Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurosci., 24 March 2022
Sec. Perception Science

The Right Inferior Frontal Gyrus Plays an Important Role in Unconscious Information Processing: Activation Likelihood Estimation Analysis Based on Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging

\r\nJilong ShiJilong Shi1Haojie HuangHaojie Huang2Ruichen Jiang,Ruichen Jiang1,3Xuechen MaoXuechen Mao1Qin HuangQin Huang1Anmin Li*Anmin Li1*
  • 1School of Psychology, Shanghai University of Sport, Shanghai, China
  • 2Department of Physical Education, Xiamen University, Xiamen, China
  • 3School of Teacher Education, Anqing Normal University, Anqing, China

Unconsciousness is a kind of brain activity that occurs below the level of consciousness, and the masked priming paradigm is a classic paradigm to study unconscious perceptual processing. With the deepening of unconscious perception research, different researchers mostly use different experimental materials and different masked priming paradigms in a single experiment but not for the comprehensive analysis of the unconscious information processing mechanism itself. Thus, the purpose of this study is to conduct a comprehensive analysis through a cross-experimental paradigm, cross-experimental materials, and cross-experimental purposes. We used activation likelihood estimation to test functional magnetic resonance imaging studies, involving 361 subjects, 124 foci in eight studies representing direct comparison of unconscious processing with baseline, and 115 foci in 10 studies representing direct comparison of unconscious priming effects. In the comparison of unconscious processing and baseline, clusters formed in the left superior parietal gyrus, the right insular gyrus, and the right inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) triangular part after correcting for familywise error (FWE). In the comparison of priming effects, clusters formed in only the right IFG triangular part after correcting for FWE. Here, we found that ventral and dorsal pathways jointly regulate unconscious perceptual processes, but only the ventral pathway is involved in the regulation of unconscious priming effects. The IFG triangular part is involved in the regulation of unconscious perceptual processing and unconscious priming effects and may be an important brain area in unconscious information processing. These preliminary data provide conditions for further study of the neural correlation of unconscious information processing.

Introduction

In general, consciousness includes all mental activities, such as human thoughts, feelings, cognition, and memory, while unconsciousness contains deeper mental processes that are not perceived by consciousness. Therefore, unconsciousness is the brain activity that occurs below the level of consciousness. In human behavior, a visual or facial message that flashes for tens of milliseconds can still have an impact on human consciousness. However, when a word, a figure, or a face is close to other visual stimuli in space and time, it becomes indistinguishable, even beyond human perception of the information. This is a visual phenomenon called masking (Dehaene et al., 2001). Evett and Humphreys (1981) first used the masked priming paradigm to measure unconscious perception. Their masked priming paradigm has four levels of structure: forward mask – priming stimulus – target stimulus – backward mask. However, this method controls the presentation time of the target stimulus and prime stimulus of 50 ms, which is prone to produce a fusion effect: the target stimulus and prime stimulus cannot be distinguished at all (Kouider and Dehaene, 2007).

With increased unconscious perception research, researchers have improved the four-level structure of the masked priming paradigm to improve its credibility (Greenwald et al., 1996). To date, the masked priming paradigm may be divided into three types: the forward masking paradigm, the backward masking paradigm, and the sandwich masking paradigm, including the forward and backward masking paradigms. The forward masked priming paradigm was proposed by Forster and Davis (1984) as a three-level structure because there is no backward masking. The target stimulus itself is seen as a very powerful masking, and this structure consists of forward masking, a short prime word (60 ms or below), and a target word. The principle of the backward masking paradigm is to reduce the visibility of the prime stimulus through the backward pattern mask so that the subjects can neither recognize the prime (McCauley et al., 1980) nor make the existence-absence judgment significantly more than the opportunity (Fowler et al., 1981; Balota, 1983; Marcel, 1983; Cheesman and Merikle, 1984). Since sandwich masking includes a forward mask and backward mask, it was considered to have a more effective masking effect (Greenwald et al., 1996; Gao and Zhang, 2014). However, regardless of the masking paradigm, the spatial and temporal presentation of the stimulus could be restricted to prevent the participants from consciously processing the stimulus cue.

In unconscious perception studies, another important concept is unconscious priming. Namely, “invisible” stimuli can nonetheless affect behavioral responses to subsequent probe stimuli presented shortly after the mask (Henson et al., 2008). For example, Greenwald et al. (1995) conducted a typical experiment in which the subjects were instructed to complete a classification task, where the targets were divided into positive (e.g., “happy”) or negative (e.g., “vomit”) words, and the front of these words had a congruent prime (i.e., a word from the same category, such as the “love” preceding the target “happy” or an incongruent prime “vomit” preceding “happy”). Subjects were faster in congruent trials than in incongruent trials (Greenwald et al., 1995). Therefore, the priming effect depends on whether the prime and the target categories are congruent or incongruent, which means that unconscious priming originates from competition between the prime stimulus category and the target stimulus category, thus reflecting categorical congruity (Kouider and Dehaene, 2007). Numerous functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies have shown that the incongruent trial produced a larger increase in BOLD signals in brain regions than the congruent trial, although this was unconscious, possibly because the incongruent trial involved more cognitive resources (Dehaene et al., 2001; Nakamura et al., 2007; Kouider et al., 2010).

Numerous studies have used the masked priming paradigm to prove that people can process emotions, words, graphics, and other information presented at the unconscious level (Dehaene et al., 2001; D’Ostilio and Garraux, 2012a; Prochnow et al., 2013; Sato et al., 2016; Tacikowski et al., 2017). However, due to the differences in experimental purposes, different researchers mostly adopted different experimental materials and different masked priming paradigms for the study of a single experimental purpose and did not comprehensively analyze the unconscious information processing mechanism itself. With the application of fMRI technology in the study of unconscious information processing and the emergence of activation likelihood estimation (ALE) analysis methods (Brooks et al., 2012; Meneguzzo et al., 2014), it is possible to explore cross-experimental materials, cross-experimental paradigms, and cross-experimental purposes.

Most studies have accepted the view that the processing of cognitive information is mainly carried out by the ventral and dorsal pathways (Weiskrantz et al., 1974; Mishkin et al., 1983; Rees et al., 2002; Weiller et al., 2011). Previous studies have believed that the dorsal pathway is an unconscious perceptual process pathway that can directly connect sensory information and response parameters without consciousness (Neumann and Klotz, 1994). Although dorsal pathways can explain some simple unconscious motor processes, most cognitive activities cannot be summarized by a single visual pathway (Weiller et al., 2011). Therefore, researchers began to focus on the joint role of the ventral and dorsal pathways in unconscious perceptual processes (van Polanen and Davare, 2015). Ulrich and Kiefer (2016) used functional connectivity analysis to demonstrate that the ventral and dorsal pathways jointly regulate unconscious perceptual processes and pointed out that the two pathways mainly interact in the inferior occipital gyrus and posterior parietal lobe. However, it is well known that functional connectivity analysis relies on strong experimental hypotheses, and this study aimed to explore whether unconscious perceptual processes are mediated by both ventral and dorsal pathways at the whole brain level via ALE analysis.

In summary, this study aims to solve three key problems through cross-paradigm, cross-material, and cross-purpose experiments. First, we explored whether we can find the key brain regions specific to unconscious perceptual processes and unconscious priming effects in the masked priming paradigm. Second, we examined whether unconscious perceptual processes are regulated by both ventral and dorsal pathways. Finally, we examined whether the priming effect needs more brain regions to be involved because of the longer response time in incongruent compared to neutral or congruent conditions.

Materials and Methods

Study Selection

We selected studies through a standard search in the PubMed, Web of Science, and Baidu Scholar databases using the terms (fMRI or MRI) AND (unconscious or subliminal or subconscious) AND (mask or prime). To be included in our meta-analysis, studies met the following criteria: (a) studies were published within the last two decades, between January 2001 and August 2021, (b) published in a peer-reviewed journal, (c) masked prime paradigms were used and the study included a direct contrast between brain activation to unconscious processing and baseline or a direct contrast between brain activation to incongruent and neutral or congruent conditions, (d) priming stimulus less than 50 ms, (e) were original articles written in English, (f) used fMRI and no other brain imaging modalities so that the data could be better aggregated for meta-analysis, and (g) reported the neural activation coordinates in Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) or Talairach space. We excluded the results from patients and data from conditions focusing on pharmacological manipulation, and only healthy subjects were included (Eickhoff et al., 2009).

Selected Studies

We retrieved 135 studies, and all studies used the same search coverage, but 86 studies did not meet our inclusion criteria. Of these 49 eligible studies, eight were not included in the meta-analyses because they did not provide Talairach or MNI peak activation coordinates. Of the 41 studies to date, only 24 studies specifically analyzed unconscious processing compared to baseline (resting state) or incongruent versus neutral or congruent conditions. Of the remaining 24 studies, eight used ROI analysis, which was excluded because they used exclusive ROI analysis, a technique that analyses only small regions of the brain based on a priori hypotheses. This procedure is different from whole brain analysis, which statistically analyses the activation of the entire brain in one analysis (Meneguzzo et al., 2014). Thus, this search left 16 whole-brain fMRI studies that included the comparison of unconscious processing to baseline or incongruent versus neutral or congruent conditions using the masked priming paradigm. Six of these studies directly compared the activation of brain regions between unconscious processing and baseline, eight directly compared the activation of brain regions in incongruent and neutral or incongruent and congruent conditions, and the other two included two experimental comparisons. All included studies are shown in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. List of studies included in the ALE meta-analyses.

Definition of the Unconscious Masked Priming Paradigm

For unconscious perceptual processes, the most common measurement is the masked priming paradigm, which involves a related and effectively masked priming before or after a highly visible target stimulus, and the target stimulus is more effectively processed than an unrelated prime. Studies included in this meta-analysis included perceptual induction processing, semantic induction processing, and face recognition processing using the masked priming paradigm (Kouider and Dehaene, 2007).

Kouider and Dupoux (2001) assessed the prime awareness of several prime durations and showed that a prime is considered truly invisible only if it lasts less than 50 ms. Therefore, unconscious presentation is most often achieved by a brief stimulus onset asynchrony usually not more than 50 ms (Brooks et al., 2012; Meneguzzo et al., 2014; Opstal, 2021). According to the discussion of many scholars on the presentation time of the masked priming paradigm prime stimulus, this meta-analysis excluded studies whose prime duration exceeded 50 ms.

Quantitative Data Synthesis: Activation Likelihood Estimation

To examine unconscious activation and priming effects using the masked priming paradigm, we conducted two separate meta-analyses using BrainMap GingerALE version 3.0.2 software (Eickhoff et al., 2009). The ALE method is a voxelwise technique that provides information from convergence in the spatial location of the neural correlates across studies. Neural correlates, or foci from included studies, become the “activation likelihood” for each voxel in the brain, and for each voxel, ALE gives a score using a three-dimensional Gaussian probability density function to estimate the number of subjects in each study. The Gaussian distributions are then summed across studies to generate a map that estimates the likelihood of activation for each voxel (Laird et al., 2011; Turkeltaub et al., 2012). We applied the updated version of the ALE approach to conduct the meta-analyses using MNI coordinate “foci” from neuroimaging results and converting Talairach coordinates into MNI (SPM) for the analysis using GingerALE software (Eickhoff et al., 2010). The statistical results are presented in two thresholds. First, a familywise error (FWE)-corrected (Nichols and Hayasaka, 2003) threshold of p < 0.05 at the cluster level (cluster-forming threshold: p < 0.001 at the voxel level) through 1000 permutation tests, and then, an uncorrected threshold (p < 0.001, minimum cluster volume >200mm3). The reason why two statistical thresholds are used is to test the difference between different thresholds to obtain more reliable experimental results, We used an anatomical image overlay program called Mango (Creators, Jack Lancaster, Michael Martinez)1 and BrainNet Viewer (Creators, Mingrui Xia)2 to illustrate the results of our meta-analyses.

Results

Meta-Analysis One: Unconscious Perceptual Processing (Unconscious Processing > Baseline)

From 124 foci, 177 subjects and 12 separate experiments, 12 significant clusters were found that survived the cluster-level inference threshold (p < 0.001, uncorrected), of which three significant clusters survived the statistically more rigorous FWE-corrected analysis (p < 0.05). Cluster one was found in the left superior parietal gyrus (SPG) in BA7 (x = −28, y = −58, z = 44), cluster two was found in the right insula in BA47 (x = 32, y = 22, z = 0), and cluster three was found in the right inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) triangular part in BA45 (x = 46, y = 30, z = 20) (see Figures 1, 2 and Table 2).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Unconscious activation greater than baseline. FWE-corrected at the cluster level (p < 0.05) with a cluster-forming threshold of p < 0.001.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Unconscious perceptual processing. Unconscious activation greater than baseline. Spheres with a radius of 5 mm are visualized to present the statistical results of clusters formed. Red spheres represent results that survived FWE correction, while yellow spheres represent results that are uncorrected. SPG, Superior parietal gyrus; INS, Insular; IFGtriang, Inferior frontal gyrus triangular part; SMA, Supplementary motor area; PreCG, Precentral gyrus; SOG, Superior occipital gyrus; FFG, Fusiform gyrus; PCUN, Precuneus; L, left; R, right.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Unconscious activation greater than baseline.

Meta-Analysis Two: Unconscious Priming Effects (Incongruent > Neutral and Congruent)

From 115 foci, 238 subjects and 11 separate experiments, 10 significant clusters were found that survived the cluster-level inference threshold (p < 0.001, uncorrected), of which one significant cluster survived the statistically more rigorous FWE correction (p < 0.05). Cluster one was found in the right IFG triangular part in BA48 (x = 44, y = 24, z = 22) (see Figures 3, 4 and Table 3).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Incongruent activation was greater than neutral and congruent activation. FWE correction at the cluster level (p < 0.05) with a cluster-forming threshold of p < 0.001.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Unconscious priming effect. Incongruent activation was greater than congruent and neutral activation. Spheres with a radius of 5 mm are visualized to present the statistical results of clusters formed. Red spheres represent results that survived FWE correction, while yellow spheres represent results that are uncorrected. IFGtriang, inferior frontal gyrus triangular part; FFG, fusiform gyrus; ACG, anterior cingulate gyrus; THA, thalamus; IOG, inferior occipital gyrus; PreCG, precentral gyrus; INS, insular; ORBinf, inferior frontal gyrus opercular part; L, left; R, right.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Incongruent activation greater than neutral and congruent activation.

Discussion

We conducted a preliminary meta-analysis of fMRI studies comparing the activation of unconscious processing versus baseline and unconscious priming effects in the masked priming paradigm. In the unconscious condition, the prime stimulus and the target stimulus have longer latency periods in the incongruent condition than in the neutral and congruent conditions. We considered two studies that indicated differences in activation between incongruent and neutral conditions, since we assumed that incongruent conditions invoked more cognitive resources than both neutral and congruent conditions. However, caution must be taken in interpreting these findings in terms of categories and differences in visual stimuli, as they may influence the observed data (Meneguzzo et al., 2014). It is worth noting that, in consideration of the caution of interpreting cross-task research, we adopted a strict literature screening method in this meta-analysis in an effort to obtain more reliable research results. In fact, Brooks et al. (2012) conducted an ALE analysis based on fMRI for subliminal stimuli for the first time, but their study focused more on the brain activity level induced by different types of subliminal stimuli. However, Meneguzzo et al. (2014) also conducted an ALE analysis based on fMRI, which paid more attention to the differences in brain activities induced by supraliminal and subliminal stimuli. Since neither of these two studies only targeted at the unconscious masked priming paradigm, this study is also the first study to reveal the unconscious information processing mechanism underlying the masked priming paradigm through an ALE meta-analysis based on fMRI. The results of the meta-analysis also provided sufficient evidence for the three questions we planned to answer. Surprisingly, the right IFG triangular part plays a crucial role in unconscious information processing. In the following section, we will discuss the latest findings in this study.

The bilateral IFG is a heterogeneous region in two pathways of cognitive information processing, with the opercular part (BA44) connected mainly via the dorsal pathway, and the triangular part (BA45) and BA47 are mainly connected via the ventral pathway (Petrides and Pandya, 2007, 2009). Although the involvement of the bilateral IFG in the semantic process at the lexical level is unclear and controversial (Bookheimer, 2002), the triangular part (BA45) and orbital part (BA47) are related to the relevant features of semantic knowledge (Thompson-Schill et al., 1997). There is neuropsychological evidence that this region is involved in the control of semantic processing (Sharp et al., 2010). In the results of this meta-study, clusters were found in the IFG triangular part not only in the comparison between unconscious processing and baseline but also, more importantly, in the analysis of unconscious priming effects. In the case of incongruent processing, an unconscious perceptual process not only has a longer latency, but when considering that incongruent processing has been shown to activate more IFG regions than congruent and neutral processing (Ulrich et al., 2014; Jiang et al., 2016), this coincidence seems to provide evidence that an unconscious perceptual process can reach the semantic level. However, Ulrich et al. (2014) found in an analysis of the interaction between unconscious perceptual induction processing and semantic association that compared with perceptual induction processing, semantic induction processing had greater activations in the bilateral IFG (BA45) and the boundary part of the left anterior insula. It was further confirmed that the unconscious semantic priming effect only occurs after semantic induction processing but not after perceptual induction processing.

However, why is it the right IFG, not the left IFG, that shows the activation likelihood? It is well known that the left IFG triangular part is part of Broca’s area and is an important motor speech center (Thompson-Schill et al., 1997; Tremblay and Dick, 2016). Jackson (2021) conducted a meta-analysis of the neural correlativity of semantic control and found that semantic control depended on a distributed network composed of the IFG, post middle temporal gyrus, post inferior temporal gyrus, and dorsomedial prefrontal cortex. This network is left-dominant, with the left IFG more involved than the right IFG, and produces the strongest activation likelihood in the left IFG triangular part (Gonzalez et al., 2019; Jackson, 2021). However, although the left IFG generated more activations than the right IFG in the semantic control network, activations were still detected in the right IFG, and the role of the right IFG triangular part in semantic control remains unknown. Thus, when considering the mechanism of unconscious information processing in this study, the right IFG triangular part is likely to be a specific brain region for unconscious information processing in the masked priming paradigm. The ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (VLPFC) is known for its cognitive control functions (Miller and Cohen, 2001; Niendam et al., 2012) and plays an important role in the learning, retrieval, and maintenance of stimulate-response (S-R) rules (Toni and Passingham, 1999; Murray et al., 2000; Toni et al., 2001; Bunge et al., 2002, 2005; Crone et al., 2006), and the right IFG triangular part is a part of the VLPFC. On the one hand, in congruent trials, the relevant S-R rules and related responses have been preactivated by the prime, and the right IFG triangular part regulates response consistency in the congruent condition. On the other hand, in incongruent trials, priming activated inappropriate S-R mapping, and it is necessary to retrieve the appropriate S-R rules for the target response in a more detailed way (Ulrich and Kiefer, 2016). However, it has been shown that the IFG is recruited when competitive task representations are simultaneously activated (Bunge et al., 2002; Zhang et al., 2004; Badre et al., 2005; Moss et al., 2005; Souza et al., 2009). Thus, during incongruent trials, when considering that the prime preactivated an S-R rule inappropriate for the upcoming target, the triangular part may be involved in resolving the conflict (Ulrich and Kiefer, 2016).

In general, the dorsal (occipito – parietal) pathway is used for spatial perception (Mishkin et al., 1983; Rees et al., 2002), which is often called the “where” pathway. It has the function of “action vision” and provides strong input for the motor system to guide action (Michael et al., 2013). In this meta-analysis, we found that clusters formed in the dorsal pathway, such as the left SPG, the right superior occipital gyrus, and the right precuneus. As mentioned above, previous studies have suggested that dorsal pathways can explain some simple unconscious motor processing. Therefore, our results are consistent with previous studies (Neumann and Klotz, 1994). However, in information processing, the IFG is a heterogeneous area, and its triangular part is connected with the ventral pathway. The ventral (occipito-temporal) pathway is used for object perception and recognition (Mishkin et al., 1983; Rees et al., 2002). It is often called the “what” pathway and has the function of “recognizing vision” (Michael et al., 2013). Most studies suggest that the ventral pathway is a conscious processing pathway (Weiller et al., 2011; van Polanen and Davare, 2015). This meta-analysis found that the ventral pathway was involved in unconscious perceptual processing in the masked priming paradigm, such as the bilateral insula, right IFG triangular part, and right fusiform clusters. Although some studies have also shown that the ventral pathway participates in unconscious perceptual processes, their studies are based on the analysis of a single experimental purpose and a single experimental paradigm (Dehaene et al., 1998, 2001; Ulrich and Kiefer, 2016). In this study, ALE analysis provided strong evidence for the involvement of the ventral pathway in the regulation of unconscious perceptual processes from cross-purpose, cross-paradigm, and cross-material evidence. However, for most functions, both pathways are not mutually exclusive but rather work in parallel (Makris and Pandya, 2009; Rauschecker and Scott, 2009), constituting a loop that has to be passed at least once (Weiller et al., 2011). This loop may explain our findings that the two pathways cooperate to complete unconscious perceptual processing in the masked priming paradigm. Moreover, van Polanen and Davare (2015) proposed that the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex regulates the interaction between ventral and dorsal pathways so that the grasping action is completed. Thus, considering that the IFG is a heterogeneous region, we have reason to speculate that the interaction of the two pathways is mediated by the IFG in unconscious perceptual processing. The supplementary motor area and the precentral gyrus may participate in the preparation, planning, and execution of unconscious motor responses (Hanakawa et al., 2003; Witt et al., 2008).

However, in the regulation of the priming effect, we have different findings. Consistent with previous studies, our meta-analysis also found that the ventral pathway regulated the unconscious priming effect, such as the formation of clusters in the right IFG triangular part, the left fusiform gyrus and the left inferior occipital gyrus, but no significant clusters were found in the dorsal pathway. Moreover, to the best of our knowledge, only a few fMRI studies have shown that the unconscious prime effect involves the dorsal pathway (Aron et al., 2003; D’Ostilio and Garraux, 2012a; D’Ostilio et al., 2013). However, due to the use of arrow stimulation as a prime and target, the explanation of the priming effect is limited. Thus, there is no sufficient evidence to show that the two pathways coordinate in the unconscious priming effect in the masked priming paradigm (Ulrich and Kiefer, 2016).

The other two findings of this study are also critical. One is that the SPG formed clusters. The general theory holds that unconscious perceptual processes do not involve attention resources, and the appearance of SPG clusters challenges this view. The SPG receives attentional modulation signals from the prefrontal cortex and then applies appropriate attentional control to task-related sensory brain regions (Ulrich et al., 2013). Top–down attention control is not only a feature of spatial position and visual object but can also use the same neural mechanism for semantic information (Kiefer, 2008; Hoenig and Scheef, 2009; Grandjean et al., 2012). Thus, the discovery of SPG suggests that attention resources are involved in unconscious perceptual processes. Although SPG was found to form clusters across experiments, considering that different experimental designs have different control over conscious intervention, this result should be interpreted carefully. Another finding is that the insula forms clusters in unconscious perceptual processes and priming effects. Although the insula is known to be sensitive to both conscious and unconscious visual stimuli (Salomon et al., 2016), few studies have focused on the effect of claustrum hiding in the insula on unconscious information processing. The claustrum is hidden under the general area of the insula, which is thought to act as an on-off switch for consciousness (Crick and Koch, 2005; Brooks et al., 2012; Gattass et al., 2014; Meneguzzo et al., 2014; Chau et al., 2015). Whether the clusters formed in the insula in this study extend to the claustrum remains to be considered. However, there is a two-way connection between the claustrum and almost all areas of the cerebral cortex (Crick and Koch, 2005). Considering its bidirectional connectivity, whether or not claustrum may connect bilateral pathways and modulate unconscious perceptual processes is still a question worthy of more researchers’ attention.

Conclusion

This study preliminarily confirmed our hypothesis. We found that ventral and dorsal pathways jointly regulate unconscious perceptual processes, but only the ventral pathway is involved in the regulation of unconscious priming effects. The right IFG triangular part is the key brain regions in unconscious information processing in the masked priming paradigm, which is involved in the regulation of unconscious perceptual processes and priming effects. These preliminary data provide the conditions for further investigation of the neural correlates of unconscious information processing.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author Contributions

All authors listed have made a substantial, direct, and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.

Funding

This study was supported by funds from the National Natural Science Foundation of China (31971023).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank all members of the laboratory for their assistance in screening studies and ALE analysis.

Footnotes

  1. ^ http://ric.uthscsa.edu/mango
  2. ^ http://helab.bnu.edu.cn/brainnet-viewer

References

Aron, A. R., Schlaghecken, F., Fletcher, P. C., Bullmore, E. T., Eimer, M., Barker, R., et al. (2003). Inhibition of subliminally primed responses is mediated by the caudate and thalamus: evidence from functional MRI and Huntington’s disease. Brain 126, 713–723. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg067

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Badre, D., Poldrack, R. A., Ej Paré-Blagoev, Insler, R. Z., and Wagner, A. D. (2005). Dissociable controlled retrieval and generalized selection mechanisms in ventrolateral prefrontal cortex. Neuron 47, 907–918. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.07.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balota, D. A. (1983). Automatic semantic activation and episodic memory encoding. J. Verbal Learn. Verbal Behav. 22, 88–104. doi: 10.1016/s0022-5371(83)80008-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Binder, M., Gociewicz, K., Windey, B., Koculak, M., Finc, K., Nikadon, J., et al. (2017). The levels of perceptual processing and the neural correlates of increasing subjective visibility. Conscious. Cogn. 55, 106–125. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2017.07.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bookheimer, S. (2002). Functional MRI of language: new approaches to understanding the cortical organization of semantic processing. Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 25:151. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.25.112701.142946

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brooks, S. J., Savov, V., Allzén, E., Benedict, C., Fredriksson, R., and Schiöth, H. B. (2012). Exposure to subliminal arousing stimuli induces robust activation in the amygdala, hippocampus, anterior cingulate, insular cortex and primary visual cortex: a systematic meta-analysis of fMRI studies. NeuroImage 59, 2962–2973. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.077

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bunge, S. A., Hazeltine, E., Scanlon, M. D., Rosen, A. C., and Gabrieli, J. D. (2002). Dissociable contributions of prefrontal and parietal cortices to response selection. Neuroimage 17, 1562–1571. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1252

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bunge, S. A., Wallis, J. D., Parker, A., Brass, M., and Sakai, K. (2005). Neural circuitry underlying rule use in humans and nonhuman primates. J. Neurosci. Off. J. Soc. Neurosci. 25, 10347–10350. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2937-05.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chau, A., Salazar, A. M., Krueger, F., Cristofori, I., and Grafman, J. (2015). The effect of claustrum lesions on human consciousness and recovery of function. Conscious. cogn. 36, 256–264. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2015.06.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheesman, J., and Merikle, P. M. (1984). Priming with and without awareness. Percept. psychophys. 36, 387–395. doi: 10.3758/bf03202793

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crick, F. C., and Koch, C. (2005). What is the function of the claustrum? Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. Series B. Biol. sci. 360, 1271–1279. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2005.1661

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crone, E. A., Wendelken, C., Donohue, S. E., and Bunge, S. A. (2006). Neural evidence for dissociable components of task-switching. Cereb. cortex 16, 475–486. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhi127

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Pisapia, N., Turatto, M., Lin, P., Jovicich, J., and Caramazza, A. (2012). Unconscious priming instructions modulate activity in default and executive networks of the human brain. Cereb. cortex 22, 639–649. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr146

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dehaene, S., Naccache, L., Cohen, L., Bihan, D. L., Mangin, J. F., Poline, J. B., et al. (2001). Cerebral mechanisms of word masking and unconscious repetition priming. Nat. Neurosci. 4, 752–758. doi: 10.1038/89551

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dehaene, S., Naccache, L., Le Clec, H. G., Koechlin, E., Mueller, M., and Dehaene-Lambertz, G. (1998). Imaging unconscious semantic priming. Nature 395, 597–600. doi: 10.1038/26967

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jiang, J., Bailey, K., Xiang, L., Zhang, L., and Zhang, Q. (2016). Comparing the Neural Correlates of Conscious and Unconscious Conflict Control in a Masked Stroop Priming Task. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:297. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00297

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

D’Ostilio, K., Collette, F., Phillips, C., and Garraux, G. (2012b). Evidence for a role of a cortico-subcortical network for automatic and unconscious motor inhibition of manual responses. PLoS One 7:e48007. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0048007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

D’Ostilio, K., Deville, B., Cremers, J., Grandjean, J., Skawiniak, E., Delvaux, V., et al. (2013). Role of the supplementary motor area in the automatic activation of motor plans in de novo Parkinson’s disease patients. Neurosci. Res. 76, 173–177. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2013.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

D’Ostilio, K., and Garraux, G. (2012a). Dissociation between unconscious motor response facilitation and conflict in medial frontal areas. Eur. j. Neurosci. 35, 332–340. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2011.07941.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eickhoff, S. B., Laird, A. R., Grefkes, C., Wang, L. E., Zilles, K., and Fox, P. T. (2009). Coordinate-based activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis of neuroimaging data: a random-effects approach based on empirical estimates of spatial uncertainty. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 2907–2926. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20718

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eickhoff, S. B., Nickl-Jockschat, T., and Kurth, F. (2010). Metaanalysen in der klinischen Hirnforschung [Meta-analyses in clinical brain research]. Der Nervenarzt 81, 32–38. doi: 10.1007/s00115-009-2826-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Evett, L. J., and Humphreys, G. W. (1981). The use of abstract graphemic information in lexical access. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. A 33, 325–350. doi: 10.1111/bjop.12071

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Forster, K. I., and Davis, C. (1984). Repetition priming and frequency attenuation in lexical access. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Memory Cogn. 10, 680–698. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e32834ca576

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fowler, C. A., Wolford, G., Slade, R., and Tassinary, L. (1981). Lexical access with and without awareness. J. Exp. Psychol. General 110, 341–362. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.110.3.341

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gao, Y., and Zhang, H. (2014). Unconscious processing modulates creative problem solving: evidence from an electrophysiological study. Conscious. cogn. 26, 64–73. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2014.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gattass, R., Soares, J. G., Desimone, R., and Ungerleider, L. G. (2014). Connectional subdivision of the claustrum: two visuotopic subdivisions in the macaque. Front. sys. Neurosci. 8:63. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2014.00063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gonzalez, Alam, T., Karapanagiotidis, T., Smallwood, J., and Jefferies, E. (2019). Degrees of lateralisation in semantic cognition: evidence from intrinsic connectivity. NeuroImage 202:116089. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116089

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grandjean, J., D’Ostilio, K., Phillips, C., Balteau, E., Degueldre, C., Luxen, A., et al. (2012). Modulation of brain activity during a Stroop inhibitory task by the kind of cognitive control required. PLoS One 7:e41513. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0041513

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenwald, A. G., Draine, S. C., and Abrams, R. L. (1996). Three cognitive markers of unconscious semantic activation. Science 273, 1699–1702. doi: 10.1126/science.273.5282.1699

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenwald, A. G., Klinger, M. R., and Schuh, E. S. (1995). Activation by marginally perceptible (”subliminal”) stimuli: dissociation of unconscious from conscious cognition. J. Exp. psychol. General 124, 22–42. doi: 10.1037//0096-3445.124.1.22

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hanakawa, T., Immisch, I., Toma, K., Dimyan, M. A., Van Gelderen, P., and Hallett, M. (2003). Functional properties of brain areas associated with motor execution and imagery. J. Neurophysiol. 89, 989–1002. doi: 10.1152/jn.00132.2002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Henson, R. N., Mouchlianitis, E., Matthews, W. J., and Kouider, S. (2008). Electrophysiological correlates of masked face priming. NeuroImage 40, 884–895. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoenig, K., and Scheef, L. (2009). Neural correlates of semantic ambiguity processing during context verification. NeuroImage 45, 1009–1019. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.12.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jackson, R. L. (2021). The neural correlates of semantic control revisited. NeuroImage 224:117444. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117444

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kiefer, M. (2008). Top-down modulation of unconscious ‘automatic’ processes: a gating framework. Adv. cogn. psychol. 3, 289–306. doi: 10.2478/v10053-008-0032-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kouider, S., de Gardelle, V., Dehaene, S., Dupoux, E., and Pallier, C. (2010). Cerebral bases of subliminal speech priming. NeuroImage 49, 922–929. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.08.043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kouider, S., and Dehaene, S. (2007). Levels of processing during non-conscious perception: a critical review of visual masking. Philosoph. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. Series B Biol. sci. 362, 857–875. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2007.2093

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kouider, S., and Dupoux, E. (2001). A functional disconnection between spoken and visual word recognition: evidence from unconscious priming. Cognition 82, B35–B49. doi: 10.1016/s0010-0277(01)00152-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laird, A. R., Eickhoff, S. B., Fox, P. M., Uecker, A. M., Ray, K. L., and Saenz, J. J. Jr., et al. (2011). The BrainMap strategy for standardization, sharing, and meta-analysis of neuroimaging data. BMC Res. Notes 4:349. doi: 10.1186/1756-0500-4-349

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luo, Q., Peng, D., Jin, Z., Xu, D., Xiao, L., and Ding, G. (2004). Emotional valence of words modulates the subliminal repetition priming effect in the left fusiform gyrus: an event-related fMRI study. NeuroImage 21, 414–421. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.09.048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Makris, N., and Pandya, D. N. (2009). The extreme capsule in humans and rethinking of the language circuitry. Brain struct. funct. 213, 343–358. doi: 10.1007/s00429-008-0199-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marcel, A. J. (1983). Conscious and unconscious perception: experiments on visual masking and word recognition. Cogn. psychol. 15, 197–237. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(83)90009-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCauley, C., Parmelee, C. M., Sperber, R. D., and Carr, T. H. (1980). Early extraction of meaning from pictures and its relation to conscious identification. J. Exp. psychol. Hum. percept. Perform. 6, 265–276. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.6.2.265

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meneguzzo, P., Tsakiris, M., Schioth, H. B., Stein, D. J., and Brooks, S. J. (2014). Subliminal versus supraliminal stimuli activate neural responses in anterior cingulate cortex, fusiform gyrus and insula: a meta-analysis of fMRI studies. BMC Psychol. 2:52. doi: 10.1186/s40359-014-0052-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Michael, S. G., Richard, B. I., and George, R. M. (2013). Cognitive Neuroscience: The Biology of the Mind, 4th Edn. New York, NY: W. W. Norton.

Google Scholar

Miller, E. K., and Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 24, 167–202. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mishkin, M., Ungerleider, L. G., and Macko, K. A. (1983). Object vision and spatial vision: two cortical pathways. Trends Neurosci. 6, 414–417. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(83)90190-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moss, H. E., Abdallah, S., Fletcher, P., Bright, P., Pilgrim, L., Acres, K., et al. (2005). Selecting among competing alternatives: selection and retrieval in the left inferior frontal gyrus. Cereb. cortex 15, 1723–1735. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhi049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murawski, C., Harris, P. G., Bode, S., Domínguez, D. J. F., and Egan, G. F. (2012). Led into temptation? Rewarding brand logos bias the neural encoding of incidental economic decisions. PLoS One 7:e41535. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0034155

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murray, E. A., Bussey, T. J., and Wise, S. P. (2000). Role of prefrontal cortex in a network for arbitrary visuomotor mapping. Exp. Brain Res. 133, 114–129. doi: 10.1007/s002210000406

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nakamura, K., Dehaene, S., Jobert, A., Le Bihan, D., and Kouider, S. (2007). Task-specific change of unconscious neural priming in the cerebral language network. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104, 19643–19648. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0704487104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nakamura, K., Kouider, S., Makuuchi, M., Kuroki, C., Hanajima, R., Ugawa, Y., et al. (2010). Neural control of cross-language asymmetry in the bilingual brain. Cerebral cortex 20, 2244–2251. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp290

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neumann, O., and Klotz, W. (1994). “Motor responses to nonreportable, masked stimuli: Where is the limit of direct parameter specification?,” in Tention and Performance 15: Conscious and Nonconscious Information Processing, eds C. Umiltà and M. Moscovitch (Cambridge: The MIT Press)

Google Scholar

Nichols, T., and Hayasaka, S. (2003). Controlling the familywise error rate in functional neuroimaging: a comparative review. Stat. Methods Med. Res. 12, 419–446. doi: 10.1191/0962280203sm341ra

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Niendam, T. A., Laird, A. R., Ray, K. L., Dean, Y. M., Glahn, D. C., and Carter, C. S. (2012). Meta-analytic evidence for a superordinate cognitive control network subserving diverse executive functions. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 12, 241–268. doi: 10.3758/s13415-011-0083-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Opstal, F. V. (2021). The same-different task as a tool to study unconscious processing. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 37, 35–40. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2020.06.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petrides, M., and Pandya, D. N. (2007). Efferent association pathways from the rostral prefrontal cortex in the macaque monkey. J. Neurosci. Off. j. Soc. Neurosci. 27, 11573–11586. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2419-07.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petrides, M., and Pandya, D. N. (2009). Distinct parietal and temporal pathways to the homologues of Broca’s area in the monkey. PLoS Biol. 7:e1000170. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1000170

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pichon, S., Guex, R., and Vuilleumier, P. (2016). Influence of Temporal Expectations on Response Priming by Subliminal Faces. PLoS One 11:e0164613. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0164613

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prochnow, D., Kossack, H., Brunheim, S., Müller, K., Wittsack, H. J., Markowitsch, H. J., et al. (2013). Processing of subliminal facial expressions of emotion: a behavioral and fMRI study. Soc. Neurosci. 8, 448–461. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2013.812536

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rauschecker, J. P., and Scott, S. K. (2009). Maps and streams in the auditory cortex: nonhuman primates illuminate human speech processing. Nat. Neurosci. 12, 718–724. doi: 10.1038/nn.2331

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rees, G., Kreiman, G., and Koch, C. (2002). Neural correlates of consciousness in humans. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 261–270. doi: 10.1038/nrn783

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salomon, R., Ronchi, R., Dönz, J., Bello-Ruiz, J., Herbelin, B., Martet, R., et al. (2016). The insula mediates access to awareness of visual stimuli presented synchronously to the heartbeat. J. Neurosci. 36, 5115–5127. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4262-15.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sato, W., Kochiyama, T., Uono, S., and Toichi, M. (2016). Neural mechanisms underlying conscious and unconscious attentional shifts triggered by eye gaze. NeuroImage 124, 118–126. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.08.061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sharp, D. J., Awad, M., Warren, J. E., Wise, R. J., Vigliocco, G., and Scott, S. K. (2010). The neural response to changing semantic and perceptual complexity during language processing. Hum. Brain Mapp. 31, 365–377. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20871

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Souza, M. J., Donohue, S. E., and Bunge, S. A. (2009). Controlled retrieval and selection of action-relevant knowledge mediated by partially overlapping regions in left ventrolateral prefrontal cortex. NeuroImage 46, 299–307. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.01.046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tacikowski, P., Berger, C. C., and Ehrsson, H. H. (2017). Dissociating the Neural Basis of Conceptual Self-Awareness from Perceptual Awareness and Unaware Self-Processing. Cereb. cortex 27, 3768–3781. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhx004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson-Schill, S. L., D’Esposito, M., Aguirre, G. K., and Farah, M. J. (1997). Role of left inferior prefrontal cortex in retrieval of semantic knowledge: a reevaluation. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 94, 14792–14797. doi: 10.1073/pnas.94.26.14792

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Toni, I., and Passingham, R. E. (1999). Prefrontal-basal ganglia pathways are involved in the learning of arbitrary visuomotor associations: a PET study. Exp. Brain Res. 127, 19–32. doi: 10.1007/s002210050770

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Toni, I., Ramnani, N., Josephs, O., Ashburner, J., and Passingham, R. E. (2001). Learning arbitrary visuomotor associations: temporal dynamic of brain activity. NeuroImage 14, 1048–1057. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0894

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tremblay, P., and Dick, A. S. (2016). Broca and Wernicke are dead, or moving past the classic model of language neurobiology. Brain Lang. 162, 60–71. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2016.08.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turkeltaub, P. E., Eickhoff, S. B., Laird, A. R., Fox, M., Wiener, M., and Fox, P. (2012). Minimizing within-experiment and within-group effects in Activation Likelihood Estimation meta-analyses. Hum. Brain Mapp. 33, 1–13. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21186

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ulrich, M., Adams, S. C., and Kiefer, M. (2014). Flexible establishment of functional brain networks supports attentional modulation of unconscious cognition. Hum. Brain Mapp. 35, 5500–5516. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22566

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ulrich, M., Hoenig, K., Grön, G., and Kiefer, M. (2013). Brain activation during masked and unmasked semantic priming: commonalities and differences. J. cogn. Neurosci. 25, 2216–2229. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00449

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ulrich, M., and Kiefer, M. (2016). The Neural Signature of Subliminal Visuomotor Priming: Brain Activity and Functional Connectivity Profiles. Cereb. cortex 26, 2471–2482. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhv070

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Polanen, V., and Davare, M. (2015). Interactions between dorsal and ventral streams for controlling skilled grasp. Neuropsychologia 79, 186–191. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.07.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weiller, C., Bormann, T., Saur, D., Musso, M., and Rijntjes, M. (2011). How the ventral pathway got lost: and what its recovery might mean. Brain Lang. 118, 29–39. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2011.01.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weiskrantz, L., Warrington, E. K., Sanders, M. D., and Marshall, J. (1974). Visual capacity in the hemianopic field following a restricted occipital ablation. Brain 97, 709–728. doi: 10.1093/brain/97.1.709

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Witt, S. T., Laird, A. R., and Meyerand, M. E. (2008). Functional neuroimaging correlates of finger-tapping task variations: an ALE meta-analysis. NeuroImage 42, 343–356. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.04.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, J., Cao, Z., Xu, X., and Chen, G. (2012). The amygdala is involved in affective priming effect for fearful faces. Brain cogn. 80, 15–22. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2012.04.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, J. X., Feng, C. M., Fox, P. T., Gao, J. H., and Tan, L. H. (2004). Is left inferior frontal gyrus a general mechanism for selection? NeuroImage 23, 596–603. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.06.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: unconsciousness, masked priming, activation likelihood estimation, right inferior frontal gyrus, priming effect

Citation: Shi J, Huang H, Jiang R, Mao X, Huang Q and Li A (2022) The Right Inferior Frontal Gyrus Plays an Important Role in Unconscious Information Processing: Activation Likelihood Estimation Analysis Based on Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging. Front. Neurosci. 16:781099. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2022.781099

Received: 22 September 2021; Accepted: 28 February 2022;
Published: 24 March 2022.

Edited by:

Britt Anderson, University of Waterloo, Canada

Reviewed by:

Micah Alan Johnson, University of California, United States
Samantha Jane Brooks, Liverpool John Moores University, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2022 Shi, Huang, Jiang, Mao, Huang and Li. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Anmin Li, anminli@sus.edu.cn

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.