Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurosci., 03 August 2022
Sec. Brain Imaging Methods

Remote cortical atrophy and language outcomes after chronic left subcortical stroke with aphasia

  • 1Department of Neurology and Stroke Center, Guangdong Provincial Key Laboratory of Diagnosis and Treatment of Major Neurological Diseases, National Key Clinical Department and Key Discipline of Neurology, The First Affiliated Hospital, Sun Yat-sen University, Guangzhou, China
  • 2Department of Radiology, The First Affiliated Hospital, Sun Yat-sen University, Guangzhou, China

Objective: Subcortical stroke can cause a variety of language deficits. However, the neural mechanisms underlying subcortical aphasia after stroke remain incompletely elucidated. We aimed to determine the effects of distant cortical structures on aphasia outcomes and examine the correlation of cortical thickness measures with connecting tracts integrity after chronic left subcortical stroke.

Methods: Thirty-two patients and 30 healthy control subjects underwent MRI scanning and language assessment with the Western Aphasia Battery-Revised (WAB-R) subtests. Among patients, the cortical thickness in brain regions that related to language performance were assessed by the FreeSurfer software. Fiber tracts connecting the identified cortical regions to stroke lesions were reconstructed to determine its correlations with the cortical thickness measures across individual patient.

Results: Cortical thickness in different parts of the left fronto-temporo-parietal (FTP) regions were positively related to auditory-verbal comprehension, spontaneous speech and naming/word finding abilities when controlling for key demographic variables and lesion size. Cortical thickness decline in the identified cortical regions was positively correlated with integrity loss of fiber tracts connected to stroke lesions. Additionally, no significant difference in cortical thickness was found across the left hemisphere between the subgroup of patients with hypoperfusion (HP) and those without HP at stroke onset.

Conclusions: These findings suggest that remote cortical atrophy independently predicts language outcomes in patients with chronic left subcortical stroke and aphasia and that cortical thinning in these regions might relate to integrity loss of fiber tracts connected to stroke lesions.

Introduction

Aphasia is observed in about one third of patients with ischemic or hemorrhagic stroke. Empirically, aphasia is attributed to lesions in cortical regions such as Broca’s and Wernicke’s areas after stroke. In a follow-up computed tomography investigation, stroke lesion has been demonstrated to predict the outcomes of post-stroke aphasia at a modest degree (Worrall et al., 2001). In contrast, language deficits have been reported in patients with subcortical lesions in the internal capsule, putamen, thalamus, basal ganglia, and periventricular white matter (Naeser et al., 1982; Kuljic-Obradovic, 2003; Dronkers et al., 2017). At present, the underlying mechanisms of subcortical aphasia remain incompletely known.

Accumulating evidence has suggested subcortical aphasia to be resultant of cortical hypoperfusion (HP) in brain regions that support language function. Previous studies have showed that the degree of cortical HP is associated with language outcomes in patients with acute subcortical stroke (Olsen et al., 1986; Perani et al., 1987; Okuda et al., 1994; Hillis et al., 2002). The severity of subcortical aphasia has been found to relate to the degree of cortical HP (Choi et al., 2007). In contrast, language deficits can be recovered when cortical perfusion is regained (Vallar et al., 1988). Even in the chronic stage of subcortical stroke, patients might have language deficits that relate to persistent cortical HP (Peñaloza et al., 2014). However, a recent study has showed that cortical HP was not observed in most patients with left thalamic infarction and aphasia (Sebastian et al., 2014). These findings imply that potential alternative mechanisms might be involved in subcortical aphasia.

It has been shown that subcortical stroke can lead to structural changes in remote cortical regions. The role of remote cortical changes in functional outcomes is controversial. Motor functional recovery is related to gray matter volumes of cortical regions (Dang et al., 2013), or cortical thickness measures (Lotan et al., 2019) following subcortical stroke. Other studies have showed remote cortical atrophy that determined by connectivity to the primary lesion was not related to motor deficits after subcortical stroke (Cheng et al., 2015; Liu et al., 2020). With respect to aphasia, secondary cortical degeneration may result from impairment of white matter tracts that are crucial for reorganization after stroke (Duering et al., 2015). Integrity of arcuate fasciculus and uncinate fasciculus can predict the severity and recovery of subcortical aphasia (Noh et al., 2021; Zhang et al., 2021). While great efforts have been devoted to understanding the neural basis for subcortical aphasia, the effects of subcortical stroke on cortical thickness and language outcomes remain incompletely investigated. As the left fronto-temporo-parietal (FTP) cortex is commonly implicated in language processes. It might be expected that cortical regions beyond the subcortical lesion may undergo atrophy due to disconnection and have a negative impact on language outcomes following subcortical stroke in the left hemisphere.

To text this hypothesis, we aimed to determine the potential correlates of cortical thickness with language outcomes in patients with chronic left subcortical stroke and aphasia. By combining tractography with cortical thickness measures, we further explored the possible relationships between cortical thickness changes and integrity of fiber tracts connected to subcortical lesions. We hypothesized that subcortical stroke induced cortical thickness changes in connecting brain regions and related to language outcomes at the chronic stage of subcortical stroke.

Materials and methods

Participants

Thirty-two patients with chronic left subcortical stroke (ischemic or hemorrhagic) and history of aphasia were recruited with the following inclusion criteria: (i) Native Chinese speakers; (ii) over 6 months after stroke; (iii) unilateral subcortical lesion; (iv) ability to perform tests; and (v) absence of prior diagnosis of neurological disease. Based on medical records, all patients had aphasia at the time of stroke and received varied types of speech–language rehabilitation. Thirty age-matched healthy subjects with no history of neurological disease were recruited in the study.

The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the First Affiliated Hospital of Sun Yat-sen University, and written informed consent was obtained from all participants before enrollment in the study.

Language assessment

Language performance of all participants was assessed using the Western Aphasia Battery-Revised (WAB-R) (Shewan and Kertesz, 1980), which was translated into Chinese version. The language assessment was administrated and scored by two neurologists (S.H.F and H.J.T). The WAB-R includes separate subtests that provide composite scores for spontaneous speech, repetition, naming/word finding, and auditory-verbal comprehension. The total of these composite scores is the aphasia quotient, a measure of overall aphasia severity ranging from 0 to 100.

Image acquisition

Image data were acquired on a Siemens 3T Trio scanner with a 12-channel coil, and three-dimensional T1-weighted structural images were acquired using the magnetization-prepared rapid gradient-echo imaging sequence: TR (relaxation time) = 1,900 ms; TE (echo time) = 2.56 ms; flip angle = 9°; field of view (FOV) = 250 mm × 250 mm; 192 contiguous 1-mm sagittal slices; voxel size = 1 mm × 1 mm × 1 mm). Diffusion-weighted images were acquired using the single-shot echo-planar imaging sequence: TR = 8,700 ms; TE = 90 ms; flip angle = 90°; FOV = 240 mm × 240 mm; 64 3-mm sagittal slices; voxel size = 2 mm × 2 mm × 3 mm; and 64 diffusion volumes weighted with bmax = 1,100 s/mm2 and one volume with no diffusion gradient (b0 = 0 s/mm2).

Data preprocessing

Structural data preprocessing

Subcortical stroke lesions were segmented on the T1-weighted images in native space using MRIcron software1 and independently confirmed by two neurologists (S.H.F and S.H.X). The raw lesion maps were then normalized to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space so as to allow direct comparisons across individuals, using the enantiomorphic approach (Nachev et al., 2008) with minor modifications by using the unified segmentation approach in SPM12 with CAT12.2 The lesion overlap map is shown in Figure 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Lesion overlap map of patients with chronic subcortical stroke. Lesions from 32 patients were normalized to the MNI space. The n-value denotes the number of patients with a lesion in each voxel (maximum 20 out of 32).

Support vector regression-based lesion-symptom mapping (SVR-LSM), a multivariate lesion-symptom mapping approach, was employed to examine critical subcortical areas in the left hemisphere in which damage relates to language deficits as previously described (DeMarco and Turkeltaub, 2018). SVR-LSM analyses were performed for spontaneous speech, naming/word finding, repetition and auditory-verbal comprehension, respectively. Only voxels damaged in at least 10% of patients were included in the analysis. Probabilistic maps created using 10,000 permutations of the behavioral scores were thresholded at a voxel-level of P < 0.005 and cluster-level of P < 0.05.

The structural MRI data were preprocessed using the automated surface-based analysis package FreeSurfer (version 7.1.13) with standard procedures and parameters. Cortical thickness measures by FreeSurfer software have been confirmed by histological and manual measurements (Fischl et al., 2004). The semiautomatic processes included motion correction, intensity normalization, non-linear registration, white and gray matter segmentation, and surface mesh representation of the cortex. All images were visually inspected, and cortical segmentations were manually corrected if necessary. Cortical thickness maps were registered to the FreeSurfer surface template (fsaverage) and smoothed with a full-width half maximum Gaussian kernel of 15 mm.

Diffusion data preprocessing

Diffusion-weighted images were preprocessed following the standard protocols using the FMRIB software library (FSL4). The main procedure included the following steps: skull removal with the brain extraction tool, correction for head motion and eddy current distortions using FMRIB’s Diffusion Toolbox, building diffusion tensor models, and calculation of fractional anisotropy (FA) maps using the diffusion tensor imaging fractional intensity threshold (DTIFIT) tools.

White matter connectivity reconstruction

For fiber tracking, the cortical regions that exhibited association with language outcomes were transformed back to the native DWI space, and the transformed cortical regions and lesion mask in native space were defined as paired regions of interest (ROIs). To reconstruct the fiber tracts in the contralesional hemisphere, we defined homologous ROIs in the contralateral hemisphere based on the mirrored stroke lesion and cortical regions for each patient. The representative ROIs in subject native space are shown in Supplementary Figure 1. Connections between the lesion and cortical ROIs or their mirrored homologs were reconstructed using the probabilistic tracking of crossing fibers (probtrackx) and Bayesian estimation of diffusion parameters (bedpostx) algorithms implemented in FSL as previously described (Smith et al., 2004; Behrens et al., 2007). Briefly, fiber tracking was conducted using subcortical lesion as seed and cortical regions as target in native space of individual patient. From each mask voxel, 5,000 streamline samples with a step length of 0.5 mm and a curvature threshold of 0.2 were generated to map the probabilistic connection pattern. The tract density map was obtained by dividing by the total number of streamline samples. Fiber tracking was performed in both directions from seed to target and backward, and the connection maps were averaged. The final tract density map was obtained by dividing by the total number of streamlined samples, and then thresholded at 1% to exclude spurious connections as suggested in the previous studies (Duering et al., 2012; Cheng et al., 2015, 2020; Xing et al., 2018). Thereafter, FA values of reconstructed connections were extracted for individual patients. For visual inspection, the resultant tracts were normalized to the MNI space to show the connections within each voxel of the tract across patients with the non-linear parameters using the FNIRT tool in FSL.

Statistical analysis

To determine cortical thickness association with language measures, cortical thickness maps were entered into separate multiple regression analyses using spontaneous speech, naming/word finding, repetition, and auditory-verbal comprehension as explanatory variables controlling for key demographic factors and lesion size as confounding variables. Multiple comparisons were corrected at a voxel-wise threshold of p < 0.005 and a cluster-wise level of p < 0.05, as empirically determined by Monte Carlo Simulation.

To clarify how the nuisance covariates included in the cortical thickness analysis contributed to language outcomes with and without cortical thickness measure in the model, the variables were further introduced into separate hierarchical linear regression with language outcomes as the dependent variables. Separate univariate analyses were performed to address group differences in mean cortical thickness in each identified cluster between patients and healthy subjects by controlling for the above demographic variables. Paired t-tests were conducted to compare white matter measures between the left and right hemispheres. Further, partial correlation analyses were performed to explore the relationships between cortical thickness in each cluster and mean FA values of the tracts connecting stroke lesions and cortical clusters by excluding the indicated demographic variables and lesion size. These analyses were performed using SPSS (version 22).

Results

Demographic details and language scores

The present study included 32 patients with subcortical stroke (24 infarcts and 8 hemorrhages) and 30 healthy control subjects. The demographic details and language scores of participants are shown in Table 1 and Supplementary Table 1. There were no significant differences in age, gender, educational level, or handedness between patients and healthy subjects (all p > 0.05). The mean time from stroke onset was 14.94 months (interquartile range = 7.8–35.7 months), and the mean lesion volume was 6.74 cm3 (interquartile range = 3.5–13.3 cm3). Among all patients, 26 suffered from aphasia and 6 patients were fully recovered. In aphasic patients, 25 (78%) were anomic aphasia. Significant differences were observed in the outcomes of naming/word finding [F(1, 56) = 18.31, p < 0.001], spontaneous speech [F(1, 56) = 8.87, p = 0.004] and auditory-verbal comprehension [F(1, 56) = 10.34, p = 0.002], but not repetition [F(1, 56) = 2.17, p = 0.146] in patients compared with healthy subjects when controlling for age, gender, educational level, and handedness.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic details and language performance in patients and healthy subjects.

Identification of critical areas related to language outcomes

We explored the potential associations of subcortical lesion location with different language scores by SVR-LSM. The results showed that no subcortical region in the left hemisphere in which damage relates to language scores was identified across patients (Data are not shown).

Next, separate regression analyses were performed to identify cortical thickness changes related to language scores by factoring out confounding variables. The correlation analysis showed education and lesion size significantly related to language outcomes (see Supplementary Table 2). Considering the possible impacts of age and time from stroke onset on cerebral volume (Nitkunan et al., 2011; Walhovd et al., 2011; Brodtmann et al., 2012; Takao et al., 2012), we included age, education level, time from stroke onset and lesion size as the key nuisances in the separate regression model. The results showed that cortical thickness in the left inferior parietal cortex and the posterior superior temporal gyrus was positively related to the auditory-verbal comprehension score (Figure 2Aa and Table 2). In contrast, cortical thickness in the left medial/lateral orbitofrontal gyrus was positively related to spontaneous speech outcome (Figure 2Ba and Table 2). As for naming ability, cortical thickness in the left inferior precentral gyrus, pars orbitalis, and left posterior cingulum was positively related to the naming/word finding score (Figures 2Ca,Cd and Table 2). In addition, cortical thickness in the right lateral orbitofrontal cortex, pars orbitalis, and pars triangularis was positively related to naming/word finding performance (Figure 2Da and Table 2). No negative relationships were found between cortical thickness and language performance.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Cortical thickness in brain regions related to language outcomes. Surface representation of the significant clusters of cortical thickness related to the auditory-verbal comprehension score in the left inferior parietal cortex and superior temporal gyrus (Aa), the spontaneous speech score in the left orbitofrontal gyrus (Ba), and the naming/word finding score in the left inferior precentral gyrus (Ca), left posterior cingulate gyrus (Cd), and right orbitofrontal gyrus (Da). All analyses were performed by controlling for demographic variables and lesion size, and corrected at a voxel-wise threshold of P < 0.005 and a cluster-wise level of P < 0.05, determined by Monte Carlo Simulation. (Ab–Cb,Ce,Db) Scatter plot showing partial regression using language scores as dependent variables and cortical thickness (CT) of clusters as independent factors by controlling for the above key variables (all P < 0.001). (Ac–Cc,Cf,Dc) Cortical thickness changes in each identified cluster in patients relative to controls factoring out demographic variables.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Clusters of cortical thickness related to language performance in patients.

To clarify the relationships between aphasia outcomes, cortical thickness measures, and confounding factors included in the above cortical thickness analyses, we next performed separate hierarchical regressions using the language scores as dependent variables. Age, education level, time from stroke onset and lesion size were entered first, followed by cortical thickness measures. When cortical thickness measure was excluded, the hierarchical regression analyses showed that only lesion size significantly predicted the abilities of auditory-verbal comprehension [t(27) = −4.11, p < 0.001], spontaneous speech [t(27) = −2.79, p = 0.010], and naming/word finding [t(26) = −3.56, p = 0.001]. When cortical thickness measure was added to the models, cortical thickness in the respective left hemisphere clusters was the only significant predictor for the corresponding language scores (auditory-verbal comprehension cluster: R2 change = 0.214, p < 0.001; spontaneous speech cluster: R2 change = 0.258, p < 0.001; naming/word finding cluster 1: R2 change = 0.171, p = 0.001 and cluster 2: R2 change = 0.065, p = 0.024). For naming, when cortical thickness measures in the clusters were all entered to the model, only the right hemisphere cluster was a significant predictor [t(24) = 2.20, p = 0.001]. Residual plots of the relationships between cortical thickness in the identified clusters and language performance scores are shown in Figures 2Ab–Cb,Ce,Db.

Cortical thickness differences in patients compared with healthy subjects

Mean cortical thickness in the identified clusters was compared between aphasic patients and healthy subjects by factoring out age, gender, educational level, and handedness. Mean cortical thickness was significantly lower in patients than in healthy subjects in the left hemisphere clusters related to the outcomes of auditory-verbal comprehension [F(1, 56) = 5.13, p = 0.027; Figure 2Ac] and spontaneous speech [F(1, 56) = 5.47, p = 0.023; Figure 2Bc]. For naming/word finding, mean cortical thickness was significantly lower in patients than in healthy subjects in the left hemisphere clusters [cluster 1: F(1, 56) = 18.49, p < 0.001; cluster 2: F(1, 56) = 7.31, p = 0.009; Figures 2Cc,f]. In contrast, no significant difference was observed between groups in the mean cortical thickness in the right hemisphere cluster [F(1, 56) = 0.06, p = 0.804; Figure 2Dc].

Correlation of connecting tract integrity with cortical thickness in patients

White matter tracts connecting stroke lesion to the identified cortical regions and the mirrored homologous tracts were successfully reconstructed for all patients. The representative fiber tracts in a common space across patients are showed in Figures 3Aa–Da. To determine the potential correlates between cortical thickness in the identified regions with integrity of the reconstructed tracts, separate hierarchical regression analyses were performed with cortical thickness in each cluster as a dependent variable. The demographic variables, lesion size and FA values of connecting tracts were added to the models as independent factors. The results showed that mean FA values of the left hemisphere tracts were positively related to cortical thickness in the left hemisphere clusters related to the scores of auditory-verbal comprehension [t(24) = 2.59, p = 0.016], spontaneous speech [t(24) = 2.58, p = 0.017], and naming/word finding [cluster 1: t(24) = 2.80, p = 0.010; cluster 2: t(24) = 2.74, p = 0.011]. Residual plots of the relationships between cortical thickness in the identified clusters and language performance scores are shown in Figures 3Ab–Db. No significant relationships were found between cortical thickness in the right hemisphere cluster for naming/word finding and lesion size or the interhemispheric tracts connecting to lesions (both p > 0.20). In addition, mean FA value of each tract was significantly lower in the left hemisphere than in the right hemisphere homolog [t(31) = −5.49 ∼ –7.17, all p < 0.001] (Figures 3Ac–Dc).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Integrity of white matter tracts related to cortical thickness in connecting brain regions. (Aa–Da) Reconstruction of fiber tracts connecting stroke lesions to cortical regions of interest (red) and the corresponding fiber tracts with mirrored stroke lesions and cortical regions related to language performance. Violet–red indicates the left hemisphere tracts and red–yellow indicates the mirrored tracts. All fiber tracking was thresholded at 1% of overall connectivity. (Ab–Db) Partial correlations between mean fractional anisotropy (FA) values of left fiber tracts and the corresponding cortical thickness measures when controlling for demographic variables and lesion size. (Ac–Dc) Paired comparisons of mean FA values of the left fiber tracts relative to their mirrored tracts (all P < 0.001).

Comparisons of cortical thickness in subgroups with different perfusion status

To explore the impacts of perfusion status on cortical thickness, we further compared the differences in cortical thickness between patients without hypoperfusion (NHP) and with HP according to perfusion computer tomography or perfusion weighted images at stroke onset. Female patients were more [χ2(1) = 4.50, p = 0.03] and the lesion size was larger in the HP group than in the NHP group [t(30) = −2.12, p = 0.04]. No significant difference was detected for language performance (all p > 0.80) (see Supplementary Table 3). When controlling for demographic variables and lesion size, a reduction of cortical thickness was identified in the right cuneus regions (cluster size = 1177.58 mm2; coordinates: x = 5.6, y = −67.7, z = 14.0; p = 0.0002) in the HP group relative to the NHP group. There was no significant difference in the left hemisphere between the two subgroups (Figure 4). A post hoc analysis was performed to compare cortical thickness in each cluster identified as above to relate to language performance by controlling for key variables. The results showed that the mean cortical thickness of the individual clusters was not different between the two subgroups (all p > 0.11).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Comparisons of cortical thickness in patients with different perfusion status at stroke onset. Surface representation of mean cortical thickness for hemispheres in subgroups without hypoperfusion (A) and with hypoperfusion (B). Color bar indicates cortical thickness changes in millimeter. (C) Representation of the statistically significant clusters showing a decline in cortical thickness in the right cuneus (cluster size = 1177.58 mm2; coordinates: x = 5.6, y = −67.7, z = 14.0; P = 0.0002, corrected for multiple comparisons), when controlling for demographic variables and lesion size.

Discussion

We found that cortical thickness changes in the left FTP regions were associated with language comprehension and production outcomes independent of inter-individual variability. Cortical thickness in the left FTP regions was lower in stroke patients with aphasia than in healthy subjects. Additionally, the cortical thinning was related to loss of microstructural integrity in fiber tracts connecting stroke lesions to cortical regions. Together, these findings suggest that remote secondary effects of subcortical stroke on the connecting cortical regions relate to language outcomes in chronic subcortical stroke.

Associations of cortical thickness with subcortical aphasia outcomes

We found relationships between declined cortical thickness and subcortical aphasia outcomes by covarying key confounding variables. Specifically, auditory-verbal comprehension was predicted by cortical thickness in the left temporoparietal regions including the posterior temporal gyrus and inferior parietal cortex. It has been clarified that verbal comprehension relies on cortex considerably beyond the traditional location of Wernicke’s area in the posterior temporal gyrus (Turken and Dronkers, 2011; DeWitt and Rauschecker, 2012). Previous studies have suggested that the left inferior parietal cortex, especially the angular gyrus, is an essential area for speech comprehension as well as production (Mesulam, 1998; Vigneau et al., 2006). In contrast, Hickok and Poeppel (2007) proposed a model of the anatomy of language that excludes the angular gyrus from speech comprehension and production. However, it has been included by more recent studies (Lau et al., 2008; Hartwigsen et al., 2015). With respect to speech production, we found that declined thickness in the left orbitofrontal lobe negatively predicted speech outcome following chronic subcortical stroke. To support this, the PET study in healthy subjects has showed that activity in response to speech production to be localized in the left lateral and medial orbitofrontal cortex (Awad et al., 2007). Additionally, our results showed that reductions of thickness in the left inferior precentral gyrus/pars opercularis gyrus and posterior cingulate were strongly predictive of naming outcome. To support this, the left precentral cortex and inferior frontal gyrus are demonstrated to associate with action naming and the phonology component during object naming (Schwartz et al., 2012). Our results also accord with the observations in the previous study that increased activation could be observed within the left posterior cingulate area in healthy subjects during a picture identification task (Szaflarski et al., 2011).

Homotopic regions are traditionally thought to hinder language recovery by transcallosal disinhibition of the lesioned hemisphere (Belin et al., 1996; Blank et al., 2003; Naeser et al., 2005; Winhuisen et al., 2005). However, recent studies reveal beneficial effects of language homologs in the right hemisphere to post-stroke language recovery (Xing et al., 2016; Hope et al., 2017). Similarly, we found cortical thickness of the right lateral orbitofrontal cortex to be also related to naming outcome. This finding is further supported by a previous study in which damage to the right prefrontal cortex impaired the language ability of choosing appropriate words (Taubner et al., 1999). However, cortical thickness in the right hemisphere regions showed no considerable difference relative to controls and was neither related to lesion size nor integrity of connecting fiber tracts in the present study. This likely reflects premorbid inter-individual differences in the right hemisphere regions that protect from language deficits after left subcortical stroke. Patients with greater premorbid cortical thickness in the right hemisphere might have better recovery of language deficits after left subcortical stroke.

Correlates of connecting tract integrity with cortical thickness in subcortical stroke

The current study demonstrated that there was a significant reduction of cortical thickness in the left hemisphere regions that related to language outcomes among patients as compared to the control subjects. These results might be indicative of secondary cortical atrophy in regions remote from stroke lesions as indicated in previous studies (Zhang et al., 2014). However, the exact mechanisms for structural impairment of the remote cortex remain unclear. It is assumed that fiber disconnection may account for this finding. By combining structural measurements with tractography, we found positive relationships between cortical thickness in the identified cortex and microstructural integrity of the connecting fiber tracts to subcortical lesions, but no significant correlation was seen the contralateral homologous tracts. Moreover, there was marked reductions of FA values in the tracts of lesional hemisphere relative to their contralateral homologs. These results might be reflective of secondary cortical effects of connecting fibers disruption after chronic subcortical stroke, as proposed by the diaschisis theory (Duering et al., 2012). Supporting this, previous studies have shown that focal cerebral infarction can induce progressive neuronal damage in the ipsilateral thalamus and substantia nigra remote from the lesion site (Nakane et al., 1992; Herve et al., 2005). Our results are also consistent with the findings of several longitudinal studies showing reduced thickness in the connecting cortical regions as a resultant of acute subcortical stroke (Cheng et al., 2015; Duering et al., 2015) or cortical remodeling in the bilateral hemisphere after basal ganglia infarction over several months (Liu et al., 2020). Additionally, retrograde neural degeneration of the retinal nerve fiber has been demonstrated among patients with occipital lobe stroke (Jindahra et al., 2012). Given the reconstructed tracts represent the anatomical connections between cortex and stroke lesions, these data implicate a possible link between subcortical aphasic stroke and ability to engage remote cortex in language processing secondary to fiber degeneration. Nevertheless, the casual effects of white matter disconnection on remote cortical atrophy cannot be determined from our results. The pathological mechanisms underlying the damage of connecting tracts are unclear. It might include retrograde or anterograde degeneration, dendrite shrinkage, and neuronal apoptosis because of loss of synaptic input (Siffrin et al., 2010).

Alternatively, acute subcortical aphasia might be resultant of concurrent cortical HP (Olsen et al., 1986; Wallesch et al., 1997). SPECT and perfusion weighted imaging measures revealed that focal lesion could cause remote cortical HP and hypometabolism (Carrera and Tononi, 2014). No significant differences in language scores were found between the two subgroups when controlling for key variables in the present study. In addition, the cortical thickness analysis showed no deference in cortical thickness of the left hemisphere in patients with HP relative to those with normal perfusion. This suggests that atrophy in the connected cortex was not driven by HP. However, cortical hypometabolism is also suggested in cases with subcortical aphasia (Halkar et al., 1997; de Boissezon et al., 2005; Sebastian et al., 2014). Additionally, specific cortical-subcortical circuits have been suggested to associate with language processing (Teichmann et al., 2015; Akinina et al., 2019; Jacquemot and Bachoud-Lévi, 2021). Thus, multiple neural mechanisms might contribute to aphasia outcomes after chronic subcortical stroke.

Limitations

There are several limitations in the present study. First, this is a cross-sectional investigation that might preclude the empirical evidence of remote cortical atrophy mediated by neurodegeneration of the connecting fiber tracts. Further longitudinal study is expected to confirm the present findings by providing the dynamics of secondary neurodegeneration in remote cortical regions. Secondly, the DTI findings should be interpreted with caution. The extracellular volume effects of free water have been proposed to contaminate and invalidate DTI metrics (Pierpaoli et al., 1996; Pasternak et al., 2009). Additionally, it has been suggested that free-water correction would improve DTI-based tract reconstruction and tissue specificity (Metzler-Baddeley et al., 2012; Hoy et al., 2014; Guder et al., 2021). Considering the fact that atrophy and neuroinflammation can still be observed even several months after stroke, free-water correction will strengthen the reliability of DTI analysis especially in stroke patients. Thirdly, the number of patients with different perfusion status was relatively small in the subgroup analyses, which also restricted the generalization of the conclusions. Further study is need to confirm the present findings with a larger sample of populations. Fourthly, speech therapy experience across patients might drive recruitment of cerebral cortex capable of supporting language function, thereby affects the language outcome. However, the type and dose of speech therapy are not easily quantifiable as a confounding factor in the current regression analyses. Nevertheless, our results are not likely driven by a specific language therapy because the nature and quantity of speech therapy varies between individuals. Lastly, the lack of targeted therapeutic intervention, such as the restoration of cerebral perfusion, was a limitation of the study. Direct intervention of cerebral flow should permit inferences on the HP effects on cortical thickness changes or language outcomes as previously reported (Hillis et al., 2002). Notably, our results were obtained by using FreeSurfer software that could detect changes in cortical thickness at the submillimeter scale. Additionally, the present study demonstrates the feasibility of investigating the neural base for subcortical aphasia with a combination of surface-based thickness analysis with connecting fiber tracking.

Conclusion

Our data provide direct evidence that cortical thickness in the left FTP regions is correlated with language outcomes in patients with chronic subcortical stroke and aphasia. The loss of microstructural integrity in connecting fiber tracts to stroke lesions might serve as the underlying mechanism for remote cortical atrophy in subcortical aphasia. These findings will improve the understanding of cerebral reorganization and language recovery after chronic subcortical stroke.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Institutional Review Board of the First Affiliated Hospital of Sun Yat-sen University. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study. Written informed consent was obtained from the individual(s) for the publication of any potentially identifiable images or data included in this article.

Author contributions

HT was responsible for data collection, analysis, and manuscript drafting. SF, XN, PX, and ZL collected the data. SX designed the work, analyzed the data, and drafted and revised the manuscript. All authors gave their final approval of the version to be published and agreed to be accountable for all aspects of the work.

Funding

This study was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (81971118 and 82171281), the Natural Science Foundation of Guangdong Province of China (2021A1515010094), the Science and Technology Planning Project of Guangzhou in China (201904010313), the Special Funds of Public Interest Research and Capacity Building of Guangdong Province (2014B020212003), the National Key Clinical Department and National Key Discipline, the Guangdong Provincial Key Laboratory for Diagnosis and Treatment of Major Neurological Diseases (2017B030314103), the Southern China International Cooperation Base for Early Intervention and Functional Rehabilitation of Neurological Diseases (2015B050501003), the Guangdong Provincial Engineering Center For Major Neurological Disease Treatment, and the Guangdong Provincial Translational Medicine Innovation Platform for Diagnosis and Treatment of Major Neurological Disease.

Acknowledgments

We thank our participants for their involvement in the study.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnins.2022.853169/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

  1. ^ https://www.nitrc.org/projects/mricron
  2. ^ http://www.neuro.uni-jena.de/cat/
  3. ^ http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu
  4. ^ http://www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/

References

Akinina, Y., Dragoy, O., Ivanova, M. V., Iskra, E. V., Soloukhina, O. A., Petryshevsky, A. G., et al. (2019). Grey and white matter substrates of action naming. Neuropsychologia 131, 249–265. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2019.05.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Awad, M., Warren, J. E., Scott, S. K., Turkheimer, F. E., and Wise, R. J. (2007). A common system for the comprehension and production of narrative speech. J. Neurosci. 27, 11455–11464. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5257-06.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Behrens, T. E., Berg, H. J., Jbabdi, S., Rushworth, M. F., and Woolrich, M. W. (2007). Probabilistic diffusion tractography with multiple fibre orientations: What can we gain? Neuroimage 34, 144–155. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.09.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belin, P., Van Eeckhout, P., Zilbovicius, M., Remy, P., François, C., Guillaume, S., et al. (1996). Recovery from nonfluent aphasia after melodic intonation therapy: A PET study. Neurology 47, 1504–1511. doi: 10.1212/wnl.47.6.1504

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blank, S. C., Bird, H., Turkheimer, F., and Wise, R. J. (2003). Speech production after stroke: The role of the right pars opercularis. Ann. Neurol. 54, 310–320. doi: 10.1002/ana.10656

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brodtmann, A., Pardoe, H., Li, Q., Lichter, R., Ostergaard, L., and Cumming, T. (2012). Changes in regional brain volume three months after stroke. J. Neurol. Sci. 322, 122–128. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2012.07.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carrera, E., and Tononi, G. (2014). Diaschisis: Past, present, future. Brain 137, 2408–2422. doi: 10.1093/brain/awu101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheng, B., Dietzmann, P., Schulz, R., Boenstrup, M., Krawinkel, L., Fiehler, J., et al. (2020). Cortical atrophy and transcallosal diaschisis following isolated subcortical stroke. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 40, 611–621. doi: 10.1177/0271678X19831583

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheng, B., Schulz, R., Bonstrup, M., Hummel, F. C., Sedlacik, J., Fiehler, J., et al. (2015). Structural plasticity of remote cortical brain regions is determined by connectivity to the primary lesion in subcortical stroke. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 35, 1507–1514. doi: 10.1038/jcbfm.2015.74

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Choi, J. Y., Lee, K. H., Na, D. L., Byun, H. S., Lee, S. J., Kim, H., et al. (2007). Subcortical aphasia after striatocapsular infarction: Quantitative analysis of brain perfusion SPECT using statistical parametric mapping and a statistical probabilistic anatomic map. J. Nucl. Med. 48, 194–200.

Google Scholar

Dang, C., Liu, G., Xing, S., Xie, C., Peng, K., Li, C., et al. (2013). Longitudinal cortical volume changes correlate with motor recovery in patients after acute local subcortical infarction. Stroke 44, 2795–2801. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.113.000971

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Boissezon, X., Demonet, J. F., Puel, M., Marie, N., Raboyeau, G., Albucher, J. F., et al. (2005). Subcortical aphasia: A longitudinal PET study. Stroke 36, 1467–1473. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.0000169947.08972.4f

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DeMarco, A. T., and Turkeltaub, P. E. (2018). A multivariate lesion symptom mapping toolbox and examination of lesion-volume biases and correction methods in lesion-symptom mapping. Hum. Brain Mapp. 39, 4169–4182. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24289

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DeWitt, I., and Rauschecker, J. P. (2012). Phoneme and word recognition in the auditory ventral stream. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 109, E505–E514. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1113427109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dronkers, N. F., Ivanova, M. V., and Baldo, J. V. (2017). What Do Language Disorders Reveal about Brain-Language Relationships? From Classic Models to Network Approaches. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 23, 741–754. doi: 10.1017/S1355617717001126

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duering, M., Righart, R., Csanadi, E., Jouvent, E., Hervé, D., Chabriat, H., et al. (2012). Incident subcortical infarcts induce focal thinning in connected cortical regions. Neurology 79, 2025–2028. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182749f39

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duering, M., Righart, R., Wollenweber, F. A., Zietemann, V., Gesierich, B., and Dichgans, M. (2015). Acute infarcts cause focal thinning in remote cortex via degeneration of connecting fiber tracts. Neurology 84, 1685–1692. doi: 10.1212/wnl.0000000000001502

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischl, B., Salat, D. H., van der Kouwe, A. J., Makris, N., Segonne, F., Quinn, B. T., et al. (2004). Sequence-independent segmentation of magnetic resonance images. Neuroimage 23, S69–S84. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guder, S., Pasternak, O., Gerloff, C., and Schulz, R. (2021). Strengthened structure-function relationships of the corticospinal tract by free water correction after stroke. Brain Commun. 3:fcab034. doi: 10.1093/braincomms/fcab034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Halkar, R. K., Sisterhen, C., Ammons, J., Galt, J. R., and Alazraki, N. P. (1997). Tc-99m ECD SPECT imaging in aphasia caused by subcortical infarct. Clin. Nucl. Med. 22, 850–851. doi: 10.1097/00003072-199712000-00010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hartwigsen, G., Golombek, T., and Obleser, J. (2015). Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation over left angular gyrus modulates the predictability gain in degraded speech comprehension. Cortex 68, 100–110. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2014.08.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herve, D., Molko, N., Pappata, S., Buffon, F., LeBihan, D., Bousser, M. G., et al. (2005). Longitudinal thalamic diffusion changes after middle cerebral artery infarcts. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 76, 200–205. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2004.041012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hickok, G., and Poeppel, D. (2007). The cortical organization of speech processing. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 8, 393–402. doi: 10.1038/nrn2113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hillis, A. E., Wityk, R. J., Barker, P. B., Beauchamp, N. J., Gailloud, P., Murphy, K., et al. (2002). Subcortical aphasia and neglect in acute stroke: The role of cortical hypoperfusion. Brain 125, 1094–1104. doi: 10.1093/brain/awf113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hope, T. M. H., Leff, A. P., Prejawa, S., Bruce, R., Haigh, Z., Lim, L., et al. (2017). Right hemisphere structural adaptation and changing language skills years after left hemisphere stroke. Brain 140, 1718–1728. doi: 10.1093/brain/awx086

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoy, A. R., Koay, C. G., Kecskemeti, S. R., and Alexander, A. L. (2014). Optimization of a free water elimination two-compartment model for diffusion tensor imaging. Neuroimage 103, 323–333. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.09.053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jacquemot, C., and Bachoud-Lévi, A. C. (2021). Striatum and language processing: Where do we stand? Cognition 213:104785. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104785

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jindahra, P., Petrie, A., and Plant, G. T. (2012). The time course of retrograde trans-synaptic degeneration following occipital lobe damage in humans. Brain 135, 534–541. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr324

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuljic-Obradovic, D. C. (2003). Subcortical aphasia: Three different language disorder syndromes? Eur. J. Neurol. 10, 445–448. doi: 10.1046/j.1468-1331.2003.00604.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lau, E. F., Phillips, C., and Poeppel, D. (2008). A cortical network for semantics: (de)constructing the N400. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 920–933. doi: 10.1038/nrn2532

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, H., Peng, X., Dahmani, L., Wang, H., Zhang, M., Shan, Y., et al. (2020). Patterns of motor recovery and structural neuroplasticity after basal ganglia infarcts. Neurology 95:e1174–e1187. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000010149

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lotan, E., Tavor, I., Barazany, D., Ben-Amitay, S., Hoffmann, C., Tsarfaty, G., et al. (2019). Selective atrophy of the connected deepest cortical layers following small subcortical infarct. Neurology 92, e567–e575. doi: 10.1212/wnl.0000000000006884

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mesulam, M. M. (1998). From sensation to cognition. Brain 121, 1013–1052. doi: 10.1093/brain/121.6.1013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Metzler-Baddeley, C., O’Sullivan, M. J., Bells, S., Pasternak, O., and Jones, D. K. (2012). How and how not to correct for CSF-contamination in diffusion MRI. Neuroimage 59, 1394–1403. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.08.043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nachev, P., Coulthard, E., Jager, H. R., Kennard, C., and Husain, M. (2008). Enantiomorphic normalization of focally lesioned brains. Neuroimage 39, 1215–1226. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.10.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Naeser, M. A., Alexander, M. P., Helm-Estabrooks, N., Levine, H. L., Laughlin, S. A., and Geschwind, N. (1982). Aphasia with predominantly subcortical lesion sites: Description of three capsular/putaminal aphasia syndromes. Arch. Neurol. 39, 2–14. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1982.00510130004002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Naeser, M. A., Martin, P. I., Nicholas, M., Baker, E. H., Seekins, H., Kobayashi, M., et al. (2005). Improved picture naming in chronic aphasia after TMS to part of right Broca’s area: An open-protocol study. Brain Lang. 93, 95–105. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2004.08.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nakane, M., Teraoka, A., Asato, R., and Tamura, A. (1992). Degeneration of the ipsilateral substantia nigra following cerebral infarction in the striatum. Stroke 23, 328–332. doi: 10.1161/01.str.23.3.328

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nitkunan, A., Lanfranconi, S., Charlton, R. A., Barrick, T. R., and Markus, H. S. (2011). Brain atrophy and cerebral small vessel disease: A prospective follow-up study. Stroke 42, 133–138. doi: 10.1161/strokeaha.110.594267

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Noh, J. S., Lee, S., Na, Y., Cho, M., Hwang, Y. M., Tae, W.-S., et al. (2021). Integrity of arcuate fasciculus is a good predictor of language impairment after subcortical stroke. J. Neurolinguist. 58:100968. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2020.100968

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Okuda, B., Tanaka, H., Tachibana, H., Kawabata, K., and Sugita, M. (1994). Cerebral blood flow in subcortical global aphasia. Perisylvian cortical hypoperfusion as a crucial role. Stroke 25, 1495–1499. doi: 10.1161/01.str.25.7.1495

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Olsen, T. S., Bruhn, P., and Oberg, R. G. (1986). Cortical hypoperfusion as a possible cause of ‘subcortical aphasia’. Brain 109, 393–410. doi: 10.1093/brain/109.3.393

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pasternak, O., Sochen, N., Gur, Y., Intrator, N., and Assaf, Y. (2009). Free water elimination and mapping from diffusion MRI. Magn. Reson. Med. 62, 717–730. doi: 10.1002/mrm.22055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peñaloza, C., Rodríguez-Fornells, A., Rubio, F., De Miquel, M. A., and Juncadella, M. (2014). Language recovery and evidence of residual deficits after nonthalamic subcortical stroke: A 1 year follow-up study. J. Neurolinguist. 32, 16–30. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2014.08.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perani, D., Vallar, G., Cappa, S., Messa, C., and Fazio, F. (1987). Aphasia and neglect after subcortical stroke. A clinical/cerebral perfusion correlation study. Brain 110, 1211–1229. doi: 10.1093/brain/110.5.1211

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pierpaoli, C., Jezzard, P., Basser, P. J., Barnett, A., and Di Chiro, G. (1996). Diffusion tensor MR imaging of the human brain. Radiology 201, 637–648. doi: 10.1148/radiology.201.3.8939209

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwartz, M. F., Faseyitan, O., Kim, J., and Coslett, H. B. (2012). The dorsal stream contribution to phonological retrieval in object naming. Brain 135, 3799–3814. doi: 10.1093/brain/aws300

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sebastian, R., Schein, M. G., Davis, C., Gomez, Y., Newhart, M., Oishi, K., et al. (2014). Aphasia or Neglect after Thalamic Stroke: The Various Ways They may be Related to Cortical Hypoperfusion. Front. Neurol. 5:231. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2014.00231

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shewan, C. M., and Kertesz, A. (1980). Reliability and validity characteristics of the Western Aphasia Battery (WAB). J. Speech Hear. Disord. 45, 308–324. doi: 10.1044/jshd.4503.308

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siffrin, V., Vogt, J., Radbruch, H., Nitsch, R., and Zipp, F. (2010). Multiple sclerosis - candidate mechanisms underlying CNS atrophy. Trends Neurosci. 33, 202–210. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2010.01.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, S. M., Jenkinson, M., Woolrich, M. W., Beckmann, C. F., Behrens, T. E., Johansen-Berg, H., et al. (2004). Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. Neuroimage 23, S208–S219. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Szaflarski, J. P., Eaton, K., Ball, A. L., Banks, C., Vannest, J., Allendorfer, J. B., et al. (2011). Poststroke aphasia recovery assessed with functional magnetic resonance imaging and a picture identification task. J. Stroke Cerebrovasc. Dis. 20, 336–345. doi: 10.1016/j.jstrokecerebrovasdis.2010.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Takao, H., Hayashi, N., and Ohtomo, K. (2012). A longitudinal study of brain volume changes in normal aging. Eur. J. Radiol. 81, 2801–2804. doi: 10.1016/j.ejrad.2011.10.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taubner, R. W., Raymer, A. M., and Heilman, K. M. (1999). Frontal-opercular aphasia. Brain Lang. 70, 240–261. doi: 10.1006/brln.1999.2157

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Teichmann, M., Rosso, C., Martini, J. B., Bloch, I., Brugieres, P., Duffau, H., et al. (2015). A cortical-subcortical syntax pathway linking Broca’s area and the striatum. Hum. Brain Mapp. 36, 2270–2283. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22769

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turken, A. U., and Dronkers, N. F. (2011). The neural architecture of the language comprehension network: Converging evidence from lesion and connectivity analyses. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 5:1. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2011.00001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vallar, G., Perani, D., Cappa, S. F., Messa, C., Lenzi, G. L., and Fazio, F. (1988). Recovery from aphasia and neglect after subcortical stroke: Neuropsychological and cerebral perfusion study. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 51, 1269–1276. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.51.10.1269

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vigneau, M., Beaucousin, V., Herve, P. Y., Duffau, H., Crivello, F., Houde, O., et al. (2006). Meta-analyzing left hemisphere language areas: Phonology, semantics, and sentence processing. Neuroimage 30, 1414–1432. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.11.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walhovd, K. B., Westlye, L. T., Amlien, I., Espeseth, T., Reinvang, I., Raz, N., et al. (2011). Consistent neuroanatomical age-related volume differences across multiple samples. Neurobiol. Aging 32, 916–932. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2009.05.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wallesch, C. W., Johannsen-Horbach, H., Bartels, C., and Herrmann, M. (1997). Mechanisms of and misconceptions about subcortical aphasia. Brain Lang. 58, 403–409. doi: 10.1006/brln.1997.1805

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Winhuisen, L., Thiel, A., Schumacher, B., Kessler, J., Rudolf, J., Haupt, W. F., et al. (2005). Role of the contralateral inferior frontal gyrus in recovery of language function in poststroke aphasia: A combined repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation and positron emission tomography study. Stroke 36, 1759–1763. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.0000174487.81126.ef

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Worrall, B. B., Farace, E., Hillis, A. E., Hutson, R. K., Wityk, R., Saver, J. L., et al. (2001). Correlation of aphasia and/or neglect with cortical infarction in a subpopulation of RANTTAS. Cerebrovasc. Dis. 11, 257–264.

Google Scholar

Xing, S., Lacey, E. H., Skipper-Kallal, L. M., Jiang, X., Harris-Love, M. L., Zeng, J., et al. (2016). Right hemisphere grey matter structure and language outcomes in chronic left hemisphere stroke. Brain 139, 227–241. doi: 10.1093/brain/awv323

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xing, S., Mandal, A., Lacey, E. H., Skipper-Kallal, L. M., Zeng, J., and Turkeltaub, P. E. (2018). Behavioral Effects of Chronic Gray and White Matter Stroke Lesions in a Functionally Defined Connectome for Naming. Neurorehabil. Neural. Repair 32, 613–623. doi: 10.1177/1545968318780351

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, B., Chang, J., Park, J., Tan, Z., Tang, L., Lyu, T., et al. (2021). Uncinate fasciculus and its cortical terminals in aphasia after subcortical stroke: A multi-modal MRI study. Neuroimage Clin. 30:102597. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2021.102597

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, J., Meng, L., Qin, W., Liu, N., Shi, F. D., and Yu, C. (2014). Structural damage and functional reorganization in ipsilesional m1 in well-recovered patients with subcortical stroke. Stroke 45, 788–793. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.113.003425

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: aphasia, cortical thickness, diffusion tensor imaging, subcortical stroke, outcomes

Citation: Tang H, Fan S, Niu X, Li Z, Xiao P, Zeng J and Xing S (2022) Remote cortical atrophy and language outcomes after chronic left subcortical stroke with aphasia. Front. Neurosci. 16:853169. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2022.853169

Received: 12 January 2022; Accepted: 18 July 2022;
Published: 03 August 2022.

Edited by:

Julia Stephen, Mind Research Network (MRN), United States

Reviewed by:

Sara Pillay, Medical College of Wisconsin, United States
Guglielmo Puglisi, University of Milan, Italy

Copyright © 2022 Tang, Fan, Niu, Li, Xiao, Zeng and Xing. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Shihui Xing, xingshih@mail.sysu.edu.cn

These authors have contributed equally to this work and share first authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.