavioral changes are observable, especially given that our sample included individuals with untreated mild ARHL.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Institutional Review Board of the University of Illinois Urbana-Champaign (protocol # 17067). The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

SS: Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. SJ: Visualization, Writing – review & editing. RM: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Resources, Supervision, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This research was funded by the Center on Health, Aging, and Disability Pilot grant from the University of Illinois Urbana-Champaign and the American Speech-Language-Hearing Foundation grant.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Lydia Nguyen and Elizabeth Lydon for their feedback on the initial draft of the manuscript, and all study participants for their voluntary participation.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnins.2024.1382613/full#supplementary-material

References

Almeida, O. P., and Almeida, S. A. (1999). Short versions of the geriatric depression scale: a study of their validity for the diagnosis of a major depressive episode according to ICD-10 and DSM-IV. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 14, 858–865. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1099-1166(199910)14:10<858::AID-GPS35>3.0.CO;2-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Anderson, B. A. (2013). A value-driven mechanism of attentional selection. J. Vis. 13:7. doi: 10.1167/13.3.7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Armstrong, N. M., An, Y., Doshi, J., Erus, G., Ferrucci, L., Davatzikos, C., et al. (2019). Association of Midlife Hearing Impairment with Late-Life Temporal Lobe Volume Loss. JAMA Otolaryngol. Head Neck Surg. 145, 794–802. doi: 10.1001/jamaoto.2019.1610

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Baltes, P. B., and Lindenberger, U. (1997). Emergence of a powerful connection between sensory and cognitive functions across the adult life span: a new window to the study of cognitive aging? Psychol. Aging 12, 12–21. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.12.1.12

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Barr, R. A., and Giambra, L. M. (1990). Age-related decrement in auditory selective attention. Psychol. Aging 5, 597–599. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.5.4.597

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Benedek, M., Schickel, R. J., Jauk, E., Fink, A., and Neubauer, A. C. (2014). Alpha power increases in right parietal cortex reflects focused internal attention. Neuropsychologia 56, 393–400. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.02.010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bonmassar, C., Pavani, F., Spinella, D., Frau, G. N., and van Zoest, W. (2022). Does age-related hearing loss deteriorate attentional resources? Aging Neuropsychol Cogn. 30, 601–619. doi: 10.1080/13825585.2022.2067319

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Botvinick, M., and Braver, T. (2015). Motivation and cognitive control: from behavior to neural mechanism. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 66, 83–113. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015044

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Braver, T. S. (2012). The variable nature of cognitive control: a dual-mechanisms framework shifting the emphasis to variability in cognitive control. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 106–113. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.12.010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brewster, K. K., and Rutherford, B. R. (2021). Hearing loss, psychiatric symptoms, and cognitive decline: An increasingly important triad in older adults. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 29, 554–556. doi: 10.1016/j.jagp.2020.10.015

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Campbell, J., and Sharma, A. (2013). Compensatory changes in cortical resource allocation in adults with hearing loss. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 7:71. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2013.00071

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Campbell, J., and Sharma, A. (2014). Cross-modal re-Organization in Adults with early stage hearing loss. PLoS One 9:e90594. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0090594

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Carhart, R., and Jerger, J. (1959). Preferred method for clinical determination of pure-tone thresholds. J. Speech Hear. Disord. 24, 330–345. doi: 10.1044/jshd.2404.330

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Castel, A. D. (2007). The adaptive and strategic use of memory by older adults: evaluative processing and value-directed remembering [internet]. Psychol. Learn. Motiv. 48, 225–270. doi: 10.1016/S0079-7421(07)48006-9

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Castel, A. D., Balota, D., and McCabe, D. P. (2009). Memory efficiency and the strategic control of attention at encoding: impairments of value-directed remembering in Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychology 23, 297–306. doi: 10.1037/a0014888

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Castel, A. D., Farb, N. A. S., and Craik, F. I. M. (2007). Memory for general and specific value information in younger and older adults: measuring the limits of strategic control. Mem. Cognit. 35, 689–700. doi: 10.3758/BF03193307

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Castel, A. D., Humphreys, K. L., Lee, S. S., Galván, A., Balota, D. A., and McCabe, D. P. (2011). The development of memory efficiency and value-directed remembering across the life span: a cross-sectional study of memory and selectivity. Dev. Psychol. 47, 1553–1564. doi: 10.1037/a0025623

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cavanagh, J. F., and Frank, M. J. (2014). Frontal theta as a mechanism for cognitive control. Trends Cogn. Sci. 18, 414–421. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2014.04.012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cavanagh, J. F., and Shackman, A. J. (2015). Frontal midline theta reflects anxiety and cognitive control: meta-analytic evidence. J. Physiol. Paris 109, 3–15. doi: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2014.04.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

CHABA (1988). Speech understanding and aging. Working group on speech understanding and aging. Committee on hearing, bioacoustics, and biomechanics, commission on behavioral and social sciences and education, National Research Council. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 83, 859–895.

Google Scholar

Chavant, M., and Kapoula, Z. (2022). Presbycusis and the aging of eye movement: common attention mechanisms. Brain Sci. 12:107. doi: 10.3390/brainsci12010107

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cooper, P. S., Wong, A. S. W., Fulham, W. R., Thienel, R., Mansfield, E., Michie, P. T., et al. (2015). Theta frontoparietal connectivity associated with proactive and reactive cognitive control processes. NeuroImage 108, 354–363. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.12.028

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Craik, F. I. M. (2007). The role of cognition in age-related hearing loss. J. Am. Acad. Audiol. 18, 539–547. doi: 10.3766/jaaa.18.7.2

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dai, L., Best, V., and Shinn-Cunningham, B. G. (2018). Sensorineural hearing loss degrades behavioral and physiological measures of human spatial selective auditory attention. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 115, E3286–E3295. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1721226115

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Delorme, A., and Makeig, S. (2004). EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J. Neurosci. Methods 134, 9–21. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Deng, Y., Reinhart, R. M., Choi, I., and Shinn-Cunningham, B. G. (2019). Causal links between parietal alpha activity and spatial auditory attention. eLife 8:8. doi: 10.7554/eLife.51184

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 64, 135–168. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dimitrijevic, A., Smith, M. L., Kadis, D. S., and Moore, D. R. (2019). Neural indices of listening effort in noisy environments. Sci. Rep. 9:11278. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-47643-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Doelling, K. B., Arnal, L. H., Ghitza, O., and Poeppel, D. (2014). Acoustic landmarks drive delta–theta oscillations to enable speech comprehension by facilitating perceptual parsing. NeuroImage 85, 761–768. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.06.035

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Elliott, B. L., McClure, S. M., and Brewer, G. A. (2020). Individual differences in value-directed remembering. Cognition 201:104275. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104275

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Eschmann, K. C. J., Bader, R., and Mecklinger, A. (2020). Improving episodic memory: frontal-midline theta neurofeedback training increases source memory performance. NeuroImage 222:117219. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117219

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Etard, O., and Reichenbach, T. (2019). Neural speech tracking in the Theta and in the Delta frequency band differentially encode clarity and comprehension of speech in noise. J. Neurosci. 39, 5750–5759. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1828-18.2019

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Foxe, J., and Snyder, A. (2011). The role of alpha-band brain oscillations as a sensory suppression mechanism during selective attention. Front. Psychol. 2:154. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00154

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fuglsang, S. A., Märcher-Rørsted, J., Dau, T., and Hjortkjær, J. (2020). Effects of sensorineural hearing loss on cortical synchronization to competing speech during selective attention. J. Neurosci. 40, 2562–2572. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1936-19.2020

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fumuro, T., Matsuhashi, M., Miyazaki, T., Inouchi, M., Hitomi, T., Matsumoto, R., et al. (2015). Alpha-band desynchronization in human parietal area during reach planning. Clin. Neurophysiol. 126, 756–762. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2014.07.026

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gillingham, S. M., Vallesi, A., Pichora-Fuller, M. K., and Alain, C. (2018). Older adults with hearing loss have reductions in visual, motor and attentional functioning. Front. Aging Neurosci. 10:10. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2018.00351

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Grandchamp, R., and Delorme, A. (2011). Single-trial normalization for event-related spectral decomposition reduces sensitivity to noisy trials. Front. Psychol. 2:236. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00236

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Griffiths, T. D., Lad, M., Kumar, S., Holmes, E., McMurray, B., Maguire, E. A., et al. (2020). How can hearing loss cause dementia? Neuron 108, 401–412. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2020.08.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Guerreiro, M. J. S., and Van Gerven, P. W. M. (2017). Disregarding hearing loss leads to overestimation of age-related cognitive decline. Neurobiol. Aging 56, 180–189. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2017.05.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Haile, L. M., Kamenov, K., Briant, P. S., Orji, A. U., Steinmetz, J. D., Abdoli, A., et al. (2021). Hearing loss prevalence and years lived with disability, 1990–2019: findings from the global burden of disease study 2019. Lancet 397, 996–1009. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(21)00516-X

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Heinrichs-Graham, E., Walker, E. A., Taylor, B. K., Menting, S. C., Eastman, J. A., Frenzel, M. R., et al. (2022). Auditory experience modulates fronto-parietal theta activity serving fluid intelligence. Brain Commun. 4:fcac093. doi: 10.1093/braincomms/fcac093

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Huber, M., Roesch, S., Pletzer, B., Lukaschyk, J., Lesinski-Schiedat, A., and Illg, A. (2020). Cognition in older adults with severe to profound sensorineural hearing loss compared to peers with normal hearing for age. Int. J. Audiol. 59, 254–262. doi: 10.1080/14992027.2019.1687947

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Humes, L. E. (2013). Understanding the speech-understanding problems of older adults. Am. J. Audiol. 22, 303–305. doi: 10.1044/1059-0889(2013/12-0066)

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Humes, L. E., and Dubno, J. R. (2010). “Factors affecting speech understanding in older adults” in The aging auditory system [internet]. eds. S. Gordon-Salant, R. D. Frisina, A. N. Popper, and R. R. Fay (New York, NY: Springer New York), 211–257.

Google Scholar

Humes, L. E., Kidd, G. R., and Fogerty, D. (2017). Exploring use of the coordinate response measure in a multitalker babble paradigm. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 60, 741–754. doi: 10.1044/2016_JSLHR-H-16-0042

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Humes, L. E., Lee, J. H., and Coughlin, M. P. (2006). Auditory measures of selective and divided attention in young and older adults using single-talker competition. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 120, 2926–2937. doi: 10.1121/1.2354070

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hyafil, A., Fontolan, L., Kabdebon, C., Gutkin, B., and Giraud, A. L. (2015). Speech encoding by coupled cortical theta and gamma oscillations. eLife 4:4. doi: 10.7554/eLife.06213

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Itthipuripat, S., Wessel, J. R., and Aron, A. R. (2013). Frontal theta is a signature of successful working memory manipulation. Exp. Brain Res. 224, 255–262. doi: 10.1007/s00221-012-3305-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Johne, M., Helgers, S. O. A., Alam, M., Jelinek, J., Hubka, P., Krauss, J. K., et al. (2022). Processing of auditory information in forebrain regions after hearing loss in adulthood: behavioral and electrophysiological studies in a rat model. Front. Neurosci. 16:966568. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2022.966568

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kawasaki, M., Kitajo, K., and Yamaguchi, Y. (2010). Dynamic links between theta executive functions and alpha storage buffers in auditory and visual working memory. Eur. J. Neurosci. 31, 1683–1689. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07217.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Killion, M. C., Niquette, P. A., Gudmundsen, G. I., Revit, L. J., and Banerjee, S. (2004). Development of a quick speech-in-noise test for measuring signal-to-noise ratio loss in normal-hearing and hearing-impaired listeners. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 116, 2395–2405.

Google Scholar

Klimesch, W., Doppelmayr, M., Pachinger, T., and Ripper, B. (1997). Brain oscillations and human memory: EEG correlates in the upper alpha and theta band. Neurosci. Lett. 238, 9–12. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3940(97)00771-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Klimesch, W., Sauseng, P., and Hanslmayr, S. (2007). EEG alpha oscillations: the inhibition–timing hypothesis. Brain Res. Rev. 53, 63–88. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2006.06.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Koivisto, M., and Grassini, S. (2016). Neural processing around 200 ms after stimulus-onset correlates with subjective visual awareness. Neuropsychologia 84, 235–243. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.02.024

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, F. R. (2011). Hearing loss and cognition among older adults in the United States. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 66, 1131–1136. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glr115

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, F. R., Ferrucci, L., An, Y., Goh, J. O., Doshi, J., Metter, E. J., et al. (2014). Association of hearing impairment with brain volume changes in older adults. NeuroImage 90, 84–92. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.12.059

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lindenberger, U., and Baltes, P. (1994). Sensory functioning and intelligence in old age: a strong connection. Psychol. Aging 9, 339–355. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.9.3.339

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Loughrey, D. G., Jordan, C., Ibanez, A., Parra, M. A., Lawlor, B. A., and Reilly, R. B. (2023). Age-related hearing loss associated with differences in the neural correlates of feature binding in visual working memory. Neurobiol. Aging 132, 233–245. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2023.09.016

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Loughrey, D. G., Parra, M. A., and Lawlor, B. A. (2019). Visual short-term memory binding deficit with age-related hearing loss in cognitively normal older adults. Sci. Rep. 9:12600. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-49023-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Luck, S. J. (2023). “Event-related potentials” in APA handbook of research methods in psychology: Foundations, planning, measures, and psychometrics. 2nd ed (Washington: American Psychological Association), 605–630.

Google Scholar

Mackie, M. A., Van Dam, N. T., and Fan, J. (2013). Cognitive control and attentional functions. Brain Cogn. 82, 301–312. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2013.05.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Madashetty, S., Palaniswamy, H. P., and Rajashekhar, B. (2024). The impact of age-related hearing loss on working memory among older individuals: An event-related potential study. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Dis. Extra. 14, 1–13. doi: 10.1159/000538109

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Makeig, S. (1993). Auditory event-related dynamics of the EEG spectrum and effects of exposure to tones. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 86, 283–293. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(93)90110-H

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Makeig, S., Debener, S., Onton, J., and Delorme, A. (2004). Mining event-related brain dynamics. Trends Cogn. Sci. 8, 204–210. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.03.008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Messel, M. S., Raud, L., Hoff, P. K., Stubberud, J., and Huster, R. J. (2021). Frontal-midline theta reflects different mechanisms associated with proactive and reactive control of inhibition. NeuroImage 241:118400. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118400

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mitchell, D. J., McNaughton, N., Flanagan, D., and Kirk, I. J. (2008). Frontal-midline theta from the perspective of hippocampal “theta”. Prog. Neurobiol. 86, 156–185. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2008.09.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Murphy, D. H., Schwartz, S. T., and Castel, A. D. (2024). Value-directed retrieval: the effects of divided attention at encoding and retrieval on memory selectivity and retrieval dynamics. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 50, 17–38. doi: 10.1037/xlm0001264

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nasreddine, Z., Phillips, N., Bédirian, V., Charbonneau, S., Whitehead, V., Collin, I., et al. (2005). The Montreal cognitive assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 53, 695–699. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication. Age-related hearing loss. NIDCD Fact Sheet. (2023) Available at: https://www.nidcd.nih.gov/health/age-related-hearing-loss (Accessed December 11, 2023).

Google Scholar

Nguyen, L. T., Lydon, E. A., Shende, S. A., Llano, D. A., and Mudar, R. A. (2022). Disrupted value-directed strategic processing in individuals with mild cognitive impairment: behavioral and neural correlates. Geriatrics 7:56. doi: 10.3390/geriatrics7030056

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nguyen, L. T., Marini, F., Shende, S. A., Llano, D. A., and Mudar, R. A. (2020). Investigating EEG theta and alpha oscillations as measures of value-directed strategic processing in cognitively normal younger and older adults. Behav. Brain Res. 391:112702. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2020.112702

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nguyen, L. T., Marini, F., Zacharczuk, L., Llano, D. A., and Mudar, R. A. (2019). Theta and alpha band oscillations during value-directed strategic processing. Behav. Brain Res. 367, 210–214. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2019.03.052

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nguyen, L., Shende, S., Llano, D., and Mudar, R. (2020). EEG measures of value-directed strategic processing in older adults with and without mild cognitive impairment. Innov Aging [Internet]. 4, 292–293. doi: 10.1093/geroni/igaa057.937

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nigbur, R., Ivanova, G., and Stürmer, B. (2011). Theta power as a marker for cognitive interference. Clin. Neurophysiol. 122, 2185–2194. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2011.03.030

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nuesse, T., Steenken, R., Neher, T., and Holube, I. (2018). Exploring the link between cognitive abilities and speech recognition in the elderly under different listening conditions. Front. Psychol. 9:9(MAY). doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00678

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Petersen, E. B., Wöstmann, M., Obleser, J., Stenfelt, S., and Lunner, T. (2015). Hearing loss impacts neural alpha oscillations under adverse listening conditions. Front. Psychol. 6:177. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00177

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pfurtscheller, G., and Lopes da Silva, F. H. (1999). Event-related EEG/MEG synchronization and desynchronization: basic principles. Clin. Neurophysiol. 110, 1842–1857. doi: 10.1016/S1388-2457(99)00141-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ponticorvo, S., Manara, R., Cassandro, E., Canna, A., Scarpa, A., Troisi, D., et al. (2022). Cross-modal connectivity effects in age-related hearing loss. Neurobiol. Aging 111, 1–13. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2021.09.024

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Powell, D. S., Oh, E. S., Reed, N. S., Lin, F. R., and Deal, J. A. (2022). Hearing loss and cognition: What we know and where we need to go. Front. Aging Neurosci. 13:769405. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2021.769405

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Price, C. N., Alain, C., and Bidelman, G. M. (2019). Auditory-frontal channeling in α and β bands is altered by age-related hearing loss and relates to speech perception in noise. Neuroscience 423, 18–28. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2019.10.044

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pscherer, C., Wendiggensen, P., Mückschel, M., Bluschke, A., and Beste, C. (2023). Alpha and theta band activity share information relevant to proactive and reactive control during conflict-modulated response inhibition. Hum. Brain Mapp. 44, 5936–5952. doi: 10.1002/hbm.26486

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Puschmann, S., Sandmann, P., Bendixen, A., and Thiel, C. M. (2014). Age-related hearing loss increases cross-modal distractibility. Hear. Res. 316, 28–36. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2014.07.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ren, F., Luo, J., Ma, W., Xin, Q., Xu, L., Fan, Z., et al. (2019). Hearing loss and cognition among older adults in a Han Chinese cohort. Front. Neurosci. 13:632. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2019.00632

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rönnberg, J., Danielsson, H., Rudner, M., Arlinger, S., Sternäng, O., Wahlin, Å., et al. (2011). Hearing loss is negatively related to episodic and semantic long-term memory but not to short-term memory. Hear. Res. 54, 705–726. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2010/09-0088)

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rönnberg, J., Sharma, A., Signoret, C., Campbell, T. A., and Sörqvist, P. (2022b). Editorial: cognitive hearing science: investigating the relationship between selective attention and brain activity. Front. Neurosci. 16:1098340. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2022.1098340

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rönnberg, J., Signoret, C., Andin, J., and Holmer, E. (2022a). The cognitive hearing science perspective on perceiving, understanding, and remembering language: the ELU model. Front. Psychol. 13:13. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.967260

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryan, D. B., Eckert, M. A., Sellers, E. W., Schairer, K. S., McBee, M. T., Ridley, E. A., et al. (2023). Performance monitoring and cognitive inhibition during a speech-in-noise task in older listeners. Semin. Hear. 44, 124–139. doi: 10.1055/s-0043-1767695

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneider, B. A., and Pichora-Fuller, M. K. (2000). “Implications of perceptual deterioration for cognitive aging research” in The handbook of aging and cognition. 2nd ed (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers), 155–219.

Google Scholar

Schwartz, S. T., Siegel, A. L. M., Eich, T. S., and Castel, A. D. (2023). Value-directed memory selectivity relies on goal-directed knowledge of value structure prior to encoding in young and older adults. Psychol. Aging 38, 30–48. doi: 10.1037/pag0000720

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sharma, A., and Glick, H. (2016). Cross-modal re-Organization in Clinical Populations with hearing loss. Brain Sci. 6:4. doi: 10.3390/brainsci6010004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shechter Shvartzman, L., Lavie, L., and Banai, K. (2022). Speech perception in older adults: An interplay of hearing, cognition, and learning? Front. Psychol. 13:13. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.816864

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shende, S. A., Lydon, E. A., Husain, F. T., and Mudar, R. A. (2022). Inhibitory control and value-directed strategic attention in persons with mild age-related hearing loss. Aging Health Res. 2:100074. doi: 10.1016/j.ahr.2022.100074

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shende, S. A., and Mudar, R. A. (2023). Cognitive control in age-related hearing loss: A narrative review. Hear. Res. 436:108814. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2023.108814

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shende, S. A., Nguyen, L. T., Lydon, E. A., Husain, F. T., and Mudar, R. A. (2021). Cognitive flexibility and inhibition in individuals with age-related hearing loss. Geriatrics 6:22. doi: 10.3390/geriatrics6010022

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shinn-Cunningham, B. G., and Best, V. (2008). Selective attention in normal and impaired hearing. Trends Amplif. 12, 283–299. doi: 10.1177/1084713808325306

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Slade, K., Plack, C. J., and Nuttall, H. E. (2020). The effects of age-related hearing loss on the brain and cognitive function. Trends Neurosci. 43, 810–821. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2020.07.005

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stucky, S. R., Wolf, K. E., and Kuo, T. (2010). The economic effect of age-related hearing loss: national, state, and local estimates, 2002 and 2030. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 58, 58, 618–619. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2010.02746.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stuphorn, V., and Emeric, E. E. (2012). Proactive and reactive control by the medial frontal cortex. Front. Neuroeng. 5:5. doi: 10.3389/fneng.2012.00009

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sur, S., and Sinha, V. (2009). Event-related potential: An overview. Ind. Psychiatry J. 18, 70–73. doi: 10.4103/0972-6748.57865

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tarawneh, H. Y., Jayakody, D. M. P., Sohrabi, H. R., Martins, R. N., and Mulders, W. H. A. M. (2022). Understanding the relationship between age-related hearing loss and Alzheimer’s disease: a narrative review. J. Alzheimers Dis. Rep. 6, 539–556. doi: 10.3233/ADR-220035

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tillman, T. W., and Carhart, R. (1966). An expanded test for speech discrimination utilizing CNC monosyllabic words: Northwestern University Auditory Test No. 6). Brooks Air Force Base, TX: USAF School of Aerospace Medicine, 1–12.

Google Scholar

Torrente, M. C., Vergara, R., Moreno-Gómez, F. N., Leiva, A., San Martin, S., Belkhiria, C., et al. (2022). Speech perception and dichotic listening are associated with hearing thresholds and cognition, respectively, in unaided presbycusis. Front. Aging Neurosci. 14:10. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2022.786330

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Trajkovic, J., Di Gregorio, F., Avenanti, A., Thut, G., and Romei, V. (2023). Two oscillatory correlates of attention control in the alpha-band with distinct consequences on perceptual gain and metacognition. J. Neurosci. 43, 3548–3556. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1827-22.2023

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Gerven, P. W. M., and Guerreiro, M. J. S. (2016). Selective attention and sensory modality in aging: curses and blessings. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10, 1–7. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00147

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wisniewski, M. G., Zakrzewski, A. C., Bell, D. R., and Wheeler, M. (2021). EEG power spectral dynamics associated with listening in adverse conditions. Psychophysiology 58:e13877. doi: 10.1111/psyp.13877

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

World Health Organization. World report on Hearing [Internet]. (2021). Available at: https://www.who.int/publications/i/item/9789240020481 (Accessed January 2024, 8).

Google Scholar

Zhao, R, Hu, Y, Ni, G, and Bai, Y. The influence of age-related hearing loss on working memory based on EEG analysis. In: 2022 16th ICME international conference on complex medical engineering (CME). IEEE; (2022). p. 282–285.

Google Scholar

Zhao, R., Yue, T., Xu, Z., Zhang, Y., Wu, Y., Bai, Y., et al. (2023). Electroencephalogram-based objective assessment of cognitive function level associated with age-related hearing loss. Geroscience 46, 431–446. doi: 10.1007/s11357-023-00847-w

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: age-related hearing loss, strategic processing, cognitive control, alpha, theta, neural oscillations, older adults, event-related electroencephalography

Citation: Shende SA, Jones SE and Mudar RA (2024) Alpha and theta oscillations on a visual strategic processing task in age-related hearing loss. Front. Neurosci. 18:1382613. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2024.1382613

Received: 05 February 2024; Accepted: 28 June 2024;
Published: 16 July 2024.

Edited by:

Jyrki Ahveninen, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School, United States

Reviewed by:

Kaisu Lankinen, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School, United States
James W. Dias, Medical University of South Carolina, United States
Jasmine Tan, Mass General Brigham, United States

Copyright © 2024 Shende, Jones and Mudar. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Shraddha A. Shende, sashend@ilstu.edu; Raksha A. Mudar, raksha@illinois.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.