Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurosci., 23 June 2022
Sec. Auditory Cognitive Neuroscience
This article is part of the Research Topic Towards an Understanding of Tinnitus Heterogeneity, Volume II View all 27 articles

Recovering Hidden Responder Groups in Individuals Receiving Neurofeedback for Tinnitus

  • 1Department of Psychology, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
  • 2Research Priority Program “ESIT—European School of Interdisciplinary Tinnitus Research,” Zurich, Switzerland
  • 3Department of Otorhinolaryngology, University Hospital Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
  • 4Neuroscience Center Zurich (ZNZ), ETH Zürich, Zurich, Switzerland
  • 5University Research Priority Program “Dynamics of Healthy Aging,” University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland

The widespread understanding that chronic tinnitus is a heterogeneous phenomenon with various neural oscillatory profiles has spurred investigations into individualized approaches in its treatment. Neurofeedback, as a non-invasive tool for altering neural activity, has become increasingly popular in the personalized treatment of a wide range of neuropsychological disorders. Despite the success of neurofeedback on the group level, the variability in the treatment efficacy on the individual level is high, and evidence from recent studies shows that only a small number of people can effectively modulate the desired aspects of neural activity. To reveal who may be more suitable, and hence benefit most from neurofeedback treatment, we classified individuals into unobserved subgroups with similar oscillatory trajectories during the treatment and investigated how subgroup membership was predicted by a series of characteristics. Growth mixture modeling was used to identify distinct latent subgroups with similar oscillatory trajectories among 50 individuals suffering from chronic subjective tinnitus (38 male, 12 female, mean age = 47.1 ± 12.84) across 15 neurofeedback training sessions. Further, the impact of characteristics and how they predicted the affiliation in the identified subgroups was evaluated by including measures of demographics, tinnitus-specific (Tinnitus Handicap Inventory) and depression variables, as well as subjective quality of life subscales (World Health Organization—Quality of Life Questionnaire), and health-related quality of life subscales (Short Form-36) in a logistic regression analysis. A latent class model could be fitted to the longitudinal data with a high probability of correctly classifying distinct oscillatory patterns into 3 different groups: non-responder (80%), responder (16%), and decliner (4%). Further, our results show that the health-related wellbeing subscale of the Short Form-36 questionnaire was differentially associated with the groups. However, due to the small sample size in the Responder group, we are not able to provide sufficient evidence for a distinct responder profile. Nevertheless, the identification of oscillatory change-rate differences across distinct groups of individuals provides the groundwork from which to tease apart the complex and heterogeneous oscillatory processes underlying tinnitus and the attempts to modify these through neurofeedback. While more research is needed, our results and the analytical approach presented may bring clarity to contradictory past findings in the field of tinnitus research, and eventually influence clinical practice.

Introduction

Chronic tinnitus is a variable phenomenon characterized by a heterogeneous appearance (Cederroth et al., 2019). Existing data suggest an extensive degree of individual differences and fluctuations in tinnitus, which have hampered both basic and clinical research (Hall et al., 2018). Previous research has aimed to disentangle the complex heterogeneity of the audiological phantom percept into causal risk factors, such as gender, age, ototoxic medication, and related hearing loss (Davis and El Refaie, 2000; Martines et al., 2010) or tinnitus characteristics (e.g., loudness, pitch, side of perception, and duration). Further presumed causes of the heterogeneity are comorbidities, which may start with anxiety (McCormack et al., 2015) or insomnia (Lasisi and Gureje, 2011; Wallhäusser-Franke et al., 2013), continue to hyperacusis (Goebel and Floezinger, 2008), and escalate to depression (McKenna et al., 1991; Zöger et al., 2006; Zirke et al., 2013; Trevis et al., 2018). Other epiphenomena, including tinnitus-related distress (Hesser and Andersson, 2014; Brüggemann et al., 2016), personality traits (Konareva, 2006; Simões et al., 2019), and tinnitus-specific brain oscillation accompanied with structural and functional alterations in auditory and non-auditory brain areas (Schlee et al., 2009; Adjamian et al., 2014) have been considered to contribute to or moderate the various manifestations of the phantom percept. Finally, all possible combinations of the mentioned phenomena complement the heterogeneous appearance (Henry et al., 2005; Vanneste et al., 2010; Joos et al., 2012; Vanneste and De Ridder, 2012; Meyer et al., 2014). As efforts to disentangle the heterogeneity have increased, so has the recognition of the complexity of an effective treatment approach (Scott and Lindberg, 2000; Hoare et al., 2011; Pryce et al., 2019).

A treatment approach for such a heterogeneous phenomenon that is appropriate for all those suffering from tinnitus—a “one size fits all” solution, so to speak—has not yet been identified (Landgrebe et al., 2012; Baguley et al., 2013; Hall et al., 2019). The difficulty arises as the heterogeneous appearance of tinnitus persists in its response to treatment with complex and variable trajectories (Tyler et al., 2008; Hall et al., 2016; Riha et al., 2021). However, a group of treatment modalities has recently inspired extensive research; namely, neurofeedback (NFB) (Guerra et al., 2019). In most cases, this technique offers a non-invasive window on the brain and provides a tool to pinpoint and alter subject-specific brain function and dysfunction, thus offering potential for improvement of a number of (clinical) conditions, such as ADHD, depression, epilepsy, and anxiety, among others (for an overview, see Hampson et al., 2019). In the treatment of tinnitus, NFB training has been associated with reductions in self-reported tinnitus-related distress and loudness (Dohrmann et al., 2007a,b; Crocetti et al., 2011; Güntensperger et al., 2019; Jensen et al., 2020). The underlying mechanism of NFB is based on the reinforcement of individual brain activity patterns that are recorded via electroencephalography (EEG) or functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), analyzed and fed-back to the participant in real time. The feedback modality can be either visual, acoustic, or tactile and is based on the principles of operant conditioning (Skinner, 1938). The participant is rewarded when the brain signal reaches a predefined value. Despite the great potential of NFB as an option in the treatment for several conditions, the practical application still encounters considerable drawbacks.

One potential source of drawback is the general ability of an individual to modify their cortical activity, which is referred to as the inefficacy problem (The inefficacy problem is apparent in both EEG- and fMRI-based NFB; yet rooted in diverse technical approaches and difficulties. Gevensleben et al., 2009; Weber et al., 2011; Huster et al., 2014; Rogala et al., 2016; Alkoby et al., 2018). This failure to control has been described in numerous NFB trials and other brain-computer interface (BCI) applications (for a review, see Alkoby et al., 2018). Alkoby et al. (2018) note that in most NFB studies approximately 16–57% of the participants are successful in self-regulating their EEG activity. A further consideration is that there is no consensus yet about how to quantify effectiveness, thus the definition of a responder is not consistent within the NFB and BCI literature and still lacks a general standard across studies and research fields (Gruzelier, 2014a,b). In addition, the question has been raised whether the ability to deliberately modify the oscillatory activity is necessarily linked to the NFB training outcome, or vice versa; for example, the reduction of symptoms such as tinnitus distress and loudness (Rogala et al., 2016). Existing evidence indicates that generally the outcome of NFB treatment is related to combined effects of pre-treatment, neuroanatomical or oscillatory, and treatment-specific factors. Among the pre-treatment factors are age and sex (Riha et al., 2021), personality traits (Simões et al., 2019), and psychological factors such as motivation (Diaz Hernandez et al., 2018), mood, attention, and anxiety (Koush et al., 2017), which further influence different (oscillatory) baseline conditions for NFB training. For a systematic review of how psychological factors contribute to NFB outcome, we refer the reader to Kadosh and Staunton (2019). Baseline neuroanatomical or oscillatory determinants have included gray and white matter volumes (Enriquez-Geppert et al., 2013; Ninaus et al., 2015), as well as the means of eyes-open resting-state EEG power before the training (Wan et al., 2014; Nan et al., 2015; Reichert et al., 2015). Further factors in the design of the training protocol (e.g., duration of each training and training schedule), and the NFB learning strategy (Kober et al., 2013; Witte et al., 2013) may contribute to overall NFB success. Indeed, the evaluation of early training sessions can be used to predict future training progress (Weber et al., 2011; Enriquez-Geppert et al., 2013). In addition to those already mentioned, Weber et al. (2020) have provided an extensive summary of predictors of NFB training outcome in their systematic review. However, even this is not an exhaustive list, and the conflicting results provided only underscore the need for further research in the field of NFB training.

For the present research and in the light of the inefficacy problem, the first question was whether the tinnitus individuals studied were able to alter their brain activity in the predefined direction. If the desired change in neural activity was apparent across the NFB training, the individual is considered a Responder in this report, independent of tinnitus-related changes. Due to the pronounced variation of the oscillatory fingerprint in tinnitus and the variation in the response to NFB, the main purpose of this study was to identify unobserved subgroups of individuals that had similar EEG training trajectories across all sessions. By disentangling the heterogeneity of training trajectories into subgroups, we further investigated which potentially modifiable clinical factors predicted group affiliation prior to the NFB training. Thus, we aimed at identifying the underlying characteristics that were associated with successful oscillatory modification, and thereby recognizing the possible Responders to NFB. This research thus constitutes the conceptual groundwork for identifying subgroups of individuals that are more or less responsive to the given intervention, in the sense of being able to alter one’s brain activity. Further, it contributes to the understanding of inter-individual differences in NFB progress, knowledge which may then be applied in the development of individually tailored NFB protocols with the aim of increasing the therapy’s effectiveness.

Materials and Methods

Study Sample

The study sample in this analysis was derived from the clinical trial by Güntensperger et al. (2017, 2020, 2019), the largest NFB study in tinnitus research to date. The authors’ main goal was to examine the efficacy and possible distinctions of two different NFB approaches in the treatment of tinnitus; namely, traditional surface-based NFB vs. tomographic NFB (Güntensperger et al., 2020). The protocol complied with the Declaration of Helsinki, was approved by the relevant Ethics Committee (Kantonale Ethikkommission Project KEK-ZH-Nr. 2014-0594), and further registered online at ClinicalTrials.gov (NCT02383147) and kofam.ch (SNCTP000001313). The trial took place in 2017 and 2018, and comprised two baseline visits, 15 weekly NFB sessions of 15 min duration each, a post-treatment visit, as well as two follow-up appointments 3 and 6 months after completion of training. Fifty individuals with chronic tinnitus were able to complete the NFB study, including 38 males and 12 females aged 47.1 ± 12.84 (M ± SD) years (Güntensperger et al., 2019, 2020). Each participant gave their written informed consent prior to partaking in the experimental trials. We refer to the original referenced publication for an in-depth description of the study protocol, and the ancillary publication by Riha et al. (2021) for properties of applied measures and their predictive value regarding the progression of NFB training. Table 1 provides an overview of characteristics, health, and tinnitus characteristics of the study sample.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic, health, and tinnitus characteristics of study sample.

Brain Oscillation and Tinnitus

A common finding in brain imaging resting-state EEG studies of patients suffering from chronic tinnitus is an increased delta (3–4 Hz) wave activity and a reduction in alpha (8.5–12 Hz) oscillation in the auditory cortex region compared to healthy subjects (Weisz et al., 2007a,b, 2011; De Ridder et al., 2015). In chronic tinnitus, the cause of these established, spontaneous oscillatory alterations has been linked to sensory deprivation; namely, deafferentation due to hearing loss (Llinás et al., 1999; Eggermont and Roberts, 2004; Møller, 2007; Weisz et al., 2007b; Eggermont, 2012). The consequence of these bottom-up and top-down abnormalities is an imbalance in excitatory-inhibitory neuronal activity along the tonotopic axis in the affected regions (Møller, 2007; Hong et al., 2016). Among others, such adaptions are described in the theoretical frameworks of the thalamocortical dysrhythmia model (TCM; Llinás et al., 1999; Mahmoudian et al., 2013) and the synchronization by loss of inhibition model (SLIM; Weisz et al., 2007b). Thus, a frequently used NFB training protocol for tinnitus aims to reduce delta and increase the individual alpha activity to attenuate tinnitus and tinnitus-related symptoms (Dohrmann et al., 2007b; Crocetti et al., 2011; Güntensperger et al., 2019, 2020; Jensen et al., 2020). Using a rewarding alpha and inhibiting delta protocol resulted in encouraging training outcomes in previous NFB trials. For an overview of this and other NFB protocols in the treatment of tinnitus (see Güntensperger et al., 2017).

Güntensperger’s NFB trial from 2017 to 2018 applied the previously described protocol who additionally acquired the neuro-dynamic data for this analysis as mentioned before (Güntensperger et al., 2019, 2020). The measures used in this report were the EEG power values from alpha and delta, recorded with fronto-central electrode positions (FC1, FC2, F3, F4) prior to each of the 15 NFB training sessions (thus unrelated to the training itself). The resting-state activity was recorded by splitting it in eyes-closed and -open segments, whereby we focused on the latter according to the recommendations of the European tinnitus research network, TINNET (Working Group 3).1 The EEG data derived from each of the 15 recordings was pre-processed and the EEG power averaged for each participant across the four electrodes, according to the main interest in this analysis, the individual trajectories. In order to examine the individual training trajectories, the ratio between the desired increase of alpha and decrease of delta power (alpha/delta ratio; ADR) was calculated and compared across time points. The interested reader is referred to publications by Güntensperger et al. (2019, 2020) and Riha et al. (2020, 2021) for in-depth descriptions of the EEG recording procedure and pre-processing pipelines. To resolve the possible confusion around the different publications evolving from this data set we further refer to Figure 1 for a comprehensive overview.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Study Design of the clinical NFB trial by Güntensperger et al. (2019, 2020). Based on the original data a number of publications evolved, however, focusing on different aspects of the data set, according to the hypotheses. The used data sets are highlighted by a color coded circle with a number, which correspond to the publications in the first row. TSCHQ, Tinnitus sample case history questionnaire; THI, Tinnitus Handicap Inventory; TQ, Tinnitus Questionnaire; PRISM, Pictorial Representation of Illness and Self Measure; BDI, Beck Depression Inventory; SCL K 9, Symptom Checklist; WHO, World Health Organization Quality of Life; SF 36, Short Form Health Questionnaire.

Analytical Procedure

Oscillatory Training Trajectories

This analysis follows on from a previous analysis, in which we investigated the oscillatory trajectories of delta and alpha and their relation to influential factors across the NFB training (Riha et al., 2021). The applied latent growth curve (LGC) analysis revealed a linear pattern of change and a significant individual variability in the two frequency bands over time: The desired enhancement of alpha was found, while slow wave delta was stable in most individuals throughout the NFB training. These results raised questions that inspired this explorative follow-on analysis with the aim of identifying unobserved subgroups (latent classes) with similar ADR patterns in the variability of longitudinal linear trajectories.

Here, we used a growth mixture modeling (GMM) approach (Muthen and Muthen, 2000; Kaplan, 2004; Muthén, 2004; Jung and Wickrama, 2008; Ram and Grimm, 2009; Berlin et al., 2014; Geifman et al., 2018) to statistically differentiate meaningful or naturally occurring subgroups according to the trends in repeated measures of the ADR (see Figure 2 for the individual ADR trajectories). In simple terms, by including the categorical variable of “class,” the GMM approach is able to determine the optimal number of classes, the number of people in each class, as well as the growth factors (intercept and slope) of each different trajectory. We employed an exploratory approach and fitted models with an increasing number of classes to ascertain the optimum latent class model. To estimate the number of latent classes, we followed recommended approaches including the comparison of various model fit statistics, substantive meaning and interpretability of each class (Wickrama et al., 2016). We inspected the Akaike (1974) and the Bayesian information criterions (BIC; Schwarz, 1978), the sample-size adjusted BIC (SSABIC; Sclove, 1987), entropy values and the Lo-Mendel-Rubin likelihood ratio test value (LMR-LRT; Jung and Wickrama, 2008). For interpretation, lower AIC, BIC, and SSABIC values indicate a more parsimonious and better fitting model, whereas higher entropy values signal better class separation (Nylund et al., 2007). Models were estimated by full maximum likelihood (FML) and robust standard errors (MLR) to non-normality and non-independence of observations.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Individual raw data trajectories of the ADR across the 15 measurement occasions. Each thin line represents an individual oscillatory ADR trajectory.

Class Membership

In a second step and since latent classes (i.e., the identified subgroups) are categorical, we applied Firth’s logistic regression (Firth, 1993) with the penalization of log-likelihood (Heinze, 2006) to estimate the association with a list of characteristics in a small sample (Heinze and Puhr, 2010). This kind of logistic regression is designed to handle datasets that are small, imbalanced or separated. The estimates represent the logarithm of the odds of being in a latent class vs. being in the reference class, while assessing the overall model fit and predictive accuracy. Moreover, we reported the Nagelkerke R2 and Hosmer and Lemeshow test value as quality markers for this analysis. For the model’s diagnostic properties of sensitivity and specificity, we used the receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curve and the area under the curve (AUC) as a measure of predictive ability. The list of characteristics that we considered to possibly mark class membership were acquired face-to-face during the two baseline visits, and include age, sex, tinnitus duration in months, as well as scores from a tinnitus-related symptom scale (THI: Tinnitus Handicap Inventory—German version; Kleinjung et al., 2007) and depression scales (BDI: Beck Depression Inventory; Hautzinger et al., 1994); SCL-K-9: short form of the Symptom Checklist—(Klaghofer and Brähler, 2001). Additionally, the subscales of the Quality of Life questionnaire from the World Health Organization (WHOQOL: World Health Organization Quality of Life-BREF—German version; Angermeyer et al., 2002) and the health-related questions from the Short Form-36 (SF-36—German version; Bullinger et al., 1995) were possible indicators. For a more detailed description of this list of characteristics, we refer to our preceding analysis (Riha et al., 2021). Further, the complete test battery used in the clinical study by Güntensperger and colleagues followed the guidelines of the Tinnitus Research Initiative (TRI; Landgrebe et al., 2012).

For reasons of completeness, we included the categorical feature of sex, and encoded it as dichotomous (0 = female; 1 = male). Questionnaire items in the logistic regression were treated as continuous measures and were mean-centered prior to the analysis (Hox, 2002). P-values below 0.05 were considered to indicate statistical significance. All analysis was performed using R statistical software, version 4.0.0 (R Core Team, 2019). The following packages were used: “lcmm” for the growth mixture model analysis (Proust-Lima et al., 2017), the “logistf” package for Firth’s logistic regression (Heinze and Ploner, 2004), and plots were created using “ggplot2” (Wickham, 2016).

Results

The considerable individual variability in the ADR trend among all tinnitus sufferers across the NFB training is shown in Figure 2 (raw data). To ensure that we identified the model of change that best represented the 15 training sessions, we conducted a GMM analysis. This approach was chosen to extract unobserved subgroups of tinnitus sufferers with homogenous change trajectories. In Figure 3 the predicted means of the 1-, 2-, 3-, and 4-class model can be compared. Additionally, Table 2 provides the AIC, BIC, SSBIC values and the entropy results for the estimated class models. The explorative model fitting procedure resulted in a 3-class model, and the decision was facilitated by the recommendation for fit indices. The 3-class model is favored by the AIC of −2885.939 and the size adjusted BIC of −2900.661 (which involves smaller penalties), in combination with the entropy being closer to 1 (entropy = 0.87). All other models were rejected as they did not provide any additional explanatory value for estimating the patterns of change.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Predicted trajectories across 15 NFB training sessions. (A) 1-Class model, (B) 2-class model, (C) the favored 3-class model, with Class 1 corresponding to the red, Class 2 to green, and Class 3 to the blue line, (D) 4-class model.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Model selection criteria of the Growth Mixture Model (GMM) analysis.

As can be seen in Figure 3C, Class 1 (red line) is distinguished by having almost the same level of ADR at the beginning as at the end of the NFB treatment. This class can be considered a non-responder class. Class 2 (green line) had a moderate initial ADR with a notable decrease in the slope over time and thus indicates the Decliner class. Based on the significant growth factors from the first to the final NFB session that equal an increase in ADR, Class 3 (blue line) will be referred to as the Responder class in the following. Further, Class 3 revealed the highest initial ADR. (Although not shown for all classes in Figure 4, there was notable overlap of trajectories, implying that there was considerable fluctuation of individual ADR trajectories within each class.) Classification of individuals based on their most likely class membership resulted in class counts and proportions of 40 (80%) in the non-responder class, two individuals (4%) in the Decliner class, and eight individuals (16%) in the Responder class. The quality of classification can be further indicated by the calculation of posterior probabilities for allocation in a certain class. Individuals of Class 1 had a 94% posterior probability of being correctly classified in the non-responder class, and only 2% posterior probability of being assigned to Class 2, or 4% to Class 3. Similar posterior probabilities were classified for individuals in Class 2 with 90% being in the Decliner class (9% for Class 1 and 0% for Class 3), as well as in Class 3 with 93% being in the Responder class (6% for Class 1 and 0% for Class 2). Even though the 3-class model was favored by the fit indices, unequal class sizes were created. Following statistical justification and interpretability of specifics of class membership, the Decliners, comprising of solely two individuals, were excluded from the remaining analysis, leaving a final sample of 48 individuals that include Responders and non-responders. (We refer the interested reader to Appendix Table 1 for descriptive characteristics of the two individuals of the Decliners class).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Alpha and delta trajectories in Classes 1 and 3.

In a next step, the two remaining classes, Responder and non-responder, were assessed for indicators of class membership. An overview of the class-specific indicator occurrence is shown in Figure 5. After checking for normal distributions and homogeneous variance, the Firth’s binary logistic regression was performed with class membership (responder vs. non-responder) as dependent variable.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Characteristics of non-responder (class 1) vs. responder (class 3). Sex (male = 1; female = 0); THI, Tinnitus Handicap Inventory; BDI, Beck Depression Inventory; SCL-K-9, Symptom Checklist; WHO, World Health Organization Quality of Life Questionnaire with 4 subscales: phys, physiological, psych, psychological; social, envir; environmental; SF-36, Short Form Health Questionnaire with 8 subscales; PFI, Physical Functioning Index; RPI, Role-Physical Index; BPI, Bodily Pain Index; GHP, General Health Perceptions Index; VI, Vitality Index; SFI, Social Functioning Index; REI, Role-Emotional Index; MHI, Mental Health Index.

Table 3 shows the logistic coefficients for the regression of class membership, with non-responder (dummy coded 0) as the reference class. The Nagelkerke R2 value was 0.53, and the Hosmer-Lemeshow test value was 0.42. Neither tinnitus distress, depression characteristics, nor general quality of life questionnaire items were significant indicators for class membership. Only the Mental Health index (MHI), a subscale of the SF-36, reached significant negative influence (OR = 0.77, SE = 0.12, p < 0.05) on class membership. This suggests that each one-unit of increase in the MHI will decrease the log odds of being in the Responder class by 0.266, and the p-value indicates that the MHI is significant in determining class membership. We refer the reader to Figure 6 for a graphic representation of the probabilities of group membership. As already mentioned, the significant independent variable is a subscale of the SF-36 health questionnaire. For each subscale, the standard scores were calculated with higher percentage scores indicating either a higher level of functioning or less disability.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Firth’s binary logistic regression with dichotomized dependent variables (0 = non-responder; 1 = responder) of class membership (n = 48).

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Probability plots illustrating how the class membership probabilities change with the given value of the Mental Health Index (MHI), a subscale of the SF-36 health questionnaire.

Taken together, the results indicate that individuals can be classified into different latent classes based on their 15 weekly EEG recordings taken prior to each NFB training session, and that a 3-class solution provided the best fit among GMM models. Health-related subscale responses on the SF-36 provided the best indicators, whereas the tinnitus distress (THI), depression (BDI), and general quality of life questionnaire (WHO-BREF, SCL-K-9) did not reach significance, providing no additional predictive value for class membership.

Discussion

According to the definition applied in this report, the only requirement to be classified a Responder is the ability to modify one’s neural oscillations. It should be emphasized here that we are aware of the publication by Gruzelier (2014b), in which it was suggested that a trio of specificities—frequency band, topographical, and outcome specificity—should be fulfilled for a NFB intervention to be labeled successful. While respecting Gruzelier’s consideration to develop a methodological standard in the NFB community, we believed it necessary to highlight the deficits of NFB and therefore to take a step back in the theoretical framework. This is why we chose the statistical tool of growth mixture modeling, which allowed us to quantify the extent of NFB inefficacy in our sample of chronic tinnitus sufferers. In addition, we attempted to predict the probability of being a Responder and whether patterns of change were constrained or dictated by underlying characteristics that had not been previously explored.

By removing subjectivity and making use of all available EEG data, we recovered hidden patients’ trajectories in response to NFB treatment for tinnitus. Based on a representative sample of individuals, we disentangled heterogeneous oscillatory trajectories and identified meaningful subgroups showing similar ADR patterns across 15 weekly sessions. Developing this direction further, we applied a GMM approach that yielded an optimum of three different latent classes, which we named Decliners, non-responders and responders. Decliners exhibited decreases in ADR during the treatment; however, there were only two individuals in this class, which we therefore excluded from further analysis as a precautionary measure. The majority of the participants (80%) were in the non-responder class, defined as those who started and continued the NFB training on the same, unvarying ADR level. Finally, the Responder class comprised eight individuals (16%) who showed the desired increase of the ADR across the NFB training sessions. The findings in our study are generally consistent with previous tinnitus research findings that have shown high variability of treatment response (Kleinjung et al., 2005; Henry et al., 2006; Rossi et al., 2007). Our findings are also in line with NFB research that has indicated large intra-individual differences in EEG patterns (Dohrmann et al., 2007b; Riha et al., 2020) and training trajectories (Riha et al., 2021). Lastly, we found that a certain number of individuals were successful in modifying their EEG activity (Responders), a finding consistent with current literature (Weber et al., 2011; Kouijzer et al., 2013; Enriquez-Geppert et al., 2014; Alkoby et al., 2018).

When looking at the results broken down by latent classes, in Figure 3C, it can be noted that the Responder class (blue line) exhibited the highest initial ADR. However, the existing ADR trajectories are ambiguous as it is unclear whether the ratio change over time is influenced by one or both frequency bands. Separated for alpha and delta power, as shown in Figure 4, it is apparent that the Responder class (Class 3) indeed revealed the desired increase in the alpha-, and decrease in the delta-band. This novel finding caused us to view our past findings from a new perspective, as we had previously only observed change in the alpha-band when including the whole sample in the analysis, without acknowledging the individual trajectories. Moreover, the Responder class’s alpha trajectory started at a significantly higher initial alpha power level compared to the non-responder class (Class 1).

The observed higher initial alpha power in the Responder class follows a concept outlined in the neural efficiency hypothesis (Haier et al., 1992; Doppelmayr et al., 2005). According to this hypothesis in the context of NFB, Vernon and colleagues suggest that “if alpha makes completion of a task more efficient by inhibiting non-essential processing, then a greater level of available alpha may enable the individual to inhibit more non-essential activity, which in turn may facilitate performance […]” (Vernon et al., 2009, p. 216). In contrast, low levels of alpha waves reflect a state of excitation (Klimesch et al., 2007). In addition, it has been suggested that alpha enhancement training may lead to higher outgoing connectivity in a neighboring region of the trained area (Hartmann et al., 2014) as it works as a communication vector across cortical areas (Haegens et al., 2015). Expanding on and supporting these lines of thought, our results indicate that individuals with an increased initial alpha power are more likely to be able to actively inhibit irrelevant processes, thus making them more efficient in altering their brain activity during NFB treatment and hence belonging to the Responder class.

The prediction of class membership in the second step, logistic regression analysis, was based on multiple characteristics that represent an approximation to the comprehensive picture of the individual’s general and tinnitus-related quality of life, as well as their health-related wellbeing. The latter construct provided the strongest group of markers, derived from the Short-Form Health Questionnaire (SF-36). Of its eight health-related quality of life dimensions, the MHI represented the strongest predictor. The five item MHI subscale of the SF-36 was developed to measure psychological distress and wellbeing (Ware and Sherbourne, 1992). The subscale’s items relate to anxiety, depression, loss of behavioral or emotional control, and psychological wellbeing. Scoring follows a 0–100 range from low for feelings of nervousness and depression all of the time to high for feeling peaceful, happy, and calm all of the time. The range of the scale allows for the valid discrimination of psychiatric patients from those with other medical conditions (Berwick et al., 1991). The unique effect of the MHI subscale was very small but may be clinically relevant and is in accordance with the results of other studies indicating the effect of psychological wellbeing and (healthy) mental states on the course and outcome of treatments in various pathologies (Carver et al., 2005; Hasler, 2016; Guidi et al., 2018). It is, however, important to differentiate between the effect on a positive treatment outcome and the ability to learn to self-regulate the brain activity. In this analysis, poorer psychological wellbeing was found to predict this ability. The question which then arises is why would poor subjective wellbeing be a prerequisite for the alteration of oscillatory patterns?

Researchers have contrasting views on the influence of psychological factors on the individual ability to modulate EEG patterns. Hammer et al. (2012) have suggested that NFB/BCI performance can only be predicted to a limited extent by psychological parameters. Similar findings were reported by Marxen et al. (2016), who noted that depression has no statistically significant relationship with regulation during fMRI-based NFB training. Given the lack of an association between class membership and depression in our results, we can support these previous findings to some extent. However, taking into consideration the fronto-central position of the electrodes in our study and the coarse spatial resolution of EEG in general, the signal detected cannot assuredly be associated with only the primary auditory cortices; other, non-auditory areas may have contributed as well. The neural correlate for feelings of nervousness and depression, as the lower scores of the MHI are defined (Ware and Sherbourne, 1992), can be represented by specific oscillatory patterns in the tinnitus distress network encompassing the ventromedial prefrontal cortex, the parahippocampus, as well as the insula and anterior cingulate cortices (ACC) (Jastreboff, 1990; Lockwood et al., 1998; Mühlau et al., 2006; Moisset and Bouhassira, 2007; Vanneste et al., 2010; De Ridder et al., 2011; Vanneste and De Ridder, 2012). Previous studies have indicated the importance of emotional factors in the experience of tinnitus (Andersson et al., 1999; van der Loo et al., 2011; Joos et al., 2012; Brüggemann et al., 2016; Meyer et al., 2017), and that happiness is associated with temporal parietal regions, while sadness activates limbic and paralimbic structures (Jastreboff, 1990; George et al., 1995). Other scholars found evidence that activities of the paralimbic cortex including the left insula and the rostral and pregenual ACC were of significant predictive value for the change of distress (measured by the THI) in tinnitus retraining therapy (Kim et al., 2016). This was confirmed by resting-state EEG data indicating that the level of distress is further correlated with alpha oscillation over these areas (Vanneste et al., 2010). A recent report supports the notion that if the oscillatory activity of the ACCs is insufficient prior to the initial fitting and wearing of hearing aids in the treatment for tinnitus, the phantom perception cannot be improved by the devices (Han et al., 2020). These latter findings accord with our results and guide the attention back to the described top-down inhibiting processes of alpha oscillations. As previously mentioned, contemporary research on NFB has indicated that higher resting-state alpha is associated with increased probability of learning to modify the targeted brain waves during treatment (Klimesch et al., 2007; Gruzelier, 2014a; Wan et al., 2014).

Furthermore, the insula and the ACC are key regions of the salience network (SN) which mediates filtering and detecting salient stimuli (Seeley et al., 2007; Menon, 2015). Simply put, the SN first filters the constant stream of incoming stimuli according to their perceptional features (Peters et al., 2005). As Menon (2015) states, stimuli are more likely to be perceived as salient if they “include deviants embedded in a constant stream, surprising stimuli, and stimuli that are pleasurable and rewarding, self-relevant, or emotionally engaging” (p. 597). Once a salient stimuli is detected, the network’s robust connections recruit other brain networks and facilitate access to attention and working-memory resources (Sridharan et al., 2008). A shift of attention from external to internal processes is suggested, resulting in the representation of a subjective and conscious state, as well as the emotional value of the external stimuli (Seeley et al., 2007; Goulden et al., 2014). Thus, the SN is further associated with internally oriented mental processes and interoceptive awareness, which is associated with autonomic processes such as heartbeat, skin conductance, and respiration. In the context of tinnitus, it has been suggested that a persistent state of awareness may lead to the misattribution of salience to a stimulus, and that this could explain the genesis and maintenance of a conscious auditory percept to a non-existent sound (Rinne et al., 2009; Sadaghiani et al., 2009; De Ridder et al., 2014). Driven by the persistent awareness, the SN seems to act as a multisensory integration site of different tinnitus aspects and attributes, thus making it a core modulator of tinnitus-related distress and subjective wellbeing (van der Loo et al., 2011; De Ridder et al., 2014; Shi et al., 2018).

While our interpretation of results builds on the approach presented in this report, the neural component of conscious, health-related wellbeing and its oscillatory activity or fluctuations could have a number of other potential causes. The challenge in interpreting these effects lies in determining whether they are associated with the generation and chronification of the tinnitus percept, or whether they are associated with tinnitus-related reactions and/or compensations on the individual level. Disentangling wellbeing into its constituent parts and considering our data, we cannot clearly differentiate between tinnitus-related and health-related wellbeing, nor can we identify which of these potential mechanisms might be most relevant. Since the dynamics of neural oscillations reflect perceptual, sensory, cognitive and emotional events, the precise details of these mechanisms warrant further attention. However, our results supported the general assertion that mental wellbeing—as derived from the items of the MHI in this analysis—is decisive for the course and outcome of an NFB treatment. Indeed, the effects were determined at both ends of the defined continuum, at levels of both low and high wellbeing.

Limitations

The inefficacy problem, as one ambiguity concerning NFB, has been the focus of this report. However, other pertinent points in this treatment approach remain to be considered. The most important points are first, that the underlying mechanisms of NFB are not entirely understood and the discussion of its effects is ongoing (Fovet et al., 2017; Schabus, 2017; Thibault et al., 2017; Witte et al., 2018). Second, the demands of temporal expenditure for both participants and clinicians in NFB mean that more distinct and clinically applicable predictors for the ability to learn the regulation of brain activity are urgently needed. The ultimate point we mention here refers to clinical study protocols; for example, duration and frequency of training, feedback modality, and the lack of a blinded control or placebo group (Vernon et al., 2009; Cortese et al., 2016; Omejc et al., 2019; Jensen et al., 2020). Detailed information on all aspects of the discussion orbiting NFB are unfortunately beyond the scope of this report and we refer to existing publications (Gruzelier, 2014b; Rogala et al., 2016; Hampson et al., 2019). It would be certainly not correct to view the limiting factors exclusively from the aspect of NFB, but rather it is necessary to raise awareness about the inferences in this report. Our results are restricted to oscillatory patterns prior to several NFB training sessions and are sensitive to and dependant on the variation of the sample. Additionally, the number of individuals in our sample who underwent a longitudinal clinical NFB trial would be considered moderate yet, for analysis in the GMM framework, it is in the lower ranges. Hence, the observations and inferences presented here can only be treated as qualitative on incidental results. Access to data collected on a larger, more diverse group would give better estimates of this potential dependence. Additionally, we must ask future researchers to consider and include intervention-specific outcomes (Hall et al., 2019), such as hearing thresholds, openness to technical novelties, measures from MRI examinations, and other clinically applicable measures as possible predictors for failure to control in their analysis.

Conclusion

Our findings support the idea that the treatment of tinnitus with NFB is a promising technique. However, individuals displayed heterogeneous trajectories during the training while low levels of health-related wellbeing seemed to be a prerequisite for the ability to modify the brain activity in the desired direction. In addition, our efforts to identify individual trajectories and thus bring clarity to the existing literature through the application of GMM would not have been possible if we had treated the individuals in our study of NFB treatment for tinnitus as a single group and used mean level data as adopted in previous studies. Our data-driven approach in this report presents a step toward enabling the translation of scientific findings into suitability for everyday medical practice, bettering the definition of tinnitus “subtypes” in heterogeneous treatment responses, and hence supporting precision medicine. To help achieve the vision of NFB becoming part of precision medicine, both the technology and the general understanding of tinnitus-specific brain activity require continued research, with special consideration being given to health-related wellbeing.

Data Availability Statement

The data is not yet openly available due to ongoing analysis. Thus, we regret that the data is not publicly accessible at this time. Requests to access these datasets should be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Kantonale Ethikkommission Project KEK-ZH-Nr. 2014-0594. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

DG carried out the neurofeedback training and the data acquisition. CR designed the model and the computational framework, analyzed the data, and wrote the manuscript. MM provided comments on the final draft. TK helped in supervise the project. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This project was received funding from the European Union’s Horizon 2020 Research and Innovation Program (Marie Skłodowska-Curie grant agreement no. 722046).

Author Disclaimer

The views expressed in this article are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the funders.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Footnotes

  1. ^ http://www.tinnet.tinnitusresearch.net/

References

Adjamian, P., Hall, D. A., Palmer, A. R., Allan, T. W., and Langers, D. R. M. (2014). Neuroanatomical abnormalities in chronic tinnitus in the human brain. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 45, 119–133. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.05.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Akaike, H. (1974). A new look at the statistical model identification. IEEE Trans. Automat. Contr. 19, 716–723. doi: 10.1109/TAC.1974.1100705

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alkoby, O., Abu-Rmileh, A., Shriki, O., and Todder, D. (2018). Can we predict who will respond to neurofeed-back? A review of the inefficacy problem and existing predictors for successful EEG neurofeedback learning. Neuroscience 378, 155–164. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.12.050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andersson, G., Lyttkens, L., and Larsen, H. C. (1999). Distinguishing levels of tinnitus distress. Clin. Otolar Yngol. Allied Sci. 24, 404–410. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2273.1999.00278.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Angermeyer, C., Kilian, R., and Matschinger, H. (2002). Deutschsprachige version der WHO instrumente zur erfassung von lebensqualität WHOQOL-100 und WHOQOL-BREFM – rezension. Zeitschrift Für Medizinische Psychologie 11, 44–48.

Google Scholar

Baguley, D., McFerran, D., and Hall, D. (2013). Tinnitus. Lancet 382, 1600–1607. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(13)60142-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berlin, K. S., Parra, G. R., and Williams, N. A. (2014). An introduction to latent variable mixture modeling (part 2): longitudinal latent class growth analysis and growth mixture models. J. Pediatr. Psychol. 39, 188–203. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jst085

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berwick, D. M., Murphy, J. M., Goldman, P. A., Ware, J. E., Barsky, A. J., and Weinstein, M. C. (1991). Performance of a five-item mental health screening test. Med. Care 29, 169–176. doi: 10.1097/00005650-199102000-00008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brüggemann, P., Szczepek, A. J., Rose, M., McKenna, L., Olze, H., and Mazurek, B. (2016). Impact of multiple factors on the degree of tinnitus distress. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:341. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00341

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bullinger, M., Kirchberger, I., and Ware, J. (1995). Der deutsche SF-36 health survey übersetzung und psychometrische testung eines krankheitsübergreifenden instruments zur erfassung der gesundheitsbezogenen lebensqualität. Zeitschrift für Gesundheitswissenschaften J. Public Health 3:21. doi: 10.1007/BF02959944

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carver, C. S., Smith, R. G., Antoni, M. H., Petronis, V. M., Weiss, S., and Derhagopian, R. P. (2005). Optimistic personality and psychosocial well-being during treatment predict psychosocial well-being among long-term survivors of breast cancer. Health Psychol. 24, 508–516. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.24.5.508

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cederroth, C. R., Gallus, S., Hall, D. A., Kleinjung, T., Langguth, B., Maruotti, A., et al. (2019). Editorial: towards an understanding of tinnitus heterogeneity. Front. Aging Neurosci. 11:53. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2019.00053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cortese, S., Ferrin, M., Brandeis, D., Holtmann, M., Aggensteiner, P., Daley, D., et al. (2016). Neurofeedback for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: meta-analysis of clinical and neuropsychological outcomes from randomized controlled trials. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 55, 444–455. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2016.03.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crocetti, A., Forti, S., and Del Bo, L. (2011). Neurofeedback for subjective tinnitus patients. Auris Nasus Larynx 38, 735–738. doi: 10.1016/j.anl.2011.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, A., and El Refaie, A. (2000). “Epidemiology of tinnitus,” in Tinnitus Handbook, ed. R. S. Tyler (San Diego, CA: Singular Thomson Learning), 1–23.

Google Scholar

De Ridder, D., Elgoyhen, A. B., Romo, R., and Langguth, B. (2011). Phantom percepts: tinnitus and pain as persisting aversive memory networks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 8075–8080. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1018466108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Ridder, D., Vanneste, S., Langguth, B., and Llinas, R. (2015). Thalamocortical dysrhythmia: a theoretical update in tinnitus. Front. Neurol. 6:124. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2015.00124

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Ridder, D., Vanneste, S., Weisz, N., Londero, A., Schlee, W., Elgoyhen, A. B., et al. (2014). An integrative model of auditory phantom perception: tinnitus as a unified percept of interacting sepa-rable subnetworks. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 44, 16–32. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.03.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diaz Hernandez, L., Rieger, K., and Koenig, T. (2018). Low motivational incongruence predicts successful EEG resting-state neurofeedback performance in healthy adults. Neuroscience 378, 146–154. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.12.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dohrmann, K., Weisz, N., Schlee, W., Hartmann, T., and Elbert, T. (2007b). Neurofeedback for treating tinnitus. Prog. Brain Res. 166, 473–485. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(07)66046-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dohrmann, K., Elbert, T., Schlee, W., and Weisz, N. (2007a). Tuning the tinnitus percept by modification of synchronous brain activity. Restor. Neurol. Neurosci. 25, 371–378.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Doppelmayr, M., Klimesch, W., Hödlmoser, K., Sauseng, P., and Gruber, W. (2005). Intelligence related upper alpha desynchronization in a semantic memory task. Brain Res. Bull. 66, 171–177. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2005.04.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eggermont, J. J., and Roberts, L. E. (2004). The neuroscience of tinnitus. Trends Neurosci. 27, 676–682. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2004.08.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eggermont, R. J. L. (2012). Ringing Ears: The Neuroscience of Tinnitus. Lausanne: Frontiers Media SA, 261.

Google Scholar

Enriquez-Geppert, S., Huster, R. J., Scharfenort, R., Mokom, Z. N., Vosskuhl, J., Figge, C., et al. (2013). The morphology of midcingulate cortex predicts frontal-midline theta neu-rofeedback success. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:453. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00453

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Enriquez-Geppert, S., Huster, R. J., Scharfenort, R., Mokom, Z. N., Zimmermann, J., and Herrmann, C. S. (2014). Modulation of frontal-midline theta by neurofeedback. Biol. Psychol. 95, 59–69. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.02.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Firth, D. (1993). Bias reduction of maximum likelihood estimates. Biometrika 80:27. doi: 10.2307/2336755

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fovet, T., Micoulaud-Franchi, J.-A., Vialatte, F.-B., Lotte, F., Daudet, C., Batail, J.-M., et al. (2017). On assessing neurofeedback effects: should double-blind replace neurophysiological mechanisms? Brain 140:e63. doi: 10.1093/brain/awx211

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geifman, N., Lennon, H., and Peek, N. (2018). Patient stratification using longitudinal data – application of latent class mixed models. Stud. Health Technol. Inform. 247, 176–180.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

George, M. S., Ketter, T. A., Parekh, P. I., Horwitz, B., Herscovitch, P., and Post, R. M. (1995). Brain activity during transient sadness and happiness in healthy women. Am. J. Psychiatry 152, 341–351. doi: 10.1176/ajp.152.3.341

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gevensleben, H., Holl, B., Albrecht, B., Schlamp, D., Kratz, O., Studer, P., et al. (2009). Distinct EEG effects related to neurofeedback training in children with ADHD: a randomized controlled trial. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 74, 149–157. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2009.08.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goebel, G., and Floezinger, U. (2008). Pilot study to evaluate psychiatric co-morbidity in tinnitus patients with and without hyperacusis. Audiol. Med. 6, 78–84. doi: 10.1080/16513860801959100

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goulden, N., Khusnulina, A., Davis, N. J., Bracewell, R. M., Bokde, A. L., McNulty, J. P., et al. (2014). The salience network is responsible for switching between the default mode network and the central executive network: replication from DCM. Neuroimage 99, 180–190. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.05.052

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gruzelier, J. H. (2014a). EEG-neurofeedback for optimising performance. I: a review of cognitive and affective outcome in healthy participants. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 44, 124–141. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.09.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gruzelier, J. H. (2014b). EEG-neurofeedback for optimising performance. III: a review of methodological and theoretical considerations. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 44, 159–182. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.03.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guerra, A., Feurra, M., Pellegrino, G., and Brittain, J.-S. (2019). Investigating and modulating physiological and pathological brain oscillations: the role of oscillatory activity in neural plasticity. Neural Plast. 2019:9403195. doi: 10.1155/2019/9403195

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guidi, J., Rafanelli, C., and Fava, G. A. (2018). The clinical role of well-being therapy. Nord. J. Psychiatry 72, 447–453. doi: 10.1080/08039488.2018.1492013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Güntensperger, D., Kleinjung, T., Neff, P., Thüring, C., and Meyer, M. (2020). Combining neurofeedback with source estimation: evaluation of an sLORETA neurofeedback protocol for chronic tinnitus treatment. Restor. Neurol. Neurosci. 28, 283–299. doi: 10.3233/RNN-200992

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Güntensperger, D., Thüring, C., Kleinjung, T., Neff, P., and Meyer, M. (2019). Investigating the efficacy of an individualized alpha/delta neurofeedback protocol in the treatment of chronic tinnitus. Neural Plast. 2019, 1–15. doi: 10.1155/2019/3540898

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Güntensperger, D., Thüring, C., Meyer, M., Neff, P., and Kleinjung, T. (2017). Neurofeedback for tinnitus treatment – review and current concepts. Front. Aging Neurosci. 9:386. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00386

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haegens, S., Barczak, A., Musacchia, G., Lipton, M. L., Mehta, A. D., Lakatos, P., et al. (2015). Laminar profile and physiology of the α rhythm in primary visual, auditory, and somatosensory re-gions of neocortex. J. Neurosci. 35, 14341–14352. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0600-15.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haier, R. J., Siegel, B., Tang, C., Abel, L., and Buchsbaum, M. S. (1992). Intelligence and changes in regional cerebral glucose metabolic rate following learning. Intelligence 16, 415–426. doi: 10.1016/0160-2896(92)90018-M

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hall, D. A., Fackrell, K., Li, A. B., Thavayogan, R., Smith, S., Kennedy, V., et al. (2018). A narrative synthesis of research evidence for tinnitus-related complaints as reported by patients and their significant others. Health Qual. Life Outcomes 16:61. doi: 10.1186/s12955-018-0888-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hall, D. A., Haider, H., Szczepek, A. J., Lau, P., Rabau, S., Jones-Diette, J., et al. (2016). Systematic review of outcome domains and instruments used in clinical trials of tinnitus treatments in adults. Trials 17:270. doi: 10.1186/s13063-016-1399-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hall, D. A., Hibbert, A., Smith, H., Haider, H. F., Londero, A., Mazurek, B., et al. (2019). One size does not fit all: developing common standards for outcomes in early-phase clinical trials of sound-, psychology-, and pharmacology-based interventions for chronic subjective tinnitus in adults. Trends Hear. 23:2331216518824827. doi: 10.1177/2331216518824827

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hammer, E. M., Halder, S., Blankertz, B., Sannelli, C., Dickhaus, T., Kleih, S., et al. (2012). Psychological predictors of SMR-BCI performance. Biol. Psychol. 89, 80–86. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hampson, M., Ruiz, S., and Ushiba, J. (2019). Neurofeedback. Neuroimage 218:116473. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116473

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Han, J. J., de Ridder, D., Vanneste, S., Chen, Y.-C., Koo, J.-W., and Song, J.-J. (2020). Pre-treatment ongoing cortical oscillatory activity predicts improvement of tinnitus after partial peripheral reafferentation with hearing aids. Front. Neurosci. 14:410. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2020.00410

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hartmann, T., Lorenz, I., Müller, N., Langguth, B., and Weisz, N. (2014). The effects of neurofeedback on oscillatory processes related to tinnitus. Brain Topogr. 27, 149–157. doi: 10.1007/s10548-013-0295-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hasler, G. (2016). Well-being: an important concept for psychotherapy and psychiatric neuroscience. Psychother. Psychosom. 85, 255–261. doi: 10.1159/000447268

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hautzinger, M., Bailer, M., and Worall, H. (1994). Beck-Depressions-Inventar (BDI): Bearbeitung der deutschen Ausgabe. Testhandbuch, 2nd Edn. Bern: Huber, 32.

Google Scholar

Heinze, G. (2006). A comparative investigation of methods for logistic regression with separated or nearly separated data. Stat. Med. 25, 4216–4226. doi: 10.1002/sim.2687

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heinze, G., and Ploner, M. (2004). A SAS Macro, S-PLUS Library and R package to Perform Logistic Regression Without Convergence Problems. Section for Clinical Biometrics, CeMSIIS. Vienna: Medical University of Vienna.

Google Scholar

Heinze, G., and Puhr, R. (2010). Bias-reduced and separation-proof conditional logistic regression with small or sparse data sets. Stat. Med. 29, 770–777. doi: 10.1002/sim.3794

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Henry, J. A., Dennis, K. C., and Schechter, M. A. (2005). General review of tinnitus. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 48, 1204–1235. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2005/084)

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Henry, J. A., Schechter, M. A., Zaugg, T. L., Griest, S., Jastreboff, P. J., Vernon, J. A., et al. (2006). Outcomes of clinical trial: tinnitus masking versus tinnitus retraining therapy. J. Am. Acad. Audiol. 17, 104–132. doi: 10.3766/jaaa.17.2.4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hesser, H., and Andersson, G. (2014). Dimensional or categorical approach to tinnitus severity: an item re-sponse mixture modeling analysis of tinnitus handicap. Int. J. Behav. Med. 21, 982–988. doi: 10.1007/s12529-013-9375-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoare, D. J., Kowalkowski, V. L., Kang, S., and Hall, D. A. (2011). Systematic review and meta-analyses of ran-domized controlled trials examining tinnitus management. Laryngoscope 121, 1555–1564. doi: 10.1002/lary.21825

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hong, S. K., Park, S., Ahn, M.-H., and Min, B.-K. (2016). Top-down and bottom-up neurodynamic evidence in patients with tinnitus. Hear. Res. 342, 86–100. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2016.10.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hox, J. J. (2002). Multilevel Analysis: Techniques and Applications. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, 304.

Google Scholar

Huster, R. J., Mokom, Z. N., Enriquez-Geppert, S., and Herrmann, C. S. (2014). Brain-computer interfaces for EEG neurofeedback: peculiarities and solutions. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 91, 36–45. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2013.08.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jastreboff, P. J. (1990). Phantom auditory perception (tinnitus): mechanisms of generation and perception. Neurosci. Res. 8, 221–254. doi: 10.1016/0168-0102(90)90031-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jensen, M., Hüttenrauch, E., Schmidt, J., Andersson, G., Chavanon, M.-L., and Weise, C. (2020). Neurofeedback for tinnitus: study protocol for a randomised controlled trial assessing the specificity of an alpha/delta neurofeedback training protocol in alleviating both sound perception and psychological distress in a cohort of chronic tinnitus sufferers. Trials 21:382. doi: 10.1186/s13063-020-04309-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Joos, K., Vanneste, S., and de Ridder, D. (2012). Disentangling depression and distress networks in the tinnitus brain. PLoS One 7:e40544. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0040544

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jung, T., and Wickrama, K. A. S. (2008). An introduction to latent class growth analysis and growth mixture modeling. Soc. Pers. Psych. Comp. 2, 302–317. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2007.00054.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kadosh, K. C., and Staunton, G. (2019). A systematic review of the psychological factors that influence neu-rofeedback learning outcomes. Neuroimage 185, 545–555. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.10.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaplan, D. (ed.) (2004). The Sage Handbook of Quantitative Methodology for the Social Sciences. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage, 511.

Google Scholar

Kim, S. H., Jang, J. H., Lee, S.-Y., Han, J. J., Koo, J.-W., Vanneste, S., et al. (2016). Neural substrates predicting short-term improvement of tinnitus loudness and distress after modified tinnitus retraining therapy. Sci. Rep. 6:29140. doi: 10.1038/srep29140

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klaghofer, R., and Brähler, E. (2001). Konstruktion und teststatistische prüfung einer kurzform der SCL-90–R [Construction and test statistical evaluation of a short version of the SCL-90–R]. Zeitschrift Für Klinische Psychologie Psychiatrie Psychotherapie 49, 115–124.

Google Scholar

Kleinjung, T., Eichhammer, P., Langguth, B., Jacob, P., Marienhagen, J., Hajak, G., et al. (2005). Long-term effects of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) in patients with chronic tinnitus. Otolaryngol. Head Neck Surg. 132, 566–569. doi: 10.1016/j.otohns.2004.09.134

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kleinjung, T., Fischer, B., Langguth, B., Sand, P., Hajak, G., Dvorakova, J., et al. (2007). Validierung einer deutschsprachigen version des “Tinnitus handicap inventory”. Psychiat. Prax 34, 140–142. doi: 10.1055/s-2006-940218

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klimesch, W., Sauseng, P., and Hanslmayr, S. (2007). EEG alpha oscillations: the inhibition-timing hypothesis. Brain Res. Rev. 53, 63–88. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2006.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kober, S. E., Witte, M., Ninaus, M., Neuper, C., and Wood, G. (2013). Learning to modulate one’s own brain activity: the effect of spontaneous mental strategies. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:695. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00695

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Konareva, I. N. (2006). Correlations between the psychological peculiarities of an individual and the efficacy of a single neurofeedback session (by the EEG characteristics). Neurophysiology 38, 201–208. doi: 10.1007/s11062-006-0047-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kouijzer, M. E. J., van Schie, H. T., Gerrits, B. J. L., Buitelaar, J. K., and de Moor, J. M. H. (2013). Is EEG-biofeedback an effective treatment in autism spectrum disorders? A randomized controlled trial. Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeedback 38, 17–28. doi: 10.1007/s10484-012-9204-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koush, Y., Meskaldji, D.-E., Pichon, S., Rey, G., Rieger, S. W., Linden, D. E. J., et al. (2017). Learning control over emotion networks through connectivity-based neurofeedback. Cereb. Cortex 27, 1193–1202. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhv311

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Landgrebe, M., Azevedo, A., Baguley, D., Bauer, C., Cacace, A., Coelho, C., et al. (2012). Methodological aspects of clinical trials in tinnitus: a proposal for an international standard. J. Psychos. Res. 73, 112–121. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2012.05.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lasisi, A. O., and Gureje, O. (2011). Prevalence of insomnia and impact on quality of life among community elderly subjects with tinnitus. Ann. Otol. Rhinol. Laryngol. 120, 226–230. doi: 10.1177/000348941112000402

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Llinás, R. R., Ribary, U., Jeanmonod, D., Kronberg, E., and Mitra, P. P. (1999). Thalamocortical dysrhythmia: a neurological and neuropsychiatric syndrome characterized by magnetoencephalography. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 96, 15222–15227. doi: 10.1073/pnas.96.26.15222

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lockwood, A. H., Salvi, R. J., Coad, M. L., Towsley, M. L., Wack, D. S., and Murphy, B. W. (1998). The functional neuroanatomy of tinnitus: evidence for limbic system links and neural plasticity. Neurology 50, 114–120. doi: 10.1212/wnl.50.1.114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mahmoudian, S., Farhadi, M., Najafi-Koopaie, M., Darestani-Farahani, E., Mohebbi, M., Dengler, R., et al. (2013). Central auditory processing during chronic tinnitus as indexed by topographical maps of the mismatch negativity obtained with the multi-feature paradigm. Brain Res. 1527, 161–173. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2013.06.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martines, F., Bentivegna, D., Martines, E., Sciacca, V., and Martinciglio, G. (2010). Assessing audiological, pathophysiological and psychological variables in tinnitus patients with or without hearing loss. Eur. Arch. Oto Rhino Laryngol. 267, 1685–1693. doi: 10.1007/s00405-010-1302-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marxen, M., Jacob, M. J., Müller, D. K., Posse, S., Ackley, E., Hellrung, L., et al. (2016). Amygdala regulation following fMRI-neurofeedback without instructed strategies. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:183. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00183

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCormack, A., Edmondson-Jones, M., Fortnum, H., Dawes, P. D., Middleton, H., Munro, K. J., et al. (2015). Investigating the association between tinnitus severity and symptoms of depression and anxiety, while controlling for neuroticism, in a large middle-aged UK population. Int. J. Audiol. 54, 599–604. doi: 10.3109/14992027.2015.1014577

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McKenna, L., Hallam, R. S., and Hinchcliffef, R. (1991). The prevalence of psychological disturbance in neuro-otology outpatients. Clin. Otolaryngol. Allied Sci. 16, 452–456. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2273.1991.tb02091.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Menon, V. (2015). “Salience network,” in Brain Mapping: An Encyclopedic Reference, Vol. 1–3, ed. A. W. Toga (London: Academic Press), 597–611.

Google Scholar

Meyer, M., Luethi, M. S., Neff, P., Langer, N., and Büchi, S. (2014). Disentangling tinnitus distress and tinnitus presence by means of EEG power analysis. Neural Plast. 2014:468546. doi: 10.1155/2014/468546

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meyer, M., Neff, P., Grest, A., Hemsley, C., Weidt, S., and Kleinjung, T. (2017). EEG oscillatory power dissoci-ates between distress- and depression-related psychopathology in subjective tinnitus. Brain Res. 1663, 194–204. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2017.03.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moisset, X., and Bouhassira, D. (2007). Brain imaging of neuropathic pain. Neuroimage 37(Suppl. 1) S80–S88. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Møller, A. R. (2007). “The role of neural plasticity in tinnitus,” in Tinnitus: Pathophysiology and Treatment: Pathophysiology and Treatment, 1st Edn, Vol. 166, eds A. R. Moller, B. Langguth, G. Hajak, T. Kleinjung, and A. Cacace (Amsterdam: Elsevier textbooks), 37–544. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(07)66003-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mühlau, M., Rauschecker, J. P., Oestreicher, E., Gaser, C., Röttinger, M., Wohlschläger, A. M., et al. (2006). Structural brain changes in tinnitus. Cereb. Cortex 16, 1283–1288. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhj070

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muthén, B. (2004). “Latent variable analysis: growth mixture modeling and related techniques for longitudinal data,” in The Sage Handbook of Quantitative Methodology for the Social Sciences, ed. D. Kaplan (Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage), 346–369.

Google Scholar

Muthen, B., and Muthen, L. K. (2000). Integrating person-centered and variable-centered analyses: growth mixture modeling with latent trajectory classes. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 24, 882–891. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2000.tb02070.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nan, W., Wan, F., Vai, M. I., and Da Rosa, A. C. (2015). Resting and initial beta amplitudes predict learning ability in beta/theta ratio neurofeedback training in healthy young adults. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9:677. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00677

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ninaus, M., Kober, S. E., Witte, M., Koschutnig, K., Neuper, C., and Wood, G. (2015). Brain volumetry and self-regulation of brain activity relevant for neurofeedback. Biol. Psychol. 110, 126–133. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2015.07.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nylund, K. L., Asparouhov, T., and Muthén, B. O. (2007). Deciding on the number of classes in latent class analysis and growth mixture modeling: a monte carlo simulation study. Struct. Equ. Model. Multidiscipl. J. 14, 535–569. doi: 10.1080/10705510701575396

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Omejc, N., Rojc, B., Battaglini, P. P., and Marusic, U. (2019). Review of the therapeutic neurofeedback method using electroencephalography: EEG Neurofeedback. Bosnian J. Basic Med. Sci. 19, 213–220. doi: 10.17305/bjbms.2018.3785

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peters, R. J., Iyer, A., Itti, L., and Koch, C. (2005). Components of bottom-up gaze allocation in natural images. Vis. Res. 45, 2397–2416. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2005.03.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Proust-Lima, C., Philipps, V., and Liquet, B. (2017). Estimation of extended mixed models using latent classes and latent processes: the R package lcmm. J. Stat. Soft. 78, 1–56. doi: 10.18637/jss.v078.i02

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pryce, H., Moutela, T., Bunker, C., and Shaw, R. (2019). Tinnitus groups: a model of social support and social connectedness from peer interaction. Br. J. Health Psychol. 24, 913–930. doi: 10.1111/bjhp.12386

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

R Core Team (2019). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Core Team.

Google Scholar

Ram, N., and Grimm, K. J. (2009). Growth mixture modeling: a method for identifying differences in longitu-dinal change among unobserved groups. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 33, 565–576. doi: 10.1177/0165025409343765

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reichert, J. L., Kober, S. E., Neuper, C., and Wood, G. (2015). Resting-state sensorimotor rhythm (SMR) power predicts the ability to up-regulate SMR in an EEG-instrumental conditioning paradigm. Clin. Neurophysiol. 126, 2068–2077. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2014.09.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riha, C., Güntensperger, D., Kleinjung, T., and Meyer, M. (2020). Accounting for heterogeneity: mixed-effects models in resting-state EEG data in a sample of tinnitus sufferers. Brain Topogr. 33, 413–424. doi: 10.1007/s10548-020-00772-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riha, C., Güntensperger, D., Oschwald, J., Kleinjung, T., and Meyer, M. (2021). Application of latent growth curve modeling to predict individual trajectories during neurofeedback treatment for tinnitus. Prog. Brain Res. 263, 109–136. doi: 10.1016/bs.pbr.2021.04.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rinne, T., Koistinen, S., Salonen, O., and Alho, K. (2009). Task-dependent activations of human auditory cortex during pitch discrimination and pitch memory tasks. J. Neurosci. 29, 13338–13343. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3012-09.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rogala, J., Jurewicz, K., Paluch, K., Kublik, E., Cetnarski, R., and Wrobel, A. (2016). The do’s and don’ts of neurofeedback training: a review of the controlled studies using healthy adults. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:301. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00301

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rossi, S., de Capua, A., Ulivelli, M., Bartalini, S., Falzarano, V., Filippone, G., et al. (2007). Effects of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation on chronic tinnitus: a randomised, crossover, double blind, placebo controlled study. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 78, 857–863. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2006.105007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sadaghiani, S., Hesselmann, G., and Kleinschmidt, A. (2009). Distributed and antagonistic contributions of ongoing activity fluctuations to auditory stimulus detection. J. Neurosci. 29, 13410–13417. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2592-09.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schabus, M. (2017). Reply: on assessing neurofeedback effects: should double-blind replace neurophysiological mechanisms? Brain 140:e64. doi: 10.1093/brain/awx212

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schlee, W., Hartmann, T., Langguth, B., and Weisz, N. (2009). Abnormal resting-state cortical coupling in chronic tinnitus. BMC Neurosci. 10:11. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-10-11

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwarz, G. (1978). Estimating the dimension of a model. Ann. Stat. 6, 461–464. doi: 10.1214/aos/1176344136

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sclove, S. L. (1987). Application of model-selection criteria to some problems in multivariate analysis. Psychometrika 52, 333–343. doi: 10.1007/BF02294360

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scott, B., and Lindberg, P. (2000). Psychological profile and somatic complaints between help-seeking and non-help-seeking tinnitus subjects. Psychosomatics 41, 347–352. doi: 10.1176/appi.psy.41.4.347

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seeley, W. W., Menon, V., Schatzberg, A. F., Keller, J., Glover, G. H., Kenna, H., et al. (2007). Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. J. Neurosci. 27, 2349–2356. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5587-06.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shi, L., Sun, J., Wu, X., Wei, D., Chen, Q., Yang, W., et al. (2018). Brain networks of happiness: dynamic functional connectivity among the default, cognitive and salience networks relates to sub-jective well-being. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 13, 851–862. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsy059

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simões, J., Schlee, W., Schecklmann, M., Langguth, B., Farahmand, D., and Neff, P. (2019). Big five personality traits are associated with tinnitus improvement over time. Sci. Rep. 9:18234. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-53845-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skinner, B. F. (1938). The Behaviour of Organisms: An Experimental Analysis. New York, NY: D. Appleton & Company.

Google Scholar

Sridharan, D., Levitin, D. J., and Menon, V. (2008). A critical role for the right fronto-insular cortex in switching between central-executive and default-mode networks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 105, 12569–12574. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0800005105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thibault, R. T., Lifshitz, M., and Raz, A. (2017). Neurofeedback or neuroplacebo? Brain 140, 862–864. doi: 10.1093/brain/awx033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trevis, K. J., McLachlan, N. M., and Wilson, S. J. (2018). A systematic review and meta-analysis of psychological functioning in chronic tinnitus. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 60, 62–86. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2017.12.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tyler, R., Coelho, C., Tao, P., Ji, H., Noble, W., Gehringer, A., et al. (2008). Identifying tinnitus sub-groups with cluster analysis. Am. J. Audiol. 17, S176–S184. doi: 10.1044/1059-0889(2008/07-0044)

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van der Loo, E., Congedo, M., Vanneste, S., van de Heyning, P., and de Ridder, D. (2011). Insular lateralization in tinnitus distress. Autonom. Neurosci. Basic Clin. 165, 191–194. doi: 10.1016/j.autneu.2011.06.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vanneste, S., and De Ridder, D. (2012). The auditory and non-auditory brain areas involved in tinnitus. An emergent property of multiple parallel overlapping subnetworks. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 6:31. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2012.00031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vanneste, S., Plazier, M., van der Loo, E., de van Heyning, P., Congedo, M., and de Ridder, D. (2010). The neural correlates of tinnitus-related distress. Neuroimage 52, 470–480. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.04.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vernon, D., Dempster, T., Bazanova, O., Rutterford, N., Pasqualini, M., and Andersen, S. (2009). Alpha neu-rofeedback training for performance enhancement: reviewing the methodology. J. Neurother. 13, 214–227. doi: 10.1080/10874200903334397

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wallhäusser-Franke, E., Schredl, M., and Delb, W. (2013). Tinnitus and insomnia: is hyperarousal the common denominator? Sleep Med. Rev. 17, 65–74. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2012.04.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wan, F., Nan, W., Vai, M. I., and Rosa, A. (2014). Resting alpha activity predicts learning ability in alpha neu-rofeedback. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:500. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00500

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ware, J. E., and Sherbourne, C. D. (1992). The MOS 36-ltem short-form health survey (SF-36). Med. Care 30, 473–483. doi: 10.1097/00005650-199206000-00002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weber, E., Köberl, A., Frank, S., and Doppelmayr, M. (2011). Predicting successful learning of SMR neurofeedback in healthy participants: methodological considerations. Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeedback 36, 37–45. doi: 10.1007/s10484-010-9142-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weber, L. A., Ethofer, T., and Ehlis, A.-C. (2020). Predictors of neurofeedback training outcome: a systematic review. Neuroimage 27:102301. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2020.102301

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weisz, N., Müller, S., Schlee, W., Dohrmann, K., Hartmann, T., and Elbert, T. (2007b). The neural code of auditory phantom perception. J. Neurosci. 27, 1479–1484. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3711-06.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weisz, N., Dohrmann, K., and Elbert, T. (2007a). The relevance of spontaneous activity for the coding of the tinnitus sensation. Prog. Brain Res. 166, 61–70. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(07)66006-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weisz, N., Hartmann, T., Müller, N., Lorenz, I., and Obleser, J. (2011). Alpha rhythms in audition: cognitive and clinical perspectives. Front. Psychol. 2:73. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wickham, H. (2016). ggplot2: Elegant Graphics for Data Analysis, 2nd Edn. Cham: Springer International Publishing, 260.

Google Scholar

Wickrama, K. A. S., Lee, T. K., O’Neal, C. W., and Lorenz, F. O. (2016). Higher-Order Growth Curves And mixture Modeling with Mplus: A Practical Guide. New York, NY: Routledge Taylor & Francis Group, 326.

Google Scholar

Witte, M., Kober, S. E., Ninaus, M., Neuper, C., and Wood, G. (2013). Control beliefs can predict the ability to up-regulate sensorimotor rhythm during neurofeedback training. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:478. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00478

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Witte, M., Kober, S. E., and Wood, G. (2018). Noisy but not placebo: defining metrics for effects of neurofeedback. Brain 141:e40. doi: 10.1093/brain/awy060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zirke, N., Seydel, C., Szczepek, A. J., Olze, H., Haupt, H., and Mazurek, B. (2013). Psychological comorbidity in patients with chronic tinnitus: analysis and comparison with chronic pain, asthma or atopic dermati-tis patients. Qual. Life Res. 22, 263–272. doi: 10.1007/s11136-012-0156-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zöger, S., Svedlund, J., and Holgers, K.-M. (2006). Relationship between tinnitus severity and psychiatric disorders. Psychosomatics 47, 282–288. doi: 10.1176/appi.psy.47.4.282

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Appendix

TABLE A1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE A1. Demographic, health, and tinnitus characteristics of the two individuals of the decliner class.

Keywords: tinnitus, neurofeedback (NFB), inefficacy problem, EEG, brain computer interface, growth mixture model, responder, heterogeneity

Citation: Riha C, Güntensperger D, Kleinjung T and Meyer M (2022) Recovering Hidden Responder Groups in Individuals Receiving Neurofeedback for Tinnitus. Front. Neurosci. 16:867704. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2022.867704

Received: 01 February 2022; Accepted: 23 May 2022;
Published: 23 June 2022.

Edited by:

Julian Keil, University of Kiel, Germany

Reviewed by:

Derek James Hoare, University of Nottingham, United Kingdom
Agnieszka J. Szczepek, Charité Universitätsmedizin Berlin, Germany

Copyright © 2022 Riha, Güntensperger, Kleinjung and Meyer. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Constanze Riha, constanzeriha@gmail.ch, orcid.org/0000-0002-6006-7018

These authors have contributed equally to this work and share last authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.