Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurosci., 22 December 2022
Sec. Translational Neuroscience
This article is part of the Research Topic Traumatic Stress Disorders View all 7 articles

Individual behavioral profiling as a translational approach to assess treatment efficacy in an animal model of post-traumatic stress disorder

\r\nIshita SarkarIshita Sarkar1Maja Snippe-StraussMaja Snippe-Strauss2Adi Tenenhaus ZamirAdi Tenenhaus Zamir1Amir BenhosAmir Benhos2Gal Richter-Levin,,*Gal Richter-Levin1,2,3*
  • 1Sagol Department of Neurobiology, University of Haifa, Haifa, Israel
  • 2School of Psychological Sciences, University of Haifa, Haifa, Israel
  • 3The Integrated Brain and Behavior Research Center, University of Haifa, Haifa, Israel

A major challenge in treating post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) continues to be the large variability in responsiveness to pharmacotherapy. Only 20–30% of patients experience total remission to a specific treatment, while others demonstrate either partial remission or no response. However, this heterogeneity in response to pharmacotherapy has not been adequately addressed in animal models, since these analyze the averaged group effects, ignoring the individual variability to treatment response, which seriously compromises the translation power of such models. Here we examined the possibility of employing an “individual behavioral profiling” approach, originally developed to differentiate between “affected” and “exposed-unaffected” individuals in an animal model of PTSD, to also enable dissociating “responders” or “non-responders” after SSRI (fluoxetine) treatment. Importantly, this approach does not rely on a group averaged response to a single behavioral parameter, but considers a cluster of behavioral parameters, to individually characterize an animal as either “responder” or “non-responder” to the treatment. The main variable to assess drug efficacy thus being the proportion of “responders” following treatment. Alteration in excitatory/inhibitory (E/I) balance has been proposed as being associated with stress-related psychopathology. Toward a functional proof of concept for our behaviorally-based characterization approach, we examined the expression patterns of α1 and α2 subunits of GABAA receptor, and GluN1 and GluN2A subunits of the NMDAR receptor in the ventral hippocampus, as well as electrophysiologically local circuit activity in the dorsal dentate gyrus (DG). We demonstrate that with both parameters, treatment “responders” differed from treatment “non-responders,” confirming the functional validity of the behavior-based categorization. The results suggest that the ability to respond to fluoxetine treatment may be linked to the ability to modulate excitation-inhibition balance in the hippocampus. We propose that employing the “individual behavioral profiling” approach, and the resultant novel variable of the proportion of “recovered” individuals following treatment, offers an effective translational tool to assess pharmacotherapy treatment efficacy in animal models of stress and trauma-related psychopathology.

Introduction

While treatment efficacy in humans is measured by comparing an individual’s behavioral and physiological traits to an averaged healthy population, the accepted approach of measuring efficacy of pharmacological treatments in animal models of psychiatric disorders refers to averaged group results (Katz and Hersh, 1981; Bodnoff et al., 1989; Rudolph and Feiger, 1999; Dulawa et al., 2004). This contradiction is particularly evident in studies assessing the effects of pharmacotherapy in animal models of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and the actual response rate to pharmacotherapy in PTSD patients. For instance, Wilson et al. (2014), reported that chronic sertraline treatment in animal model of PTSD did not alleviate anxiety-like behavior as evinced by no increase in exploration of the open arms in elevated-platform test after treatment (Wilson et al., 2014). Similarly, previous work from our lab had shown that chronic fluoxetine treatment failed to rescue PTSD-like symptoms in adult rats post-trauma (Ariel et al., 2017). In both studies, the behavioral response to sertraline or fluoxetine was reported as an averaged group effect of the treated animals. However, humans statistics suggest that while the majority of PTSD patients do not respond to treatment, a considerable fraction of 30–40% patients do respond to pharmacotherapy (Ravindran and Stein, 2010) and a significant minority of 20–30% of patients attain complete remission (Steckler and Risbrough, 2012). This individual variability in treatment response is obscured in animal models using group averages, thus limiting their translational power.

A way to overcome this limitation, however, can be if each animal is profiled individually based on their behavior. The concept of profiling animals individually based on certain behavioral “cut-off” values was first established by Cohen et al. (2007) and later modified by our lab to categories trauma-exposed animals as PTSD-affected or PTSD-unaffected, in reference to the behavior of a control population (Ardi et al., 2016). In the current study, we aimed to adapt the individual behavioral profiling (IBP) approach in order to address the heterogeneity in treatment response in an animal model of PTSD (Richter-Levin et al., 2015). We administered chronic fluoxetine treatment to the PTSD-affected animals and employed the IBP approach to classify them as treatment responders or non-responders.

Fluoxetine belongs to the class of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) and is used as a first line (off-label) medication for treating PTSD patients (Management of Post-Traumatic Stress Working Group, 2010). However, effectiveness of fluoxetine is dubious since different studies either acclaim it as beneficial in civilians and veterans (Connor et al., 1999) or report its inefficiency (Martenyi et al., 2007). Probably, part of the confusion regarding the efficacy of SSRIs results from the fact that, since only about 30% of patients show full remission, the averaged efficacy of the drug is not optimal. However, this averaged view obscures the efficacy of the drug for those who respond well to the treatment.

Post-traumatic stress disorder is characterized by impaired GABAergic (Girgenti et al., 2021) and glutamatergic transmission (Krystal et al., 2017), especially in the hippocampus. This disruption in the excitation-inhibition (E/I) balance has been extensively investigated in animal models of PTSD. Traumatic stress in juvenility has been reported to induce anxiety-like behavior and altered α2, α5, and γ2 subunits expression of GABAA receptor in the hippocampus (Lu et al., 2017). Increased α1 expression in the ventral hippocampus has been associated with resilience to PTSD-like symptoms in an animal model of PTSD (Ardi et al., 2019). Similarly, a study in predator-exposed rat model of PTSD reported decreased expression of GluN1 subunit of NMDA receptor in the dorsal hippocampus following trauma, which resulted in increased nitric oxide synthase in neurons, which was implicated in several anxiety-disorders (Ayhan et al., 2016). Increased expression of GluN1 and GluN2A subunits of NMDA receptor in the frontal and pre-frontal cortex post trauma was also observed in an acute foot-shock model of PTSD (Bonini et al., 2016). Further, previous research from our lab showed that exposure to trauma and/or re-exposure to trauma reminder increased feed-forward inhibition in the local inhibitory circuits and impaired long-term potentiation (LTP) in the dorsal dentate gyrus (dDG) in rat-model of PTSD (Ardi et al., 2014). Together the evidence suggests that disruption of the E/I homeostasis is one of the major underlying causes of PTSD pathophysiology (Chambers et al., 1999; Bremner et al., 2000; Reul and Nutt, 2008; Meyerhoff et al., 2014; Michels et al., 2014; Rosso et al., 2014).

Fluoxetine is known to influence E/I balance by altering GABA and glutamate mediated neurotransmission (Fang et al., 2018; Lazarevic et al., 2019; Sheth et al., 2019). It can increase GABAA receptor activity through novel modulatory sites in α1-α6 subunits except α5 (Robinson et al., 2003). Additionally α2 subunit is essential for manifesting anxiolytic effects of fluoxetine (Benham et al., 2017). Fluoxetine also modulates NMDA receptor activity by selectively inhibiting GluN2B containing receptors (Kiss et al., 2012). Chronic fluoxetine administration enhances maturation of synapses and dendritic arborization by the increasing concentration of GluN2A subunits in the PFC (Ampuero et al., 2019).

In the current study we employed a rat model of PTSD that combined juvenile stress and trauma exposure in adulthood (Ardi et al., 2016). This set-up simulates early life adversity that increases the risk of developing psychiatric disorders (Heim and Nemeroff, 2001) and a severe acute trauma that mimics trauma exposure in PTSD patients. In the first set of experiments we employed IBP to classify animals as trauma affected or unaffected. Trauma affected animals were administered chronic fluoxetine treatment in drinking water for a period of 30 days. At the end of the treatment, the animals were categorized as treatment responders or non-responders according to a second round of IBP (IBP I). As a biochemical proof of concept for IBP I and in order to better understand the influence of fluoxetine on E/I balance, we measured the expression levels of GABAAα1, GABAAα2 and GluN1, GluN2A receptor subunits in the ventral hippocampus in responders and non-responders. In the second set of experiments, we checked the flexibility and strength of the IBP approach by incorporating a different set of behavioral parameters. We used IBP approach II to classify animals as trauma affected and later distinguished them as treatment responders or non-responders after fluoxetine treatment. As a testbed for IBP II and to investigate the alteration of E/I balance at neural circuit level, we measured changes in the local circuit activity and LTP in the dDG in treatment responders and non-responders.

We hypothesized that both sets of IBP would effectively delineate between treatment responders and non-responders. The percentage of responsivity to fluoxetine would be close to that observed in human PTSD patients receiving SSRIs. We further expected to see differential expression pattern of the GABAA, and NMDAR receptor subunits, and altered electrophysiological properties between responders and non-responders, which may point to the involvement of altered I/E balance in treatment responsiveness.

Materials and methods

Animals

Male Sprague Dawley rats of post-natal day 22 were ordered from Harlan laboratories, Jerusalem. Animals were group housed (22 ± 2°C, 12 h light dark cycle) with food and water ad libitum. Five days of acclimatization period was provided to all animals before the start of the experiments. All experiments were performed according to the NIH guide for care and use of laboratory animals and approved by the University of Haifa ethical committee.

An animal model of post-traumatic stress disorder

In the current study we employed the previously established rat model of PTSD, which combines exposure to juvenile stress (JVS) with a later exposure to underwater trauma (UWT) in adulthood (Richter-Levin et al., 2015). Briefly, animals were exposed to the juvenile stress protocol (Horovitz et al., 2012) for a period of three consecutive days during post-natal days (PND) 27-29. The protocol is comprised of three different stressors (i) Forced swim (15 min) on day 1, (ii) elevated platform (30 min, 3 times, 1-h break between each session) on day 2, and (iii) restrain stress (2 h) on day 3. After that, the animals were left undisturbed for a period of 30 days. The control group was not exposed to JVS.

On PND 60 the animals were brought and habituated for 5 min in the UWT room. The water filled plastic tank for UWT (diameter 70 cm, height 50 cm, water depth 40 cm, water temperature 22 ± 2°C) was part of the water associated zero maze (WAZM) set-up (Ritov and Richter-Levin, 2014). The animals were allowed 5 min of exploration the WAZM platform. Immediately after that, they were restrained under water, inside the water tank, for 45 s by a special net (20 × 20 × 15 cm). The control groups were not immersed in water and were returned to their cages after exploration.

Behavioral tests

Water associated zero maze

This behavioral test set-up was previously established in our lab to measure anxiety caused by traumatic re-experiencing animals (Ritov and Richter-Levin, 2014). It consisted of an annular platform (90 cm diameter; 10 cm width), made out of black plywood, and joined to the UWT plastic tank. The annular platform had two opposite closed quadrants (with walls 35 cm height) and two open quadrants (with borders 5 mm height). For the tests, rats were first habituated to the room for 5 min and then were placed into one of the open quadrants facing a closed part of the apparatus. Rats were allowed to explore the arena for 5 min. During this time, behavior of the rat was tracked, recorded and analyzed by the Etho-Vision system (Noldus Information Technology, Wageningen, Netherlands).

Open field test

Open field test was used to measure anxiety-like behavior in animals by analyzing the total distance traveled or time spent in the center of the OFT box (Gould et al., 2009). Animals were first habituated for 5 min to the OFT room before starting the experiment. The rats were then placed at the corner of the open field box (90 cm × 90 cm × 50 cm, dim white light illumination) and allowed to explore the arena freely for 5 min (Avital and Richter-Levin, 2005).

Social recognition test

Social recognition test measures the social recognition memory of an animal by determining the preference index of the subject animal for an unfamiliar animal over a familiar animal (Eagle et al., 2013; Gur et al., 2014; Jacobs et al., 2016). This test is based on the observations that rodents tend to prefer exploring socially novel partner over familiar (Thor and Holloway, 1982). In this study we measured short-term social recognition memory. SRT was conducted a day following the OFT, in the same arena. At the end of the OFT recording, the animals were habituated with corrals (9 cm in diameter, slotted with holes and covered with Plexiglas) kept in a diagonally opposite direction for 5 min. These corrals were used to place the stimuli for SRT the next day. On the day of SRT, the subject rats were first familiarized to a juvenile conspecific kept in one of the corrals for a period of 15 min. After the familiarization session all the animals were returned to home-cage for 30 min. After the 30 min the subject animal was brought back from its home cage and placed again in the arena for 5 min with the previous familiar juvenile conspecific and a novel unfamiliar juvenile conspecific (Gur et al., 2014). The time spent investigating each corral was recorded and measured using the EthoVision XT8 tracking system (Gur et al., 2014). The position of familiar and unfamiliar animals was changed between animals to minimize positional bias. Preference index for unfamiliar animal in SRT was calculated by using the following formula (Eagle et al., 2013):

(Timespent(unfamiliar)-Timespent(familiar)Timespent(unfamiliar)+Timespent(familiar))x 100

Elevated plus maze

The EPM set-up consisted of two opposing open (anxiogenic) and closed (anxiolytic) arms which is used to measure anxiety-like behavior in rodents (Walf and Frye, 2007). Animals were brought from their home-cage and habituated to the EPM room for 5 min. The test was carried out by placing the rats in the center of the EPM maze (110 cm × 110 cm, 70 m above the floor; full light illumination) facing an open arm and allowing them to explore the maze freely for 5 min. The behavior of the animal was analyzed by EthoVision XT8 video tracking system (Noldus, Wageningen, Netherlands).

Fluoxetine administration

Twenty-four-hour water consumption was measured in animals for three consecutive days prior to drug delivery. Fresh solutions were given twice a week using Fluoxetine stock solution of (3 mg/ml) (Vetmarket, Petah tikva, Israel). Animals were weighed before and given fluoxetine at a dose of 10 mg/kg/day in drinking water after calculating the concentration based on their body weight and average daily intake of water (Ariel et al., 2017). The treatment was given for 30 days. Fluoxetine was given in opaque bottles due to its light sensitivity.

Experiment 1

Experimental paradigm I

Animals were exposed to JVS at PND 27 and UWT at PND 60. Behavioral tests, including WAZM and EPM, were conducted 2 weeks post UWT to differentiate animals into trauma affected or unaffected. Trauma affected animals were given fluoxetine treatment for a period of 30 days in drinking water. At the end of the treatment, WAZM and EPM test were repeated to classify animals as treatment responders or non-responders. Animals were sacrificed and brain tissues were harvested at the conclusion of the behavioral tests. Western blot analysis was conducted to check expression of α1, α2 subunit of GABAA receptor and GluN1 and GluN2A subunit of NMDAR in the (i) ventral dentate gyrus (vDG), (ii) ventral CA1 (vCA1), (iii) ventral CA3 (vCA3) (Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Schematic representation of experimental paradigm 1.

Individual behavioral profiling I

Based on the performance on WAZM and EPM, 7 behavioral parameters that were reflective of explorative activity and anxiety levels were chosen (Supplementary Figure 1). One standard deviation was used to decide the cut-off value for each behavioral parameter based on the performance of the control animals. Animal whose behavioral parameter value was either lower or higher than the cut-off value was considered affected in that specific parameter. Post-UWT, animals which were found to be affected in at least 4 out of 7 parameters were classified as trauma affected. Similarly post treatment animals were required to exhibit less than 4 affected parameters to be considered treatment responders. The following parameters were used to classify the animals as trauma affected and unaffected or fluoxetine responders and non-responders:

   i) Total distance traveled in WAZM

  ii) Time spent in closed arms of WAZM

 iii) Distance traveled in closed arms of WAZM

 iv) Distance traveled in open arms of WAZM

  v) Total freezing in WAZM

 vi) Total freezing in EPM

vii) Anxiety index in EPM closed arm time

(timeinclosedarms+timeincenterTotaltime)x 100

Experimental groups

Rats were randomly assigned to one these two groups: controls (n = 16) and JVS + UWT i.e., Trauma exposed (n = 93). IBP 1 was used to classify animals as trauma-affected or trauma-unaffected. The trauma-affected) rats (n = 53) were treated with fluoxetine for 30 days. Post-treatment animals were further classified as treatment responders or non-responders, again based on IBP 1. Brain tissue was harvested and the ventral hippocampus dissected out from a subset of these animals. Western-blot experiment was conducted for responders which showed complete symptom remission (n = 6), non-responders (n = 4) that continued to be severely affected on all behavioral parameters and unaffected control animals (n = 5).

Brain harvesting and tissue collection

Animals were sacrificed and the brains were snap frozen in dry ice to keep the protein stability intact. On the cryostat, frozen tissue punches were collected (diameter 1 mm, depth 1.5 mm) from vDG, vCA1, and vCA3—7.6 from Bregma, horizontal orientation of the brain.

Protein preparation and western blot analysis

One milliliter of lysis buffer was prepared for tissue homogenization (HEPES 10 mM, EDTA 2 mM, EGTA 2 mM, DTT 0.5 mM, protease inhibitor 1:100 dilution, phosphatase inhibitor 1:100 dilution, SDS 0.5% and double distilled water). Hundred microliter of this solution was used to homogenize tissue samples. Protein concentration was estimated using Bradford assay.

Ten microgram of protein was loaded on a 10% SDS-polyacrylamide gel for electrophoresis (SDS-PAGE). The proteins from the gel were transferred to nitrocellulose membrane using semi-dry transfer technique. The proteins on the membrane were visualized using ponceau staining. The membrane was cut according to the molecular weight marker for the protein of interest. The nitrocellulose membrane was washed with double distilled water and 1× TBST to remove ponceau staining. The membrane was later blocked with 5% milk in 1× TBST for 2.5 h. After blocking, the membrane was washed three time with 1× TBST. The membranes were incubated overnight at 4°C with rabbit polyclonal to GABAA α1 1: 5,000 (Abcam—ab33299), rabbit polyclonal to GABAA α2 1: 1,000 (Abcam—ab72445), rabbit polyclonal to GluN1 1:1,000 (Abcam—ab521717), rabbit polyclonal GluN2A 1:1,000 (Abcam—ab169873) or rabbit polyclonal GAPDH 1:10,000 (Abcam—ab9485). The next day, membranes were washed thrice with 1× TBST and incubated for 1 h with Goat α-rabbit secondary antibody 1: 10,000 (Abcam—ab6721). The membranes were washed thrice and developed using ECL plus substrate for chemiluminiscence. Since the molecular weight of α1 and α2 of GABAA receptor are the same, two separate gels were run for each sample. The optical density of the signals was measured using Quantity1 analysis software. The optical density for each band was first normalized to background intensity and then with its respective GAPDH signal. The optical density was then normalized to mean density of the control group for each target protein and region.

Experiment 2

Experimental paradigm 2

Animals were subjected to the same protocol of JVS at PND 27 and adulthood UWT at PND 60. Two weeks post UWT, animals were categorized into trauma affected or unaffected based on their behavior in WAZM, OFT, and SRT. Trauma affected animals were administered fluoxetine treatment for 30 days in drinking water. At the end of treatment animals were subjected to WAZM, OFT, SRT, and EPM. The animals were behaviorally profiled based on their behavior in WAZM, EPM, and SRT. At the end of the behavioral battery the animals were taken for in-vivo electrophysiological study to investigate local circuit activity and long term potentiation in the dorsal dentate gyrus (dDG) (Figure 2).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Schematic representation of experimental paradigm 2.

Individual behavioral profiling 2

In order to classify animals as trauma affected or unaffected post UWT, behavioral parameters were selected from WAZM, SRT, OFT. The behavioral parameters of WAZM, SRT, EPM were used to classify animals as treatment responders or non-responders. The rationale for replacing OFT with EPM at the post treatment time point was the observation that control animals did not remain curious enough to explore the same arena on their second exposure to OFT. We replaced the two parameters of OFT that measured explorative activity and anxiety-like behavior, i.e., (i) total distance traveled and (ii) time spent in OFT center at the post trauma time point with two similar readouts from EPM at the post-treatment time point namely (i) total distance traveled in EPM and (ii) Anxiety index EPM open arm time. Similar to IBP1, behavioral cut-off values based on one standard deviation from the performance of the control animals were determined for each parameter and for each time point. Animals affected in at least 4 out of 7 parameters were considered trauma affected post trauma and those who showed recovery in at least 4 parameters after fluoxetine treatment were considered treatment responders.

The following parameters were used post-trauma to classify the animals as trauma affected and unaffected.

   i) Distance traveled in closed arms of WAZM

  ii) Time spent in open arms of WAZM

 iii) Distance traveled in open arms of WAZM

 iv) Total freezing in WAZM

  v) Preference index of SRT

 vi) Total distance traveled in OFT

vii) Time spent in OFT center

In addition to the first five parameters mentioned above the following two parameters of EPM were used at the post-treatment time point.

   i) Total distance traveled in EPM

  ii) Anxiety index in EPM closed arm time

Experimental groups

This experimental set of animals was divided into two groups: controls (n = 16) and Trauma exposed (n = 72). IBP 2 was used to classify animals as trauma-affected or trauma-unaffected. Fluoxetine was administered to trauma-affected rats (n = 35) for a duration of 30 days. IBP 2 was used to classify animals as treatment responders or non-responders post-fluoxetine treatment. These animals were later subjected to electrophysiological tests to measure local circuit activity and long-term potentiation in dDG. For measuring local circuit activity, the following number of animals were analyzed: controls (n = 14), responders (n = 12), and non-responders (n = 18). Animals which either died during the protocol, were outliers, or did not have stable baseline activity were excluded from the analysis. For the LTP measurement the data of controls (n = 11), responders (n = 8), and non-responders (n = 13). Animals were excluded from analysis if baseline was found unstable.

Electrophysiology

Animals were anesthetized (40% urethane, 5% chloral, hydrate in saline, max. 0.5 ml/100 g i.p.) and placed in a stereotaxic apparatus. Body temperature (maintained at 37 ± 0.5°C with Homeothermic Blanket System, Harvard apparatus, Dover, MA, USA) and level of anesthesia were monitored and adjusted if necessary during the whole experiment. The scalp was opened and of small bur holes (1 mm diameter) were drilled. A glass recording electrode (tip diameter 2–5 μm, filled with a 2 M NaCl solution) was inserted into the dorsal DG (coordinates: –4 mm AP, 2.5 mm ML from Bregma and appr. –3.7 mm DV from brain surface) and a bipolar 125 μm stimulating electrode was placed in the perforant path (PP; coordinates: 8.0 mm AP and 4 mm ML from bregma, –3 mm DV from brain surface. The final depth of the electrodes was adjusted to yield maximal excitatory postsynaptic potentials (EPSP), evoked by a single pulse delivered to the ipsilateral PP (0.1 ms duration; rectangular monophasic). The characteristic features of the depth profile of the DG response thereby served as an indicator for correct electrode location. Evoked field potentials were amplified (×1,000), bandpass filtered at 0.1–1,000 Hz (AM-Systems amplifier), digitized at 10 kHz (CED, Cambridge, UK) and stored to disk for off-line analysis using Spike-2 software (version 4.24, CED, Cambridge, UK), where the field EPSP slope and the population spike (PS) amplitude were measured.

After electrode insertion, recordings were allowed to stabilize for 20 min before recording of an input-output curve commenced (stimulation intensities from 0.4 to 3.2 mA, 0.2 ms duration) to determine the baseline stimulation intensity (40% of maximal field potential response evoked). After 30 min of baseline recording (at 0.1 Hz), different protocols were applied to determine local circuit activity:

a) Frequency-dependent inhibition (FDI): 10 pulses at 0.1 Hz followed by 10 pulses at 1 Hz. The average of EPSP and PS slope of 10 pulses at 0.1 Hz was compared to the average of 10 pulses given at 1Hz (Maroun and Richter-Levin, 2002).

b) Paired-pulse inhibition (PPI): The PPI protocol was used to measure feed-back inhibition or facilitations by giving 5 sets of two pulses with an inter-stimulus interval (ISI) of 15, 30, and 80 ms. Five minutes of baseline stimulation was given before each protocol. The responses to the 2nd pulse were averaged and compared to the average responses to the baseline pulses.

For each protocol, the inhibition strength was expressed as percentage of the baseline response, for the population spike amplitude and the EPSP slope, respectively.

For measuring LTP, baseline recordings of EPSP were assessed during 20 min, at frequency of 0.1 Hz. LTP was induced by theta burst stimulation (TBS) (Ardi et al., 2014): three sets of 10 trains, each with 10 pulses (100 Hz), were administrated with 200 ms inter-train interval, and 1 min inter-set interval. LTP was recorded for 60 min after TBS.

Statistical analysis

Data is presented as mean ± SEM. The normality of each data set was analyzed using Shapiro–Wilk test. If the data was found to be normally distributed, One-way ANOVA was employed. Bonferroni’s post-hoc comparison was used if the p value was found to be significant for ANOVA. In case the data was found to be distributed not normally, Kruskal–Wallis test was employed. Dunn’s post-hoc comparison was used if the p value was found to be significant. Distribution of trauma affected and unaffected animals post UWT was compared with that of controls using Fisher exact test. Distribution of affected unaffected and responder and non-responders between experiment 1 and 2 were compared using Fisher exact test.

Results

Result experiment 1

Individual behavioral profiling-1 effectively differentiated trauma-affected animals from trauma-unaffected animals

Individual behavioral profiling-1 revealed that not every animal that was exposed to JVS+UWT was trauma affected. Nevertheless, the majority of trauma-exposed animals developed PTSD-like symptoms (∼65.6%), while around (34.6%) of the animals remained unaffected (Figure 3B). Proportion of trauma affected individuals was found to be significantly higher in the trauma-exposed group than the controls group (Fisher exact test, p < 0.0001). Only 12.5 % animals of the control group were found to be affected (Figure 3A).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. IBP1 classified animals into affected or unaffected in different groups. (A) Percentage distribution of affected animals and unaffected animals in control group. (B) Percentage of affected animals was found to be significantly higher in trauma-exposed animals than controls at 2 weeks post UWT [(Fisher exact test, p*** < 0.001); Control (n = 16), trauma-exposed (n = 93)]. (C) Total distance traveled by trauma affected animal was less than controls and unaffected animals [Kruskal–Wallis test (H (3) = 67.36, p < 0.0001); Dunn’s post-hoc comparison, p*** < 0.001]. (D) Time spent in the closed arms of WAZM was more by the trauma affected animals than controls and trauma unaffected animals [Kruskal–wallis test (H (3) = 34.53, p < 0.0001); Dunn’s post-hoc comparison, p*** < 0.001]. (E) Distance traveled in the closed arms of WAZM was least in the trauma affected animals [Ordinary-One-Way ANOVA [F(2,106) = 27.31, p < 0.0001]; Bonferroni post-hoc comparison p*** < 0.0001]. (F) Distance traveled in the open arms of WAZM was least in the trauma affected animals [Kruskal–Wallis test (H (3) = 61.24, p < 0.0001); Dunn’s post-hoc multiple comparison p*** < 0.0001]. (G) Total freezing in WAZM was not different between controls, trauma affected and unaffected animals [Ordinary one-way ANOVA (F(2,106) = 2.686, p = ns)]. (H) Total time spent freezing in the EPM was higher in the trauma affected than controls and trauma unaffected [Ordinary one-way ANOVA (F(2,106) = 35.13, p < 0.0001); Bonferroni’s multiple comparison p*** < 0.0001]. (I) Anxiety index in EPM was highest in the trauma affected [Kruskal–Wallis test (H (3) = 19.06, p < 0.0001); Dunn’s multiple comparison p* < 0.05, p***0.001]. In all the behavioral parameters of WAZM and EPM no significant difference was observed between the controls and trauma unaffected individuals. All values are represented as mean ± SEM. Controls (n = 16), trauma affected (n = 64), trauma unaffected (n = 29).

The average group effect on behavior parameters indicated that the trauma-affected animals were severely affected in most of the behavioral measures. (i) Total distance traveled in WAZM revealed significant main effect of the groups [H (3) = 67.36, p < 0.0001] as measured by Kruskal–Wallis test. Dunn’s post-hoc comparison further revealed that the trauma-affected animals traveled significantly less distance than both the control (p < 0.0001) and unaffected animals (p < 0.0001) (Figure 3C). (ii) Time spent in the closed arms of the WAZM was compared across the three groups and Kruskal–Wallis test showed significant difference between groups [H (3) = 34.53, p < 0.0001]. Dunn’s post-hoc multiple comparison verified that the trauma affected animals spent more time in the anxiogenic closed arms of WAZM than control (p < 0.0001) and unaffected (p < 0.0001) animals (Figure 3D). (iii) Distance traveled in closed arms of the WAZM was found to be significantly different across the groups as shown by Ordinary-One-Way ANOVA [F(2,106) = 27.31, p < 0.0001]. Bonferroni post-hoc comparison showed that the affected animals traveled less in the WAZM closed arms than both control (p < 0.0001) and unaffected (p < 0.0001) animals (Figure 3E). Significant difference between the three groups was found in the iv) distance traveled in open arms by the Kruskal–Wallis test [H (3) = 61.24, p < 0.0001]. Dunn’s post-hoc multiple comparison further revealed that distance traveled in WAZM open arms by affected group was less than both control (p < 0.0001) and unaffected animals (p < 0.0001) (Figure 3F). No main effect of group was found by ordinary one-way ANOVA in the (v) total time spent freezing in WAZM across the 3 groups (Figure 3G). In the EPM test, significant main effect of group was found in (vi) total time spent freezing in EPM using ordinary one-way-ANOVA test across the three groups [F(2,106) = 35.13, p < 0.0001] Bonferroni’s multiple comparison test showed increased freezing of affected animals than control (p < 0.0001) and unaffected (p < 0.0001) animals (Figure 3H). Kruskal–Wallis test showed significant difference between groups in (vii) anxiety index in closed arms of EPM [H (3) = 19.06, p < 0.0001]. Dunn’s post-hoc test revealed affected animals had an increased anxiety index as compared to control (p < 0.05) and unaffected animals (p < 0.001) (Figure 3I). In all the parameters no significant difference was found in the behavior between the control and unaffected animals. This showed that the unaffected animals were behaviorally similar to control animals. The observation that the trauma-affected animals had worse average group effects on behavioral parameters than controls and unaffected animals reinforced the fact that IBP1 effectively categorized affected and unaffected animals post-UWT.

Individual behavioral profiling-1 efficiently identified treatment-responders from treatment non-responders after fluoxetine treatment

Individual behavioral profiling-1 successfully identified animals that responded to the fluoxetine treatment. Around 49% of the trauma affected animals that received fluoxetine treatment showed recovery in ≥4 out of 7 behavioral parameters (Figure 4B). Fifty-one of the trauma-affected animals did not show recovery and were classified as non-responder (Figure 4B). This finding corroborated human statistical data indicating that only a subset of PTSD patients respond to therapy. IBP-1 also profiled the control animals as affected or unaffected with the percentage of unaffected animals much higher than affected animals (Figure 4A).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. IBP1 classified JVS+UWT affected animals as treatment responders and non-responders. (A) Percentage of affected and unaffected animals in the control group at the post-fluoxetine time point. (B) Percentage distribution of treatment responders and non-responders in the trauma affected group after fluoxetine treatment. Controls (n = 16), trauma affected animals administered fluoxetine (n = 53). (C) Total distance traveled in WAZM by the non-responders was the least while responders and control traveled similar distance [Kruskal–Wallis test (H (3) = 41.51, p < 0.0001); Dunn’s post-hoc comparison p*** < 0.0001]. (D) Time spent in the WAZM closed arms was the highest by the non-responders but responders and controls spent similar time [Kruskal–Wallis test (H (3) = 20.62, p < 0.0001); Dunn’s post-hoc comparison p*** < 0.0001, p* < 0.05)]. (E) Distance traveled in the closed arms was the least by non-responders but responders and controls traversed similar distance [Kruskal–Wallis test (H (3) = 28.11, p < 0.0001); Dunn’s post-hoc comparison p** < 0.01, p*** < 0.0001]. (F) Distance traveled in the open arm of the WAZM was least by the non-responders while responders and controls covered similar distances [Kruskal–Wallis test (H (3) = 27.21, p < 0.0001); Dunn’s post-hoc comparison p*** < 0.00001]. (G) Total time spent freezing in the WAZM was highest amongst the non-responders but responders and controls spent similar time freezing [Kruskal–Wallis test (H (3) = 34.13, p < 0.0001); Dunn’s post-hoc comparison p*** < 0.00001]. (H) Time spent freezing in the EPM was not statistically different between controls, responders and non-responders [Kruskal–Wallis test (H (3) = 3.573, p = ns]. (I) Anxiety index in the EPM was highest in the non-responders while responders and controls had similar anxiety index [Kruskal–Wallis test (H (3) = 11.85, p < 0.01); Dunn’s post-hoc comparison p* < 0.05]. All data represented as mean ± SEM. Controls (n = 16), treatment responders (n = 26), treatment non-responders (n = 27).

The behavior profiling was further supported by the differences observed in the group average effect of responders and non-responders on behavior. The average group effect of the treatment responders on most behavioral parameters was either found to be similar to healthy control animals or better than the non-responders. Kruskal–Wallis test showed significant difference between the three groups in the (i) total distance traveled in WAZM [H (3) = 41.51, p < 0.0001]. Dunn’s post-hoc comparison revealed that the non-responders traveled significantly less distance than both controls (p < 0.0001) and responders (p < 0.00001). The responders and controls traversed similar distance in the WAZM (Figure 4C). Similarly, significant main effect of groups was found in (ii) time spent in closed arms as compared by Kruskal–Wallis test [H (3) = 20.62, p < 0.0001]. Dunn’s post-hoc comparison revealed that non-responders spent significantly more time in the closed arms of the WAZM than controls (p < 0.00001) and responders (p = 0.0151). No significant difference was found in the time spent in closed arms between the controls and responders (Figure 4D). The average group effect was measured for (iii) distance traveled in closed arms of WAZM using Kruskal–Wallis test and was found to be significantly different between the groups [H(3) = 28.11, p < 0.00001]. Dunn’s post-hoc comparison showed that non-responders traversed significantly less distance in the closed arms of the WAZM than controls (p = 0.0019) and responders (p < 0.0001). The controls and responders traversed similar distance in the closed arms and no significant difference was found between them (Figure 4E). Significant group differences were also found in the iv) distance traveled in the open arm of the WAZM as analyzed by Kruskal–Wallis test [H (3) = 27.21, p < 0.0001]. Dunn’s post-hoc comparison revealed that the non-responders traveled significantly less distance in the open arm as compared to controls (p < 0.00001) and responders (p < 0.00001). No significant difference was found between the controls and responders in the distance traveled in the open arms (Figure 4F). Similar trend was observed in the (v) total time spent freezing in the WAZM by the three groups. Significant main effect of groups was found in Kruskal–Wallis test [H (3) = 34.13, p < 0.0001) and Dunn’s post-hoc comparison showed that the controls are behaviorally same as responders but the non-responders spent significantly more time freezing than the controls (p < 0.00001) and responders (p < 0.00001) (Figure 4G). No significant main effect of groups was found in the (vi) total time spent freezing in the EPM between the groups (Figure 4H). However, significant differences was found between the groups in the (vii) anxiety index time in EPM as measured by Kruskal–Wallis test [H (3) = 11.85, p < 0.01]. Dunn’s post-hoc comparison further revealed that the non-responders had significantly higher anxiety index than the controls (p = 0.0117) and responders (p = 0.0102). No significant difference was found in the anxiety index between the controls and responders (Figure 4I). The results clearly showed that behaviorally, treatment-responders were similar to controls, while non-responders continued to perform worse in all behavioral parameters than both controls and responders.

Biochemical proof of concept of individual behavioral profiling-1: Changes in GABAAα2, GluN1, and GluN2A expression in the ventral hippocampus after treatment

vDG

No significant main effect of groups was found on the expression of GABAAα1 in vDG (Figures 5A,C,D). The receptor subunit expression of GABAAα2, GluN1 and Glun2A was found to be significantly altered between controls, responders and non-responders (Figures 5B,D–F). Expression patterns of GABAAα2 revealed significant differences between the groups [F(2, 12) = 15.15, p = 0.0005]. Bonferroni’s post-hoc test showed that the responders had higher expression levels of α2 than both controls (p = 0.0006) and non-responders (p = 0.0087). No significant difference was found between controls and non-responders (Figure 5E). This demonstrated that, in contrast to controls and non-responders, the responders regulated the GABAAα2 expression differently that likely evoked a unique adaptive response in them. Ordinary-one way ANOVA revealed significant differences in the expression pattern of GluN1 between the groups [F(2, 12) = 8.486, p = 0.005]. Bonnferoni’s post-hoc test revealed that the responders had similar expression pattern as control animals. The non-responders had lower expression of GluN1 than controls (p = 0.0264) and responders (p = 0.005) (Figure 5C). The result indicated that the responders regulated their GluN1 levels as controls, which the non-responders failed to do and hence were unable to elicit response to fluoxetine. Statistically significant difference in the expression patterns of GluN2A was found between groups by ordinary-one way-ANOVA [F(2, 12) = 7.679, p = 0.0071] (Figures 5F–H). Bonferroni’s post-hoc test revealed that responders had significantly higher levels of GluN2A than controls (p = 0.0061) indicating a unique adaptive response. Though the average expression level of GluN2A of non-responders was lesser than that of responders, no significant statistical difference was found between the non-responders and responders (p = 0.3390) or controls (p = 0.2485).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Experssion profile of GABAAα1, GABAAα2, GluN1, and GluN2A in the ventral hippocampus between controls, treatment responders and non-responders. (A) Gel images of GABAAα1, GluN1, and GAPDH for controls, treatment responders and non-responders in the vDG. (B) Gel images of GABAAα2, GluN2A, and GAPDH for controls, treatment responders and non-responders in the vDG. (C) GABAAα1 expression level was similar between the theree groups in vDG [Ordinary one-way ANOVA (F(2,12) = 1.717, p = ns)]. (D) GluN1 levels was the least in the non-responders while controls and responders had almost similar level of expression in vDG [Ordinary one-way ANOVA (F(2,12) = 8.486, p = 0.005); Bonferroni’s post-hoc test p* < 0.05, p** < 0.01]. (E) GABAAα2 levels were highest in the responders while controls and responders showed similar level of expression in vDG [Ordinary one-way ANOVA (F(2,12) = 15.15, p = 0.0005); Bonferroni’s post-hoc test p** < 0.01, p*** < 0.0001]. (F) GluN2A expression in the responders was significantly higher than controls but no significant difference was found with respect to non-responders in vDG [Ordinary one-way ANOVA (F(2,12) = 7.679, p = 0.0071); Bonferroni’s post-hoc test p** < 0.01]. (G) Gel images of GABAAα1, GluN1 and GAPDH for controls, treatment respnders and non-responders in the vCA1. (H) Gel images of GABAAα2, GluN2A, and GAPDH for controls, treatment respnders and non-responders in the vCA1. (I) GABAAα1 expression level was similar between the theree groups in vCA1 [Ordinary one-way ANOVA (F(2,12) = 0.4875, p = ns)]. (J) GluN1 expression levels of non-responders was significantly less than the controls in vCA1 [Ordinary one-way ANOVA (F(2,12) = 5.054, p = 0.0256); Bonferroni’s post-hoc test p* < 0.05]. The levels of GluN1 between responders and non-responders though not statistically significant showed a strong trend (p = 0.093). (K) GABAAα2 expression level was similar between the theree groups in vCA1 [Ordinary one-way ANOVA (F(2,12) = 2.410, p = ns)]. (L) GluN2A expression levels of non-responders was significantly less than the controls in vCA1 [Ordinary one-way ANOVA (F(2,12) = 4.150, p = 0.0427); Bonferroni’s post-hoc test p* < 0.05]. The levels of GluN1 between responders and controls were similar and no statistically significant difference was found between responders and non-responders. (M) Gel images of GABAAα1, GluN1, and GAPDH for controls, treatment respnders and non-responders in the vCA3. (N) Gel images of GABAAα2, GluN2A and GAPDH for controls, treatment respnders and non-responders in the vCA3. (O) GABAAα1 expression level was similar between the theree groups in vCA3 [Ordinary one-way ANOVA (F(2,12) = 1.015, p = ns)]. (P) GluN1 expression levels of non-responders was significantly less than the controls in vCA3 [Ordinary one-way ANOVA (F(2,12) = 5.054, p = 0.0256); Bonferroni’s post-hoc test p* < 0.05]. The levels of GluN1 between responders and non-responders though not statistically significant showed a strong trend (p = 0.093). (Q) GABAAα2 levels was the least in the non-respnders while controls and responders had almost similar level of expression in vCA3 [Ordinary one-way ANOVA (F(2,12) = 5.771, p = 0.0175); Bonferroni’s post-hoc test p* < 0.05]. (R) GluN2A expression levels of non-responders was significantly less than the controls in vCA3 [Ordinary one-way ANOVA (F(2,12) = 4.150, p = 0.0427); Bonferroni’s post-hoc test p* < 0.05]. The levels of GluN1 between responders and controls were similar and no statistically significant difference was found between responders and non-responders. All data represented as mean ± SEM. Controls (n = 5), treatment responders (n = 6), treatment non-responders (n = 4).

vCA1

In vCA1 expression pattern of GABAAα1(Figure 5I) and GABAAα2 (Figure 5K) was not found to be significantly different between the three groups as analyzed by ordinary-one-way-ANOVA. However, expression levels GluN1 [F(2,12) = 5.054, p = 0.0256] (Figure 5J) and GluN2A [F(2,12) = 4.150, p = 0.0427] (Figure 5L) were found to be significantly different across groups when compared by ordinary-one way-ANOVA. Bonferroni’s post-hoc test revealed that the non-responders had significantly lower expression of GluN1 than controls (p = 0.0291) and also exhibited a similarly strong trend of lower expression when compared to responders (p = 0.093) (Figure 5J). The expression profile of GluN1 was similar between responders and controls (Figure 5J). Expression of GluN2A was significantly lower in non-responders than controls (p = 0.0428). There was no significant difference in the expression profile of responders compared to controls or non-responders (Figure 5L). Together the results suggested that the anxiety-like behavior in the non-responders may partially be due to low expression levels of GluN1 and GluN2A which they failed to normalize after fluoxetine treatment.

vCA3

GABAAα1 expression levels was not found to be significantly altered between the groups as revealed by Kruskal–Wallis test (Figures 5M–O). However, expression of GABAAα2 was significantly altered between groups [F(2, 12) = 5.771, p = 0.0175]. Bonferroni’s post-hoc comparison showed that the non-responders had significantly lower expression levels than controls (p = 0.0293) and responders (p = 0.0355). Expression level between controls and responders was found to be similar (Figure 5Q). This indicated that the responders had a different expression pattern of α2 from the non-responders which highlights a biochemical difference between the two groups. Further, inability of non-responders to rescue the expression of GABAAα2 to the levels similar to controls and responders might had contributed to the failure to respond to fluoxetine. GluN1 expression levels was found to be significantly altered between the groups as measured by ordinary-one way-ANOVA [F(2, 12) = 5.054, p = 0.0256]. Bonferroni post-hoc test revealed that the non-responders had significantly lower expression levels than controls (p = 0.0291). A similar trend was also observed between non-responders and responders (p = 0.0930) (Figure 5P). Expression levels of GluN2A was found to be significantly different between the groups by ordinary-one way-ANOVA [F(2, 12) = 4.150, p = 0.0427]. Bonferroni’s post-hoc comparison showed non-responders had significantly lower GluN2A expression than controls (p = 0.0428). There was no significant difference in the expression profile of responders compared to controls or non-responders (Figure 5R). Decreased expression of GluN1 and GluN2A in the non-responders might be contributing factor to its inability to respond to treatment. The average expression of GluN1 and GluN2 responders though statistically insignificant was approximately higher by ∼35 % than the non-responders.

Results experiment 2

Individual behavioral profiling-2 effectively distinguished trauma-affected from unaffected individuals, as verified by average behavioral performance of affected individuals

Individual behavioral profiling-2 revealed that 57% of trauma-exposed animals were affected (Figure 6B), compared to 12.5% of controls (Figure 6A). According to Fisher exact test, the percentage of affected animals in the JVS+UWT group was significantly greater than the controls (p = 0.0017). The distribution of affected and unaffected individuals obtained using IBP-2 was compared to that obtained from IBP-2 using Fisher exact test but no statistically significant difference was identified (p = 0.1423).

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Individual behavioral profiling-2 (IBP2) classified animals into affected or unaffected in different groups. (A) Percentage distribution of affected animals and unaffected animals in control group. (B) Percentage of affected animals was found to be significantly higher in trauma-exposed animals than controls at 2 weeks post UWT [(Fisher exact test, p*** < 0.001); Control (n = 16), trauma-exposed (n = 72)]. (C) Distance traveled in the closed arms of WAZM was least in the trauma affected animals [Kruskal–Wallis test H (3) = 18.25, (p = 0.0001); Dunn’s post-hoc test p** < 0.01, p*** < 0.0001]. (D) Time spent in the open arms of WAZM was less by the trauma affected animals than controls and trauma unaffected animals [Kruskal–Wallis test H (3) = 27.03, (p < 0.0001); Dunn’s post-hoc test p** < 0.01, p*** < 0.0001]. (E) Distance traveled in the open arms of WAZM was least in the trauma affected animals [Kruskal–Wallis test H (3) = 39.43, (p < 0.0001); Dunn’s post-hoc test p*** < 0.0001]. (F) Total time spent freezing in WAZM was highest by the trauma affected animals [Ordinary one-way ANOVA (F(2,85) = 17.69, p < 0.0001); Bonferroni’s post-hoc test p*** < 0.0001]. (G) Total distance travelled in the OFT was least by the trauma affected animals [Kruskal–Wallis test H (3) = 21.22, (p < 0.0001); Dunn’s post-hoc test p** < 0.01, p*** < 0.001]. (H) Time spent in the anxiogenic canter of the OFT was least by the trauma affected animals [Kruskal–Wallis test H (3) = 33.98, (p < 0.0001); Dunn’s post-hoc test p*** < 0.001]. (I) Preference index for unfamiliar animal in the SRT was not significantly different between the three groups. The controls and trauma unaffected individuals had no statistically different performance in any of the behavioral parameters [Ordinary-one way-ANOVA (F(2, 85) = 2.210, p = ns)]. All values are mean ± SEM. Controls (n = 16), trauma affected (n = 41), trauma unaffected (n = 31).

The average group effect on the 7 behavioral parameters was compared between the trauma affected, unaffected and control animals. Statistically significant difference was found in the (i) distance traveled in the closed arms of WAZM between the groups [H(3) = 18.25, p = 0.0001], as measured by Kruskal–Wallis test. Dunn’s post-hoc multiple comparison showed that the affected individual traveled significantly less distance in closed arms than controls (p = 0.0039) and unaffected animals (p = 0.0006). The controls and unaffected animals traveled similar difference in the closed arms of WAZM and no significant difference was found between the two groups (p > 0.0999) (Figure 6C). (ii) Time spent in the open arms of the WAZM was compared using Kruskal–Wallis test and was found to be significantly different across groups [H(3) = 27.03, p < 0.0001]. Dunn’s post-hoc multiple comparison showed that the affected animals spent significantly less time in the anxiogenic open arms than the controls (p = 0.0011) and unaffected animals (p < 0.00001). The controls and unaffected animals spent similar time in the open arms of the WAZM with no significant statistical difference found between them (p > 0.0999) (Figure 6D). (iii) Distance traveled in the open arms of the WAZM was found to be significantly different between groups when measured using Kruskal–Wallis test [H(3) = 39.43, p < 0.0001). Dunn’s post-hoc test showed that the affected animals traveled less distance in the anxiogenic open arms than controls (p = 0.0001) and unaffected animals (p < 0.00001). No significant difference was found between the controls and unaffected animals (p > 0.9999) and both the groups appeared to travel similar distance in the open arms of WAZM (Figure 6E). Statistically significant difference was found between groups in the (iv) total time spent freezing in the WAZM using ordinary-one way-ANOVA and was found to be significantly different between groups [F(2,82) = 17.69, p < 0.0001]. Bonferroni’s post-hoc test revealed that the affected individual spent significantly more time freezing in the WAZM than controls (p < 0.00001) and unaffected animals (p < 0.00001). No significant difference was found between the controls and unaffected individual in terms of total time spent freezing in the WAZM (p > 0.9999) (Figure 6F). (v) Total distance covered in the OFT was compared between the three groups using Kruskal–Wallis test and was found to be significantly different [H(3) = 21.22, p < 0.00001]. Dunn’s post-hoc comparison test showed that the affected individuals traveled significantly less distance in the OFT as compared to controls (p = 0.0002) and unaffected animals (p = 0.0018). There was no significant difference in the total distance traveled in OFT between the controls and the unaffected (p = 0.5335) (Figure 6G). (vi) Time spent in the center of OFT was compared between groups and was found to be significantly different using Kruskal–Wallis test [H(3) = 33.98, p < 0.00001]. Dunn’s post-hoc comparison revealed that the affected individuals spent significantly less time in the anxiogenic center of the OFT than controls [p = 0.0003] and unaffected individuals [p < 0.00001]. No statically significant difference was found between the controls and unaffected (p > 0.9999). No significant difference was found between the groups in the (vii) preference index to novel animal in SRT using ordinary-one way-ANOVA [F(2, 82) = 2.210, p = 0.1160] (Figures 6H,I).

Together, the results indicated that the behaviorally profiled affected animals displayed increased anxiety-like behavior. The animals which were profiled as unaffected by IBP-2 were behaviorally similar to the healthy controls giving further validation to the behavioral profiling approach.

Individual behavioral profiling-2 correctly identified treatment-responders from non-responders as verified by average behavioral performance of the responders

Individual behavioral profiling-2 successfully distinguished treatment responders from non-responders after fluoxetine therapy. Thirty-seven percent of the affected population was found to be treatment responders and 63% did not respond to the treatment (Figure 7B). The control population was also screened for any affected individuals, however the percentage of affectedness remained low at 18.5% (Figure 7A). No statistically significant difference was found in the distribution of responders and non-responders between experiment 1 and experiment 2 as compared by Fisher’s test (p = 0.2842).

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Individual behavioral profiling 2 (IBP2) classified JVS+UWT affected animals as treatment responders and non-responders. (A) Percentage of affected and unaffected animals in the control group at the post-fluoxetine time point. (B) Percentage distribution of treatment responders and non-responders in the JVS+UWT affected group after fluoxetine treatment. (C) Distance travelled in the closed arms of the WAZM was less by the non-responders than the controls. [Kruskal–Wallis test H (3) = 12.42, (p = 0.002); Dunn’s post-hoc test p** < 0.01]. No statistical significance was found between responders and controls or non-responders. (D) Time spent in the open arms of WAZM was less by the non-responders than both controls and responders [Kruskal–Wallis test H (3) = 22.99, (p < 0.0001); Dunn’s post-hoc test p** < 0.01, p*** < 0.0001]. Responders had no significant difference with controls. (E) Distance travelled in the open arm of the WAZM was least by the non-responders while responders and controls covered similar distances [Kruskal–Wallis test H (3) = 29.21, (p < 0.0001); Dunn’s post-hoc test p* < 0.05, p*** < 0.0001]. (F) Total time spent freezing in the WAZM was higher in the non-responders than controls. [Ordinary one-way ANOVA (F(2,48) = 10.52, p = 0.0002); Bonferroni’s post-hoc test p*** < 0.0001]. No significant difference was observed in the time spent freezing in WAZM between responders and controls or non-responders. (G) Total distance travelled in the EPM was least in the non-responders while responders and controls spent similar distance [Ordinary one-way ANOVA (F(2,48) = 17.23, p < 0.00001); Bonferroni’s post-hoc test p*** < 0.0001]. (H) Anxiety index in the EPM in the non-responders was higher than the controls [Kruskal–Wallis test H (3) = 10.85, (p = 0.0044); Dunn’s post-hoc test p** < 0.01]. No significant difference was observed between responders and controls or non-responders. (I) Preference index for unfamiliar animal in the SRT was not significantly different between the three groups controls [Kruskal–Wallis test (H (3) = 1.979, p = ns)]. All data represented as mean ± SEM. Controls (n = 16) treatment responders (n = 13), treatment non-responders (n = 22).

The average group effect of the responders, non-responders and controls was measured for the 7 behavioral parameters chosen. (i) Distance travelled in the closed arms of WAZM was compared across groups using Kruskal–Wallis test and was found to be significantly different [H (3) = 12.42, p = 0.0020]. Dunn’s post-hoc comparison showed that the non-responders traveled significantly less than controls in the closed arms (p = 0.0013). The mean rank difference between responders and controls was 9.670, and between responders and non-responders was 7.524, although neither difference was statistically significant (p = 0.23367, p = 0.3967 respectively) (Figure 7C). (ii) Time spent in the open arms of the WAZM was found to be significantly different across groups using Kruskal–Wallis test [H (3) = 22.99, p < 0.00001). Dunn’s post-hoc test revealed no significant difference in the time spent in the anxiogenic open arms between the responders and controls (p = 0.8052). However, the non-responders spent significantly less time in the anxiogenic open arms of the WAZM than controls (p < 0.00001) and responders (p = 0.0036) (Figure 7D). iii) Distance travelled in the open arms of the WAZM was found to be significantly different between the groups as seen by Kruskal–Wallis test [H (3) = 29.21, p < 0.00001]. Further post-hoc comparison using Dunn’s test revealed that the non-responders travelled significantly less in the open arms than the controls (p < 0.00001) and the responders (p = 0.0141). No statistically significant difference was found between responders and controls (p = 0.0788) (Figure 7E). iv) Total time spent freezing in the WAZM was compared between the groups using ordinary-one way ANOVA and was found to be significantly different [F(2, 48) = 10.52, p = 0.0002]. Bonferroni’s post-hoc test showed that the non-responders spent significantly more time freezing in the WAZM than the controls (p = 0.0001) (Figure 7F). (v) Total distance traveled in EPM was found to be significantly different between the groups as compared by ordinary-one way- ANOVA [F(2, 48) = 17.23, p < 0.00001]. Bonferroni’s post-hoc test revealed that non-responders traveled significantly less distance in the EPM than controls (p < 0.00001) and responders (p = 0.0001). No statistically significant difference was observed between the responders and the controls (Figure 7G). (vi) Anxiety index in the closed arms of EPM was observed to be significantly different between the groups as measured by sung Kruskal–Wallis test [H(3) = 10.85, p = 0.0044]. Dunn’s post-hoc comparison showed that the non-responders exhibited significantly higher anxiety index than the controls (p = 0.0042). No significant difference was observed between responders and controls (p = 0.9197) or non-responders (p = 0.1622) (Figure 7H). No statistical difference was observed between the responders and controls (p = 0.0937) or non-responders (p = 0.1390). Kruskal–Wallis test revealed no significant difference between groups in the vii) preference index for novel animal in SRT (Figure 7I). The findings revealed that responders recovered in most behavioral parameters and were either behaviorally equivalent to controls or better than non-responders. This demonstrated that IBP-2 was effective in detecting responders within the fluoxetine-treated group.

Electrophysiological proof of concept of individual behavioral profiling-2: Increased local circuit activity in dDG of the responders

The feed-forward inhibition in the dDG was measured using FDI protocol but the EPSP slope was not found to be significant between the group as measured by Kruskal–Wallis test [H(3) = 2.609, p = 0.2713] (Figure 8A). No statistically significant difference was found in the FDI population spike (PS) amplitude between groups as revealed by ordinary-one way-ANOVA [F(2, 41) = 0.3740] (Figure 8A). However increased feed-back inhibition was observed in the responders when the local circuit activity was measured using PPI protocol. PS amplitude after 15ms ISI of PPI protocol was significantly different between groups as seen by using Kruskal–Wallis test [H (3) = 10.27, p = 0.0059]. Dunn’s multiple comparison revealed that the responders had increased local-circuit inhibition after 15ms protocol in PPI as evinced by significantly low PS amplitude as compared to controls (p = 0.0066) and non-responders (p = 0.0373). No statistically significant difference was observed between controls and non-responders (p > 0.9999) (Figure 8B). PS amplitude induced by PPI 30 ms protocol was significantly different between groups [H (3) = 9.366, p = 0.0093]. Dunn’s multiple comparison showed that the responders has significantly reduced population spike induction than controls (p = 0.0268) and non-responders (p = 0.0161). Majority of control and non-responder animals showed facilitation in the local-circuit after 30ms ISI between pulses unlike most of the responders that exhibited feed-back inhibition. No statistically significant difference was observed between controls and non-responders (p > 0.9999). The EPSP slope induced by 15ms (Figure 8B) and 30 ms (Figure 8C) protocol of PPI were found to be non-significant between groups as measured by Kruskal–Wallis test [H(3) = 4.230, p = 0.1207] and [H(3) = 3.792, p = 0.1502], respectively. No significant difference was observed between groups in EPSP slope or PS amplitude induced by PPI 80ms protocol as measured by ordinary-one way-ANOVA [F(2,41) = 0.7357, p = 0.4857] and [F(2,41) = 0.7695, p = 0.4700], respectively (Figure 8D).

FIGURE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 8. Increased inhibition in PPI at inter-stimulus interval of 15ms and 30 ms in responders. (A, left) Representative traces of FDI for controls, responders and non-responders showing feed-forward inhibition. (Right) Bar graphs of perecenatge change in EPSP slope [Kruskal–Wallis test (H(3) = 2.609, p = 0.2713)] and PS amplitude between the groups showed no change [Ordinary-one way-ANOVA (F(2, 41) = 0.3740)]. (B, left) Representative traces of PPI at 15ms ISI for controls, responders and non-responders showing feed-back inhibition. (Right) Bar graph representing perecenatge change in EPSP slope did not show change any difference [Kruskal–Wallis test (H(3) = 4.230, p = 0.1207)]. Decreased PS amplitude in the responders than controls and non-responders indicated incrased feed-back inhibition the DG of the responders [Kruskal–Wallis test (H(3) = 10.27, p = 0.0059); Dunn’s post-hoc comparison p** < 0.01. p* < 0.05]. (C, left) Representative traces of PPI at 30 ms ISI for controls, responders and non-responders. Majority of control and non-responders showed fasciliation at 30 ms ISI while responders showed feed-back inhibition. (Right) Bar graph representing perecenatge change in EPSP slope did not show any difference between groups [Kruskal–Wallis test (H(3) = 3.792, p = ns)]. Decreased PS amplitude at 30ms ISI in the responders than controls and non-responders indicated incrased feed-back inhibition the DG of the responders [Kruskal–Wallis test (H(3) = 9.366, p = 0.0093); Dunn’s post-hoc comparison p* < 0.05]. (D, left) Representative traces of PPI at 80ms ISI for controls, responders and non-responders showing fasciltation. (Right) Bar graphs of perecenatge change in EPSP slope [Ordinary one-way ANOVA (F(2,41) = 0.7357, p = 0.4857)] and PS amplitude between the groups showed no change [Ordinary one-way ANOVA (F(2,41) = 0.7695, p = 0.4700)]. Controls (n = 14), treatment responders (n = 12) and treatment non-responders (n = 18).

No differences in EPSP or PS amplitude was discovered after TBS in LTP induction protocol as well (Supplementary Figure 2). Together, increased feed-back inhibition in the dDG local circuit was displayed only by the responders and was restricted to decreased PS amplitude in PPI 15 and PPI 30 protocol. This not only highlighted electrophysiological difference between the responders and the others but also highlighted a unique functional adaptive response of the responders to fluoxetine.

Discussion

It is well-established that only a relatively small number of PTSD patients respond well to psychopharmacological treatment. It is estimated that only about 20-30% of them show full remission (Ravindran and Stein, 2010; Steckler and Risbrough, 2012). Despite that, the accepted approach of measuring efficacy of pharmacological treatments in animal models of psychiatric disorders refers to averaged group results (Katz and Hersh, 1981; Bodnoff et al., 1989; Rudolph and Feiger, 1999; Dulawa et al., 2004). Using group averages, and obscuring individual variability in treatment response in animal models significantly compromises their translational power. To address this drawback, we aimed in the current study to adapt a “Behavioral Profiling” analysis approach, originally developed for differentiating between trauma-exposed-Affected and trauma-exposed-Unaffected individuals (Ardi et al., 2016), in order to enable differentiating between treatment-responders and treatment-non-responders individuals.

We demonstrate here, for the first time, the effectiveness of employing this approach for assessing treatment efficacy in an animal model of PTSD. IBP 1 and IBP2 properly estimated that less than 50% of the treated animals were responsive to fluoxetine. These outcomes are consistent with PTSD patients’ overall response rate to SSRIs, which is around 60%, with only 20–30% of patients attaining full remission (Berger et al., 2009).

The results also serve to further support the efficacy of the ‘Behavioral Profiling’ approach in differentiating between affected and Unaffected individuals following exposure to trauma. Post-categorisation analysis clearly revealed that the group average effects of the trauma-affected animals were worse than those of controls and trauma-unaffected animals in the majority of IBP1 and IBP-2 behavioral characteristics, which included both exploratory and anxiety-like behaviors.

In a similar way, the majority of IBP1’s behavioral parameters also showed a clear distinction between responders and non-responders. The non-responders fared worse than both controls and responders, while the responders appeared to behave similarly to the controls. The non-responders were also found to be negatively affected in all behavioral parameters of IBP2 and performed worse than controls in all parameters with the exception of preference index in SRT. A distinct difference in performance was seen between the responders and non-responders in the time spent in open arms in the open arms of WAZM, total distance covered in the open arms of the WAZM and the total distance traveled in the EPM. In the other parameters the average group effect of the responders, though not significantly different, was closer to controls than the non-responders. These results supported the classification performed also by the IBP2. Additionally, we did not observe any difference in the percentage of affected population after trauma and percentage of fluoxetine responders, as assessed by IBP-1 or IBP-2. This is an important finding since it demonstrates the power of the ‘Behavioral Profiling’ approach. The classification of animals as trauma affected or unaffected, or treatment responders or non-responders is not dependent on the exact behavioral criteria used but rather on the principle approach. This gives our individual behavioral profile method an extremely adaptable advantage, enabling it to be expanded into other behavioral paradigms to assess the effectiveness of various therapies using a variety of animal models and behavioral tests.

The functional importance of the individual behavioral profiling technique was further proven by showing that the behavioral classification of animals into responders and non-responders is also associated with pertinent alterations in the E/I balance.

Biochemical investigation of E/I balance found a distinct difference between responders and non-responders by evaluating expression levels of receptor subunits of GABAA and NMDAR. The most prevalent and significant difference between the responders and non-responders was in GABAAα2 expression levels in the vDG and vCA3. GABAAα2 has been linked to anxiolytic behavior (Pham et al., 2009; Raud et al., 2009; Wisłowska-Stanek et al., 2013) as well as facilitating anxiolytic effects of benzodiazepines and barbiturates (Low et al., 2000; Morris et al., 2006; Dixon et al., 2008). Polymorphism of GABAAα2 is also associated with childhood trauma and risk of developing PTSD in adulthood (Nelson et al., 2009). Additionally, earlier research from our group revealed elevated levels of GABAAα2 in the vCA1 of trauma-exposed but resilient animals (Ardi et al., 2016). Together, these studies indicate a neuroprotective role of GABAAα2. Increased levels of GABAAα2 in the vDG and vCA3 of the hippocampus in responders compared to non-responders may reflect a unique mechanism of functional adaptation in the responders in response to treatment that non-responders do not attain. The responders exhibited elevated levels of GABAAα2 in the vDG also compared with controls, showing that despite being behaviourally similar, the responders demonstrate active coping mechanisms, not found in controls, presumably enabling them to behaviourally respond well. No change in expression patterns of GABAAα1 was seen between the groups. GABAAα1 is the most abundant subunit of GABAA receptor ∼60% (Benke et al., 2004) and may have intact global expression. However, further investigation is required to determine whether synapse or cell-specific changes have occurred.

The expression patterns of the NMDAR subunit changed concurrently amongst the groups, as well. In comparison to responders, non-responders had lower levels of GluN1 expression in the vDG. This difference between responders and non-responders further serves to functionally validate the significance of the behavioral profiling approach. Additionally, we observed that the ventral hippocampus of the non-responders consistently had lower levels of GluN1 expression than controls. Similar downtrend of expression was seen in the vCA1 and vCA3 as compared to the responders. Downregulation of GluN1 in the ventral hippocampus has been observed after chronic stress (Pacheco et al., 2017) and in hippocampus after prenatal stress (Sun et al., 2013). Therefore, lower GluN1 expression in non-responders might indicate a maladaptive change; nevertheless, further in-depth research is required in order to establish this possibility. In addition, Glun2A expression was lower in the vCA1 and vCA3 of non-responders compared to controls. Alteration in the levels GluN2A has been reported after stress and was related to anxiety like behaviors (Calabrese et al., 2012; Goulart et al., 2021). An increase in synaptic GuN1 and GluN2A in the hippocampus is also essential for dendritic arborisation, memory consolidation (Cercato et al., 2017) and contextual fear learning (Acutain et al., 2021). The most crucial fact is that GluN1 and GluN2A both have the potential to impact the induction of plasticity (Shipton and Paulsen, 2014; Baez et al., 2018; Zhou and Duan, 2018), and changes in their levels in non-responders are indicative of potential changes in synaptic plasticity which may lead to altered memory.

Together, the different pattern of alteration in the GABAAα2, GluN1, and GluN2A in the ventral hippocampus between responders and non-responders suggests a shift in E/I balance. Increased expression of GluN2A in the responders’ ventral DG compared to the controls further supports the idea that the responders are actively using coping mechanisms which involve E/I balance alterations, in order to establish a new stable state and return to normal behavioral patterns.

Electrophysiological investigation of the local circuit activity of the dorsal dentate gyrus further revealed indications for a shift in E/I balance in the responders. Antidepressant treatments, including fluoxetine, are known to alter local circuit activity in the dDG (Reisi et al., 2014). We did not observe any changes in feed-forward inhibition, as measured by FDI but did find altered PPI (Increased inhibition of the PS amplitude at 15 and 30 ms ISI), highlighting that changes in the local circuit in the responders may be specific to feedback inhibition. Increased inhibition of the PS amplitude of the granular cells is thought to be mediated through basket cells. It is considered that the inhibition induced during these shorter ISIs is mediated by fast acting GABAA receptors, while longer ISIs result in the eventual recruitment of GABAB receptors, leading to facilitation (Tuff et al., 1983; Albertson and Joy, 1987). We observed that at the 30ms ISI controls and non-responders mainly exhibited facilitation of the PS, but responders continued to show increased inhibition, again suggesting enhanced feedback inhibition in this group. Such alterations of the local circuit and of E/I balance, which were specific to responders, indicate changes in the computation properties of the region, resulting in altered processing of information in the dDG. Specific alterations in the GABA and NMDAR receptor subunits as well as specific changes in the local circuit activity solely in the responders point to the deployment of an active coping mechanism. We did not observe any changes in LTP between groups, suggesting that mechanisms of long-term memory storage may not have been affected, but further research is required in order to verify this.

Together, we show that behavioral categorisation into responders and non-responders is an important research approach, since it differentiates between different neural outcomes following drug treatment, as was exemplified by the differential effects on E/I balance both in the ventral and dorsal hippocampus. The results call for the need to adopt a “Behavioral Profiling” analysis approach when evaluating the efficacy and the neural outcome of drug treatment in animal models of psychiatric disorders.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The animal study was reviewed and approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of the University of Haifa.

Author contributions

IS: behavior experiments and analysis, western blots and analysis, analysis of electrophysiology experiments, and manuscript writing. MS-S: part of the electrophysiological experiments and analysis. AT and AB: part of the electrophysiological experiments. GR-L: supervising experiments and manuscript writing. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This study was supported by a United States – Israel Binational Science Foundation (BSF) (#2019261) to GR-L and Jelena Radulovic, and a German Research Foundation (DFG) award (#457171758) to GR-L and HansJuergen Volkmer.

Acknowledgments

We thank HansJuergen Volkmer and Jelena Radulovic for very helpful discussions.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnins.2022.1071482/full#supplementary-material

References

Acutain, M. F., Griebler Luft, J., Vazquez, C. A., Popik, B., Cercato, M. C., Epstein, A., et al. (2021). Reduced expression of hippocampal GluN2A-NMDAR increases seizure susceptibility and causes deficits in contextual memory. Front. Neurosci. 15:644100. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2021.644100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Albertson, T. E., and Joy, R. M. (1987). Increased inhibition in dentate gyrus granule cells following exposure to GABA-uptake blockers. Brain Res. 435, 283–292. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(87)91611-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ampuero, E., Cerda, M., Härtel, S., Rubio, F. J., Massa, S., Cubillos, P., et al. (2019). Chronic fluoxetine treatment induces maturation-compatible changes in the dendritic arbor and in synaptic responses in the auditory cortex. Front. Pharmacol. 10:804. doi: 10.3389/fphar.2019.00804

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ardi, Z., Albrecht, A., Richter-Levin, A., Saha, R., and Richter-Levin, G. (2016). Behavioral profiling as a translational approach in an animal model of posttraumatic stress disorder. Neurobiol. Dis. 88, 139–147. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2016.01.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ardi, Z., Richter-Levin, A., Xu, L., Cao, X., Volkmer, H., Stork, O., et al. (2019). The role of the GABAA receptor Alpha 1 subunit in the ventral hippocampus in stress resilience. Sci. Rep. 9:13513. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-49824-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ardi, Z., Ritov, G., Lucas, M., and Richter-Levin, G. (2014). The effects of a reminder of underwater trauma on behaviour and memory-related mechanisms in the rat dentate gyrus. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 17, 571–580. doi: 10.1017/S1461145713001272

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ariel, L., Inbar, S., Edut, S., and Richter-Levin, G. (2017). Fluoxetine treatment is effective in a rat model of childhood-induced post-traumatic stress disorder. Transl. Psychiatry 7:1260. doi: 10.1038/s41398-017-0014-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Avital, A., and Richter-Levin, G. (2005). Exposure to juvenile stress exacerbates the behavioural consequences of exposure to stress in the adult rat. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 8, 163–173. doi: 10.1017/S1461145704004808

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ayhan, B. G., Aykac, A., Gur, K., Aydın, B., Secgin, E., Seven, I., et al. (2016). The role of GluN1 activated nitric oxide synthase in a rat model of post-traumatic stress disorder. Marmara Med. J. 29, 67–72. doi: 10.5472/MMJoa.2902.01

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baez, M. V., Cercato, M. C., and Jerusalinsky, D. A. (2018). NMDA receptor subunits change after synaptic plasticity induction and learning and memory acquisition. Neural Plastic. 2018:5093048. doi: 10.1155/2018/5093048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benham, R. S., Hewage, N. B., Suckow, R. F., Engin, E., and Rudolph, U. (2017). Prodepressant- and anxiogenic-like effects of serotonin-selective, but not noradrenaline-selective, antidepressant agents in mice lacking α2-containing GABAA receptors. Behav. Brain Res. 332, 172–179. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2017.05.063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benke, D., Fakitsas, P., Roggenmoser, C., Michel, C., Rudolph, U., and Mohler, H. (2004). Analysis of the presence and abundance of GABAA receptors containing two different types of α subunits in murine brain using point-mutated α subunits. J. Biol. Chem. 279, 43654–43660. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M407154200

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berger, W., Mendlowicz, M. V., Marques-Portella, C., Kinrys, G., Fontenelle, L. F., Marmar, C. R., et al. (2009). Pharmacologic alternatives to antidepressants in posttraumatic stress disorder: A systematic review. Prog. Neuro-Psychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 33, 169–180. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2008.12.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bodnoff, S. R., Suranyi-Cadotte, B., Quirion, R., and Meaney, M. J. (1989). A comparison of the effects of diazepam versus several typical and atypical anti-depressant drugs in an animal model of anxiety. Psychopharmacology 97, 277–279. doi: 10.1007/BF00442264

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bonini, D., Mora, C., Tornese, P., Sala, N., Filippini, A., La Via, L., et al. (2016). Acute footshock stress induces time-dependent modifications of AMPA/NMDA protein expression and AMPA phosphorylation. Neural Plastic. 2016:7267865. doi: 10.1155/2016/7267865

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bremner, J. D., Innis, R. B., Southwick, S. M., Staib, L., Zoghbi, S., Charney, D. S., et al. (2000). Decreased benzodiazepine receptor binding in prefrontal cortex in combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 157, 1120–1126. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.7.1120

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Calabrese, F., Guidotti, G., Molteni, R., Racagni, G., Mancini, M., and Riva, M. A. (2012). Stress-induced changes of hippocampal NMDA receptors: Modulation by duloxetine treatment. PLoS One 7:e37916. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0037916

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cercato, M. C., Vázquez, C. A., Kornisiuk, E., Aguirre, A. I., Colettis, N., Snitcofsky, M., et al. (2017). GluN1 and GluN2A NMDA receptor subunits increase in the hippocampus during memory consolidation in the rat. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 10:242. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00242

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chambers, R. A., Bremner, J. D., Moghaddam, B., Southwick, S. M., Charney, D. S., Krystal, J. H., et al. (1999). Glutamate and post-traumatic stress disorder: toward a psychobiology of dissociation. Semin. Clin. Neuropsychiatry 4, 274–278. doi: 10.153/SCNP00400274

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, H., Kaplan, Z., Matar, M. A., Loewenthal, U., Zohar, J., and Richter-Levin, G. (2007). Long-lasting behavioral effects of juvenile trauma in an animal model of PTSD associated with a failure of the autonomic nervous system to recover. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 17, 464–477. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2006.11.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Connor, K. M., Sutherland, S. M., Tupler, L. A., Malik, M. L., and Davidson, J. R. (1999). Fluoxetine in post-traumatic stress disorder. Randomised, double-blind study. Br. J. Psychiatry 175, 17–22. doi: 10.1192/bjp.175.1.17

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dixon, C. I., Rosahl, T. W., and Stephens, D. N. (2008). Targeted deletion of the GABRA2 gene encoding α2-subunits of GABAA receptors facilitates performance of a conditioned emotional response, and abolishes anxiolytic effects of benzodiazepines and barbiturates. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 90, 1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2008.01.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dulawa, S. C., Holick, K. A., Gundersen, B., and Hen, R. (2004). Effects of chronic fluoxetine in animal models of anxiety and depression. Neuropsychopharmacology 29, 1321–1330. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300433

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eagle, A. L., Fitzpatrick, C. J., and Perrine, S. A. (2013). Single prolonged stress impairs social and object novelty recognition in rats. Behav. Brain Res. 256, 591–597. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2013.09.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fang, Q., Li, Z., Huang, G. D., Zhang, H. H., Chen, Y. Y., Zhang, L. B., et al. (2018). Traumatic stress produces distinct activations of GABAergic and glutamatergic neurons in amygdala. Front. Neurosci. 12:387. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2018.00387

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Girgenti, M. J., Wang, J., Ji, D., Cruz, D., Alvarez, V., Benedek, D., et al. (2021). Transcriptomic organization of the human brain in post-traumatic stress disorder. Nat. Neurosci. 24, 24–33. doi: 10.1038/s41593-020-00748-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goulart, V. G., Rocha-Mendonça, H., Maisonnette, S., Pandolfo, P., Landeira-Fernandez, J., Campello-Costa, P., et al. (2021). Differential expression of glutamatergic receptor subunits in the hippocampus in carioca high- and low-conditioned freezing rats. Mol. Cell. Neurosci. 116:103666. doi: 10.1016/j.mcn.2021.103666

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gould, T. D., Dao, D. T., and Kovacsics, C. E. (2009). The open field test. Neuromethods 1916, 99–103. doi: 10.1007/978-1-60761-303-9_1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gur, R., Tendler, A., and Wagner, S. (2014). Long-term social recognition memory is mediated by oxytocin-dependent synaptic plasticity in the medial amygdala. Biol. Psychiatry 76, 377–386. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.03.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heim, C. M., and Nemeroff, C. B. (2001). The role of childhood trauma in the neurobiology of mood and anxiety disorders: Preclinical and clinical studies. Biol. Psychiatry 49, 1023–1039. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(01)01157-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Horovitz, O., Tsoory, M. M., Hall, J., Jacobson-Pick, S., and Richter-Levin, G. (2012). Post-weaning to pre-pubertal (‘juvenile’) stress: A model of induced predisposition to stress-related disorders. Neuroendocrinology 95, 56–64. doi: 10.1159/000331393

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jacobs, S., Huang, F., Tsien, J. Z., and Wei, W. (2016). Social recognition memory test in rodents. Bio-Protocol 6:e1804. doi: 10.21769/bioprotoc.1804

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Katz, R. J., and Hersh, S. (1981). Amitriptyline and scopolamine in an animal model of depression. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 5, 265–271. doi: 10.1016/0149-7634(81)90008-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kiss, J. P., Szasz, B. K., Fodor, L., Mike, A., Lenkey, N., Kurkó, D., et al. (2012). GluN2B-containing NMDA receptors as possible targets for the neuroprotective and antidepressant effects of fluoxetine. Neurochem. Int. 60, 170–176. doi: 10.1016/j.neuint.2011.12.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krystal, J. H., Abdallah, C. G., Averill, L. A., Kelmendi, B., Harpaz-Rotem, I., Sanacora, G., et al. (2017). Synaptic Loss and the Pathophysiology of PTSD: Implications for Ketamine as a Prototype Novel Therapeutic. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 19:74. doi: 10.1007/s11920-017-0829-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lazarevic, V., Mantas, I., Flais, I., and Svenningsson, P. (2019). Fluoxetine suppresses glutamate-and gaba-mediated neurotransmission by altering snare complex. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 20:4247. doi: 10.3390/ijms20174247

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Low, K., Crestani, F., Keist, R., Benke, D., Brünig, I., Benson, J. A., et al. (2000). Molecular and neuronal substrate for the selective attenuation of anxiety. Science 290, 131–134. doi: 10.1126/science.290.5489.131

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lu, C. Y., Liu, X., Jiang, H., Pan, F., Ho, C. S., and Ho, R. C. (2017). Effects of traumatic stress induced in the juvenile period on the expression of gamma-aminobutyric acid receptor Type A subunits in adult rat brain. Neural Plastic. 2017:5715816. doi: 10.1155/2017/5715816

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Management of Post-Traumatic Stress Working Group (2010). VA/DoD Clinical Practice Guideline For Management Of Post-Traumatic Stress. Washington, DC: Department of Veterans Affairs.

Google Scholar

Maroun, M., and Richter-Levin, G. (2002). Local circuit plasticity in the rat dentate gyrus: Characterization and aging-related impairment. Neuroscience 116, 201–211. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4522(02)00045-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martenyi, F., Brown, E. B., and Caldwell, C. D. (2007). Failed efficacy of fluoxetine in the treatment of posttraumatic stress disorder: Results of a fixed-dose, placebo-controlled study. J. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 27, 166–170. doi: 10.1097/JCP.0b013e31803308ce

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meyerhoff, D. J., Mon, A., Metzler, T., and Neylan, T. C. (2014). Cortical gamma-aminobutyric acid and glutamate in posttraumatic stress disorder and their relationships to self-reported sleep quality. Sleep 37, 893–900. doi: 10.5665/sleep.3654

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Michels, L., Schulte-Vels, T., Schick, M., O’Gorman, R. L., Zeffiro, T., Hasler, G., et al. (2014). Prefrontal GABA and glutathione imbalance in posttraumatic stress disorder: Preliminary findings. Psychiatry Res. 224, 288–295. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2014.09.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morris, H. V., Dawson, G. R., Reynolds, D. S., Atack, J. R., and Stephens, D. N. (2006). Both alpha2 and alpha3 GABAA receptor subtypes mediate the anxiolytic properties of benzodiazepine site ligands in the conditioned emotional response paradigm. Eur. J. Neurosci. 23, 2495–2504. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.04775.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nelson, E. C., Agrawal, A., Pergadia, M. L., Lynskey, M. T., and Todorov, A. A. (2009). Association of childhood trauma exposure and GABRA2 polymorphisms with risk of posttraumatic stress disorder in adults. Mol. Psychiatry 23, 2495–2504. doi: 10.1038/mp.2008.81

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pacheco, A., Aguayo, F. I., Aliaga, E., Muñoz, M., García-Rojo, G., Olave, F. A., et al. (2017). Chronic stress triggers expression of immediate early genes and differentially affects the expression of AMPA and NMDA subunits in dorsal and ventral hippocampus of rats. Front. Mol. Neurosci. 10:244. doi: 10.3389/fnmol.2017.00244

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pham, X., Sun, C., Chen, X., van den Oord, E. J., Neale, M. C., Kendler, K. S., et al. (2009). Association study between GABA receptor genes and anxiety spectrum disorders. Depress. Anxiety 26, 998–1003. doi: 10.1002/da.20628

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raud, S., Sütt, S., Luuk, H., Plaas, M., Innos, J., Kõks, S., et al. (2009). Relation between increased anxiety and reduced expression of alpha1 and alpha2 subunits of GABAA receptors in Wfs1-deficient mice. Neurosci. Lett. 460, 138–142. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2009.05.054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ravindran, L. N., and Stein, M. B. (2010). The pharmacologic treatment of anxiety disorders: A review of progress. J. Clin. Psychiatry 71, 839–854. doi: 10.4088/JCP.10r06218blu

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reisi, P., Reisi, P., Alaei, H., and Javanmard, S. H. (2014). Effects of amitriptyline and fluoxetine on synaptic plasticity in the dentate gyrus of hippocampal formation in rats. Adv. Biomed. Res. 3:199. doi: 10.4103/2277-9175.142044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reul, J. M. H. M., and Nutt, D. J. (2008). Glutamate and cortisol – A critical confluence in PTSD? J. Psychopharmacol. 22, 469–472. doi: 10.1177/0269881108094617

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richter-Levin, G., Horovitz, O., and Tsoory, M. M. (2015). “The early adolescent or “juvenile stress” translational animal model of posttraumatic stress disorder,” in Future Directions in Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder: Prevention, Diagnosis, and Treatment, eds M. Safir, H. Wallach, and A. Rizzo (Boston, MA: Springer), doi: 10.1007/978-1-4899-7522-5_3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ritov, G., and Richter-Levin, G. (2014). Water associated zero maze: A novel rat test for long term traumatic re-experiencing. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 8:1. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, R. T., Drafts, B. C., and Fisher, J. L. (2003). Fluoxetine increases GABAA receptor activity through a novel modulatory site. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Therap. 304, 978–984. doi: 10.1124/jpet.102.044834

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosso, I. M., Weiner, M. R., Crowley, D. J., Silveri, M. M., Rauch, S. L., Jensen, J. E., et al. (2014). Insula and anterior cingulate GABA levels in posttraumatic stress disorder: Preliminary findings using magnetic resonance spectroscopy. Depress. Anxiety 31, 115–123. doi: 10.1002/da.22155

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rudolph, R. L., and Feiger, A. D. (1999). A double-blind, randomized, placebo-controlled trial of once-daily venlafaxine extended release (XR) and fluoxetine for the treatment of depression. J. Affect. Disord. 142, S8–S21. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(99)00067-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheth, C., Prescot, A. P., Legarreta, M., Renshaw, P. F., McGlade, E., and Yurgelun-Todd, D. (2019). Reduced gamma-amino butyric acid (GABA) and glutamine in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) of veterans exposed to trauma. J. Affect. Disord. 248, 166–174. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2019.01.037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shipton, O. A., and Paulsen, O. (2014). GluN2A and GluN2B subunit-containing NMDA receptors in hippocampal plasticity. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 369:20130163. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2013.0163

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steckler, T., and Risbrough, V. (2012). Pharmacological treatment of PTSD – Established and new approaches. Neuropharmacology 62, 617–627. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2011.06.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sun, H., Guan, L., Zhu, Z., and Li, H. (2013). Reduced levels of NR1 and NR2A with depression-like behavior in different brain regions in prenatally stressed juvenile offspring. PLoS One 8:e81775. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0081775

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thor, D. H., and Holloway, W. R. (1982). Social memory of the male laboratory rat. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 96, 1000–1006. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.96.6.1000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tuff, L. P., Racine, R. J., and Adamec, R. (1983). The effects of kindling on GABA-Mediated inhibition in the dentate gyrus of the rat. I. Paired-pulse depression. Brain Res. 277, 79–90. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(83)90909-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walf, A. A., and Frye, C. A. (2007). The use of the elevated plus maze as an assay of anxiety-related behavior in rodents. Nat. Protoc. 2, 322–328. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2007.44

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, C. B., McLaughlin, L. D., Ebenezer, P. J., Nair, A. R., Dange, R., Harre, J. G., et al. (2014). Differential effects of sertraline in a predator exposure animal model of post-traumatic stress disorder. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 8:256. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00256

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wisłowska-Stanek, A., Lehner, M., Skórzewska, A., Krza̧ścik, P., Maciejak, P., Szyndler, J., et al. (2013). Changes in the brain expression of alpha-2 subunits of the GABA-A receptor after chronic restraint stress in low- and high-anxiety rats. Behav. Brain Res. 253, 337–345. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2013.07.042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhou, L., and Duan, J. (2018). The C-terminus of NMDAR GluN1-1a subunit translocates to nucleus and regulates synaptic function. Front. Cell. Neurosci. 12:334. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2018.00334

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: PTSD, individual behavioral profiling, fluoxetine, treatment-responders, excitation-inhibition balance, animal model of PTSD

Citation: Sarkar I, Snippe-Strauss M, Tenenhaus Zamir A, Benhos A and Richter-Levin G (2022) Individual behavioral profiling as a translational approach to assess treatment efficacy in an animal model of post-traumatic stress disorder. Front. Neurosci. 16:1071482. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2022.1071482

Received: 16 October 2022; Accepted: 25 November 2022;
Published: 22 December 2022.

Edited by:

Jiansong Zhou, Central South University, China

Reviewed by:

Sebastiano Alfio Torrisi, University of Catania, Italy
Raquel Romay-Tallon, University of Illinois at Chicago, United States

Copyright © 2022 Sarkar, Snippe-Strauss, Tenenhaus Zamir, Benhos and Richter-Levin. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Gal Richter-Levin, Z2FscmljaHRlcmxldmluQGdtYWlsLmNvbQ==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.