Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurosci., 23 June 2022
Sec. Neural Technology
This article is part of the Research Topic Smart Nano-Architectures for Neuroscience and Neuroengineering: From Properties to Applications View all 5 articles

Probing Neuro-Endocrine Interactions Through Remote Magnetothermal Adrenal Stimulation

\r\nLisa Y. Maeng&#x;&#x;Lisa Y. Maeng1†‡Dekel Rosenfeld&#x;&#x;Dekel Rosenfeld2†‡Gregory J. SimandlGregory J. Simandl3Florian Koehler,Florian Koehler2,6Alexander W. Senko,Alexander W. Senko2,4Junsang Moon,Junsang Moon2,4Georgios Varnavides,Georgios Varnavides2,4Maria F. MurilloMaria F. Murillo1Adriano E. Reimer,Adriano E. Reimer1,3Aaron WaldAaron Wald3Polina Anikeeva,,*
Polina Anikeeva2,4,5*§Alik S. Widge,*
Alik S. Widge1,3*§
  • 1Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School, Charlestown, MA, United States
  • 2Research Laboratory of Electronics and McGovern Institute for Brain Research, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA, United States
  • 3Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, United States
  • 4Department of Materials Science and Engineering, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA, United States
  • 5Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA, United States
  • 6Department of Electrical Engineering and Computer Science, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA, United States

Exposure to stressful or traumatic stimuli may alter hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis and sympathoadrenal-medullary (SAM) reactivity. This altered reactivity may be a component or cause of mental illnesses. Dissecting these mechanisms requires tools to reliably probe HPA and SAM function, particularly the adrenal component, with temporal precision. We previously demonstrated magnetic nanoparticle (MNP) technology to remotely trigger adrenal hormone release by activating thermally sensitive ion channels. Here, we applied adrenal magnetothermal stimulation to probe stress-induced HPA axis and SAM changes. MNP and control nanoparticles were injected into the adrenal glands of outbred rats subjected to a tone-shock conditioning/extinction/recall paradigm. We measured MNP-triggered adrenal release before and after conditioning through physiologic (heart rate) and serum (epinephrine, corticosterone) markers. Aversive conditioning altered adrenal function, reducing corticosterone and blunting heart rate increases post-conditioning. MNP-based organ stimulation provides a novel approach to probing the function of SAM, HPA, and other neuro-endocrine axes and could help elucidate changes across stress and disease models.

Introduction

Multiple psychiatric disorders, particularly stress and trauma-related conditions, are associated with dysregulation in the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis and sympatho-adrenomedullary (SAM) system (Yehuda et al., 1992; Liberzon et al., 1999; de Kloet et al., 2005; McEwen, 2005; Heim et al., 2018; Iob et al., 2020), which mediates homeostatic stress responses. Systemically-released adrenal stress hormones, cortisol (CORT; corticosterone in rodents) and epinephrine (E), are key elements in these pathways. During exposure to a stressor, the immediate SAM response is driven by the hypothalamus, which stimulates the sympathetic nervous system that then sends the signals to the adrenal medulla to release epinephrine. This “fight or flight” response is short-lived compared to HPA activity (Wong et al., 2012). Once the HPA axis is activated, homeostasis is maintained through negative feedback to prevent overstimulation. Circulating adrenal hormones reduce central secretion of their triggers such as corticotropin-releasing hormone (CRH) or adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH) (Ulrich-Lai and Herman, 2009). Disruption of these homeostatic loops and/or persistent SAM activation, often by prolonged stress, is commonly found in depression and post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) (Yehuda et al., 1991; de Kloet et al., 2005, 2006; Dwyer et al., 2020). The consequences of chronic stress exposure or HPA/SAM dysregulation are mixed, with some studies reporting hyper-responsivity (increased CORT release), and others reporting hypo-responsivity, or a blunted CORT response (Pitman and Orr, 1990; Nemeroff, 1996; Yehuda, 2006), or no difference (Klaassens et al., 2012). These inconsistencies may reflect individual differences in stress sensitivity and highlight the need for a more nuanced understanding of adrenal function under stress. Specifically, methods are needed for probing the capacity for adrenal release and feedback adaptation at multiple points along the stress and recovery trajectory.

Circulating adrenal hormones also affect learning. Memories formed during high emotional arousal can persist and be reactivated more efficiently than memories formed during low arousal (Cahill and McGaugh, 1998). These memory-enhancing effects can be adaptive or problematic, depending on the specific memory and its degree of generalization. For instance, trauma-related disorders involve formation of extinction-resistant emotional memories that lead to pathological avoidance habits (American Psychiatric Association [APA], 2013). There is great interest in finding ways to alter extinction learning processes to oppose these traumatic memories (Kida, 2019). Pre-clinical studies have suggested timed brain stimulation (Milad and Quirk, 2002; Vidal-Gonzalez et al., 2006; Bukalo et al., 2015; Likhtik and Paz, 2015), glutamatergic agonists (Norberg et al., 2008; La Buissonnière-Ariza et al., 2020), and timed increases in adrenal hormones (de Kloet et al., 2005, 2006; McEwen, 2005; Klaassens et al., 2012; Iob et al., 2020) as potential strategies for augmenting extinction. Invasive brain stimulation presents a challenge to routine clinical practice, and pharmacologic strategies have shown limited effectiveness in formal trials (Storch et al., 2007; Litz et al., 2012; Ressler, 2020). Part of the challenge is that manipulations may need to precisely coincide with the formation of extinction memories (Milad and Quirk, 2002). This is the basis of a recently-approved brain stimulation treatment for obsessive-compulsive disorder (Carmi et al., 2019). Methods for precisely timed adrenal release would enable pre-clinical studies of HPA and SAM effects on extinction learning.

We recently demonstrated an approach for temporally precise adrenal hormone control: direct, wireless adrenal gland stimulation (Rosenfeld et al., 2020). Biocompatible, non-toxic magnetic nanoparticles (MNPs) composed of iron oxide can be injected into adrenal glands (or almost any peripheral organ). In humans or larger animal models, that injection could be performed under X-ray, magnetic resonance, or ultrasound guidance, i.e., as a minimally invasive procedure. In the presence of alternating magnetic fields (AMFs), the MNPs dissipate heat, opening endogenous thermosensitive calcium-permeable ion channels from the transient receptor potential (TRP) family and depolarizing electrogenic cells (Figure 1A). This approach increases circulating epinephrine and corticosterone in rats (Rosenfeld et al., 2020), controlled by calcium influx into adrenal cells. This technology offers advantages over other means of probing peripheral organ function. It permits access to tissues where chronic indwelling hardware (e.g., electrodes, catheters) may be difficult to implant or secure. It can be applied without a tether, implying potential use in multi-animal assays such as social interaction. Magnetic fields readily penetrate deep into tissue, and using magnetic nanomaterials as transducers enables spatially restricted stimulation. This contrasts with optical and ultrasonic approaches, where resolution and penetration depth are inversely correlated (Yizhar et al., 2011; Bystritsky and Korb, 2015; Niu et al., 2018; Christiansen et al., 2019; Fekete et al., 2020). Magnetic activation has advantages over other forms of hardware-free control such as chemogenetics, as it permits tight temporal control over organ stimulation (Shahriari et al., 2020).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Experimental design. (A) When an alternating magnetic field (AMF) is applied, the magnetic nanoparticles dissipate heat that causes the opening of heat-sensitive calcium-permeable ion channels (TRPV1 receptors). The calcium influx causes E and CORT release from adrenal cells. (B) Schematic diagram of the procedures that were performed in order. 1. Adrenal MNP injections. 2. Heart rate monitoring before and during magnetothermal stimulation. 3. Aversive conditioning and extinction. 4. Serum collections during magnetothermal stimulation for hormone analyses. The experimental timeline was as follows: After adrenal MNP injections, heart rate monitoring took place during MNP stimulation for 2 days (days 1 and 2) before the 3-day behavioral testing (days 3–5). Five days later, blood was collected for serum hormone (E and CORT) measurements (days 10 and 11). (C–F) Adrenal injection specifications and histology. (C) The specific loss power (SLP) of MNPs at a frequency of 624 kHz was estimated for two amplitudes (13 and 14 kA/m) that were measured within the coil. (D) Finite element modeling of temperature increases at 2 small-volume MNP injection sites within the adrenal gland at 20-s, 40-s, and 60-s of AMF applied. Scale bar = 1 mm. (E) Representative mosaic of an adrenal gland section with two MNP injections (two black holes). Scale bar = 500 μm. (F) A map of MNP and WNP (control) injection sites in the adrenal gland across all animals (n = 16).

As adrenal hormones have been shown to enhance extinction behavior and heart rate, we expected that using MNP technology to stimulate adrenal hormone release would elicit similar enhancements. In this study, we demonstrate this technology’s ability to probe HPA axis/SAM function at a specific timepoint and modulate behavior. We show that an acute stressor (threat conditioning) alters adrenal function and provide pilot evidence that MNP-activated timed adrenal hormone release can facilitate explorations of stress-related physiological and behavioral responses.

Materials and Methods

Nanoparticle Synthesis and Characterization

Iron oxide nanoparticles (NPs), either magnetic (active MNP) or non-magnetic wüstite (control NP) were synthesized by altering the solvent ratios during synthesis. First, we mixed sodium oleate (95%, TCI America) and 30 mmol of FeCl3⋅6H2O (99%, Acros Organics), heated to reflux (60°C) in a solvent mixture of hexane, ethanol and ddH2O for 1 h under N2. The mixture was heated to 110°C and dried overnight on a hotplate. MNPs were prepared by degassing the iron-oleate mixture at 90°C for 1 h in 2:1 (volume ratio) of 1-octadecene (90%, 10 ml) and dibenzyl ether (98%, 5 ml) and heated to 200°C under N2. Control NPs were prepared similarly but with 20 ml of 1-octadecene as solvent. Mixtures were then heated to 200°C under N2 and then to reflux at ∼325°C for 30 min. Both types of particles were 20–22 nm in diameter. They underwent surface functionalization using poly(ethylene glycol) grafted with poly(maleic anhydride-alt-1-octadecene) (Rosenfeld et al., 2020). NP concentration was measured using inductively coupled plasma-optical emission spectroscopy (ICP-OES,Agilent 5100 DVD), where in vivo injection concentration was ∼40 mg[Fe]/ml. For estimating the specific loss power (SLP, heating efficiency per gram of iron) of the NPs based on the dynamic hysteresis model (Christiansen et al., 2014), we used a similar calorimetry method described previously in our work (Chen et al., 2013). An optical fiber probe (Omega HHTFO-101) was used to measure temperature change in the ferrofluid solution inside a 7.5 mm gap of a toroidal ferromagnetic core that is driven by a custom-built series resonant circuit generating AMF with frequency f = 515 kHz and amplitude range of H0 = 13–15 kA/m. The SLP of MNPs in those conditions was estimated by 880 ± 38 W/g[Fe] (mean ± standard deviation, std.) while the same conditions applied to control NPs yield a negligible SLP of 8 ± 4 W/g[Fe] (Rosenfeld et al., 2020).

Finite Element Modeling

To estimate the needed volume and concentration of MNPs and the number of injection sites for the in vivo experiments that will allow the MNPs to reach the threshold temperature of TRPV1 ion channel (42°C), we created a Finite Element Modeling (FEM) of heat distribution based on the gland dimensions, blood perfusion and fat layer surrounding the gland. The model was developed using Pennes’ bio-heat equation (Rosenfeld et al., 2020). We modified the previous model to fit the current experiment (Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Parameters for finite element modeling (FEM).

The chosen dimensions for the glands were modeled as an ellipsoid of 5.5 × 2.2 × 2.2 mm3 (with additional fat layer) and the following equations:

ρACATt=KA2T+ρbCbwb(T-Tb)+PA{adrenaltissue}
PFCFTt=KA2T+ρbCbwb(T-Tb)+PF{fattissue}

Subjects

We tested magnetothermal stimulation in male Long Evans rats weighing 250–300 g (Charles River, Wilmington, MA). The current study extends previous findings (Rosenfeld et al., 2020) by investigating this MNP technology’s ability to modulate behavior. Only males were used to maintain a comparable sample to that of the previous study. As sex differences have been reported (and reviewed) in aversive and extinction behaviors (Milad et al., 2009; Lebron-Milad et al., 2012; Maeng and Milad, 2015; Velasco et al., 2019), it will be critical that future studies examine whether MNP effects differ in females.

For this methodological proof of concept, we did not plan a fully-powered study but estimated the number of animals per group based on expected attrition. Rather, we planned for 20 animals per group (active MNP vs. control NP). Of these 40 initial rats, 28 completed the full protocol and were retained in at least some aspects of the analysis; Figure 2 diagrams the reasons for attrition per analysis. Rats were maintained on a 12 h light/dark cycle and ad libitum chow and water. Experiments were performed during the light phase of the light/dark cycle. All procedures were approved by the Subcommittee on Research Animal Care at the Massachusetts General Hospital (an Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Summary chart of animal exclusions by analysis. A total of 28 rats were used in this study. Of these 28 animals, a number of animals were excluded from each of the heart rate, serum, and behavioral analyses for the reasons described in the diagram.

Adrenal Magnetic Nanoparticle Injections

All rats were injected with intraperitoneal (i.p.) atropine (0.01 mg/kg) for respiratory support. About 20 min after injection, the rats were placed into an induction chamber and anesthetized with isoflurane. After induction, animals were injected with subcutaneous (s.c.) narcotic pain reliever buprenorphine (0.05 mg/kg), an anti-inflammatory non-narcotic pain reliever flunixin meglumine (2.5 mg/kg, s.c.), and intramuscular (i.m.) antibiotic enrofloxacin (5 mg/kg). To minimize the surgical burden each animal experienced, only the left adrenal gland was injected with MNPs or control NPs. The left adrenal gland was targeted because it is more easily accessible and has a larger medullary volume, with comparable secretory function to the right adrenal gland (De Nardi et al., 2017). The incision site was shaved and cleansed with alternating Betadine and ethanol. After the incision was made, the left adrenal gland was visualized and isolated (Rosenfeld et al., 2020). A Hamilton syringe containing nanoparticles was connected to a stereotaxic infusion pump and injected 1 μl (0.1 μl/min) into the gland in at least two different locations for a total volume of 2 μl, targeting both the adrenal medulla and cortex. The needle was kept inside the gland for 10 min post-injection to prevent leakage of the ferrofluid solution. Surgeries were performed by LYM, MFM, and DR. LYM and DR were aware of which injections contained control NP vs. MNP, but MFM was blinded. The data analysis was similarly performed blind to condition (see below). Rats recovered from surgery for at least 7 days before entering the experiment.

Magnetothermal Stimulation

We measured HPA axis function by acute magnetothermal stimulation of awake, restrained rats (Figure 1B). Rats were placed inside a plastic restraint tube, to which they had all been habituated. The habituation procedure (multiple sessions of exposure to the tube and coil apparatuses) was important to minimize stress due to handling and the procedure used for the magnetothermal stimulation. The tube was then placed inside the core of an in vivo AMF stimulation apparatus, which was a replica of that used in Rosenfeld et al. (2020). The coil was driven by a custom resonant circuit and actively water cooled. The SLP (heating efficiency) of MNPs at the frequency of 624 kHz was estimated for two amplitudes that were measured within the coil (13–14 kA/m; Figure 1C) and were matched to reach an SLP of at least 600 W/g in MNPs as in the prior study (Rosenfeld et al., 2020). Control NPs under the same conditions yielded a negligible SLP (Rosenfeld et al., 2020). For all stimulation procedures, rats received 1 min of active stimulation (longer than the 40 s used in the previous study due to the rats’ larger/older status). Stimulation parameters were selected based on the amount of heat and its spread in the adrenal gland (Figure 1D), according to the FEM.

Heart Rate Monitoring

To verify that adrenal hormone (particularly epinephrine) release occurred in response to MNP heating, we measured stimulation-induced heart rate changes. A pulse oximetry sensor was attached to the paw after each animal was restrained, and heart rate (bpm) was recorded before, during, and after stimulation using PhysioSuite (Kent Scientific, Torrington, CT). The experimenter performing stimulation and heart rate procedures (MFM) was blind to each rat’s experimental condition (MNP vs. control NP).

Behavior

To assess the effects of adrenal stimulation on emotionally valenced learning, we employed a tone-shock conditioning/extinction/recall paradigm that we (Maeng et al., 2017b,a) and others (Quirk et al., 2000; Milad and Quirk, 2002) have previously used to test putative learning and memory enhancers. The experimenter (MFM) was blind to each rat’s experimental condition. Similar to prior experiments, behavior included habituation to the apparatus (to minimize novelty or stress effects in response to the environment), tone-shock conditioning, tone-only extinction, and tone-only extinction recall (Figure 1B). The behavioral testing procedure was run across 3 consecutive days. For each phase, an 82 dB tone conditioned stimulus (CS) was presented. On day 1, all rats underwent a habituation phase that consisted of 5 trials of tone CS-alone presentations. Habituation was immediately followed by the aversive conditioning phase. Conditioning consisted of 7 trials of tone CS and shock unconditioned stimulus (US) pairings. In the conditioning trials, each tone culminated with a 0.5 mA foot shock administered by the grid floor in the operant chamber. The extinction phase took place on day 2, consisting of 20 tone-CS alone trials. Immediately prior to extinction, each animal received 1 min of magnetothermal stimulation within the coil described above. Pre- and post-stimulation blood samples were collected 6 and 7 days after extinction. We quantified defensive behavior (freezing) by video analysis (ANY-maze, Stoelting Co., Wood Dale, IL). On day 3, the Recall phase comprised 3 CS-alone trials. During each phase, video recordings were analyzed to assess freezing behavior during the experimental trials. Freezing behavior was analyzed via ANY-maze (Stoelting Co., Wood Dale, IL, United States) post-experimentation using video recordings taken within the operant chamber during each behavioral testing phase (habituation, conditioning, extinction, and recall).

Serum Hormone Quantification

Five days after behavioral testing, we further verified hormone release by lateral tail vein blood collections before and immediately after stimulation. To increase vasodilation of the lateral tail vein and thus blood flow, tails were warmed for 2–4 min with hand warmers. The animals were anesthetized with 0.25 mg/kg ketamine to insert a tail vein catheter, then allowed to recover for blood collection. After the catheter was implanted, it was flushed with saline and blocked with heparin (0.2 ml). Blood samples were drawn after removing the heparin block. After at least an hour of sitting at room temperature for the blood to clot, the samples were centrifuged at 5,000 rpm for 5 min at room temperature. We chose to perform this collection only after the conditioning and extinction paradigm because we had already verified pre-conditioning hormone release properties (Rosenfeld et al., 2020). Methodological limitations surrounding repeated blood sampling, such as the brief stimulation time window, unintended stress effects on subsequent behavioral measures, etc., did not allow us to perform repeated blood sample collections during the behavioral test phases. Serum hormone levels were quantified via ELISA (MyBioSource, Epinephrine: MBS264776, Corticosterone: MBS761865) and a plate reader to determine the optical density. For each tested hormone, the absolute values were calculated according to a calibration curve, obtained separately for each tested plate.

Histology and Image Analysis of Adrenal Glands

Following the experiment, rats were sacrificed via injections of pentobarbital/phenytoin at 150 mg/kg (Beuthanasia-D C IIIN, Merck Animal Health, Patterson Veterinary, Devens, MA) and transcardial perfusion with 0.9% saline and 4% paraformaldehyde at a rate of about 21 ml/min. To confirm the nanoparticle injection locations, the adrenal glands in agarose were sectioned using a vibratome with a thickness of 40 μm, mounted on glass slides, and imaged in a laser scanning confocal microscope Fluoview FV1000, Olympus). The percentage of nanoparticle coverage of each adrenal sub-region was determined from mosaic scans of the entire adrenal slice via image analysis (Rosenfeld et al., 2020). Post-processing transformed each adrenal slice to an ellipse with the same semi-axes. This permitted labeling of adrenal sub-regions: medulla, zona glomerulosa (ZG), zona fasciculata (ZF) and zona reticularis (ZR). A map of injection locations was generated across 16 adrenal glands injected with NPs and defined the percentage of area covered with MNPs for each injected gland (Figures 1E,F).

Statistical Analysis

All analyses were performed using RStudio (RStudio Team, 2020; R Core Team, 2021) with packages betareg (Cribari-Neto and Zeileis, 2010), broom (Robinson et al., 2021), fitdistrplus (Delignette-Muller and Dutang, 2015), ggplot2 (Wickham, 2016), ggpubr (Kassambara, 2020), imputeTS (Moritz and Bartz-Beielstein, 2017), plotrix (Lemon, 2006), readxl (Wickham and Bryan, 2019), and tidyverse (Wickham et al., 2019). As noted above, this study was not powered to detect significant between-group differences, and we present parametric statistics primarily to demonstrate possible effect sizes. The complete code for all analyses, with corresponding data files, is available online at https://github.com/tne-lab/magnex. For all analyses, we identified appropriate distributions for statistical testing by visualization of the Cullen-Frey graph in fitdistrplus (Delignette-Muller and Dutang, 2015).

Statistical Analysis – Heart Rate

We analyzed heart rate data before, during, and after stimulation for Days 1 and 2 (pre-conditioning) and Day 4 (post-conditioning) separately, retaining only animals with adequate data and verified NP placement. Due to animal movement and occasional loss of the pulse oximetry signal, heart rate tracings contained gaps. We imputed these missing values using Kalman smoothing (“imputeTS” package, “na_kalman” function), using the default parameters. Post-imputation, data were scaled by dividing each measurement in the time series by the mean pre-stimulation heart rate. Day 1 and 2 data were averaged at each time point for each respective animal. Day 4 data was left separate because measurements were obtained after the conditioning phase and thus were expected to have had different underlying adrenal biology. We retained only animals where we were able to successfully impute missing data. We excluded animals in the active MNP group if the histology showed no particles in the medulla. This left n = 11 control and n = 9 active rats for this analysis. We summed samples within the stimulation time range (300–360 s) to compute the area under the heart rate curve (AUC). These values best fit a gamma distribution, and thus we compared active and control conditions using a generalized linear model with gamma distribution, identity link function and a single independent variable (treatment condition).

Statistical Analysis – Serum

After excluding animals for experimental failures and outlier values, we analyzed the mean of the serum hormone levels across the 2 days of collection. We converted the data to a post-stim/pre-stim ratio, which we compared between active and control animals with a two-sample t-test on the log of the post/pre ratio. Serum analyses assessed epinephrine and corticosterone levels immediately prior to and after stimulation. 9 animals were excluded from analysis due to blood sampling issues, e.g., catheter malfunctions. For the remaining animals, the serum values were best described by a beta distribution. We then excluded animals with outlier pre-stimulation hormone levels, defined by fitting a beta distribution to all pre-stimulation hormone values (corticosterone and epinephrine separately). We excluded the serum samples whose baseline was extremely unlikely, i.e., p < 0.0005 based on the fitted distribution. This method excluded 1 additional animal. Finally, animals without active MNP placement in the medulla for epinephrine, or the cortex for corticosterone release, were also excluded (animals retained for E, control: n = 9, active: n = 8; CORT, control: n = 9, active: n = 9). We then converted the data to a post-stim/pre-stim ratio. These values were consistent with a log-normal distribution for each hormone, hence active and control animals were compared with a two-sample t-test on the log of the post/pre ratio.

Statistical Analysis – Behavior

We quantified freezing behavior across the 3-day paradigm as the percentage of the 30 s CS tone that was spent freezing. The behavioral paradigm consisted of four testing phases: habituation (day 1), conditioning (day 1), extinction (day 2), and recall (day 3). We analyzed freezing with a beta regression model, which is designed for data spanning the 0.00 ≤ x ≤ 1.00 interval. This beta regression analysis was selected because it was a more appropriate fit to the distribution of the data than a repeated measures ANOVA. For each animal, the raw freezing scores were adjusted to span this full range using a min-max normalization [(x - min)/(max - min)]. Normalizations were done separately for each phase of the testing procedure. These normalized data were scaled by dividing by each rat’s individual baseline freezing, defined as the average of the final 3 trials of the habituation phase (Trial 3–5). Finally, we smoothed the normalized trial-to-trial data using a centered moving average with window size 3. Post-normalization, we confirmed that freezing data fit a beta distribution. Data then were analyzed in a beta regression using trial, testing phase, and treatment (fixed effects, including 2-way and 3-way interaction terms) as explanatory variables. Before the analysis, we excluded animals that did not initially condition, and thus that could not have experienced extinction learning. Conditioning failures were determined by visual inspection of the conditioning behavior curve, by an investigator (ASW) who was blind to each rat’s treatment assignment. We also excluded animals in the active MNP group where both cortex and medulla were particle-free. These approaches excluded 5 rats (21.7% of total), with n = 23 rats retained in the analysis (control: n = 13; active: n = 10). For the analysis comparing the final three trials (Trials 18–20) of extinction, we confirmed that these averages followed a normal distribution before applying a t-test.

Results

Adrenal Magnetic Nanoparticle Injection Verification

Histological results verifying the location of the NP adrenal injections are illustrated in Figures 1E,F. Most of the injected NPs were found in the ZR, medulla, and ZF, while none of the NPs were found in the ZG (data tables available on GitHub).

Magnetic Nanoparticle Influences on Heart Rate

Prior to tone-shock conditioning, animals injected with active MNPs showed adrenal engagement in response to an AMF stimulus, evidenced by an increased heart rate during stimulation compared to controls (Figure 3A). The between-group difference did not reach significance in this small sample [t(14) = 0.807, p = 0.433]. Further, aversive conditioning changed adrenal responsivity. After conditioning (but before extinction on day 4), MNP stimulation produced similar changes in heart rate between the control and active MNP animals [t(14) = −0.025, p = 0.98] (Figure 3B). Heart rate rose more slowly in the animals injected with active MNPs, and there was greater variability as stimulation continued (increasing width of error bars specifically during the stimulation period).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Magnetothermal adrenal stimulation effects on heart rate and hormone levels. (A,B) Heart rate measurement during magnetothermal stimulation on (A) days 1 and 2 before conditioning and (B) on day 4 after conditioning. (C,D) Serum hormone analysis. (C) Corticosterone and (D) epinephrine were measured before (pre) and after (post) magnetothermal stimulation. (E,F) Behavioral effects of magnetothermal adrenal stimulation. (E) Percent freezing per trial across the habituation, conditioning, extinction, and recall phases of the behavioral paradigm (controls, n = 13; active, n = 10). (F) Percent freezing averaged across the last three trials of extinction (Trials 18–20) for control and active MNP rats.

Magnetic Nanoparticle Modulation of Adrenal Hormones

After aversive conditioning and extinction, the circulating hormone response to magnetothermal stimulation was also altered. In animals injected with active MNPs, serum CORT and E both decreased from pre- to post-stimulation (Figures 3C,D). The change in CORT reached statistical significance compared to control MNPs [t(16) =−2.2, p = 0.047], whereas the change in E did not [t(14) = −1.8, p = 0.091], although significance should be evaluated with caution given the sample size. Importantly, these are the reverse of the change we previously reported in unconditioned animals (Rosenfeld et al., 2020).

Magnetic Nanoparticle Modulation of Defensive Behavior

Freezing to conditioned tones did not significantly differ between the animals injected with active MNPs and those injected with control non-magnetic NPs in any experimental phase (Figure 3E). There was a non-significant enhancement of extinction learning from adrenal stimulation at the end of extinction [t(20.4) = −0.579, p = 0.569], that did not persist to the recall phase (Figure 3F).

Discussion

We previously demonstrated remote control of adrenal hormone release via magnetically triggered heating of locally injected MNPs (Rosenfeld et al., 2020). Here, we demonstrated the use of this technology to probe the state dependence of HPA axis function. After aversive conditioning, this adrenal stimulation produced no visibly different changes in heart rate, contrary to the HR increases that were previously observed (Rosenfeld et al., 2020). This may represent exhaustion of a readily releasable pool if animals with continuously high adrenergic tone after conditioning were left with little reserve E available for release in response to subsequent stimulation (McCarty et al., 1988). This may offer a model for persistent hyper-arousal in trauma/stress-related disorders and demonstrates the potential role of this technology as a probe of neuro-endocrine interactions. MNP stimulation also decreased serum E and CORT, while in our prior study of non-conditioned animals, the same stimulation increased these hormones at the same time point (Rosenfeld et al., 2020). In addition to not being conditioned, another possible explanation for the hormone and HR differences between the two studies may be the anesthetized state. The animals in the previous study were under isoflurane anesthesia, compared to the awake and behaving state of the animals in the current study. Overall, the reduction in CORT after MNP stimulation aligns with the heart rate results, in that it again represents a blunted response compared to the previous study. These decreases potentially reflect an up-regulation of E/CORT metabolism or feedback inhibition as a result of a repeatedly activated HPA axis during conditioning, which could lead to a net decrease by the post-stimulation measurement (Kannan, 2012). These results further demonstrate that magnetothermally-driven adrenal release can probe aversive learning-related alterations of HPA and SAM function.

Behaviorally, we did not observe a significant difference in freezing, even though circulating adrenal mediators might be expected to improve extinction learning (Merz et al., 2014; Wolf et al., 2016; de Quervain et al., 2019). The present study observed a reduction in pre- to post-stimulation serum CORT levels, suggesting that the lack of a behavioral difference may be driven by a lack of sustained change in circulating mediators, or even by their suppression. The timing of activation/secretion may also be important. Previous studies suggest time-dependent hormone effects on behavior, e.g., hydrocortisone reduced defensive behavior only if given close to the time of training (Merz et al., 2018). Because our technology allows more precise control over hormone release timing compared to commonly used approaches such as a systemic injection or delivery via an osmotic pump, it could permit dissection of this time dependence.

The current approach was limited to adrenal gland activation by triggering heat-sensitive TRP channels, in a relatively non-specific way across the entire organ, in restrained animals. However, the resulting limitations can be overcome. For example, we have activated mechanosensitive ion channels by altering the MNP structure and magnetic field conditions (Gregurec et al., 2020). Moreover, magnetothermal stimulation can also inhibit cell activity by activating heat-sensitive hyperpolarizing channels (Munshi et al., 2018). To note, as biological tissues have negligible magnetic susceptibility in the frequency range of 100 kHz–10 MHz, we can achieve specific magnetothermal modulation at a frequency around 600 kHz without significant risk of general effects of the magnetic field. Controlled injections of MNPs to specific substructures within the adrenal gland, instead of the current multiple-injection approach, could increase specificity and permit the independent release of individual hormones such as androgens and aldosterone. We found a greater percentage of MNPs in the zona reticularis, but we did not measure its androgen hormone product, dehydroepiandrosterone (DHEA), which could be a consideration for future investigations. Similarly, the zona glomerulosa, which did not contain MNPs in this study, could be a target region for investigating the effects of aldosterone, another adrenal hormone implicated in stress responses and depression (Jezova et al., 2019; Izakova et al., 2020). Specificity might also be achieved using a multiplexing approach for controlling different populations of MNPs within the same organ (Moon et al., 2020). While we used restrained animals, the current coil design would permit experiments with freely moving mice (Rao et al., 2019). Freely moving rat experiments are possible, but demand scaling up of the coils and the driving power electronics (Christiansen et al., 2017).

The lack of female inclusion was a limitation of this study, but as mentioned above, deciding to use the same type of animals as in (Rosenfeld et al., 2020) was necessary as an extension of the previous findings. Inclusion of females in future MNP studies is critical as sex hormones such as estrogen, progesterone, and testosterone affect learning processes including extinction (Milad et al., 2010; Merz et al., 2012; Hwang et al., 2015; Hamson et al., 2016; Maeng et al., 2017b,a; Taxier et al., 2020) and are broadly implicated in psychiatrically relevant functions (Andreano and Cahill, 2009; Dwyer et al., 2020; Hwang et al., 2021). In addition to testing in females, this technology could also address the role of sex hormones by generalizing beyond the adrenal gland and including the gonads. Systemic hormone administration in animal studies relies on subcutaneous or intraperitoneal injections, which can be stressful and interfere with the sex hormone effects being investigated. TRP receptors are present in the ovaries, testes, and hypothalamus (Stein et al., 2004; Kunert-Keil et al., 2006; Surkin et al., 2020). While the thermal effects in this study are attributed mainly to TRPV1 activation, verified also in our previous study, other temperature-sensitive ion channels could be potentially activated, for example TRPV3 with a threshold of 31–39°C (Broad et al., 2016). Therefore, the hypothalamic-pituitary-gonadal (HPG) axis could be similarly probed by magnetothermal stimulation.

Beyond their use as probes of peripheral organ function, magnetic nanomaterials have been increasingly recognized as therapies and “theranostic” tools. They are being applied for treating cancer, diagnosing atherosclerosis, delivering drugs, etc. (Williams, 2017; Gul et al., 2019). However, their utility as tools for neuropsychiatric treatment and investigation is less defined. While intracranial or deep brain MNP stimulation has mostly been validated with motor behaviors (Kozielski et al., 2021), these particles may be a valuable tool for creating and/or manipulating models of neuropsychiatric disorders (Rao et al., 2019; Lu et al., 2020). For instance, MNP stimulation of the prelimbic cortex reduced immobility in the forced-swim test and increased sucrose consumption in stressed mice (Lu et al., 2020). Rao et al. (2019) demonstrated the use of MNPs for targeted drug delivery, where MNP-stimulated drug release increased dopamine-mediated social behavior. While these findings illustrate how MNP stimulation can be used to alter behavior via central modulation, the present study suggests a potential for modulation in the periphery, which may be more accessible and translatable.

Magnetothermal or magnetomechanical approaches are not universally useful. Chemogenetics will be simpler wherever temporal precision is not necessary. Very complex arenas will be difficult to instrument with coils for MNP activation, favoring more mature, tethered technologies such as optogenetics. The coils require specialized driving hardware and high-voltage supply lines that may not be available in all laboratory environments. It would similarly be hard to run many animals at once using this paradigm, and pharmacological/chemogenetic tools would be more suited if high throughput is needed. As nanomaterials, MNPs still need to be placed into organs of interest and may degrade, migrate, or be eliminated over time, unlike larger optogenetic fibers (Kolosnjaj-Tabi et al., 2016; Tsoi et al., 2016).

Despite the limitations described above, we have demonstrated the use of MNPs to remotely and more precisely detect and assess alterations within a peripheral stress system. While we observed effects of MNP stimulation on heart rate, serum hormone levels, and behavior that were not in the direction that we hypothesized, our findings did illustrate how MNP stimulation allowed us to identify specific changes in the functioning of a stress system that has been altered by aversive conditioning. Future research could further examine the timing of MNP peripheral stimulation effects on behavior, influences in the brain, and their potential role in the restoration of post-conditioning adrenal function. As the technology becomes more refined and widely available, these on-demand peripheral release approaches could provide valuable new tools for understanding and eventually altering the biology of mental illness beyond the brain.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets presented in this study can be found in online repositories. The names of the repository/repositories and accession number(s) can be found in the article/supplementary material.

Ethics Statement

The animal study was reviewed and approved by the Subcommittee on Research Animal Care at the Massachusetts General Hospital (an Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee).

Author Contributions

ASW and PA conceived and designed the study. MM, DR, and LM ran the behavioral experiment and serum analyses. FK, AS, JM, and GV helped with magnetic stimulation and image analysis. AR and AW performed follow-up analyses. LM and DR wrote the first draft of the manuscript. LM, DR, ASW, and PA wrote sections of the manuscript. ASW and GS performed the statistical analyses. All authors approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was funded in part by the DARPA ElectRx Program (HR0011-15-C-0155 under Dr. D. Weber) to PA and the Bose Research Grant, and the NIH BRAIN Initiative (1R01MH111872) to PA. This work made use of the MIT MRSEC Shared Experimental Facilities under award number DMR-14-19807 from the NSF. DR is a recipient of the MIT-Technion Fellowship. ASW was additionally supported by the MnDRIVE Brain Conditions Initiative.

Conflict of Interest

ASW, PA, and DR are inventors on a patent application for therapeutic uses of controlled adrenal release.

The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

American Psychiatric Association [APA] (2013). Diagnostic And Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th Edn. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

Google Scholar

Andreano, J. M., and Cahill, L. (2009). Sex influences on the neurobiology of learning and memory. Learn. Mem. 16, 248–266. doi: 10.1101/lm.918309

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Broad, L. M., Mogg, A. J., Eberle, E., Tolley, M., Li, D. L., and Knopp, K. L. (2016). TRPV3 in Drug Development. Pharmaceuticals 9:55. doi: 10.3390/ph9030055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bukalo, O., Pinard, C. R., Silverstein, S., Brehm, C., Hartley, N. D., Whittle, N., et al. (2015). Prefrontal inputs to the amygdala instruct fear extinction memory formation. Sci. Adv. 1:e1500251. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.1500251

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bystritsky, A., and Korb, A. S. (2015). A Review of Low-Intensity Transcranial Focused Ultrasound for Clinical Applications. Curr. Behav. Neurosci. Rep. 2, 60–66. doi: 10.1007/s40473-015-0039-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cahill, L., and McGaugh, J. L. (1998). Mechanisms of emotional arousal and lasting declarative memory. Trends Neurosci. 21, 294–299. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(97)01214-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carmi, L., Tendler, A., Bystritsky, A., Hollander, E., Blumberger, D. M., Daskalakis, J., et al. (2019). Efficacy and Safety of Deep Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation for Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder: a Prospective Multicenter Randomized Double-Blind Placebo-Controlled Trial. Am. J. Psychiatry 176, 931–938. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2019.18101180

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, R., Christiansen, M. G., and Anikeeva, P. (2013). Maximizing Hysteretic Losses in Magnetic Ferrite Nanoparticles via Model-Driven Synthesis and Materials Optimization. ACS Nano 7, 8990–9000. doi: 10.1021/nn4035266

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Christiansen, M. G., Howe, C. M., Bono, D. C., Perreault, D. J., and Anikeeva, P. (2017). Practical methods for generating alternating magnetic fields for biomedical research. Rev. Sci. Instrum. 88:084301. doi: 10.1063/1.4999358

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Christiansen, M. G., Senko, A. W., and Anikeeva, P. (2019). Magnetic Strategies for Nervous System Control. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 42, 271–293. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-070918-050241

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Christiansen, M. G., Senko, A. W., Chen, R., Romero, G., and Anikeeva, P. (2014). Magnetically multiplexed heating of single domain nanoparticles. Appl. Phys. Lett. 104:213103. doi: 10.1063/1.4879842

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cribari-Neto, F., and Zeileis, A. (2010). Beta regression in R. J. Stat. Softw. 34, 1–24. doi: 10.18637/jss.v034.i02

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Kloet, C. S., Vermetten, E., Geuze, E., Kavelaars, A., Heijnen, C. J., and Westenberg, H. G. M. (2006). Assessment of HPA-axis function in posttraumatic stress disorder: pharmacological and non-pharmacological challenge tests, a review. J. Psychiatr. Res. 40, 550–567. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2005.08.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Kloet, E. R., Joëls, M., and Holsboer, F. (2005). Stress and the brain: from adaptation to disease. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 6, 463–475. doi: 10.1038/nrn1683

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Nardi, F., Lefort, C., Bréard, D., Richomme, P., Legros, C., and Guérineau, N. C. (2017). Monitoring the Secretory Behavior of the Rat Adrenal Medulla by High-Performance Liquid Chromatography-Based Catecholamine Assay from Slice Supernatants. Front. Endocrinol. 8:248. doi: 10.3389/fendo.2017.00248

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Quervain, D., Wolf, O. T., and Roozendaal, B. (2019). Glucocorticoid-induced enhancement of extinction—from animal models to clinical trials. Psychopharmacology 236, 183–199. doi: 10.1007/s00213-018-5116-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delignette-Muller, M. L., and Dutang, C. (2015). fitdistrplus: an R package for fitting distributions. J. Stat. Softw. 64, 1–34.

Google Scholar

Dwyer, J. B., Aftab, A., Radhakrishnan, R., Widge, A., Rodriguez, C. I., Carpenter, L. L., et al. (2020). Hormonal Treatments for Major Depressive Disorder: State of the Art. Am. J. Psychiatry 177, 686–705. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2020.19080848

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fekete, Z., Horváth, ÁC., and Zátonyi, A. (2020). Infrared neuromodulation:a neuroengineering perspective. J. Neural Eng. 17:051003. doi: 10.1088/1741-2552/abb3b2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gregurec, D., Senko, A. W., Chuvilin, A., Reddy, P. D., Sankararaman, A., Rosenfeld, D., et al. (2020). Magnetic Vortex Nanodiscs Enable Remote Magnetomechanical Neural Stimulation. ACS Nano 14, 8036–8045. doi: 10.1021/acsnano.0c00562

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gul, S., Khan, S. B., Rehman, I. U., Khan, M. A., and Khan, M. I. (2019). A Comprehensive Review of Magnetic Nanomaterials Modern Day Theranostics. Front. Mater. 6:179. doi: 10.3389/fmats.2019.00179

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamson, D. K., Roes, M. M., and Galea, L. A. M. (2016). Sex Hormones and Cognition: neuroendocrine Influences on Memory and Learning. Compr. Physiol. 6, 1295–1337. doi: 10.1002/cphy.c150031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heim, C., Schultebraucks, K., Marmar, C. R., and Nemeroff, C. B. (2018). “Neurobiological pathways involved in fear, stress, and PTSD,” in Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder, eds C. B. Nemeroff and C. R. Marmar (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 331–351.

Google Scholar

Hwang, M. J., Zsido, R. G., Song, H., Pace-Schott, E. F., Miller, K. K., Lebron-Milad, K., et al. (2015). Contribution of estradiol levels and hormonal contraceptives to sex differences within the fear network during fear conditioning and extinction. BMC Psychiatry 15:295. doi: 10.1186/s12888-015-0673-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hwang, W. J., Lee, T. Y., Kim, N. S., and Kwon, J. S. (2021). The Role of Estrogen Receptors and Their Signaling across Psychiatric Disorders. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 22:373. doi: 10.3390/ijms22010373

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iob, E., Kirschbaum, C., and Steptoe, A. (2020). Persistent depressive symptoms, HPA-axis hyperactivity, and inflammation: the role of cognitive-affective and somatic symptoms. Mol. Psychiatry 25, 1130–1140. doi: 10.1038/s41380-019-0501-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Izakova, L., Hlavacova, N., Segeda, V., Kapsdorfer, D., Morovicsova, E., and Jezova, D. (2020). Salivary Aldosterone, Cortisol, and Their Morning to Evening Slopes in Patients with Depressive Disorder and Healthy Subjects: Acute Episode and Follow-Up 6 Months after Reaching Remission. Neuroendocrinology 110, 1001–1009. doi: 10.1159/000505921

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jezova, D., Balagova, L., Chmelova, M., and Hlavacova, N. (2019). Classical Steroids in a New Fashion: Focus on Testosterone and Aldosterone. Curr. Protein Pept. Sci. 20, 1112–1118. doi: 10.2174/1389203720666190704151254

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kannan, C. R. (2012). The Adrenal Gland. Berlin: Springer Science & Business Media.

Google Scholar

Kassambara, A. (2020). ggpubr: “ggplot2” Based Publication Ready Plots. Available Online at: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=ggpubr (accessed March 18, 2021).

Google Scholar

Kida, S. (2019). Reconsolidation/destabilization, extinction and forgetting of fear memory as therapeutic targets for PTSD. Psychopharmacology 236, 49–57. doi: 10.1007/s00213-018-5086-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klaassens, E. R., Giltay, E. J., Cuijpers, P., van Veen, T., and Zitman, F. G. (2012). Adulthood trauma and HPA-axis functioning in healthy subjects and PTSD patients: a meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology 37, 317–331. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.07.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kolosnjaj-Tabi, J., Lartigue, L., Javed, Y., Luciani, N., Pellegrino, T., Wilhelm, C., et al. (2016). Biotransformations of magnetic nanoparticles in the body. Nano Today 11, 280–284. doi: 10.1016/j.nantod.2015.10.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kozielski, K. L., Jahanshahi, A., Gilbert, H. B., Yu, Y., Erin, Ö, Francisco, D., et al. (2021). Nonresonant powering of injectable nanoelectrodes enables wireless deep brain stimulation in freely moving mice. Sci. Adv. 7:eabc4189. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.abc4189

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kunert-Keil, C., Bisping, F., Krüger, J., and Brinkmeier, H. (2006). Tissue-specific expression of TRP channel genes in the mouse and its variation in three different mouse strains. BMC Genomics 7:159. doi: 10.1186/1471-2164-7-159

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

La Buissonnière-Ariza, V., Schneider, S. C., and Storch, E. A. (2020). “Pharmacological enhancement of extinction learning,” in Clinical Handbook of Fear and Anxiety: Maintenance Processes and Treatment Mechanisms, eds J. S. Abramowitz and S. M. Blakey (Washington, DC: American Psychological Association), 345–357. doi: 10.1037/0000150-019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lebron-Milad, K., Abbs, B., Milad, M. R., Linnman, C., Rougemount-Bücking, A., Zeidan, M. A., et al. (2012). Sex differences in the neurobiology of fear conditioning and extinction: a preliminary fMRI study of shared sex differences with stress-arousal circuitry. Biol. Mood Anxiety Disord. 2:7. doi: 10.1186/2045-5380-2-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lemon, J. (2006). Plotrix: a package in the red light district of R. R-News 6, 8–12.

Google Scholar

Liberzon, I., Abelson, J. L., Flagel, S. B., Raz, J., and Young, E. A. (1999). Neuroendocrine and psychophysiologic responses in PTSD: a symptom provocation study. Neuropsychopharmacology 21, 40–50. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(98)00128-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Likhtik, E., and Paz, R. (2015). Amygdala-prefrontal interactions in (mal)adaptive learning. Trends Neurosci. 38, 158–166. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2014.12.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Litz, B. T., Salters-Pedneault, K., Steenkamp, M. M., Hermos, J. A., Bryant, R. A., Otto, M. W., et al. (2012). A randomized placebo-controlled trial of D-cycloserine and exposure therapy for posttraumatic stress disorder. J. Psychiatr. Res. 46, 1184–1190. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2012.05.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lu, Q.-B., Sun, J.-F., Yang, Q.-Y., Cai, W.-W., Xia, M.-Q., Wu, F.-F., et al. (2020). Magnetic brain stimulation using iron oxide nanoparticle-mediated selective treatment of the left prelimbic cortex as a novel strategy to rapidly improve depressive-like symptoms in mice. Zool. Res. 41, 381–394. doi: 10.24272/j.issn.2095-8137.2020.076

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maeng, L. Y., and Milad, M. R. (2015). Sex differences in anxiety disorders: interactions between fear, stress, and gonadal hormones. Horm. Behav. 76, 106–117. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2015.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maeng, L. Y., Cover, K. K., Taha, M. B., Landau, A. J., Milad, M. R., and Lebrón-Milad, K. (2017a). Estradiol shifts interactions between the infralimbic cortex and central amygdala to enhance fear extinction memory in female rats. J. Neurosci. Res. 95, 163–175. doi: 10.1002/jnr.23826

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maeng, L. Y., Taha, M. B., Cover, K. K., Glynn, S. S., Murillo, M., Lebron-Milad, K., et al. (2017b). Acute gonadotropin-releasing hormone agonist treatment enhances extinction memory in male rats. Psychoneuroendocrinology 82, 164–172. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2017.05.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCarty, R., Horwatt, K., and Konarska, M. (1988). Chronic stress and sympathetic-adrenal medullary responsiveness. Soc. Sci. Med. 26, 333–341. doi: 10.1016/0277-9536(88)90398-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McEwen, B. S. (2005). Glucocorticoids, depression, and mood disorders: structural remodeling in the brain. Metabolism 54, 20–23. doi: 10.1016/j.metabol.2005.01.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Merz, C. J., Hamacher-Dang, T. C., and Wolf, O. T. (2014). Exposure to stress attenuates fear retrieval in healthy men. Psychoneuroendocrinology 41, 89–96. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.12.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Merz, C. J., Hamacher-Dang, T. C., Stark, R., Wolf, O. T., and Hermann, A. (2018). Neural Underpinnings of Cortisol Effects on Fear Extinction. Neuropsychopharmacology 43, 384–392. doi: 10.1038/npp.2017.227

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Merz, C. J., Tabbert, K., Schweckendiek, J., Klucken, T., Vaitl, D., Stark, R., et al. (2012). Neuronal correlates of extinction learning are modulated by sex hormones. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 7, 819–830. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsr063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Milad, M. R., and Quirk, G. J. (2002). Neurons in medial prefrontal cortex signal memory for fear extinction. Nature 420, 70–74. doi: 10.1038/nature01138

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Milad, M. R., Igoe, S. A., Lebron-Milad, K., and Novales, J. E. (2009). Estrous cycle phase and gonadal hormones influence conditioned fear extinction. Neuroscience 164, 887–895. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.09.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Milad, M. R., Zeidan, M. A., Contero, A., Pitman, R. K., Klibanski, A., Rauch, S. L., et al. (2010). The influence of gonadal hormones on conditioned fear extinction in healthy humans. Neuroscience 168, 652–658. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.04.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moon, J., Christiansen, M. G., Rao, S., Marcus, C., Bono, D. C., Rosenfeld, D., et al. (2020). Magnetothermal Multiplexing for Selective Remote Control of Cell Signaling. Adv. Funct. Mater. 30:2000577. doi: 10.1002/adfm.202000577

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moritz, S., and Bartz-Beielstein, T. (2017). imputeTS: time series missing value imputation in r. R J. 9, 207–218. doi: 10.32614/RJ-2017-009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Munshi, R., Qadri, S. M., and Pralle, A. (2018). Transient Magnetothermal Neuronal Silencing Using the Chloride Channel Anoctamin 1 (TMEM16A). Front. Neurosci. 12:560. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2018.00560

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nemeroff, C. B. (1996). The corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) hypothesis of depression: new findings and new directions. Mol. Psychiatry 1, 336–342.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Niu, X., Yu, K., and He, B. (2018). On the neuromodulatory pathways of the in vivo brain by means of transcranial focused ultrasound. Curr. Opin. Biomed. Eng. 8, 61–69. doi: 10.1016/j.cobme.2018.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Norberg, M. M., Krystal, J. H., and Tolin, D. F. (2008). A Meta-Analysis of D-Cycloserine and the Facilitation of Fear Extinction and Exposure Therapy. Biol. Psychiatry 63, 1118–1126. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.01.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pitman, R. K., and Orr, S. P. (1990). Twenty-four hour urinary cortisol and catecholamine excretion in combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 27, 245–247. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(90)90654-k

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Quirk, G. J., Russo, G. K., Barron, J. L., and Lebron, K. (2000). The role of ventromedial prefrontal cortex in the recovery of extinguished fear. J. Neurosci. Off. J. Soc. Neurosci. 20, 6225–6231. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-16-06225.2000

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

R Core Team (2021). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.

Google Scholar

Rao, S., Chen, R., LaRocca, A. A., Christiansen, M. G., Senko, A. W., Shi, C. H., et al. (2019). Remotely controlled chemomagnetic modulation of targeted neural circuits. Nat. Nanotechnol. 14, 967–973. doi: 10.1038/s41565-019-0521-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ressler, K. J. (2020). Translating Across Circuits and Genetics Toward Progress in Fear- and Anxiety-Related Disorders. Am. J. Psychiatry 177, 214–222. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2020.20010055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, D., Hayes, A., and Couch, S. (2021). broom: Convert Statistical Objects into Tidy Tibbles. Available Online at: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=broom (accessed March 18, 2021).

Google Scholar

Rosenfeld, D., Senko, A. W., Moon, J., Yick, I., Varnavides, G., Gregureæ, D., et al. (2020). Transgene-free remote magnetothermal regulation of adrenal hormones. Sci. Adv. 6:eaaz3734. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.aaz3734

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

RStudio Team (2020). RStudio: Integrated Development for R. Boston, MA: PBC.

Google Scholar

Shahriari, D., Rosenfeld, D., and Anikeeva, P. (2020). Emerging Frontier of Peripheral Nerve and Organ Interfaces. Neuron 108, 270–285. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2020.09.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stein, R. J., Santos, S., Nagatomi, J., Hayashi, Y., Minnery, B. S., Xavier, M., et al. (2004). Cool (trpm8) and hot (trpv1) receptors in the bladder and male genital tract. J. Urol. 172, 1175–1178. doi: 10.1097/01.ju.0000134880.55119.cf

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Storch, E. A., Merlo, L. J., Bengtson, M., Murphy, T. K., Lewis, M. H., Yang, M. C., et al. (2007). D-cycloserine does not enhance exposure-response prevention therapy in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Int. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 22, 230–237. doi: 10.1097/YIC.0b013e32819f8480

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Surkin, P. N., Dmytrenko, G., Giorgio, N. P. D., Bizzozzero, M., Laurentiis, A. D., and Fernández−Solari, J. (2020). Participation of TRPV1 in the activity of the GnRH system in male rats. Eur. J. Neurosci. 52, 2995–3001. doi: 10.1111/ejn.14770

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taxier, L. R., Gross, K. S., and Frick, K. M. (2020). Oestradiol as a neuromodulator of learning and memory. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 21, 535–550. doi: 10.1038/s41583-020-0362-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsoi, K. M., MacParland, S. A., Ma, X.-Z., Spetzler, V. N., Echeverri, J., Ouyang, B., et al. (2016). Mechanism of hard-nanomaterial clearance by the liver. Nat. Mater. 15, 1212–1221. doi: 10.1038/nmat4718

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ulrich-Lai, Y. M., and Herman, J. P. (2009). Neural Regulation of Endocrine and Autonomic Stress Responses. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 397–409. doi: 10.1038/nrn2647

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Velasco, E., Florido, A., Milad, M., and Andero, R. (2019). Sex differences in fear extinction. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 103, 81–108. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.05.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vidal-Gonzalez, I., Vidal-Gonzalez, B., Rauch, S. L., and Quirk, G. J. (2006). Microstimulation reveals opposing influences of prelimbic and infralimbic cortex on the expression of conditioned fear. Learn. Mem. 13, 728–733. doi: 10.1101/lm.306106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wickham, H. (2016). ggplot2: Elegant Graphics for Data Analysis. New York, NY: Springer-Verlag New York.

Google Scholar

Wickham, H., and Bryan, J. (2019). readxl: Read Excel Files. Available Online at: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=readxl (accessed March 18, 2021).

Google Scholar

Wickham, H., Averick, M., Bryan, J., Chang, W., McGowan, L. D., François, R., et al. (2019). Welcome to the tidyverse. J. Open Source Softw. 4:1686. doi: 10.21105/joss.01686

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, H. M. (2017). The application of magnetic nanoparticles in the treatment and monitoring of cancer and infectious diseases. Biosci. Horiz. Int. J. Stud. Res. 10:hzx009. doi: 10.1093/biohorizons/hzx009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolf, O. T., Atsak, P., de Quervain, D. J., Roozendaal, B., and Wingenfeld, K. (2016). Stress and Memory: A Selective Review on Recent Developments in the Understanding of Stress Hormone Effects on Memory and Their Clinical Relevance. J. Neuroendocrinol. 28, 1–8. doi: 10.1111/jne.12353

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wong, D. L., Tai, T. C., Wong-Faull, D. C., Claycomb, R., Meloni, E. G., Myers, K. M., et al. (2012). Epinephrine: a short- and long-term regulator of stress and development of illness: a potential new role for epinephrine in stress. Cell. Mol. Neurobiol. 32, 737–748. doi: 10.1007/s10571-011-9768-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yehuda, R. (2006). Advances in understanding neuroendocrine alterations in PTSD and their therapeutic implications. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1071, 137–166. doi: 10.1196/annals.1364.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yehuda, R., Giller, E. L., Southwick, S. M., Lowy, M. T., and Mason, J. W. (1991). Hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal dysfunction in posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 30, 1031–1048.

Google Scholar

Yehuda, R., Southwick, S., Giller, E. L., Ma, X., and Mason, J. W. (1992). Urinary catecholamine excretion and severity of PTSD symptoms in Vietnam combat veterans. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 180, 321–325. doi: 10.1097/00005053-199205000-00006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yizhar, O., Fenno, L. E., Davidson, T. J., Mogri, M., and Deisseroth, K. (2011). Optogenetics in neural systems. Neuron 71, 9–34. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: epinephrine, corticosterone, adrenal gland, neuromodulation, hormones, magnetic nanoparticles

Citation: Maeng LY, Rosenfeld D, Simandl GJ, Koehler F, Senko AW, Moon J, Varnavides G, Murillo MF, Reimer AE, Wald A, Anikeeva P and Widge AS (2022) Probing Neuro-Endocrine Interactions Through Remote Magnetothermal Adrenal Stimulation. Front. Neurosci. 16:901108. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2022.901108

Received: 21 March 2022; Accepted: 30 May 2022;
Published: 23 June 2022.

Edited by:

Ravi L. Hadimani, Virginia Commonwealth University, United States

Reviewed by:

Vassiliy Tsytsarev, University of Maryland, College Park, United States
Tyler Scullen, Tulane University, United States

Copyright © 2022 Maeng, Rosenfeld, Simandl, Koehler, Senko, Moon, Varnavides, Murillo, Reimer, Wald, Anikeeva and Widge. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Polina Anikeeva, YW5pa2VldmFAbWl0LmVkdQ==; Alik S. Widge, YXdpZGdlQHVtbi5lZHU=

Present addresses: Lisa Y. Maeng, Department of Psychology, Developmental and Brain Sciences, University of Massachusetts Boston, Boston, MA, United States; Dekel Rosenfeld, Department of Biomedical Engineering, Faculty of Engineering, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv, Israel

These authors have contributed equally to this work and share first authorship

§These authors have contributed equally to this work and share senior authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.