Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurosci., 17 June 2024
Sec. Auditory Cognitive Neuroscience

Neural correlates of musical timbre: an ALE meta-analysis of neuroimaging data

  • 1Acoustics Research Institute, Austrian Academy of Sciences, Vienna, Austria
  • 2Systematic Musicology, Institute for Musicology, University of Cologne, Cologne, Germany

Timbre is a central aspect of music that allows listeners to identify musical sounds and conveys musical emotion, but also allows for the recognition of actions and is an important structuring property of music. The former functions are known to be implemented in a ventral auditory stream in processing musical timbre. While the latter functions are commonly attributed to areas in a dorsal auditory processing stream in other musical domains, its involvement in musical timbre processing is so far unknown. To investigate if musical timbre processing involves both dorsal and ventral auditory pathways, we carried out an activation likelihood estimation (ALE) meta-analysis of 18 experiments from 17 published neuroimaging studies on musical timbre perception. We identified consistent activations in Brodmann areas (BA) 41, 42, and 22 in the bilateral transverse temporal gyri, the posterior superior temporal gyri and planum temporale, in BA 40 of the bilateral inferior parietal lobe, in BA 13 in the bilateral posterior Insula, and in BA 13 and 22 in the right anterior insula and superior temporal gyrus. The vast majority of the identified regions are associated with the dorsal and ventral auditory processing streams. We therefore propose to frame the processing of musical timbre in a dual-stream model. Moreover, the regions activated in processing timbre show similarities to the brain regions involved in processing several other fundamental aspects of music, indicating possible shared neural bases of musical timbre and other musical domains.

1 Introduction

Music cognition research explores the neural bases of processing essential features of music, including pitch and rhythm. These features have been the primary focus within a dual-stream model. Consistent with visual processing streams (Goodale and Milner, 1992), two auditory processing streams were introduced: the auditory dorsal stream, connecting the areas in the posterior superior temporal gyrus, the inferior parietal lobes, premotor areas, and posterior parts of the inferior frontal gyrus, is considered to be involved in sequencing, whereas a ventral stream, linking the anterior superior temporal gyrus and anterior areas of the inferior frontal gyrus, is conceived of playing central role in categorical perception and auditory pattern recognition (Zatorre et al., 2007; Rauschecker, 2014). For example, concerning pitch processing, the areas related to the auditory dorsal streams are involved in the processing of interval structures (Stewart et al., 2008), the concatenation of pitches into a sequence (Green et al., 2018), the syntactic processing of chord progressions (Musso et al., 2015), while those associated with the ventral stream deal with the mapping of semantic and affective information onto pitch sequences (Rauschecker, 2014; Asano et al., 2022b). Concerning rhythm, the areas related to the dorsal auditory stream are involved in the timing (Grahn et al., 2011), sequencing (Chen et al., 2008; Kotz et al., 2018), and metrical structuring of events (Chen et al., 2008), the organization of sounds into beat patterns (Patel and Iversen, 2014), and the production of rhythms (Zatorre et al., 2007; Kotz et al., 2018), while the ventral stream areas are involved in the integration of melodic and rhythmic information (Sihvonen et al., 2022).

Another essential feature of music besides pitch and rhythm is timbre (Patel, 2008; Holmes, 2012; Hoeschele et al., 2014). Timbre, also called “sound quality” or “tone color”, is a perceptual property of sounds, which enables listeners to discriminate two sounds equal in all other parameters such as pitch, duration, and loudness (Siedenburg et al., 2016). For example, timbre allows listeners to distinguish a note plucked on a violin vs. a guitar; a violin sound plucked vs. bowed; or to discriminate the elements in the sound sequence “/p, /t, /k, /t,” when all other parameters are kept constant (Koelsch et al., 2002; Town and Bizley, 2013; Siedenburg et al., 2016). Timbre depends on multiple acoustic properties, such as the attack portion, the composition of the frequency spectrum, and the development of the spectrum in time (McAdams, 2013).

The ability to perceive timbre is considered to be a feature of human musicality (Wagner and Hoeschele, 2022). Already infants remember the timbre of unfamiliar songs better than their melody (Trainor et al., 2004), and changes in timbre considerably influence the recognition of music by adults (e.g., Radvansky et al., 1995; Lange and Czernochowski, 2013; Schellenberg and Habashi, 2015). Timbre is a quality inherent to all perceived musical instrument sounds and vocal sounds (Town and Bizley, 2013). It is fundamental for the discrimination of sounds, the identification and recognition of sound sources such as musical instruments (Handel, 1995), and the categorization of actions involved in, e.g., various types of drum strokes (Patel, 2008). Timbre is also seen as one of the primary ways of conveying emotion in music (Hailstone et al., 2009; Holmes, 2012; Bowman and Yamauchi, 2016; Juslin and Lindström, 2016).

Like pitch and rhythm, timbre is also an important structuring property of music (Patel, 2008; Goodchild et al., 2019; McAdams, 2019). As timbre allows the discrimination of sound events that occur in temporal succession (Town and Bizley, 2013; Koelsch, 2019), it has repeatedly been proposed to enable the segmental grouping of sounds into larger musical units (McAdams and Giordano, 2016; Goodchild et al., 2019; McAdams, 2019). For example, percussive and beatboxing music can be essentially regarded as a structured series of timbres, in which the musical patterns are organized based on sound events that mainly differ in timbre (Patel, 2008; Stowell and Plumbley, 2008). Moreover, repeating sequences with hierarchical dependencies among timbres are recognized even by listeners unfamiliar with these patterns (Siedenburg et al., 2016).

Previous research found that musical timbre processing is commonly attributed to the areas related to the ventral stream including the middle and anterior portions of the superior temporal gyrus (STG) and the ventral part of the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) (Rauschecker, 2014; Alluri and Kadiri, 2019). Timbre-based analyses of basic auditory features (such as the perceived “brightness”) and discrimination of sound events are thought to be mediated by central auditory cortex (AC) and directly adjacent cortices of the posterior STG (pSTG), including its upper surface posterior to the primary auditory cortex (planum temporale, PT), while stimulus categorization based on timbre is thought to be mediated by cortices in the anterior STG (aSTG) including its superior surface (planum polare, PP) anterior to the primary auditory cortex (Town and Bizley, 2013; Alluri and Kadiri, 2019; Wei et al., 2022). It was also suggested that musical timbre processing mainly depends on right-hemispheric structures (Samson et al., 2002).

To date, the contributions of the dorsal stream to (musical) timbre processing remain unclear (Alluri and Kadiri, 2019). One of the central functions of the dorsal auditory stream in music processing is the processing of pitch and rhythmic sequences. For example, in processing melodies, the dorsal stream is associated with the discrimination of pitches and the sequencing of pitch patterns in the pSTG, PT, IPL, PMC, and SMA (Rauschecker and Scott, 2009; Rauschecker, 2014) as well as the structural organization of pitch and the prediction and detection of deviations from pitch patterns in the PT, pSTG, and IFG (Zatorre et al., 2007). Likewise, timbre processing involves these operations typically implemented in the dorsal stream: Based on timbre, sounds are discriminated in a continuous stream of incoming information and organized in a specific order, which is crucial for the structuring of sequences from multiple sounds (Patel, 2008; McAdams, 2013). Moreover, on the basis of timbre, listeners can learn structural regularities of musical sequences (Tillmann and McAdams, 2004) and detect deviations from a sequence of sounds (Koelsch et al., 2002). Therefore, we hypothesize that musical timbre processing involves areas in the dorsal auditory stream. To test this hypothesis, we conduct an activation-likelihood estimation (ALE) meta-analysis on the neural correlates of musical timbre perception. Although two ALE meta-analyses on musical timbre processing have been already conducted before (Janata, 2015; Criscuolo et al., 2021), these analyses provide no clear answers regarding the cortical organization of musical timbre processing due to small sample sizes, inconsistency in study inclusion, and/or inclusion of non-musical stimuli.

Based on the previous research, we expect to identify consistent activations in (1) auditory regions on the HG (Ba 41, BA 42) that perform basic timbral analyses (Janata, 2015; Alluri and Kadiri, 2019) and (2) in ventral stream areas aSTG/PP (BA 22) for processing categorical information for object recognition (Bizley and Cohen, 2013; Janata, 2015). With timbre as a structuring property involved in segmentation, we also expect (3) dorsal stream associated activations in pSTG/PT (BA 22) (Janata, 2015) for timbre-based discrimination (Town and Bizley, 2013) and sequential processing (Clark et al., 2015). We also expect the inferior parietal lobe (IPL) and the IFG (Criscuolo et al., 2021), which were associated with musical timbre-based sensorimotor, sequential (BA 39 & BA 40; Margulis et al., 2009; Lévêque and Schön, 2015), and structural (BA 44; Koelsch et al., 2002; Koelsch, 2006) processing.

2 Method

A systematic search for neuroimaging studies on musical timbre processing was carried out in the Databases PubMed, APA PsycInfo, and Neurosynth. For the search on PubMed and APA PsycInfo, we used the following search terms: “[timbre OR timber1 OR timbral] & [fmri OR pet]”; “music & [unexpected OR expectancy OR spectral OR noise OR [noise & scanner] OR drum OR percussion] & fmri”; “instrument & [different OR [music & emotion] OR [musical & voices] OR [music & different]] & fmri”; “[reverberation OR beatboxers] & [fmri].” The search returned n = 572 results on PubMed and n = 136 results on APA PsycInfo. Search using “timbre” returned n = 2 results on Neurosynth. In total, the search yielded n = 710 results. After the removal of n = 272 duplicates, n = 438 remaining records were screened for title and abstract. Of these n = 438 screened records, n = 374 records were excluded based on the inclusion and exclusion criteria. The full texts of all remaining n = 64 records were successfully retrieved. The Flowchart (Figure 1) shows the article screening procedure.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Flowchart of the article screening procedure. *n = 14 Studies were excluded for multiple reasons.

Studies were selected based on the following inclusion and exclusion criteria. Studies were included if timbre perception by healthy adults (> 18 years old) was investigated by using musical stimuli containing no words and using fMRI or PET; an analysis of the whole brain was carried out; foci were reported in Montreal Neurological Institute-Hospital (MNI) or Talairach (TAL) (Talairach and Tournoux, 1988) coordinate system; if the analyses or contrasts were reported for [timbre > baseline] or [timbre > rest] or [musical timbre > nonmusical timbre] or [attention timbre > attention other]. Studies were excluded, if either populations under the age of 18 years or populations with a disorder or disability were investigated; the analysis was a region-of-interest (ROI) or volume-of-interest (VOI) analysis; the article was a review or did otherwise not report original experimental research; contrasts or analyses reported were other than specified by the inclusion criteria; a group comparison was carried out; only functional connectivity analyses were carried out; and the task was timbre production or imagination.

Of n = 64 full-text articles assessed for eligibility according to these criteria, n = 47 studies were excluded for the following reasons: contrast that does not meet the inclusion criteria (n = 15); only ROI analysis was carried out (n = 15); no coordinates were reported in MNI or TAL (n = 12); no musical stimuli were used (n = 7); no timbre perception task were used (n = 5); they were non-fMRI or non-PET study (n = 2); only group comparison was carried out (n = 4); they were review article (n = 1); they did not study healthy adult population (n = 1); the analysis did not cover the whole brain (n = 1); and only functional connectivity analysis was carried out (n = 1).

From the remaining 17 studies, n = 18 experiments were included in the meta-analysis. Concerning the selection of contrasts, we aimed to include contrasts in a way that broadly encompasses the various aspects of processing musical timbre. These include the processing of basic perceptual features like brightness or roughness, musical sequences based on timbre, and categorical information from isolated sounds such as stimulus identification and recognition. Due to the limited number of studies investigating timbre and the heterogeneity of the contrasts, it was not possible to focus on one specific contrast.

For studies that compared passive listening to sounds with musical timbre and passive listening to sounds with a nonmusical timbre, we chose the contrast [musical timbre > non-musical timbre]. This contrast should include the activations specific to processing musical instrument identity, which is a central aspect of musical timbre processing.

For studies that compared manipulations of timbre with other parameters, we chose the contrasts between timbre and a baseline containing sounds without any timbre manipulation [timbre > baseline]. Examples include deviations in timbre versus deviations in other musical parameters or listening to complex musical timbres versus more sine-like timbres. These contrasts should reflect musical timbre processing because activations associated with the processing of loudness and tonal or rhythmic features as well as general auditory processing should be subtracted.

For studies that compared listening to musical instrument timbres of isolated sounds to resting baselines or visual and motor control baselines, we chose the contrast [timbre > rest] and [timbre > visual & motor baseline]. These contrasts should primarily reflect spectral feature processing due to the lack of tonal or rhythmic context in single sounds. Because these contrasts encompass activations from the general processing of basic timbral features in musical sounds, they allow our analysis to also capture the correlates of processing foundational aspects of musical timbre, like brightness or roughness, otherwise lost.

For studies that compared the tracking of deviations in a sequence of timbres with passive listening and a resting baseline, we chose the contrast [timbre > rest] if no contrast against a sequence of nonmusical timbres was available. This contrast should include the activations associated with sequential and structural processing of musical timbre. We did not choose the contrast [tracking deviations > listening] because it emphasizes the activations associated with paying attention to deviations and weakens those associated with the sequential processing of musical timbre.

For studies that compared listening while attending to a musical timbre and listening while not attending to musical timbre, we chose the contrast [attention timbre > attention other]. We argue that due to the nature of the contrast and the presence of musical stimuli in both conditions, general auditory activations are ruled out, activations because of processing a class of specific stimuli (that is, music) are ruled out, and correlates of general attention-paying or resources used for attentional focus are ruled out. What should remain of these contrasts are activations specific to the attentive tracking of timbre.

For studies that correlated acoustical features with brain activations during music listening, we included timbral complexity regressors. Timbral complexity is based on measures of the Wiener entropy of the spectrum, with the lowest values (no complexity) being found for sine-like tones, while more complex spectral information results in high values (Alluri and Toiviainen, 2010). We chose this regressor instead of the other common timbre-related regressors such as those targeting the perceived “fullness” (spectral fluctuations in lower bands of the spectrum), “activity” (roughness and flux in the highest areas of the spectrum), or “brightness” (spectral centroid) (Alluri and Toiviainen, 2010) because these regressors are constrained to very specific sub-aspects of timbre processing compared to the timbral complexity regressor.

Instrumental sounds were chosen over sung stimuli to minimize the influence of activations reflecting voice processing possibly shared with the domain of speech. Only one contrast per experiment was selected to control for multiple comparisons (Müller et al., 2018). Characteristics of the included 18 experiments (n = 338 Participants (159 female), mean age = 24,51 years) are shown in Table 1. The neuroimaging method comprised fMRI (n = 17 experiments) and PET (n = 1 experiment). The task varied, with n = 7 experiments using passive listening, n = 5 using musical timbre identification, n = 2 using detection of musical timbral deviances, n = 2 using listening and covert recitation of musical timbres, n = 1 musical timbre discrimination, and n = 1 on careful listening. Stimuli from Tsai et al. (2010), and Wallmark et al. (2018a,b) partly comprised sung vocal timbres (in both studies, 25% of stimuli were sung vocal sounds without text). All other stimuli consisted of non-vocal musical timbres. The foci included both cortical and subcortical activations.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. List of the experiments included in the meta-analysis.

We conducted an activation likelihood estimation (ALE) meta-analysis. ALE is a coordinate-based statistical method that allows identifying brain areas of consistent activation across neuroimaging studies (Turkeltaub et al., 2002). The ALE was computed using GingerALE v3.0.2 (brainmap.org/ale). First, TAL coordinates were converted to the MNI system (SPM) or, if indicated, MNI (other) by the built-in conversion algorithm (Fox et al., 2013). For the calculation of the activation likelihood, a cluster-forming threshold of p < 0.001 with 5,000 permutations was chosen (Eickhoff et al., 2012). A cluster-wise family-wise error correction was performed using a threshold of p < 0.05 (Eickhoff et al., 2012; Müller et al., 2018).

3 Results

The ALE meta-analysis identified four clusters of consistent activation during musical timbre processing (Figure 2). Musical timbre processing consistently activated the bilateral posterior temporal lobes, including pSTG, PT, and HG. In the parietal lobe, inferior areas of the SMG were reliably activated, with more pronounced activations in the left hemisphere. In both hemispheres, posterior portions of the insula were activated. Finally, we also identified a right-hemispheric cluster covering the anterior insula and the aSTG/PP. Table 2 lists the peak coordinates of the four obtained clusters in MNI space.

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. ALE clusters for the musical timbre meta-analysis Template: ICBM 2009c Nonlinear Asymmetric (T1w) with ICBM 2009c brain mask (Fonov et al., 2009, 2011). The first and second horizontal row, respectively, show peaks of clusters one and two in the pSTG, HG and SMG, the third row shows the cluster three (z = −6) in the right aSTG/Insula and cluster four in the left pSTG. Labels for peaks in off-white color. aSTG, anterior superior temporal gyrus; BA, Brodmann area; HG, Heschl’s Gyrus (Transverse temporal gyrus); pSTG, posterior superior temporal gyrus; SMG, supramarginal gyrus.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Peak coordinates of the clusters found by the ALE meta-analysis in MNI.

Cluster 1 had its maximum in inferior parts of the SMG (BA40) in the left hemisphere. It extended ventrally into the pSTG and PT (BA 22). Anteroventrally, the cluster extended into the primary and secondary auditory cortex on HG and bordering areas (BA 41 and BA 42). Cluster 1 also covered parts of the posterior Insula (BA 13). Cluster 1 included foci from 11 experiments, of which four involved musical sequence processing such as tracking of a timbre (Janata et al., 2002, Exp. I) or timbral deviations (Janata et al., 2002, Exp. II; Koelsch et al., 2002), and attentively listening to a sequence of timbres and covert reciting (Tsai et al., 2012). One involved listening to a complex musical stimulus, using a timbral complexity regressor (Alluri et al., 2012). The other six experiments involved processing timbre from single sounds that were passively listened to (Halpern et al., 2004; Wallmark et al., 2018a,b) or that were the basis for a sound source identification task (Tervaniemi et al., 2006; Sturm et al., 2011; Osnes et al., 2012).

Cluster 2 was located in the right hemisphere, had its maximum on the pSTG (BA 22), and extended anteriorly into the HG and adjacent pSTG (BA 41 & BA 42), medially into the posterior Insula (BA 13), and dorsally into the SMG (BA 40). Cluster 2 included foci from 9 experiments involving musical sequences that contained timbre deviations (Janata et al., 2002), Exp. II; Koelsch et al., 2002), timbre sequences based on percussive sounds (Tsai et al., 2010, 2012), and isolated musical sounds (Halpern et al., 2004; Tervaniemi et al., 2006; Osnes et al., 2012; Whitehead and Armony, 2018; Wallmark et al., 2018a,b).

Cluster 3 was located in the right hemisphere anterior to Cluster 2, covering inferior anterior parts of the insula (BA 13), where it had its maximum. Laterally, it extended into the left aSTG (BA 22). This anteroventral cluster included foci from 7 experiments, all of which involved passive listening tasks: Passive listening to musical timbres from single sounds (Menon et al., 2002; Angulo-Perkins et al., 2014; Whitehead and Armony, 2018; Wallmark et al., 2018a,b), a timbral complexity regressor from a passive listening task (Alluri et al., 2012), or the passive listening to timbre deviations (Koelsch et al., 2002).

Cluster 4 was located in the left hemisphere with its maximum in caudal parts of the STG near the temporoparietal junction. The majority of cluster 4 covered ventral parts of the posterior insula (BA 13). It included foci from 5 experiments, of which two involved passive listening to the musical timbres (Halpern et al., 2004; Wallmark et al., 2018b) and three involved attending sequences with timbre deviations (Janata et al., 2002), Exp. II; Koelsch et al., 2002) or attentive listening to timbre sequences (Janata et al., 2002), Exp. I).

Although subcortical foci were included, the four clusters did not cover subcortical structures.

4 Discussion

4.1 Comparison with the previous studies

The goal of this study was to identify areas of consistent activation during the processing of musical timbre through an ALE meta-analysis. We identified clusters in the bilateral pSTG, SMG, and the right aSTG and anterior insula.

Our results partially confirm expectations based on the literature and the findings from two earlier ALE meta-analyses on musical timbre processing. Our analysis confirms the involvement of the bilateral STG, including primary AC and PT (Janata, 2015) and in the right IPL (Criscuolo et al., 2021), while the involvement of the cerebellum (Janata, 2015) and the IFG (Criscuolo et al., 2021; Wei et al., 2022) could not be confirmed. The cerebellum is primarily involved in timing and rhythmic processing and is currently not thought to be involved in timbre processing (Evers, 2023). The IFG is foremost associated with hierarchical/syntactic processing in music (Musso et al., 2015; Asano et al., 2021), which most studies included in our analysis did not test. Moreover, we identified an additional involvement of bilateral temporoparietal regions, the bilateral posterior insular cortex, and the right anterior insular cortex in musical timbre processing, which was not identified by previous analyses (Janata, 2015; Criscuolo et al., 2021) nor considered by reviews (Town and Bizley, 2013; Alluri and Kadiri, 2019; Wei et al., 2022). While the IPL is central for processing sequential information (Rauschecker, 2014), the posterior and anterior portions of the insula are involved in sound detection, sensory and motor processing, temporal processing and sequencing (Bamiou et al., 2003) and emotional processing (Gu et al., 2013; Koelsch, 2018), respectively. Compared to previous meta-analyses, we included far more contrasts using musical sequences, which possibly led to the pronounced involvement of the IPL and the posterior insula. Also, many of our contrasts used natural music presumably containing richer emotional and sensorimotor information than isolated sounds, which may explain the activations in the right anterior insula (for discussions, see Asano et al., 2022b) and the bilateral posterior insula.

4.2 Timbre processing in the dual streams

We particularly aimed to investigate whether the neuronal correlates of musical timbre processing can be explained in a dual-stream model. Based on the previous research, timbre processing in the dual-stream model can be described as follows (see also Figure 3, italics).

Figure 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Dual-stream model for cortical processing of musical timbre. It shows the right hemisphere. Areas reported by the previous studies on cortical processing of musical timbre are referred to in italic letters. The areas referred to in bold were identified in the present meta-analysis. AF, Arcuate Fasciculus; AG, angular gyrus; CS, central sulcus; EmC, Extreme Capsule; FAT, Frontal Aslant Tract; IFG, inferior frontal gyrus; pAF, posterior segment of the Arcuate Fasciculus; PMC, premotor cortex; PP, planum polare; pre-SMA, pre-supplementary motor area; PT, planum temporale; SLF, Superior Longitudinal Fasciculus; SMA, supplementary motor area; SMG, supramarginal gyrus; STG, superior temporal gyrus; UF, Uncinate Fasciculus.

The ventral stream areas involved in musical timbre processing are regions in the aSTG, anteroventral to the auditory cortex, which are known to mediate the recognition and identification of musical stimuli (Rauschecker, 2014; Alluri and Kadiri, 2019) and have extensive connections to regions in the IFG (Rauschecker and Scott, 2009).

The dorsal stream regions pSTG & PT, IPL and PMC & SMA, and IFG are activated by musical timbre processing in discriminating successive sounds (Warren et al., 2005; Town and Bizley, 2013; Allen et al., 2017), maintaining representations of timbres (Halpern et al., 2004) and processing sound-motor relationships underlying timbre (Margulis et al., 2009; Tsai et al., 2010, 2012; Lévêque and Schön, 2015; Krishnan et al., 2018; Wallmark et al., 2018a,b), and processing structural properties of musical sequences based on timbre (Koelsch et al., 2002; Reiterer et al., 2008), respectively.

The results of our meta-analysis highlight the involvement of the ventral stream areas and some parts of the dorsal stream areas, and complement it with bilateral activations in the insular cortex (see also Figure 3, boldface).

Cluster 1 and 2 involve dorsal stream areas such as the bilateral pSTG (BA 22) and IPL (SMG, BA 40). The experiments that investigate tracking a timbre or timbral deviations in a sequence and the attentive listening to a sequence of timbres contributed to those clusters, suggesting that these areas are involved in sequential processing based on timbre. However, looking into the details, pSTG and IPL seem to have a division of labor in sequence processing. Compared to cluster 1, which has its peak in BA 40 and partly extended into BA 22, a greater number of studies involving isolated sounds contributed to cluster 2, which has its peak in BA 22 and extends partly into BA 40. We suggest that this is because BA 22 is more associated with processing smaller units in sequencing, such as segregating incoming auditory information into spectrotemporal patterns that correspond to distinct perceptual units (Griffiths and Warren, 2002), while the IPL is more associated with processing larger units in sequencing because it maintains representations of sounds (Rauschecker, 2011, 2014).

Cluster 3 involves the right aSTG as a ventral stream area. The experiments which investigate the passive listening to isolated musical sounds mainly contributed to this cluster (5/7 contrasts; Menon et al., 2002; Angulo-Perkins et al., 2014; Wallmark et al., 2018a,b; Whitehead and Armony, 2018), suggesting that this area is associated with the processing of timbral features and the extraction of emotional and categorical information based on timbre, because no tonal or rhythmic cues are provided, but only single sounds. Based on timbre-related acoustical features of single musical sounds (e.g., spectral and temporal envelope, or the spectral centroid), listeners can extract stimulus identity and emotion (McAdams et al., 2017; Ogg and Slevc, 2019), which are typical timbre-based functions of the ventral auditory stream along the aSTG (Rauschecker, 2014).

To cluster 4, which had its peak in areas of the left pSTG near the temporoparietal junction, the contrasts of Koelsch et al. (2002), Janata et al. (2002, Exp. II), and Janata et al. (2002, Exp. I) contributed and these can be interpreted as the temporal processing and sequencing based on timbre because the studies involved attending sequences with timbre deviations and attentive listening to timbre sequences. This is consistent with the involvement of the pSTG in sound detection, sound discrimination, and sequencing in general auditory processing (Griffiths and Warren, 2002; Rauschecker and Scott, 2009). The posterodorsal parts of the STG connect to other areas in the posterior temporal lobe and the parietal lobe (Makris et al., 2017; Román et al., 2022), as identified by clusters 1 and 2, that is, other areas of the dorsal stream.

Moreover, clusters 3 and 4 involved the anterior and posterior insula (BA 13), respectively. The involvement of the anterior insula is in line with recent findings from other musical domains that assume the contribution of the insula in music processing in the dual streams (Asano et al., 2022b). Musical timbre may be processed through auditory-motor interactions mediated by the insula, which connects to areas of the motor system and the limbic system because timbre not only conveys the identity of the sound source itself but also the physical and emotional state of the sound source (Wallmark et al., 2018b). Following this line of reasoning, we suggest that the consistent activation of the insula in our analysis could be explained by the sensorimotor and emotional processing of timbre sequences and potentially encoded emotional information.

Our meta-analysis did not find the motor areas of the dorsal stream (PMC/SMA, BA 6/8). A central function of BA 6 and 8 in music processing is linking perception and production (Zatorre et al., 2007). For example, BA 6 and 8 have been proposed to represent pitch sequences as motor patterns (Rauschecker, 2014) and were associated with establishing sound-action associations based on timbre (Wallmark et al., 2018b). In our analysis, two of the contrasts involved the covert recitation of perceived timbres (Tsai et al., 2010, 2012). However, the majority of contrasts did not entail tasks that emphasized processes relating sound to action.

Although our meta-analysis did not find all regions associated with the dorsal and ventral streams, previous clinical findings support the conceptualization of musical timbre processing in the dual streams. For example, Mazzucchi et al. (1982) and Peretz et al. (1994) report that patients with lesions in the middle and anterior temporal lobes (that is, ventral auditory stream areas) suffer from impaired categorization based on timbre, while the discrimination of subsequent sounds based on timbre remained intact. Mazzoni et al. (1993), on the other hand, describe a patient with lesions in posterior temporal and inferior parietal cortices (that is, areas of the dorsal auditory stream) suffering from impaired discrimination of sequences of timbres, with intact identification of sound source categories (instruments) based on timbre. We thus suggest that the dual stream model provides a useful framework for future research on musical timbre processing.

4.3 Further implications: domain-specificity vs. -generality?

We suggested that musical timbre processing relies on areas and networks that can be explained in a dual-stream model. Similar models were proposed for music (Zatorre et al., 2007), including tonal (Musso et al., 2015), and rhythmic (Patel and Iversen, 2014) processing, and general auditory processing (Rauschecker and Scott, 2009). Recently, it was argued that cognitive systems like music can be understood as specialized ways of using domain general information processing operations performed by regions or circuits (Asano et al., 2022a). Following this view, musical timbre and other auditory processing could rely on shared neural bases implemented in the dual streams, which may be embedded in different networks including sub-regions specialized during development and/or evolution for musical timbre processing. In this case, we should see similarity between the neuronal correlates of musical timbre processing and of other auditory functions, such as general music, rhythmic, tonal, and general auditory processing. At the same time, there should be some specialization for musical timbre processing within the dual streams.

Comparison between our meta-analysis and other meta-analyses and reviews reveals that musical timbre processing indeed involves brain areas similar to music processing in general, but also tonal and rhythmic processing and general auditory processing, yet often differs concerning the involved sub-regions.

Similarities between the brain regions involved in music processing in general as indicated by two meta-analyses on passive music listening (Gordon et al., 2018; Pando-Naude et al., 2021) and musical timbre processing concern the bilateral HG and pSTG/PT, IPL, and the aSTG. This similarity between musical timbre processing and passive music listening may have resulted from the similarity of stimulus features. For example, the results of the two meta-analyses could reflect cortical correlates of processing perceived timbre because most of included studies used stimuli with complex musical timbres (e.g., musical instrument sounds).

The brain regions activated in musical rhythm processing as indicated by a meta-analysis (Heard and Lee, 2020) and timbre processing are the bilateral pSTG and the bilateral SMG, which may result from similar types of processing such as complex sequence processing (Rauschecker and Scott, 2009; Rauschecker, 2014). Within the SMG, however, rhythmic processing involves postero-dorsal areas bordering the AG, whereas the areas involved in musical timbre processing are located more inferior and anterior. A meta-analysis of beat-based rhythmic processing (Kasdan et al., 2022) and our results do not exhibit any similarities.

Pitch processing, as reported in two recent meta-analyses on tonal processing (Asano, 2019; Asano et al., 2022b), and musical timbre processing both revealed involvement of the right anterior insula, which also might result from a shared type of processing. The right anterior insula is considered to process perceived emotion in various auditory domains (Seydell-Greenwald et al., 2020; Koelsch et al., 2021), and both tonal features and musical timbre are seen as principal ways for conveying musical emotion (Athanasopoulos et al., 2021). Tonal processing, however, involves rostral portions of the right anterior insula and the aSTG, while musical timbre processing is located more posterior and inferior.

Musical timbre processing also involves brain areas similar to general auditory processing. Consistently, all areas indicated in the current meta-analysis apart from the anterior insula are involved in general auditory processing of humans and other primates (Rauschecker and Scott, 2009). These similarities could, however, potentially arise from contributions of general auditory areas to the identified clusters. This is because some of the contrasts used in the analysis, such as rest and other non-auditory baselines, may have included these areas. To further investigate this possibility, we examined the output of the ALE analysis and conducted an additional analysis of the foci in the timbre > rest and timbre > motor/visual control baseline contrasts (see supplementary materials). We examined how much these contrasts contributed to our clusters. The extra analysis finds that these contrasts contributed to, but did not dominate the clusters of our ALE analysis.

In sum (see also Table 3), the areas involved in processing musical timbre identified in the present ALE meta-analysis are similar to the areas and networks recruited for processing information in other musical domains and for general auditory processing. As we identified largely converging neural structures for processing musical timbre and processing in other domains, we presume that musical timbre processing relies on neural resources used by other auditory domains and vice versa. In future research, however, it is still necessary to study if there are some neural specializations for each domain within those shared regions to advance the discussions on domain-specificity vs. – generality (for discussions, see Asano et al., 2022a).

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Areas involved in musical timbre processing (this meta-analysis) and their relation to tonality and rhythm processing, passive music listening, and general auditory processing.

4.4 Limitations

The present study has several limitations.

First, the small number of studies included in this ALE meta-analysis poses limits to the power of this meta-analysis (Eickhoff et al., 2016; Müller et al., 2018). Due to the dearth number of published studies available for the analysis, we were able to slightly exceed the recommended minimum of 17 experiments for a reliable ALE meta-analysis (Eickhoff et al., 2016; Müller et al., 2018).

Second, tasks and contrasts greatly varied (i.e., studies were very heterogenous). That is, we pooled activations across a broad variety of processes based on timbre, resulting in a rather coarse-grained estimate of the neural correlates of musical timbre processing.

Third, we included non-auditory baseline and resting contrasts, which may have introduced activations in the AC due to general auditory processing. In 4.3, we argued that contrasts against rest and visual/motor control tasks contributed to, but did not dominate the clusters of our ALE analysis, which thus likely do not primarily reflect general auditory processing. Still, in future research, once more studies with contrasts examining musical timbre more specifically are available, it is worth doing a further meta-analysis excluding rest contrast to pursue this issue.

Fourth, the involvement of subcortical structures in musical timbre processing was not discussed because our focus was mainly put on the dual stream model of timbre processing. However, areas in the dorsal and ventral streams receive input from and project to subcortical structures, and they together implement several functions (for an overview concerning the general auditory processing, see Rauschecker, 2021). Thus, the possible interplay between the dual streams and subcortical structures for musical timbre processing should be further investigated and research on the subcortical structures should be integrated into our model in the future to provide a more complete picture.

5 Conclusion

This paper investigated the neural correlates of musical timbre processing through an ALE meta-analysis of 18 experiments from 17 neuroimaging studies. Musical timbre processing consistently involved activations in the bilateral auditory cortex (BAs 41, 42), bilateral postero-dorsal regions including the pSTG / PT (BA 22), the SMG (BA 40), and the posterior insula (BA 13), as well as antero-ventral regions in the right aSTG / PP (BA 22), and the right anterior insula (BA 13). Apart from the insula, these areas are associated with the dorsal and ventral auditory streams, providing evidence for dorsal components of cortical processing of musical timbre besides the well-known ventral auditory stream involvement. Thus, we proposed to frame musical timbre processing in a dual-stream model, which serves as a framework for future investigations into the neuronal processing of timbre. Moreover, similar to research on other musical domains, this model is complemented with an involvement of the insula. The results of our meta-analysis also suggest that musical timbre processing may rely on neural resources shared with other musical and non-musical auditory domains. Therefore, future music cognition research should elucidate timbre processing in the dual streams, not least through the examination of the relationships between the neural bases of processing musical timbre and of processing other essential features of music.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author contributions

OB: Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Visualization, Writing – original draft. RA: Validation, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This work was funded by the Acoustics Research Institute (ARI) of the Austrian Academy of Sciences.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Uwe Seifert for valuable discussions on earlier versions of this manuscript.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Footnotes

1. ^The term “timber” was searched on PubMed only because the search for “timber” in APA PsycInfo yielded a plethora of articles related to the study of wood, materials research, to other fields not concerned with music at all, and a researcher named “Timbers.”

References

Allen, E. J., Burton, P. C., Olman, C. A., and Oxenham, A. J. (2017). Representations of pitch and timbre variation in human auditory cortex. J. Neurosci. 37, 1284–1293. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2336-16.2016

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Alluri, V., and Kadiri, S. R. (2019). “Neural correlates of timbre processing” in Timbre: Acoustics, perception, and cognition. eds. K. Siedenburg, C. Saitis, S. McAdams, A. N. Popper, and R. R. Fay (Cham: Springer).

Google Scholar

Alluri, V., and Toiviainen, P. (2010). Exploring perceptual and acoustical correlates of polyphonic timbre. Music. Percept. 27, 223–242. doi: 10.1525/mp.2010.27.3.223

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Alluri, V., Toiviainen, P., Jääskeläinen, I. P., Glerean, E., Sams, M., and Brattico, E. (2012). Large-scale brain networks emerge from dynamic processing of musical timbre, key and rhythm. NeuroImage 59, 3677–3689. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2011.11.019

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Angulo-Perkins, A., Aubé, W., Peretz, I., Barrios, F. A., Armony, J. L., and Concha, L. (2014). Music listening engages specific cortical regions within the temporal lobes: differences between musicians and non-musicians. Cortex 59, 126–137. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2014.07.013

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Asano, R. (2019). Principled explanations in comparative biomusicology-toward a comparative cognitive biology of the human capacities for music and language. [Doctoral Dissertation, University of Cologne]. Available at: https://kups.ub.uni473.koeln.de/9749/

Google Scholar

Asano, R., Boeckx, C., and Fujita, K. (2022a). Moving beyond domain-specific versus domain-general options in cognitive neuroscience. Cortex 154, 259–268. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2022.05.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Asano, R., Boeckx, C., and Seifert, U. (2021). Hierarchical control as a shared neurocognitive mechanism for language and music. Cognition 216:104847. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104847

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Asano, R., Lo, V., and Brown, S. (2022b). The neural basis of tonal processing in music: an ALE Meta-analysis. Music Sci. 5:99. doi: 10.1177/20592043221109958

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Athanasopoulos, G., Eerola, T., Lahdelma, I., and Kaliakatsos-Papakostas, M. (2021). Harmonic organisation conveys both universal and culture-specific cues for emotional expression in music. PLoS One 16, e0244964–e0244917. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0244964

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bamiou, D. E., Musiek, F. E., and Luxon, L. M. (2003). The insula (island of Reil) and its role in auditory processing: literature review. Brain Res. Rev. 42, 143–154. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0173(03)00172-3

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bizley, J. K., and Cohen, Y. E. (2013). The what, where and how of auditory-object perception. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 14, 693–707. doi: 10.1038/nrn3565

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bogert, B., Numminen-Kontti, T., Gold, B., Sams, M., Numminen, J., Burunat, I., et al. (2016). Hidden sources of joy, fear, and sadness: explicit versus implicit neural processing of musical emotions. Neuropsychologia 89, 393–402. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.07.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bowman, C., and Yamauchi, T. (2016). Perceiving categorical emotion in sound: the role of timbre. Psychomusicol. Music Mind Brain 26, 15–25. doi: 10.1037/pmu0000105

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, J. L., Penhune, V. B., and Zatorre, R. J. (2008). Listening to musical rhythms recruits motor regions of the brain. Cereb. Cortex 18, 2844–2854. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn042

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Clark, C. N., Golden, H. L., and Warren, J. D. (2015). “Chapter 34 - acquired amusia” in The human auditory system. eds. M. J. Aminoff, F. Boller, and D. F. Swaab (Neurology: Elsevier). Available at: https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/B9780444626301000342

Google Scholar

Criscuolo, A., Pando-Naude, V., Bonetti, L., Vuust, P., and Brattico, E. (2021). Rediscovering the musician’s brain: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Bio Rxiv. doi: 10.1101/2021.03.12.434473

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Eickhoff, S. B., Bzdok, D., Laird, A. R., Kurth, F., and Fox, P. T. (2012). Activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis revisited. NeuroImage 59, 2349–2361. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.017

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Eickhoff, S. B., Nichols, T. E., Laird, A. R., Hoffstaedter, F., Amunts, K., Fox, P. T., et al. (2016). Behavior, sensitivity, and power of activation likelihood estimation characterized by massive empirical simulation. NeuroImage 137, 70–85. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.04.072

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Evers, S. (2023). The cerebellum in musicology: a narrative review. Cerebellum 23, 1165–1175. doi: 10.1007/s12311-023-01594-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fonov, V., Evans, A. C., Botteron, K., Almli, C. R., McKinstry, R. C., and Collins, D. L. (2011). Unbiased average age-appropriate atlases for pediatric studies. NeuroImage 54, 313–327. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.07.033

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fonov, V., Evans, A. C., McKinstry, R. C., Almli, C. R., and Collins, D. L. (2009). Unbiased nonlinear average age-appropriate brain templates from birth to adulthood. NeuroImage 47:S102. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(09)70884-5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fox, P. T., Laird, A. R., Eickhoff, S. B., Lancaster, J. L., Fox, M., Uecker, A. M., et al. (2013). User manual for GingerALE 2.3.

Google Scholar

Goodale, M. A., and Milner, A. D. (1992). Separate visual pathways for perception and action. Trends Neurosci. 15, 20–25. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(92)90344-8

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Goodchild, M., Wild, J., and McAdams, S. (2019). Exploring emotional responses to orchestral gestures. Music. Sci. 23, 25–49. doi: 10.1177/1029864917704033

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gordon, C. L., Cobb, P. R., and Balasubramaniam, R. (2018). Recruitment of the motor system during music listening: an ALE meta-analysis of fMRI data. PLoS One 13, e0207213–e0207219. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0207213

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Grahn, J. A., Henry, M. J., and McAuley, J. D. (2011). FMRI investigation of cross-modal interactions in beat perception: audition primes vision, but not vice versa. NeuroImage 54, 1231–1243. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2010.09.033

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Green, B., Jääskeläinen, I. P., Sams, M., and Rauschecker, J. P. (2018). Distinct brain areas process novel and repeating tone sequences. Brain Lang. 187, 104–114. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2018.09.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Griffiths, T. D., and Warren, J. D. (2002). The planum temporale as a computational hub. Trends Neurosci. 25, 348–353. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(02)02191-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gu, X., Hof, P. R., Friston, K. J., and Fan, J. (2013). Anterior insular cortex and emotional awareness. J. Comp. Neurol. 521, 3371–3388. doi: 10.1002/cne.23368

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hailstone, J. C., Omar, R., Henley, S. M. D., Frost, C., Kenward, M. G., and Warren, J. D. (2009). It’s not what you play, it’s how you play it: timbre affects perception of emotion in music. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 62, 2141–2155. doi: 10.1080/17470210902765957

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Halpern, A. R., Zatorre, R. J., Bouffard, M., and Johnson, J. A. (2004). Behavioral and neural correlates of perceived and imagined musical timbre. Neuropsychologia 42, 1281–1292. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUROPSYCHOLOGIA.2003.12.017

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Handel, S. (1995). “Timbre perception and auditory object identification” in Hearing. ed. B. C. J. Moore, 425–462.

Google Scholar

Heard, M., and Lee, Y. S. (2020). Shared neural resources of rhythm and syntax: an ALE meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia 137:107284. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2019.107284

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoeschele, M., Cook, R. G., Guillette, L. M., Hahn, A. H., and Sturdy, C. B. (2014). Timbre influences chord discrimination in black-capped chickadees (Poecile atricapillus) but not humans (Homo sapiens). J. Comp. Psychol. 128, 387–401. doi: 10.1037/a0037159

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Holmes, P. A. (2012). An exploration of musical communication through expressive use of timbre: the performer’s perspective. Psychol. Music 40, 301–323. doi: 10.1177/0305735610388898

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Janata, P. (2015). “Neural basis of music perception” in The human auditory system. eds. M. J. Aminoff, F. Boller, and D. F. Swaab (Amsterdam: Elsevier).

Google Scholar

Janata, P., Tillmann, B., and Bharucha, J. J. (2002). Listening to polyphonic music recruits domain-general attention and working memory circuits. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2, 121–140. doi: 10.3758/CABN.2.2.121

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Juslin, P. N., and Lindström, E. (2016). “Emotion in music performance” in The Oxford handbook of music psychology. eds. S. Hallam, I. Cross, and M. H. Thaut (Oxford: Oxford University Press).

Google Scholar

Kasdan, A. V., Burgess, A. N., Pizzagalli, F., Scartozzi, A., Chern, A., Kotz, S. A., et al. (2022). Identifying a brain network for musical rhythm: a functional neuroimaging meta-analysis and systematic review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 136:104588. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2022.104588

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Koelsch, S. (2006). Significance of Broca’s area and ventral premotor cortex for music-syntactic processing. Cortex 42, 518–520. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70390-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Koelsch, S. (2018). Investigating the neural encoding of emotion with music. Neuron 98, 1075–1079. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.04.029

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Koelsch, S. (2019). “Neural basis of music perception: melody, harmony, and timbre” in The Oxford handbook of music and the brain. eds. M. H. Thaut and D. A. Hodges (Oxford: Oxford University Press).

Google Scholar

Koelsch, S., Cheung, V. K. M., Jentschke, S., and Haynes, J.-D. (2021). Neocortical substrates of feelings evoked with music in the ACC, insula, and somatosensory cortex. Sci. Rep. 11:10119. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-89405-y

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Koelsch, S., Gunter, T. C., Cramon, D. Y. V., Zysset, S., Lohmann, G., and Friederici, A. D. (2002). Bach speaks: a cortical “language-network” serves the processing of music. NeuroImage 17, 956–966. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1154

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kotz, S. A., Ravignani, A., and Fitch, W. T. (2018). The evolution of rhythm processing. Trends Cogn. Sci. 22, 896–910. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2018.08.002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Krishnan, S., Lima, C. F., Evans, S., Chen, S., Guldner, S., Yeff, H., et al. (2018). Beatboxers and guitarists engage sensorimotor regions selectively when listening to the instruments they can play. Cereb. Cortex 28, 4063–4079. doi: 10.1093/CERCOR/BHY208

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lacadie, C., Fulbright, R. K., Arora, J., Constable, R., and Papademetris, X. (2008). Brodmann areas defined in MNI space using a new tracing tool in BioImage suite. Proceedings of the 14th Annual Meeting of the Organization for Human Brain Mapping 771.

Google Scholar

Lange, K., and Czernochowski, D. (2013). Does this sound familiar? Effects of timbre change on episodic retrieval of novel melodies. Acta Psychol. 143, 136–145. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.03.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lévêque, Y., and Schön, D. (2015). Modulation of the motor cortex during singing-voice perception. Neuropsychologia 70, 58–63. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.02.012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Makris, N., Zhu, A., Papadimitriou, G. M., Mouradian, P., Ng, I., Scaccianoce, E., et al. (2017). Mapping temporo-parietal and temporo-occipital cortico-cortical connections of the human middle longitudinal fascicle in subject-specific, probabilistic, and stereotaxic Talairach spaces. Brain Imaging Behav. 11, 1258–1277. doi: 10.1007/s11682-016-9589-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Margulis, E. H., Mlsna, L. M., Uppunda, A. K., Parrish, T. B., and Wong, P. C. M. (2009). Selective neurophysiologic responses to music in instrumentalists with different listening biographies. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 267–275. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20503

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mazzoni, M., Moretti, P., Pardossi, L., Vista, M., Muratorio, A., and Puglioli, M. (1993). A case of music imperception. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 56, 56:322. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.56.3.322

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mazzucchi, A., Marchini, C., Budai, R., and Parma, M. (1982). A case of receptive amusia with prominent timbre perception defect. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 45, 644–647. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.45.7.644

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McAdams, S. (2013). “Musical timbre perception” in The psychology of music. ed. D. Deutsch (Amsterdam: Academic Press).

Google Scholar

McAdams, S. (2019). “Timbre as a structuring force in music” in Timbre: Acoustics, perception, and cognition. eds. K. Siedenburg, C. Saitis, S. McAdams, A. N. Popper, and R. R. Fay (Cham: Springer International Publishing).

Google Scholar

McAdams, S., Douglas, C., and Vempala, N. N. (2017). Perception and modeling of affective qualities of musical instrument sounds across pitch registers. Front. Psychol. 8:242203. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00153

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McAdams, S., and Giordano, B. L. (2016). “The perception of musical timbre” in The Oxford handbook of music psychology. eds. S. Hallam, I. Cross, and M. H. Thaut. 2nd edn (Oxford: Oxford University Press).

Google Scholar

Menon, V., Levitin, D. J., Smith, B. K., Lembke, A., Krasnow, B. D., Glazer, D., et al. (2002). Neural correlates of timbre change in harmonic sounds. NeuroImage 17, 1742–1754. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1295

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Müller, V. I., Cieslik, E. C., Laird, A. R., Fox, P. T., Radua, J., Mataix-Cols, D., et al. (2018). Ten simple rules for neuroimaging meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 84, 151–161. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.11.012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Musso, M., Weiller, C., Horn, A., Glauche, V., Umarova, R., Hennig, J., et al. (2015). A single dual-stream framework for syntactic computations in music and language. NeuroImage 117, 267–283. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.05.020

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ogg, M., and Slevc, L. R. (2019). Acoustic correlates of auditory object and event perception: speakers, musical timbres, and environmental sounds. Front. Psychol. 10:450659. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01594

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Osnes, B., Hugdahl, K., Hjelmervik, H., and Specht, K. (2012). Stimulus expectancy modulates inferior frontal gyrus and premotor cortex activity in auditory perception. Brain Lang. 121, 65–69. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2012.02.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pando-Naude, V., Patyczek, A., Bonetti, L., and Vuust, P. (2021). An ALE meta-analytic review of top-down and bottom-up processing of music in the brain. Sci. Rep. 11:20813. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-00139-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Patel, A. D. (2008). Music, language, and the brain : Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Patel, A. D., and Iversen, J. R. (2014). The evolutionary neuroscience of musical beat perception: the action simulation for auditory prediction (ASAP) hypothesis. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 8:57. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2014.00057

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Peretz, I., Kolinsky, R., Tramo, M., Labrecque, R., Hublet, C., Demeurisse, G., et al. (1994). Functional dissociations following bilateral lesions of auditory cortex. Brain 117, 1283–1301. doi: 10.1093/brain/117.6.1283

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Platel, H., Price, C., Baron, J. C., Wise, R., Lambert, J., Frackowiak, R. S. J., et al. (1997). The structural components of music perception. A functional anatomical study. Brain 120, 229–243. doi: 10.1093/BRAIN/120.2.229

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Radvansky, G. A., Fleming, K. J., and Simmons, J. A. (1995). Timbre reliance in nonmusicians’ and musicians’ memory for melodies. Music. Percept. 13, 127–140. doi: 10.2307/40285691

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rauschecker, J. P. (2011). An expanded role for the dorsal auditory pathway in sensorimotor control and integration. Hear. Res. 271, 16–25. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2010.09.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rauschecker, J. P. (2014). Is there a tape recorder in your head? How the brain stores and retrieves musical melodies. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 8:149. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2014.00149

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rauschecker, J. P. (2021). Central Auditory Processing. Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Neuroscience. Available at: https://oxfordre.com/neuroscience/view/10.1093/acrefore/9780190264086.001.0001/acrefore-9780190264086-e-86

Google Scholar

Rauschecker, J. P., and Scott, S. K. (2009). Maps and streams in the auditory cortex: nonhuman primates illuminate human speech processing. Nat. Neurosci. 12, 718–724. doi: 10.1038/nn.2331

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Reiterer, S., Erb, M., Grodd, W., and Wildgruber, D. (2008). Cerebral processing of timbre and loudness: FMRI evidence for a contribution of Broca’s area to basic auditory discrimination. Brain Imaging Behav. 2, 1–10. doi: 10.1007/S11682-007-9010-3

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Román, C., Hernández, C., Figueroa, M., Houenou, J., Poupon, C., Mangin, J. F., et al. (2022). Superficial white matter bundle atlas based on hierarchical fiber clustering over probabilistic tractography data. NeuroImage 262:119550. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2022.119550

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Samson, S., Zatorre, R. J., and Ramsay, J. O. (2002). Deficits of musical timbre perception after unilateral temporal-lobe lesion revealed with multidimensional scaling. Brain 125, 511–523. doi: 10.1093/brain/awf051

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schellenberg, E. G., and Habashi, P. (2015). Remembering the melody and timbre, forgetting the key and tempo. Mem. Cogn. 43, 1021–1031. doi: 10.3758/s13421-015-0519-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Seydell-Greenwald, A., Chambers, C. E., Ferrara, K., and Newport, E. L. (2020). What you say versus how you say it: comparing sentence comprehension and emotional prosody processing using fMRI. NeuroImage 209:116509. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116509

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Siedenburg, K., Mativetsky, S., and McAdams, S. (2016). Auditory and verbal memory in north Indian tabla drumming. Psychomusicol. Music Mind Brain 26, 327–336. doi: 10.1037/pmu0000163

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sihvonen, A. J., Sammler, D., Ripollés, P., Leo, V., Rodríguez-Fornells, A., Soinila, S., et al. (2022). Right ventral stream damage underlies both poststroke aprosodia and amusia. Eur. J. Neurol. 29, 873–882. doi: 10.1111/ene.15148

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stewart, L., Overath, T., Warren, J. D., Foxton, J. M., and Griffiths, T. D. (2008). fMRI evidence for a cortical hierarchy of pitch pattern processing. PLoS One 3, e1470–e1476. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0001470

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stowell, D., and Plumbley, M. D. (2008). “Characteristics of the beatboxing vocal style” in Technical report C4DM-TR-08-01; Centre for Digital Music Department of electronic engineering queen Mary (University of London).

Google Scholar

Sturm, W., Schnitker, R., Grande, M., Huber, W., and Willmes, K. (2011). Common networks for selective auditory attention for sounds and words? An fMRI study with implications for attention rehabilitation. Restor. Neurol. Neurosci. 29, 73–83. doi: 10.3233/RNN-2011-0569

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Talairach, J., and Tournoux, P. (1988). Co-planar stereotaxic atlas of the human brain Stuttgart: Thieme.

Google Scholar

Tervaniemi, M., Szameitat, A. J., Kruck, S., Schröger, E., Alter, K., De Baene, W., et al. (2006). From air oscillations to music and speech: functional magnetic resonance imaging evidence for fine-tuned neural networks in audition. J. Neurosci. 26, 8647–8652. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0995-06.2006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tillmann, B., and McAdams, S. (2004). Implicit learning of musical timbre sequences: statistical regularities confronted with acoustical (dis)similarities. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 30, 1131–1142. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.30.5.1131

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Toiviainen, P., Alluri, V., Brattico, E., Wallentin, M., and Vuust, P. (2014). Capturing the musical brain with lasso: dynamic decoding of musical features from fMRI data. NeuroImage 88, 170–180. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2013.11.017

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Town, S., and Bizley, J. (2013). Neural and behavioral investigations into timbre perception. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 7:88. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2013.00088

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Trainor, L. J., Wu, L., and Tsang, C. D. (2004). Long-term memory for music: infants remember tempo and timbre. Dev. Sci. 7, 289–296. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00348.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsai, C. G., Chen, C. C., Chou, T. L., and Chen, J. H. (2010). Neural mechanisms involved in the oral representation of percussion music: an fMRI study. Brain Cogn. 74, 123–131. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2010.07.008

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsai, C. G., Fan, L. Y., Lee, S. H., Chen, J. H., and Chou, T. L. (2012). Specialization of the posterior temporal lobes for audio-motor processing - evidence from a functional magnetic resonance imaging study of skilled drummers. Eur. J. Neurosci. 35, 634–643. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2012.07996.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Turkeltaub, P. E., Eden, G. F., Jones, K. M., and Zeffiro, T. A. (2002). Meta-analysis of the functional neuroanatomy of single-word Reading: method and validation. NeuroImage 16, 765–780. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1131

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wagner, B., and Hoeschele, M. (2022). The links between pitch, timbre, musicality, and social bonding from cross-species research. Comp. Cogn. Behav. Rev. 17, 13–32. doi: 10.3819/CCBR.2022.170002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wallmark, Z., Deblieck, C., and Iacoboni, M. (2018a). Neurophysiological effects of trait empathy in music listening. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 12:66. doi: 10.3389/FNBEH.2018.00066

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wallmark, Z., Iacoboni, M., Deblieck, C., and Kendall, R. A. (2018b). Embodied listening and timbre: perceptual, acoustical, and neural correlates. Music. Percept. 35, 332–363. doi: 10.1525/MP.2018.35.3.332

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Warren, J. D., Jennings, A. R., and Griffiths, T. D. (2005). Analysis of the spectral envelope of sounds by the human brain. NeuroImage 24, 1052–1057. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.10.031

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wei, Y., Gan, L., and Huang, X. (2022). A review of research on the Neurocognition for timbre perception. Front. Psychol. 13:869475. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.869475

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Whitehead, J. C., and Armony, J. L. (2018). Singing in the brain: neural representation of music and voice as revealed by fMRI. Hum. Brain Mapp. 39, 4913–4924. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24333

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zatorre, R. J., Chen, J. L., and Penhune, V. B. (2007). When the brain plays music: auditory–motor interactions in music perception and production. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 8, 547–558. doi: 10.1038/nrn2152

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: timbre, ALE meta-analysis, dual-stream model, music cognition, cortical processing

Citation: Bellmann OT and Asano R (2024) Neural correlates of musical timbre: an ALE meta-analysis of neuroimaging data. Front. Neurosci. 18:1373232. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2024.1373232

Received: 19 January 2024; Accepted: 29 May 2024;
Published: 17 June 2024.

Edited by:

Dan Zhang, Tsinghua University, China

Reviewed by:

Zachary Wallmark, University of Oregon, United States
Xing Tian, New York University Shanghai, China
Xiaochen Zhang, Shanghai Jiao Tong University, China

Copyright © 2024 Bellmann and Asano. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Oliver Tab Bellmann, olivertab.bellmann@oeaw.ac.at

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.