Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurosci., 09 January 2023
Sec. Brain Imaging Methods

Potential dynamic regional brain biomarkers for early discrimination of autism and language development delay in toddlers

\r\nShengjuan Wu&#x;Shengjuan Wu1†Zhi Wen&#x;Zhi Wen2†Wenzhong YangWenzhong Yang3Chengcheng JiangChengcheng Jiang3Yurong ZhouYurong Zhou4Zhiwei ZhaoZhiwei Zhao1Aiqin ZhouAiqin Zhou1Xinglian LiuXinglian Liu1Xiaoyan WangXiaoyan Wang1Yue WangYue Wang1Hong Wang*Hong Wang1*Fuchun Lin*Fuchun Lin5*
  • 1Department of Child Health Care, Maternal and Child Health Hospital of Hubei Province, Tongji Medical College, Huazhong University of Science and Technology, Wuhan, China
  • 2Department of Radiology, Renmin Hospital of Wuhan University, Wuhan, China
  • 3Department of Radiology, Maternal and Child Health Hospital of Hubei Province, Tongji Medical College, Huazhong University of Science and Technology, Wuhan, China
  • 4Department of Radiology, Zhongnan Hospital of Wuhan University, Wuhan, China
  • 5State Key Laboratory of Magnetic Resonance and Atomic and Molecular Physics, National Center for Magnetic Resonance in Wuhan, Wuhan Institute of Physics and Mathematics, Innovation Academy for Precision Measurement Science and Technology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Wuhan, China

Background: The early diagnosis of autism in children is particularly important. However, there is no obvious objective indices for the diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder (ASD), especially in toddlers aged 1–3 years with language development delay (LDD). The early differential diagnosis of ASD is challenging.

Objective: To examine differences in the dynamic characteristics of regional neural activity in toddlers with ASD and LDD, and whether the differences can be used as an imaging biomarker for the early differential diagnosis of ASD and LDD.

Methods: Dynamic regional homogeneity (dReHo) and dynamic amplitude of low-frequency fluctuations (dALFF) in 55 children with ASD and 31 with LDD, aged 1–3 years, were compared. The correlations between ASD symptoms and the values of dReHo/dALFF within regions showing significant between-group differences were analyzed in ASD group. We further assessed the accuracy of dynamic regional neural activity alterations to distinguish ASD from LDD using receiver operating characteristic (ROC) analysis.

Results: Compared with the LDD group, the ASD group showed increased dReHo in the left cerebellum_8/Crust2 and right cerebellum_Crust2, and decreased dReHo in the right middle frontal gyrus (MFG) and post-central gyrus. Patients with ASD also exhibited decreased dALFF in the right middle temporal gyrus (MFG) and right precuneus. Moreover, the Childhood Autism Rating Scale score was negatively correlated with the dReHo of the left cerebellum_8/crust2 and right cerebellum_crust2. The dReHo value of the right MFG was negatively correlated with social self-help of the Autism Behavior Checklist score.

Conclusion: The pattern of resting-state regional neural activity variability was different between toddlers with ASD and those with LDD. Dynamic regional indices might be novel neuroimaging biomarkers that allow differentiation of ASD from LDD in toddlers.

1. Introduction

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a heterogeneous neurodevelopmental disorder, with varying clinical manifestations and disease severity. The prevalence is 1 in 44 in America,1 whereas approximately 1% in China (Sun et al., 2019). The core defects of ASD are impaired social functions, impaired language communication, rigid and repetitive behavior. Among them, language retardation may be an early concern for ASD children (Chung et al., 2011).

There are two difficulties in early diagnosis of ASD with language retardation. On the one hand, around 1–3 years old, the onset and severity of language retardation vary greatly in ASD, and there are usually no core symptoms. When language development is delayed, the score on Autism Diagnosis Scale will increase, which makes ASD and language development delay (LDD) easy to be confused. On the other hand, the prognosis of intervention varies greatly between ASD and LDD. For example, with appropriate intervention, LDD children who are diagnosed at the age of 2 years can reach typically developing peers at the age of 3–4 years (Schulte-Korne, 2014). In contrast, the prognosis of ASD is often worse than that of LDD, which involves life-long and continuous intervention and rehabilitation. Therefore, it is important to explore new indicators to distinguish ASD from LDD in the early stage.

Although lack of neurologic anatomical anomalies in conventional imaging, advanced imaging studies demonstrated that ASD is closely related to the structural and functional changes of the “social brain” (Amaral et al., 2008). The literature review about structural MRI studies reported the prefrontal cortex, amygdala, superior temporal sulcus, occipital lobe and fusiform gyrus are the key regions of the social brain circuits (Amaral et al., 2008), and categorized into areas related to social impairment, communication deficits, and repetitive behaviors. Compared with healthy controls, ASD patients performed a lower accuracy of semantic sentence comprehension task, and showed a reduced activation in Broca’s area, which involves in expressive language function, while an increased activation in Wernicke’s area, which is essential for receptive language function (Just et al., 2004). Harris et al. examined lexical semantic processing tasks and found a reduced activation in the frontal lobe, whereas a rise activation in the middle temporal gyrus (MTG) in ASD relative to controls (Harris et al., 2006). In addition, compared with TD children, there are significant differences in areas related to speech processing of ASD children aged 2–3 years (Redcay and Courchesne, 2008). Children with autism and low verbal and cognitive performance also showed decreased connectivity within the default, salience, auditory, and frontoparietal networks and decreased inter hemispheric connectivity (Gabrielsen et al., 2018). However, the pattern of brain activity and connectivity changes induced by ASD with language retardation is still unclear. Moreover, the fMRI study on LDD is rare, brain activity differences conducted by the direct comparison of ASD and LDD needs to be explored.

Resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (rs-fMRI) is a non-invasive technique to detect spontaneous activity of the human brain at rest, reflecting disease characteristics (Biswal, 2012). Existing studies have focus on static brain activity changes in ASD, there is increasing evidence of the dynamic nature of brain activity and connectivity over time (Allen et al., 2014). Dynamic characteristics of brain activity are related to cognitive function, brain development (Faghiri et al., 2018), and mental disorders (Hutchison et al., 2013; Cui et al., 2020). Dynamic FC method is also applied in children with ASD, and reports that dFC instability between the posterior cingulate gyrus and pars opercularis of the inferior frontal gyrus is associated with dysfunction of social cognitive processes (Li et al., 2020). However, the dynamic characteristics of regional activity in ASD and LDD is still unknown.

Regional homogeneity (ReHo) reflects the local synchronization of a given voxel and its nearest neighbor (Zang et al., 2004). The amplitude of low-frequency fluctuation (ALFF) reflects the power in the effective frequency range (0.01–0.08 Hz) to detect the regional intensity of spontaneous fluctuations and presenting spontaneous brain activity (Zang et al., 2007). Combining ReHo/ALFF and the “sliding window” method, dynamic ReHo (dReHo) and dynamic ALFF (dALFF), calculating the variance of ReHo and ALFF, reflect dynamic changes of spontaneous brain activity over time (Arbabshirani et al., 2017). The dReHo and dALFF method are sensitive approaches for investigating dynamic brain activity (Liao et al., 2019). The decreased temporal variability of dReHo and dALFF indicates lower flexibility of these cerebral neural activities.

We hypothesized that children aged 1–3 years with ASD or LDD would have different brain dynamic abnormalities related to the characteristics of their diseases. Therefore, in this study, we adopted a sliding window method combined with dReHo and dALFF approaches to investigate different dynamic local brain activities between children with ASD and LDD aged 1–3 years, and then further explored its correlation with clinical assessment. We also assessed the accuracy of the dReHo/dALFF alterations in distinguishing ASD from LDD using receiver operating characteristic (ROC) analysis. The aim of this study was to provide a potential biomarker for the early discrimination of toddlers with ASD from those with LDD.

2. Materials and methods

2.1. Study participants

This study was approved by the Ethics Committee of Hubei Maternal and Child Health Hospital, and informed consent was obtained from all parents prior to the study.

Children aged 1–3 years who visited the Department of Child Health Care of Hubei Maternal and Child Health Hospital because of language impairment were recruited. We examined 58 toddlers with ASD presenting language delay (40 males, 18 females; age 19–36 months) and 36 toddlers with LDD (25 males, 11 females; age 16–36 months). The two groups were well-matched in terms of age, sex, and handedness. Diagnosis of ASD in toddlers was conducted by two experienced neuropsychologists using the Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS) and Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-V). The following inclusion criteria were adopted for the ASD group: (1) met the diagnostic criteria of the DSM-V (Zulauf Logoz, 2014; Mukherjee, 2020), (2) CARS score of ≥30. Patients with epilepsy, cerebral palsy, other organic brain diseases, or neurodevelopmental disorders were excluded. None of the individuals with ASD had any known genetic syndromes associated with ASD (e.g., fragile X syndrome).

For the LDD group, the following inclusion criteria were adopted: (1) toddlers with language problems; (2) Gesell developmental schedule with a language development quotient (LDQ) < 75. The exclusion criteria included ASD, hearing impairment, dysarthria, and other related organic diseases.

2.2. Behavior measurements

The Autism Behavior Checklist (ABC) as used to evaluate the ASD symptoms. This scale has five subscales: sensory, relaxation, body consciousness, language, and social self-help. All ASD toddlers exhibited an ABC score of at least 68 (average ABC score 83.3 ± 7.0) and presented clinical symptoms of stereotypes, communication disorders, and/or language regression.

The LDQ was measured using the Gesell Scale for Children, which included five subtests, including domains of adaptability, gross motor, fine motor, language and social-emotional responses. The development quotient (DQ) of each domain were calculated for each participant. According to the full-scale DQ, the development of infants was classified as follow: normal (DQ ≥ 85), deficient (DQ < 75), and borderline (75 ≤ ∼ < 85). DQ in any single domain falling below 75 was also considered as deficient within this field.

2.3. Magnetic resonance imaging protocol

Thirty minutes before the scanning, all children were sedated by oral administration of 10% chloral hydrate (0.5 ml) and then underwent MRI scanning while falling asleep. A 3.0 T MRI scanner (United Imaging u780) with 8-channel head coil was used. The BOLD-fMRI EPI scan parameters included TR/TE, 2000/30 ms; flip angle, 90°; matrix, 64 × 64; FOV, 240 × 240 mm2; thickness/gap, 3/0 mm; slices, 40; scan time, 8 min. In total, 240 scans were collected. The high-resolution anatomic 3D T1 sequence had the following parameters: 176 sagittal slices; TR/TE/TI, 1900/2.1/900 ms; flip angle, 9°; matrix, 256 × 256; FOV, 240 × 240 mm2; and thickness/gap, 1/0 mm. This session lasted for 4 min and 26 s. A total of 180 scans were obtained. All participants were scanned for T2 and T2-FLAIR images to exclude obvious brain lesions.

2.4. Data analysis

The Data Processing and Analysis of Brain Imaging (DPABI) Toolbox2 running on MATLAB 2018b (MathWorks, Natick, MA, USA) was used to preprocess fMRI and structural images in the resting state. It included the following steps: (1) the first 10 time points were removed, and the remaining 230 consecutive volumes were used for data analyses; (2) slice timing correction and head motion correction were performed. The participants with head movements greater than 3 mm and rotation greater than 3 were excluded. In addition, in order to reduce the possible noise caused by head motion, the volume framewise displacement (FD) was taken as the head motion parameter, and participants whose FD exceeded 0.5 were excluded (Jenkinson et al., 2002). Therefore, 3 cases of ASD and 5 cases of LDD were excluded. Spatial normalization to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space by using infant brain template for zero, four, and five years, and all images were then resampled into 3 mm3 resolutions.

For the dynamic ReHo calculations, band-pass filtering (0.01–0.08 Hz) was applied to reduce the low-frequency drift and high-frequency noise. For specificity, a band-pass filter was applied only to the ReHo.

For the dynamic ALFF calculations, the following steps were sequentially performed: spatial smoothing using a 6 mm full-width-at-half-maximum (FWHM) Gaussian kernel, detrending and regressing nuisance covariates such as the head motion effect using Friston-24 model (Friston et al., 1996), white matter signals, cerebrospinal fluid signals, and global signal. Although global signal regression (GSR) remains highly debated, in this study, GSR was utilized because fMRI global signal includes non-neuronal confounds, such as and physiological artifacts, the application of GSR can exclude the influences from these confounds (Murphy and Fox, 2017).

2.5. Dynamic measurements

Dynamic local metric analysis was performed using the Temporal Dynamic Analysis (TDA) Toolbox. The sliding window method, which is sensitive to time-dependent changes and whole brain metrics variability, was used to calculate dynamic metrics. The window length is the most critical parameter in resting-state dynamic computation (Liao et al., 2019). Previous studies demonstrated that the minimum window length should be greater than 1/fmin (where fmin is the minimum frequency of the time series) to eliminate spurious fluctuations (Leonardi and Van De Ville, 2015). Therefore, we applied a sliding window length of 50 TR (100 s) and a shift step of 1 TR (2 s). This process generated 181 windows for each participant. Based on these sliding windows, we put forward two local metrics: dALFF and dReHo.

dALFF calculation: After calculating the ALFF of all voxels in the time windows, 18l window-based ALFF maps were obtained for each participant. Then, we calculated the mean and standard deviation (SD) of each voxel in all window-based ALFF maps for each participant, and obtained the corresponding coefficient of variation (CV = SD/mean). Finally, CV maps were smoothed by a 6 mm FWHM Gaussian kernel and prepared for further statistical analysis.

dReHo calculation: Individual ReHo maps were generated by calculating the Kendall coefficient of concordance (KCC) of the time courses of a given voxel with those of its neighbors (26 voxels) in a voxel-wise manner. Then, for each participant, we calculated the CV of each voxel in all window-based ReHo maps. Finally, spatially smoothed CV maps with an isotropic Gaussian kernel of 6 mm FWHM were used for further statistical analyses.

2.6. Statistical analysis

The Statistical software SPSS 22.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA) was used for all analyses. Age and clinical characteristics of toddlers with ASD and LDD were compared using a two-sample t-test. Sex differences were determined using Pearson’s chi-square test. With age, gender, and the total intracranial volume (TIV) as covariates, a two-sample t-test was applied to compare the dynamic metrics (dALFF and dReHo) of regional brain activity between toddlers with ASD and those with LDD. False discovery rate (FDR) correction was used for multiple comparisons with two-tailed, voxel level P < 0.05 as statistically significant.

2.7. Correlation analysis with behavior variables

The relationship between dynamic metrics and ABC and CARS scores were conducted by Pearson correlation analysis, with P < 0.05 considered statistically significant.

2.8. Receiver operating characteristic curve

We hypothesized that differences in dReHo and dALFF between the ASD and LDD groups could be useful diagnostic markers. ROC curves were used to analyze the average values of dReHo and dALFF in various brain areas.

2.9. Verification analysis

To validate our dALFF and dReHo findings, three different window lengths [40 TRs (80 s), 50 TRs (100 s), and 60 TRs (120 s)] were calculated in the validation analysis.

3. Results

3.1. Demographic and behavior measurements

As shown in Table 1, there were no significant differences in sex or age between the ASD and LDD groups. No significant lesions were found in the ASD and LDD groups on conventional MRI. The scores on the ABC and CARS scales in the ASD group were significantly higher than those in the LDD group (P < 0.001).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Participant characteristics discriminated according to autism spectrum disorder (ASD) or language developmental delays (LDD).

3.2. dReHo differences

Compared with the LDD group, the ASD group showed increased dReHo in the left cerebellum_8/Crust2 and the right cerebellum_Crust2, and decreased dReHo in the right middle frontal gyrus (MFG) and the right post-central gyrus (Table 2 and Figure 1).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Brain regions showing significant dynamic regional homogeneity (dReHo) differences between autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and language developmental delays (LDD) groups.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Brain areas with significant dReHo differences between autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and language development delay (LDD) groups. Regions in red-yellow are brain areas where dReHo was significantly increased in ASD compared to LDD. Regions in blue-green are brain areas where dReHo was significantly decreased in ASD compared to LDD. The results were multiple compared at the voxel-level (P < 0.05, FDR corrected). The left side of the image corresponds to the left hemisphere of the brain.

3.3. dALFF differences

Toddlers with ASD also exhibited decreased dALFF values in the right middle temporal gyrus (MTG) and right precuneus (Table 3 and Figure 2).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Brain regions showing significant dynamic amplitude of low-frequency fluctuation (dALFF) differences between autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and language developmental delays (LDD) groups.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Brain areas with significant dALFF differences between autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and language development delay (LDD) groups. Regions in red-yellow are brain areas where dALFF was significantly decreased in ASD compared to LDD. The results were multiple compared at the voxel-level (P < 0.05, FDR corrected). The left side of the image corresponds to the left hemisphere of the brain.

3.4. Correlation with behavior variables

Correlation analysis showed that the CARS score was negatively correlated with the dReHo of the left cerebellum_8/crust2 (r = −0.412, P = 0.002) and right cerebellum_crust2 (r = −0.357, P = 0.007). The dReHo value of the right MFG was negatively correlated with social self-help of the ABC score (r = −0.300, P = 0.026). No significant correlation was observed between the dALFF value and the ABC or CARS scores (Table 4 and Figure 3).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Correlation between dynamic regional homogeneity/dynamic amplitude of low-frequency fluctuation (dReHo/dALFF) values and behavioral scores Autism Behavior Checklist/Childhood Autism Rating Scale (ABC/CARS) in the autism spectrum disorder (ASD) group.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Correlation between dReHo value and behavioral scores (ABC/CARS) in autism spectrum disorder (ASD) group. (A) The CARS score was negatively correlated with the dReHo of the left cerebellum_8/crust2 (r = –0.412, P = 0.002). (B) The CARS score was negatively correlated with the dReHo of the right cerebellum_crust2 (r = –0.357, P = 0.007). (C) The dReHo value of the right MFG were negatively correlated with the social self-help of the ABC score (r = –0.300, P = 0.026).

3.5. ROC curve

We hypothesized that the dReHo and dALFF differences between the two groups might be useful diagnostic markers. The mean dReHo and dALFF values in different brain regions were analyzed using the receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curves. When the area under the curve (AUC) is 0.5–0.7 it indicates accuracy is low, if it is 0.7–0.9 accuracy is certain. The AUCs for dReHo values were as follows: cerebellum_8/crust2_L (0.76, P < 0.0001, sensitivity 58.06%, specificity 87.27%), cerebellum_crust2_R (0.77, P < 0.0001, sensitivity 70.97%, specificity 74.55%), right MFG (0.75, P = 0.0001, sensitivity 48.39%, specificity 94.55%), and right post-central gyrus (0.80, P < 0.0001, sensitivity 64.52%, specificity 87.27%) (Figure 4). Moreover, the AUCs for the dALFF values were as follows: right MTG (0.73, P = 0.003, sensitivity 54.84%, specificity 87.27%), and right precuneus (0.68, P = 0.005, sensitivity 51.61%, specificity 83.64%) (Figure 5).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curve analysis of the mean dReHo values for altered brain regions. The AUCs for dReHo values were as follows: cerebellum_8/crust2_L, 0.76; cerebellum_crust2_R, 0.77; MFG_R, 0.75; post-central gyrus, R, 0.80. MFG_R: right middle frontal gyrus; AUC: area under the curve.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curve analysis of the mean dALFF values for altered brain regions. The AUCs for dALFF values were Precuneus_R: 0.68. MTG_R: right middle temporal gyrus; AUC: area under the curve.

3.6. Verification analysis

By using three different window lengths [40 TRs (80 s), 50 TRs (100 s), and 60 TRs (120 s)] with two window steps [1 TR (2 s) and 4 TR (8 s)], we found that the differences in dReHo and dALFF between ASD and LDD were similar to our main findings. These results were provided in Supplementary Figures 18.

4. Discussion

In this study, dReHo and dALFF were used to investigate the differences in temporal variability of regional brain activity between toddlers with ASD and those with LDD. Compared with the LDD group, the ASD group showed increased dReHo in the left cerebellum_8/Crust2 and right cerebellum_Crust2, and decreased dReHo in the right MFG and right post-central gyrus. Patients with ASD also exhibited decreased dALFF values in the right MTG and right precuneus. Moreover, correlation analysis showed that the CARS score was negatively correlated with the dReHo of the left cerebellum_8/crust2 and right cerebellum_crust2. Moreover, the dReHo value of the right MFG was negatively correlated with social self-help based on the ABC score. These findings suggest that dynamic regional activity might be applied in clinical practice in the future to distinguish ASD from LDD.

Many neuroimaging studies have proposed hypotheses regarding the cortical basis of ASD with language dysfunction, one of which is the reversed functional lateralization of the language network (Herringshaw et al., 2016). Both the Broca (located in the left inferior frontal gyrus) and Wernicke (located in the left superior temporal gyrus) areas, which mediate social language processing, are located in the left hemisphere (Kana and Wadsworth, 2012). In ASD, the Broca and Wernicke areas are regarded as functionally impaired. However, compared with TD children, children with ASD show significantly stronger right hemisphere activation in speech perception tasks (Redcay and Courchesne, 2008). For example, rs-fMRI studies have found decreased ReHo in the right superior temporal sulcus, right inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), right middle frontal gyrus (MFG), right post-central gyrus, right insula, and bilateral cerebellum crust1 in ASD (Paakki et al., 2010). Our research also found decreased dReHo in the MFG and post-central gyrus and decreased dALFF in the MTG and precuneus on the right side, relative to LDD. Therefore, it is reasonable to infer those children diagnosed with ASD at the age of 1–3 years not only have impaired left language brain function but also have more extensive functional connectivity abnormalities in the right language brain region compared to children with LDD alone, which may provide certain clues for the early differential diagnosis of ASD and LDD.

The prefrontal cortex is the core area for ASD. Functional imaging studies have shown that the prefrontal cortex plays an important role in social cognition (Forbes and Grafman, 2010). Research on early brain growth in children with ASD showed increased gray matter volume in the prefrontal cortex, including the orbitofrontal cortex, IFG, MFG, and SFG. This is one of the reliable findings in ASD, and its pathological basis was found to be a significantly increased number of neurons (Courchesne et al., 2011). However, rs-fMRI studies have described an extensively weakened prefrontal-posterior functional connectivity, which may indicate impairment of cognitive, social, and speech processing in ASD. Enticott et al. found that repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) to the medial prefrontal cortex can reduce social-related impairment in patients with ASD, indicating that this may be a target region for these symptoms (Enticott et al., 2014). Consistently with previous rs-fMRI studies, we found a reduced dReHo in the right MFG. Further correlation analysis showed that the dReHo value of the right MFG in ASD children correlated with social self-help assessed via the ABC score. This indicates that the worse the local consistency of the right MFG brain region, the higher the social and self-help scores on the ABC scale, and the worse the social and self-help ability. Studies have found that the right MFG is related to high-level executive function. For example, attention, memory, planning, and judgment (Talati and Hirsch, 2005) and the decreased dReHo value of the right MFG will affect the social self-care ability of children with ASD, which is consistent with the results of this study. It also suggests that compared with children with LDD alone, the early social and self-help ability of children with ASD needs special attention. The ROC curve showed 48.39% sensitivity and 94.55% specificity to distinguish ASD from LDD. Therefore, these findings confirm the importance of the prefrontal cortex in the early diagnosis of ASD.

In addition to language-related activities and processes (McDermott et al., 2003; Whitney et al., 2011; Hickok, 2012), the MTG is also involved in action observation (Whitney et al., 2011), situational investigation (Hsu and Sonuga-Barke, 2016), complex sound processing (Hesling et al., 2005), logical reasoning (Goel et al., 1998), and dynamic facial expression recognition (Sato et al., 2012). Recently, more and more researchers have expressed the opinion that abnormalities in the “social network” (Adolphs, 2003; Blakemore, 2012) may be the neural mechanism underlying social dysfunction in ASD (Pelphrey and Carter, 2008). As one of the main nodes of the “social network” (Sato et al., 2017), MTG abnormalities have been reported many times in ASD (Assaf et al., 2013; Yerys et al., 2015; Ogawa et al., 2019). Our study also found reduced ALFF variability in the right MTG in ASD patients, which is consistent with local connectivity reduction (Itahashi et al., 2015) and morphological changes (Libero et al., 2014). Compared with LDD, the right MTG may be more related to the pathogenesis of social dysfunction, which is the core symptom of ASD.

The precuneus is a hub in the default mode network (DMN). The clinical manifestations of ASD overlap with the functions of the DMN, including emotional processing, perception of interpersonal communication, and judgment of self and others’ mental states; suggesting that the DMN may be damaged in ASD (Glerean et al., 2016). Although functional connectivity (FC) in the DMN is aberrant, either increased or decreased FC has been reported in ASD patients. We found decreased dALFF in the precuneus in ASD compared to LDD, providing additional evidence of DMN defects.

The cerebellum is one of the most common regions presenting abnormalities in ASD, and this has been variously reported from the cellular to the behavioral level. Most postmortem studies on ASD have reported reduced Purkinje cell counts in the cerebellar cortex, and specific targeting of cerebellar Purkinje cells induces ASD-related symptoms in animal models (Di Martino et al., 2014). Structural differences in the ASD cerebellum are associated with social and communication impairments, limited interest, and repetitive behaviors. At the genetic level, genes involved in ASD (e.g., SHANK3, EN2, and RORA) are commonly associated with cerebellar development (Rogers et al., 2013). The current results found that toddlers with ASD exhibited significantly higher dReHo values in the left cerebellum_8/Crust2 and the right cerebellum_Crust2 than those with LDD, with a negative correlation with CARS score. These results indicate that cerebellar development is abnormal in early ASD compared with LDD alone, and this abnormality is directly related to the severity of ASD. Considered together, these pieces of evidence led us to speculate that the cerebellum may regulate the dysfunction of the neocortical circuit by enhancing its own function, which might be a potential mechanism that explains some aspects of the ASD phenotype (Hegarty et al., 2018).

This study had some limitations. First, the heterogeneity of autism suggests that further studies and analysis of subtypes may lead to clearer imaging markers. Secondly, a recent area of development in ASD research is the classification of ASD based on resting-state data in conjunction with machine learning and deep learning techniques. In the future, by using dynamic brain parameters as inputs, we expect that the machine learning classifier will show a high level of accuracy.

In summary, the clinical manifestations of ASD with language delay in the early stages among toddlers are like those of LDD and are difficult to distinguish by clinical evaluation alone. The current study suggests the presence of decreased dReHo/dALFF in the right MFG, post-central gyrus, and MTG, and increased dReHo in the bilateral cerebellum in ASD relative to LDD. Our results may be helpful for the diagnosis of ASD. Identifying the differences in brain development between ASD and LDD will be the first step in the development of novel early screening tools, the determination of when to treat, and the evaluation of the intervention efficacy in ASD. Our next step is to follow up with the children with autism so that we can conduct a pre- and post-control study.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Ethics Committee of Hubei Maternal and Child Health Hospital. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.

Author contributions

SW: experimental design, data collection, data analysis, and article writing. ZW: data processing and analysis and article writing. HW: experimental design, data collection, article writing, and funds to support. FL: data processing and article writing. WY and CJ: data collection. YZ and ZZ: article writing. AZ and YW: data collection and statistical analysis. XL and XW: data processing and analysis and literature review. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This study was supported by the Hubei Provincial Top Medical Young Talents Project (No. 10200112027).

Acknowledgments

The authors thank all the doctors, therapists, nurses, and other staff members at the Department of Child Health Care, Maternal and Child Health Hospital of Hubei Province, who kindly provided their cooperation in this study.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnins.2022.1097244/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

  1. ^ https://www.autismspeaks.org/autism-statistics-asd
  2. ^ http://www.rfmri.org/dpabi

References

Adolphs, R. (2003). Cognitive neuroscience of human social behaviour. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 4, 165–178. doi: 10.1038/nrn1056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Allen, E. A., Damaraju, E., Plis, S. M., Erhardt, E. B., Eichele, T., and Calhoun, V. D. (2014). Tracking whole-brain connectivity dynamics in the resting state. Cereb. Cortex 24, 663–676. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs352

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Amaral, D. G., Schumann, C. M., and Nordahl, C. W. (2008). Neuroanatomy of autism. Trends Neurosci. 31, 137–145. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2007.12.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arbabshirani, M. R., Plis, S., Sui, J., and Calhoun, V. D. (2017). Single subject prediction of brain disorders in neuroimaging: Promises and pitfalls. Neuroimage 145(Pt B), 137–165. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.02.079

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Assaf, M., Hyatt, C. J., Wong, C. G., Johnson, M. R., Schultz, R. T., Hendler, T., et al. (2013). Mentalizing and motivation neural function during social interactions in autism spectrum disorders. Neuroimage Clin. 3, 321–331. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2013.09.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biswal, B. B. (2012). Resting state fMRI: A personal history. Neuroimage 62, 938–944. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.090

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blakemore, S. J. (2012). Development of the social brain in adolescence. J. R. Soc. Med. 105, 111–116. doi: 10.1258/jrsm.2011.110221

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chung, C. Y., Liu, W. Y., Chang, C. J., Chen, C. L., Tang, S. F., and Wong, A. M. (2011). The relationship between parental concerns and final diagnosis in children with developmental delay. J. Child Neurol. 26, 413–419. doi: 10.1177/0883073810381922

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Courchesne, E., Mouton, P. R., Calhoun, M. E., Semendeferi, K., Ahrens-Barbeau, C., Hallet, M. J., et al. (2011). Neuron number and size in prefrontal cortex of children with autism. JAMA 306, 2001–2010. doi: 10.1001/jama.2011.1638

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cui, Q., Sheng, W., Chen, Y., Pang, Y., Lu, F., Tang, Q., et al. (2020). Dynamic changes of amplitude of low-frequency fluctuations in patients with generalized anxiety disorder. Hum. Brain Mapp. 41, 1667–1676. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24902

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Di Martino, A., Yan, C. G., Li, Q., Denio, E., Castellanos, F. X., Alaerts, K., et al. (2014). The autism brain imaging data exchange: Towards a large-scale evaluation of the intrinsic brain architecture in autism. Mol. Psychiatry 19, 659–667. doi: 10.1038/mp.2013.78

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Enticott, P. G., Fitzgibbon, B. M., Kennedy, H. A., Arnold, S. L., Elliot, D., Peachey, A., et al. (2014). A double-blind, randomized trial of deep repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) for autism spectrum disorder. Brain Stimul. 7, 206–211. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2013.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faghiri, A., Stephen, J. M., Wang, Y. P., Wilson, T. W., and Calhoun, V. D. (2018). Changing brain connectivity dynamics: From early childhood to adulthood. Hum. Brain Mapp. 39, 1108–1117. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23896

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Forbes, C. E., and Grafman, J. (2010). The role of the human prefrontal cortex in social cognition and moral judgment. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 33, 299–324. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-060909-153230

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friston, K. J., Williams, S., Howard, R., Frackowiak, R. S., and Turner, R. (1996). Movement-related effects in fMRI time-series. Magn. Reson. Med. 35, 346–355. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910350312

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gabrielsen, T. P., Anderson, J. S., Stephenson, K. G., Beck, J., King, J. B., Kellems, R., et al. (2018). Functional MRI connectivity of children with autism and low verbal and cognitive performance. Mol. Autism 9:67. doi: 10.1186/s13229-018-0248-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glerean, E., Pan, R. K., Salmi, J., Kujala, R., Lahnakoski, J. M., Roine, U., et al. (2016). Reorganization of functionally connected brain subnetworks in high-functioning autism. Hum. Brain Mapp. 37, 1066–1079. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23084

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goel, V., Gold, B., Kapur, S., and Houle, S. (1998). Neuroanatomical correlates of human reasoning. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 10, 293–302. doi: 10.1162/089892998562744

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harris, G. J., Chabris, C. F., Clark, J., Urban, T., Aharon, I., Steele, S., et al. (2006). Brain activation during semantic processing in autism spectrum disorders via functional magnetic resonance imaging. Brain Cogn. 61, 54–68. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2005.12.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hegarty, J. P. II, Weber, D. J., Cirstea, C. M., and Beversdorf, D. Q. (2018). Cerebro-cerebellar functional connectivity is associated with cerebellar excitation-inhibition balance in autism spectrum disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 48, 3460–3473. doi: 10.1007/s10803-018-3613-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herringshaw, A. J., Ammons, C. J., DeRamus, T. P., and Kana, R. K. (2016). Hemispheric differences in language processing in autism spectrum disorders: A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Autism Res. 9, 1046–1057. doi: 10.1002/aur.1599

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hesling, I., Clement, S., Bordessoules, M., and Allard, M. (2005). Cerebral mechanisms of prosodic integration: Evidence from connected speech. Neuroimage 24, 937–947. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.11.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hickok, G. (2012). The cortical organization of speech processing: Feedback control and predictive coding the context of a dual-stream model. J. Commun. Disord. 45, 393–402. doi: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2012.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hsu, C. F., and Sonuga-Barke, E. J. (2016). Electrophysiological evidence during episodic prospection implicates medial prefrontal and bilateral middle temporal gyrus. Brain Res. 1644, 296–305. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2016.03.040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hutchison, R. M., Womelsdorf, T., Allen, E. A., Bandettini, P. A., Calhoun, V. D., Corbetta, M., et al. (2013). Dynamic functional connectivity: Promise, issues, and interpretations. Neuroimage 80, 360–378. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.079

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Itahashi, T., Yamada, T., Watanabe, H., Nakamura, M., Ohta, H., Kanai, C., et al. (2015). Alterations of local spontaneous brain activity and connectivity in adults with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder. Mol. Autism 6:30. doi: 10.1186/s13229-015-0026-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jenkinson, M., Bannister, P., Brady, M., and Smith, S. (2002). Improved optimization for the robust and accurate linear registration and motion correction of brain images. Neuroimage 17, 825–841. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(02)91132-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Just, M. A., Cherkassky, V. L., Keller, T. A., and Minshew, N. J. (2004). Cortical activation and synchronization during sentence comprehension in high-functioning autism: Evidence of underconnectivity. Brain 127(Pt 8), 1811–1821. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh199

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kana, R. K., and Wadsworth, H. M. (2012). “The archeologist’s career ended in ruins”: Hemispheric differences in pun comprehension in autism. Neuroimage 62, 77–86. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.04.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leonardi, N., and Van De Ville, D. (2015). On spurious and real fluctuations of dynamic functional connectivity during rest. Neuroimage 104, 430–436. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.09.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, Y., Zhu, Y., Nguchu, B. A., Wang, Y., Wang, H., Qiu, B., et al. (2020). Dynamic functional connectivity reveals abnormal variability and hyper-connected pattern in autism spectrum disorder. Autism Res. 13, 230–243. doi: 10.1002/aur.2212

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liao, W., Li, J., Ji, G. J., Wu, G. R., Long, Z., Xu, Q., et al. (2019). Endless fluctuations: Temporal dynamics of the amplitude of low frequency fluctuations. IEEE Trans. Med. Imaging 38, 2523–2532. doi: 10.1109/TMI.2019.2904555

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Libero, L. E., DeRamus, T. P., Deshpande, H. D., and Kana, R. K. (2014). Surface-based morphometry of the cortical architecture of autism spectrum disorders: Volume, thickness, area, and gyrification. Neuropsychologia 62, 1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McDermott, K. B., Petersen, S. E., Watson, J. M., and Ojemann, J. G. (2003). A procedure for identifying regions preferentially activated by attention to semantic and phonological relations using functional magnetic resonance imaging. Neuropsychologia 41, 293–303. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(02)00162-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mukherjee, S. B. (2020). Identification, evaluation, and management of children with autism spectrum disorder: American academy of pediatrics 2020 clinical guidelines. Indian Pediatr. 57, 959–962.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Murphy, K., and Fox, M. D. (2017). Towards a consensus regarding global signal regression for resting state functional connectivity MRI. Neuroimage 154, 169–173. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.11.052

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ogawa, R., Kagitani-Shimono, K., Matsuzaki, J., Tanigawa, J., Hanaie, R., Yamamoto, T., et al. (2019). Abnormal cortical activation during silent reading in adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Brain Dev. 41, 234–244. doi: 10.1016/j.braindev.2018.10.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paakki, J. J., Rahko, J., Long, X., Moilanen, I., Tervonen, O., Nikkinen, J., et al. (2010). Alterations in regional homogeneity of resting-state brain activity in autism spectrum disorders. Brain Res. 1321, 169–179. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.12.081

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pelphrey, K. A., and Carter, E. J. (2008). Charting the typical and atypical development of the social brain. Dev. Psychopathol. 20, 1081–1102. doi: 10.1017/S0954579408000515

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Redcay, E., and Courchesne, E. (2008). Deviant functional magnetic resonance imaging patterns of brain activity to speech in 2-3-year-old children with autism spectrum disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 64, 589–598. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.05.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rogers, T. D., Dickson, P. E., McKimm, E., Heck, D. H., Goldowitz, D., Blaha, C. D., et al. (2013). Reorganization of circuits underlying cerebellar modulation of prefrontal cortical dopamine in mouse models of autism spectrum disorder. Cerebellum 12, 547–556. doi: 10.1007/s12311-013-0462-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sato, W., Kochiyama, T., Uono, S., Yoshimura, S., Kubota, Y., Sawada, R., et al. (2017). Reduced gray matter volume in the social brain network in adults with autism spectrum disorder. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:395. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00395

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sato, W., Toichi, M., Uono, S., and Kochiyama, T. (2012). Impaired social brain network for processing dynamic facial expressions in autism spectrum disorders. BMC Neurosci. 13:99. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-13-99

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schulte-Korne, G. (2014). [Specific learning disabilities - from DSM-IV to DSM-5]. Z. Kinder Jugendpsychiatr. Psychother. 42, 369–372. doi: 10.1024/1422-4917/a000312

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sun, X., Allison, C., Wei, L., Matthews, F. E., Auyeung, B., Wu, Y. Y., et al. (2019). Autism prevalence in China is comparable to western prevalence. Mol. Autism 10:7. doi: 10.1186/s13229-018-0246-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Talati, A., and Hirsch, J. (2005). Functional specialization within the medial frontal gyrus for perceptual go/no-go decisions based on “what,” “when,” and “where” related information: An fMRI study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 17, 981–993. doi: 10.1162/0898929054475226

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whitney, C., Jefferies, E., and Kircher, T. (2011). Heterogeneity of the left temporal lobe in semantic representation and control: Priming multiple versus single meanings of ambiguous words. Cereb. Cortex 21, 831–844. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq148

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yerys, B. E., Antezana, L., Weinblatt, R., Jankowski, K. F., Strang, J., Vaidya, C. J., et al. (2015). Neural correlates of set-shifting in children with autism. Autism Res. 8, 386–397. doi: 10.1002/aur.1454

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zang, Y. F., He, Y., Zhu, C. Z., Cao, Q. J., Sui, M. Q., Liang, M., et al. (2007). Altered baseline brain activity in children with ADHD revealed by resting-state functional MRI. Brain Dev. 29, 83–91. doi: 10.1016/j.braindev.2006.07.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zang, Y., Jiang, T., Lu, Y., He, Y., and Tian, L. (2004). Regional homogeneity approach to fMRI data analysis. Neuroimage 22, 394–400. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.12.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zulauf Logoz, M. (2014). [The revision and 5th edition of the diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5): Consequences for the diagnostic work with children and adolescents]. Prax. Kinderpsychol. Kinderpsychiatr. 63, 562–576. doi: 10.13109/prkk.2014.63.7.562

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: autism spectrum disorder, language development delay, dynamic regional homogeneity, dynamic amplitude of low-frequency fluctuation, early diagnosis

Citation: Wu S, Wen Z, Yang W, Jiang C, Zhou Y, Zhao Z, Zhou A, Liu X, Wang X, Wang Y, Wang H and Lin F (2023) Potential dynamic regional brain biomarkers for early discrimination of autism and language development delay in toddlers. Front. Neurosci. 16:1097244. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2022.1097244

Received: 13 November 2022; Accepted: 19 December 2022;
Published: 09 January 2023.

Edited by:

Stephen J. Gotts, National Institute of Mental Health (NIH), United States

Reviewed by:

Changchun He, University of Electronic Science and Technology of China, China
Youxue Zhang, Chengdu Normal University, China

Copyright © 2023 Wu, Wen, Yang, Jiang, Zhou, Zhao, Zhou, Liu, Wang, Wang, Wang and Lin. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Hong Wang, www.frontiersin.org hong7834@163.com; Fuchun Lin, www.frontiersin.org fclin@wipm.ac.cn

These authors have contributed equally to this work and share first authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.