Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Neurosci., 24 May 2021
Sec. Neuroenergetics and Brain Health
This article is part of the Research Topic Polyphenols’ Action on the Brain View all 9 articles

Acute Effects of Polyphenols on Human Attentional Processes: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis

  • 1Unilever R&D, Colworth Science Park, Bedford, United Kingdom
  • 2Unilever Foods Innovation Centre, Wageningen, Netherlands
  • 3Nutrition, Dietetics and Food, Monash University, Notting Hill, VIC, Australia
  • 4Department of Nutrition and Preventive Medicine, Norwich Medical School, University of East Anglia, Norwich, United Kingdom

Background: The effects of polyphenols on cognitive functions have been extensively studied. Due to the large heterogeneity in the study designs, however, it is often difficult to interpret their efficacy. To address this issue, we conducted a systematic review and meta-analyses to examine whether acute polyphenol intake may have a beneficial effect on cognition and specifically on the accuracy and speed of attention.

Methods: PubMed and Scopus databases were systematically searched for studies published up to end of August 2020 following PRISMA guidelines (PROSPERO registration number: CRD42021232109). Only placebo-controlled human intervention trials that assessed acute effects of polyphenols on accuracy and speed of attention were included in the meta-analyses. When cognitive tasks were repeated over time, pooled means and standard deviations for intervention and placebo over repetitions separately for each task for both speed and accuracy were calculated. We also conducted separate analyses focusing only on the last repetition. Furthermore, confounding effects of age and source of polyphenols were also considered.

Results: Eighteen studies met the inclusion criteria. The pooled analysis of last task repetitions showed that the acute consumption of polyphenols improved rapid visual information processing speed in young participants (SMD = 0.26; 95%CI = [0.03–0.50]; I2= 0%; p = 0.02; k = 5). All other analyses did not reach significance.

Conclusion: The results of the current study indicate that acute polyphenol consumption might improve speed in rapid visual information processing task, a higher order task with elements of vigilance, working memory, and executive function, in young participants; however, as the current literature is inconsistent and limited, further acute intervention studies are warranted to achieve more conclusive results.

Introduction

Polyphenols constitute a large group of bioactive phytochemicals in the plant kingdom. They are classified in terms of the number of phenol rings that they encompass, the structural elements that bind the rings to each other as well as the substituents linked to them (Abbas et al., 2017). The main classes include (i) phenolic acids, (ii) stilbenes, (iii) lignans, and (iv) flavonoids. Phenolic acids are found in many fruit and vegetables, specifically in berries; stilbenes in red wine and grapes; and lignans in whole bran cereals and flaxseeds (for more examples please see Gomez-Pinilla and Nguyen, 2012; Figueira et al., 2017; Fraga et al., 2019). The main sources of flavonoids are berries, cocoa, citrus, tea, wine, and soy products (for more examples please see Gomez-Pinilla and Nguyen, 2012; Figueira et al., 2017; Fraga et al., 2019). As such, polyphenols are abundant in Mediterranean style and plant-forward diets (Figueira et al., 2017).

Although it has not been established whether polyphenols can cross the blood–brain barrier (BBB) to directly affect central mechanisms underlying cognitive processes (Schaffer and Halliwell, 2012), other physiologically plausible candidate modes of action have been identified. These include (i) reducing neuroinflammation, (ii) modulating the gut microbiota structure and function, (iii) activating the endogenous antioxidant defence system, and (iv) affecting the cardiovascular system to improve neurovascular coupling (Angeloni et al., 2020; Lamport and Williams, 2020).

Regardless of their modes of action, it has been well-documented that prolonged consumption of certain phenolic compounds and polyphenol-rich foods, and adherence to polyphenol-rich plant-forward or plant-based diets may contribute to limiting age-related neurodegeneration and preventing or slowing cognitive decline in older adults (Letenneur et al., 2007; Figueira et al., 2017; Vauzour, 2017; Lefevre-Arbogast et al., 2018; Scarmeas et al., 2018; Solfrizzi et al., 2018; Rajaram et al., 2019; Shishtar et al., 2020). A recent meta-analysis of randomised controlled clinical trials focusing on chronic polyphenol administration, however, showed that only some polyphenols may have potential beneficial effects on specific domains of cognition and mainly in cognitively healthy older individuals. These included verbal learning/memory and visuospatial ability (Potı̀ et al., 2019). On the other hand, the effects of acute polyphenol consumption on cognition have not been well-characterised (Bell et al., 2015). For instance, a number of studies have investigated the acute effects of polyphenol intake on measures of speed and accuracy of attention in recent years. Outcomes of these studies, however, are mixed. While some studies showed positive acute effects of polyphenols, particularly in tasks that measure speed of attention (Scholey et al., 2010; Field et al., 2011; Watson et al., 2015; Dietz et al., 2017), others failed to show such beneficial effects (Kennedy et al., 2010; Bondonno et al., 2014; Wightman et al., 2014). Furthermore, given the heterogeneity in study quality, design, and polyphenol type and dosage, it is challenging to reach a definite conclusion whether (i) acute polyphenol intake improves cognition when compared to a matching placebo/control, and (ii) whether this improvement is specific to a type of polyphenol, age group, and/or cognitive domain only. To address this knowledge gap, we performed a systematic review and multiple meta-analyses of controlled human studies focusing on the acute effects of polyphenol and/or polyphenol-rich food intake on speed and accuracy of attention.

Method

The protocol for this systematic review and meta-analyses was registered in the international prospective register of systematic reviews (PROSPERO, registration number: CRD42021232109) and the articles were selected according to the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) diagram (Moher et al., 2015; Shamseer et al., 2015).

Data Sources and Search Strategy

We carried out electronic literature searches on PubMed and Scopus to identify relevant studies. The search was conducted until the end of August 2020. Search strategy could be found in Supplementary Material.

Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria

One reviewer (PH) independently selected papers according to the following inclusion and exclusion criteria: Inclusion Criteria; (i) Treatment: polyphenols (no dose or type limit); (ii) Treatment duration: acute studies only (within a day); (iii) Outcome measures: accuracy and speed of attention measured by computerised tasks; (iv) Design: randomised controlled trials; (v) Participants: Any age or gender, healthy participants. Exclusion Criteria; (i) Design: Case report, letter to editor, conference paper, thesis, personal opinion, or commentary; (ii) Animal studies, in vitro and ex vivo studies, psychophysiological and/or neuroimaging studies.

Selection of Studies

The outcome measures of attention shown below were included in the systematic review/meta- analysis. We only included attention measures that were used across eight or more individual studies and these were simple reaction time (RT), choice RT, digit vigilance (DV), and rapid visual information processing (RVIP) tasks. In the simple RT task, we only used speed of reaction (ms) data. In choice RT, digit vigilance and RVIP tasks, we used both accuracy (% correct) and speed (ms) data. Short descriptions of simple RT (Wightman et al., 2012; Bondonno et al., 2014; Massee et al., 2015; Dietz et al., 2017; Haskell-Ramsay et al., 2017, 2018a; Kennedy et al., 2017; Watson et al., 2019), choice RT (Field et al., 2011; Wightman et al., 2012; Bondonno et al., 2014; Massee et al., 2015; Dietz et al., 2017; Haskell-Ramsay et al., 2017, 2018a; Kennedy et al., 2017; Watson et al., 2019), digit vigilance (Bondonno et al., 2014; Watson et al., 2015, 2019; Keane et al., 2016; Dietz et al., 2017; Haskell-Ramsay et al., 2017, 2018a; Kennedy et al., 2017), and RVIP (Kennedy et al., 2010, 2020; Scholey et al., 2010; Cropley et al., 2012; Wightman et al., 2012, 2014, 2018; Massee et al., 2015; Watson et al., 2015; Keane et al., 2016; Haskell-Ramsay et al., 2018a; Philip et al., 2019) tasks are available elsewhere.

Data Extraction

Two review authors (PH and AG) independently extracted data to evaluate and classify the quality of each study. Any disagreements were discussed between the authors until resolution. The following data was extracted by from all publications: (i) publication details: authors, year, journal; (ii) participant characteristics: number of participants recruited, number of participants included in the study, number of participants (intervention), number of participants (control), gender, and age range; (iii) study design: design and blinding; (iv) intervention characteristics: dose and type of polyphenols consumed; (v) control characteristics: presence/absence of control/placebo, control/placebo doses and types; (vi) outcome measures: accuracy and speed of attention measured by computerised tasks; (vii) remarks: notes on the factors that might affect results/data quality.

We emailed authors and asked them to provide data missing from included studies where necessary. In cases where the authors did not reply, we used calculations provided in Systematic Reviews of Interventions to obtain missing means and standard deviations (Higgins et al., 2020).

Quality and Risk of Bias Assessment

One reviewer (PH) evaluated the quality of the studies included in this review by using the Cochrane Collaboration's tool for assessing risk of bias in randomised trials (Higgins et al., 2011). For this purpose, 7 different domains were considered (random sequence generation, allocation concealment, blinding of participants and personnel, blinding of outcome assessment, incomplete outcome data, selective reporting, and other sources of bias). Publication bias was evaluated by means of visual inspection of funnel plots (constructed by inverse plotting SE against the respective SMD for each measure of speed and accuracy of attention) and Egger's regression test (with p < 0.1 indicating asymmetry) (Egger et al., 1997). Heterogeneity was assessed by means of the Cochran's Q statistic (significant at p < 0.1) and quantified by the I2 statistic (with values of 25, 50, and 75% considered to be low-, moderate-, and high-level heterogeneity, respectively) (Higgins et al., 2003).

Statistical Analysis

All statistical meta-analyses and graphical display were conducted using Jamovi 1.2.27 (The Jamovi Project, 2020) that utilises MAJOR meta-analysis module, which relies on metafor package for R (Hamilton, 2018). We selected highest polyphenol dose to include in the meta-analyses where studies had 2 or more intervention groups.

Separate random effects models were used to examine the effect of polyphenols on speed and accuracy of attention in simple RT, choice RT, digit vigilance, and RVIP tasks. All effect sizes were calculated using the standardised mean differences (SMD) due to the heterogeneity of cognitive tasks used. SMD effects sizes were interpreted using rules of thumb (<0.40 = small, 0.40–0.70 = moderate, >0.70 = large effect) (Higgins et al., 2020). We considered a statistically significant finding with p < 0.05.

Results

Summary of Included Studies

2,250 studies were identified using our defined search strategy. After screening for eligibility applying our inclusion and exclusion criteria described in the method section, 18 studies that met our criteria were included in the current review and meta-analyses (see Figure 1). Summaries of all the studies are presented in Table 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. PRISMA flowchart of the selection procedure.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Summary table of included studies.

Meta-Analyses on Speed and Accuracy of Attention

We conducted a series of meta-analyses with the aim of investigating the impact of polyphenols on both speed and accuracy of attention. In order to capture different attentional outcomes due to the disparate pharmacokinetic profiles of the polyphenols, we investigated both averaged repetitions and last repetition when cognitive tasks were repeated measures. No significant effects were observed on speed of attention in simple RT, choice RT, digit vigilance, and RVIP tasks in both averaged (Figure 2) and last repetitions (Figure 3). Similarly, no effects of polyphenol intake on accuracy of attention in choice RT, digit vigilance, and RVIP tasks in both averaged (Figure 4) and last repetitions (Figure 5) were observed.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Meta-analyses on the effects of polyphenols on speed of attention (repetitions averaged) in (A) Simple RT, (B) Choice RT, (C) Digit Vigilance, (D) RVIP tasks.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Meta-analyses on the effects of polyphenols on speed of attention (last repetition only) in (A) Simple RT, (B) Choice RT, (C) Digit Vigilance, (D) RVIP tasks.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Meta-analyses on the effects of polyphenols on accuracy of attention (repetitions averaged) in (A) Choice RT, (B) Digit Vigilance, (C) RVIP tasks.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Meta-analyses on the effects of polyphenols on accuracy of attention (last repetition only) in (A) Choice RT, (B) Digit Vigilance, (C) RVIP tasks.

Sub-group Analyses

Polyphenol Source

There was only sufficient data to conduct sub-group analyses of studies on berry polyphenols. No statistically significant effects were found for any measures of speed- or accuracy of attention in this sub-group analysis (see Supplementary Material Results sections 1 and 3).

Age Groups

In the averaged repetition analyses, no statistically significant effects on measures of speed of attention was seen in either of the age groups (see Supplementary Material Results sections 2.1 and 2.3). In the last repetition analyses, however, we found that young participants had significantly faster responses in polyphenol condition only in the RVIP task (SMD = 0.26; 95%CI = [0.03–0.50]) (Figure 6). All other last repetition analyses revealed non-significant results (see Supplementary Material Results sections 4.1 and 4.3). For measures of accuracy of attention, no statistically significant effects were found in either of the age groups (see Supplementary Material Results sections 2.2, 2.4, 4.2, and 4.4).

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Meta-analysis on the effects of polyphenols on RVIP speed (last repetition only) in young participants.

Risk of Bias and Heterogeneity

Nearly 70% of the studies were categorised as having low risk of selection (random sequence generation and allocation concealment), performance, attrition, reporting, and other sources of biases. Risk for detection bias was unclear as most of the studies did not report whether outcome assessment and statistical analyses were blinded or not (see Supplementary Figure 1).

Averaged Repetitions

For speed of attention, heterogeneity for simple RT was low (p-value for Q statistic = 0.96, I2= 0%), choice RT was low-moderate (p-value for Q statistic = 0.17, I2 = 29.64%), digit vigilance (p-value for Q statistic < 0.001, I2 = 99.45%) was high, and RVIP was low (p-value for Q statistic = 0.43, I2= 4.6%). For accuracy of attention, heterogeneity for choice RT was low (p-value for Q statistic = 0.99, I2= 0%), digit vigilance (p-value for Q statistic < 0.001, I2= 98.24%) was high, and RVIP was low (p-value for Q statistic = 0.76, I2= 0%).

Egger's linear regression test did not indicate potential presence of publication bias for speed of attention in simple RT (p = 0.65), choice RT (p = 0.70), and RVIP (p = 0.96) tasks. Similarly, no bias was detected for choice RT (p = 0.97), and RVIP (p = 0.53) task accuracies. However, we observed potential publication biases for digit vigilance speed (p = 0.001) and accuracy (p = 0.001) studies. Additionally, visual inspection of contour-enhanced funnel plots did not confirm an obvious presence of publication bias in majority of the studies with an exception for digit vigilance speed and accuracy (see Supplementary Figures 2, 4).

Last Repetition

For speed of attention, heterogeneity for simple RT was high (p-value for Q statistic < 0.001, I2= 90.1%), choice RT (p-value for Q statistic = 0.08, I2= 44.4%) and digit vigilance (p-value for Q statistic = 0.04, I2= 49.7%) was moderate, and RVIP was low (p-value for Q statistic = 0.38, I2= 14.01%). For accuracy of attention, heterogeneity for choice RT was low (p-value for Q statistic = 0.95, I2= 0%), digit vigilance (p-value for Q statistic < 0.001, I2= 98.2%) was high, and RVIP was low (p-value for Q statistic = 0.99, I2 = 0%). Heterogeneity for speed in the RVIP task in young participants was also low (p-value for Q statistic = 0.57, I2 = 0%).

Egger's linear regression test did not indicate potential presence of publication bias for speed of attention in simple RT (p = 0.44), choice RT (p = 0.82), digit vigilance (p = 0.38), and RVIP (p = 0.07) tasks. Similarly, no bias detected for choice RT (p = 0.57), and RVIP (p = 0.76) task accuracies. Also, no bias detected for RVIP speed studies in young participants (p = 0.20). However, we observed a potential publication bias for digit vigilance accuracy (p = 0.001) studies. Additionally, visual inspection of contour-enhanced funnel plots did not confirm an obvious presence of publication bias in majority of the studies with an exception for digit vigilance accuracy (see Supplementary Figures 3, 5).

Discussion

The importance of intact attentional performance in young adults and the need of maintaining attention in older adults have been well-established. This area, however, has not previously been systematically examined in relation to acute polyphenol intake. Hence, the aim of the current review and meta-analyses was to investigate the acute effects of polyphenol and/or polyphenol-rich food intake on cognitive performance, with a particular focus on speed and accuracy of attention. In this meta-analysis, summarising evidence from 18 randomised controlled intervention studies, we observed faster RVIP responses only in the subset of younger participants following acute consumption of polyphenols.

One might argue that our finding in relation to the beneficial effect of polyphenols on RVIP speed in younger participants may be attributable to caffeine's cognitive-enhancing effect (Nehlig, 2010). However, in the current meta-analyses, only two studies out of five that utilised RVIP task in young participants used small amounts of caffeine in their treatment conditions (Scholey et al., 2010; Massee et al., 2015). Therefore, it is more likely that the effect of polyphenol-rich foods on RVIP speed was driven mainly by their polyphenol content. Consistent with our results, recent meta-analyses examining the differential effects of moderator variables in relation to participant characteristics and supplementation protocols have revealed beneficial effects of polyphenols in younger (vs. older) individuals and following acute (vs. chronic) supplementation (Ammar et al., 2020a,b). As it has been shown that even healthy older adults have relatively poor attention due to cognitive ageing (Filley and Cullum, 1994; Harada et al., 2013), acute polyphenol supplementation might not be helpful to delay or reverse these effects. On the other hand, as young adults have relatively intact attention, maintaining these processes with acute polyphenol supplementation might be a more realistic goal. Although supporting evidence comes from a recent theory that identifies young adults as candidate targets for antiaging interventions due to the possibility of preventing diseases in young (Belsky et al., 2015), further high-quality research is required to examine whether starting polyphenol interventions in young adults to delay and/or prevent cognitive decline would be more advantageous or not.

It is important to note that, for this systematic review and meta-analysis, we restricted our analysis to acute effects on attentional measures. However, previous research suggests that specific cognitive processes may be differentially affected by various polyphenols (Potı̀ et al., 2019). For instance, Kesse-Guyot et al. (2012) observed a positive association between total polyphenol intake and performance in tasks that involved verbal/language skills but not with executive function. Similarly, a recent longitudinal study showed positive associations with (i) higher flavanol and flavan-3-ol intakes and global function and verbal/visual memory (ii) higher total flavonoid and flavonoid polymer intakes and visual memory, and (iii) higher flavanol intake and verbal learning (Shishtar et al., 2020). Hence, a thorough exploration as to which cognitive domains could be improved by polyphenol intake is warranted.

Apart from faster RVIP responses, no statistically significant effects were found in the other meta-analyses of digit vigilance, choice- and simple reaction time tasks. These outcomes could, in part, be explained by potential limitations of the current review: Firstly, the included studies featured a range of different polyphenols, doses and delivery formats. In this regard, differences in polyphenol bioavailability, metabolism, and structure could have had an influence on the outcomes of the meta-analyses. Indeed, different types of polyphenols vary in their pharmacokinetic profiles (Del Rio et al., 2010; Clifford et al., 2013), which may also be affected by the food and/or format in which they are consumed, as well as interindividual differences in bioavailability at the participant level (Gibney et al., 2019). In addition to bioavailability, a host of other factors (e.g., health status, genetic variance, sex, body mass index, age) have been identified that may impact interindividual variability in physiological responses to polyphenols (Cassidy and Minihane, 2017; Gibney et al., 2019; Landberg et al., 2019). Combined, heterogeneity in these factors within and between the studies included in the meta-analyses, may have inflated interindividual variation in responses, and thus limited the statistical power of studies and meta-analyses to detect potential effects. We attempted to address this limitation through sub-group analyses. However, due to lack of available data, sub-group analyses were limited to studies providing berries as a polyphenol source and, we were unable to investigate potential differences in efficacy of the different polyphenol classes. This is an important limitation since differences in efficacy between different types of polyphenol have been reported (albeit in chronic supplementation studies) (Kesse-Guyot et al., 2012; Potı̀ et al., 2019; Shishtar et al., 2020). We were also not able to conduct subgroup or meta-regression analyses to investigate potential effects of dose. The available data is not consistent at the moment, as dose-dependent effects have been suggested for some polyphenol sources (Kennedy et al., 2000; Bell et al., 2015), but not others (Scholey et al., 2010). Further investigation across polyphenol classes and doses in studies specifically designed for this purpose to provide answers in this regard. We were also not able to correct for potential confounding effects of variations in background polyphenol intake between the studies. It has been suggested that individuals most likely to participate in nutrition intervention trials, are usually health-conscious with good baseline nutritional status (Young et al., 2020). This “self-selection bias” may limit the ability to measure a potential benefit on functional status. As such, healthy diets generally have a high polyphenol content and, none of the included studies evaluated polyphenol intake status. Also, although some trials (Scholey et al., 2010; Field et al., 2011; Cropley et al., 2012; Watson et al., 2015, 2019; Keane et al., 2016; Philip et al., 2019) included a low-polyphenol run-in period, it is unclear whether this would have been sufficient to control for any potential effects of background polyphenol intake. Furthermore, the bioavailability of gut-derived polyphenol metabolites is several-fold higher than that of their parent compounds (Van Duynhoven et al., 2014). These metabolites have been implicated in the beneficial effects of polyphenol interventions (Espin et al., 2017), but would not reach the circulation within the measurement timeframe of the acute studies included in this review. In this regard, longer-term studies aimed at determining the role that polyphenol metabolites play in their reported cognitive effects would be useful and future studies are warranted.

In conclusion, the results of the current review indicate that acute polyphenol consumption might improve speed in RVIP task in young participants, however, as the current literature is inconsistent and limited, further studies are warranted to (i) replicate our current findings showing faster RVIP responses in young participants (and lack of this effect in old participants) following acute polyphenol consumption, preferably by using different polyphenol sources and doses, while controlling for background polyphenol intake and/or diet quality, and (ii) examine whether these acute improvements might translate into sustained cognitive benefits (i.e., slower age-related decline) in longitudinal settings.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary Material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding authors.

Author Contributions

PH and AG wrote the manuscript with input from AS and DV who also contributed to the revision of the manuscript critically for important intellectual content. All authors approved the submitted version.

Funding

Funds received from Unilever UK Central Resources Limited to cover publication fees.

Conflict of Interest

AG is employed by Unilever. PH was employed by Unilever, however, she is currently affiliated with University of Roehampton. AS and DV have received research funding, consultancy, travel support, and speaking honoraria from various industrial companies.

Acknowledgments

We would like to acknowledge and thank the authors who kindly shared their data.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnins.2021.678769/full#supplementary-material

References

Abbas, M., Saeed, F., Anjum, F. M., Afzaal, M., Tufail, T., Bashir, M. S., et al. (2017). Natural polyphenols: an overview. Int. J. Food Prop. 20, 1689–1699. doi: 10.1080/10942912.2016.1220393

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ammar, A., Trabelsi, K., Boukhris, O., Bouaziz, B., Muller, P., Glenn, J. M., et al. (2020a). Moderators of the impact of (poly)phenols interventions on psychomotor functions and bdnf: insights from subgroup analysis and meta-regression. Nutrients 12:2872. doi: 10.3390/nu12092872

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ammar, A., Trabelsi, K., Boukhris, O., Bouaziz, B., Müller, P., J, M.G., et al. (2020b). Effects of polyphenol-rich interventions on cognition and brain health in healthy young and middle-aged adults: systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Clin. Med. 9:5. doi: 10.3390/jcm9051598

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Angeloni, C., Businaro, R., and Vauzour, D. (2020). The role of diet in preventing and reducing cognitive decline. Curr. Opin. Psychiatry 33, 432–438. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0000000000000605

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bell, L., Lamport, D. J., Butler, L. T., and Williams, C. M. (2015). A review of the cognitive effects observed in humans following acute supplementation with flavonoids, and their associated mechanisms of action. Nutrients 7, 10290–10306. doi: 10.3390/nu7125538

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belsky, D. W., Caspi, A., Houts, R., Cohen, H. J., Corcoran, D. L., Danese, A., et al. (2015). Quantification of biological aging in young adults. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 112, E4104–4110. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1506264112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bondonno, C. P., Downey, L. A., Croft, K. D., Scholey, A., Stough, C., Yang, X., et al. (2014). The acute effect of flavonoid-rich apples and nitrate-rich spinach on cognitive performance and mood in healthy men and women. Food Funct. 5, 849–858. doi: 10.1039/C3FO60590F

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cassidy, A., and Minihane, A. M. (2017). The role of metabolism (and the microbiome) in defining the clinical efficacy of dietary flavonoids. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 105, 10–22. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.116.136051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clifford, M. N., Van Der Hooft, J. J. J., and Crozier, A. (2013). Human studies on the absorption, distribution, metabolism, and excretion of tea polyphenols. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 98, 1619S–1630S. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.113.058958

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cropley, V., Croft, R., Silber, B., Neale, C., Scholey, A., Stough, C., et al. (2012). Does coffee enriched with chlorogenic acids improve mood and cognition after acute administration in healthy elderly? A pilot study. Psychopharmacology 219, 737–749. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2395-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Del Rio, D., Stalmach, A., Calani, L., and Crozier, A. (2010). Bioavailability of coffee chlorogenic acids and green tea flavan-3-ols. Nutrients 2, 820–833. doi: 10.3390/nu2080820

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dietz, C., Dekker, M., and Piqueras-Fiszman, B. (2017). An intervention study on the effect of matcha tea, in drink and snack bar formats, on mood and cognitive performance. Food Res. Int. 99, 72–83. doi: 10.1016/j.foodres.2017.05.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Egger, M., Smith, G. D., Schneider, M., and Minder, C. (1997). Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ 315, 629–634. doi: 10.1136/bmj.315.7109.629

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Espin, J. C., Gonzalez-Sarrias, A., and Tomas-Barberan, F. A. (2017). The gut microbiota: a key factor in the therapeutic effects of (poly)phenols. Biochem. Pharmacol. 139, 82–93. doi: 10.1016/j.bcp.2017.04.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Field, D. T., Williams, C. M., and Butler, L. T. (2011). Consumption of cocoa flavanols results in an acute improvement in visual and cognitive functions. Physiol. Behav. 103, 255–260. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.02.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Figueira, I., Menezes, R., Macedo, D., Costa, I., and Dos Santos, C. N. (2017). Polyphenols beyond barriers: a glimpse into the brain. Curr. Neuropharmacol. 15, 562–594. doi: 10.2174/1570159X14666161026151545

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Filley, C. M., and Cullum, C. M. (1994). Attention and vigilance functions in normal aging. Appl. Neuropsychol. 1, 29–32. doi: 10.1207/s15324826an0101andamp;2_6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fraga, C. G., Croft, K. D., Kennedy, D. O., and Tomás-Barberán, F. A. (2019). The effects of polyphenols and other bioactives on human health. Food Funct. 10, 514–528. doi: 10.1039/C8FO01997E

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gibney, E. R., Milenkovic, D., Combet, E., Ruskovska, T., Greyling, A., Gonzalez-Sarrias, A., et al. (2019). Factors influencing the cardiometabolic response to (poly)phenols and phytosterols: a review of the cost action positive activities. Eur. J. Nutr. 58, 37–47. doi: 10.1007/s00394-019-02066-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gomez-Pinilla, F., and Nguyen, T. T. J. (2012). Natural mood foods: the actions of polyphenols against psychiatric and cognitive disorders. Nutr. Neurosci. 15, 127–133. doi: 10.1179/1476830511Y.0000000035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamilton, K. (2018). Major: Meta Analysis Jamovi R. 1.2.27 ed. Technical Report.

Google Scholar

Harada, C. N., Natelson Love, M. C., and Triebel, K. L. (2013). Normal cognitive aging. Clin. Geriatr. Med. 29, 737–752. doi: 10.1016/j.cger.2013.07.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haskell-Ramsay, C. F., Jackson, P. A., Dodd, F. L., Forster, J. S., Bérubé, J., Levinton, C., et al. (2018a). Acute post-prandial cognitive effects of brown seaweed extract in humans. Nutrients 10:85. doi: 10.3390/nu10010085

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haskell-Ramsay, C. F., Jackson, P. A., Forster, J. S., Dodd, F. L., Bowerbank, S. L., and Kennedy, D. O. (2018b). The acute effects of caffeinated black coffee on cognition and mood in healthy young and older adults. Nutrients 10:1386. doi: 10.3390/nu10101386

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haskell-Ramsay, C. F., Stuart, R. C., Okello, E. J., and Watson, A. W. (2017). Cognitive and mood improvements following acute supplementation with purple grape juice in healthy young adults. Eur. J. Nutr. 56, 2621–2631. doi: 10.1007/s00394-017-1454-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Higgins, J. P., Altman, D. G., Gotzsche, P. C., Juni, P., Moher, D., Oxman, A. D., et al. (2011). The Cochrane collaboration's tool for assessing risk of bias in randomised trials. BMJ 343:d5928. doi: 10.1136/bmj.d5928

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Higgins, J. P., Thompson, S. G., Deeks, J. J., and Altman, D. G. (2003). Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. BMJ 327, 557–560. doi: 10.1136/bmj.327.7414.557

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Higgins, J. P. T., Thomas, J., Chandler, J., Cumpston, M., Li, T., Page, M. J., et al. (2020). Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions Version 6.1 (Updated September 2020). Cochrane, 2020. doi: 10.1002/9781119536604

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keane, K. M., Haskell-Ramsay, C. F., Veasey, R. C., and Howatson, G. (2016). Montmorency Tart Cherries (Prunus Cerasus L.) modulate vascular function acutely, in the absence of improvement in cognitive performance. Br. J. Nutr. 116, 1935–1944. doi: 10.1017/S0007114516004177

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kennedy, D. O., Bonnländer, B., Lang, S. C., Pischel, I., Forster, J., Khan, J., et al. (2020). Acute and chronic effects of green oat (Avena Sativa) extract on cognitive function and mood during a laboratory stressor in healthy adults: a randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled study in healthy humans. Nutrients 12:6. doi: 10.3390/nu12061598

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kennedy, D. O., Jackson, P. A., Forster, J., Khan, J., Grothe, T., Perrinjaquet-Moccetti, T., et al. (2017). Acute effects of a wild green-oat (avena sativa) extract on cognitive function in middle-aged adults: a double-blind, placebo-controlled, within-subjects trial. Nutr. Neurosci. 20, 135–151. doi: 10.1080/1028415X.2015.1101304

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kennedy, D. O., Scholey, A. B., and Wesnes, K. A. (2000). The dose-dependent cognitive effects of acute administration of ginkgo biloba to healthy young volunteers. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 151, 416–423. doi: 10.1007/s002130000501

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kennedy, D. O., Wightman, E. L., Reay, J. L., Lietz, G., Okello, E. J., Wilde, A., et al. (2010). Effects of resveratrol on cerebral blood flow variables and cognitive performance in humans: a double-blind, placebo-controlled, crossover investigation. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 91, 1590–1597. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.2009.28641

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kesse-Guyot, E., Fezeu, L., Andreeva, V. A., Touvier, M., Scalbert, A., Hercberg, S., et al. (2012). Total and specific polyphenol intakes in midlife are associated with cognitive function measured 13 years later. J. Nutr. 142, 76–83. doi: 10.3945/jn.111.144428

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lamport, D. J., and Williams, C. M. (2020). Polyphenols and cognition in humans: an overview of current evidence from recent systematic reviews and meta-analyses. Brain Plast. 6, 139–153. doi: 10.3233/BPL-200111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Landberg, R., Manach, C., Kerckhof, F. M., Minihane, A. M., Saleh, R. N. M., De Roos, B., et al. (2019). Future prospects for dissecting inter-individual variability in the absorption, distribution and elimination of plant bioactives of relevance for cardiometabolic endpoints. Eur. J. Nutr. 58, 21–36. doi: 10.1007/s00394-019-02095-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lefevre-Arbogast, S., Gaudout, D., Bensalem, J., Letenneur, L., Dartigues, J. F., Hejblum, B. P., et al. (2018). Pattern of polyphenol intake and the long-term risk of dementia in older persons. Neurology 90, 1979–1988. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000005607

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Letenneur, L., Proust-Lima, C., Le Gouge, A., Dartigues, J., and Barberger-Gateau, P. (2007). Flavonoid intake and cognitive decline over a 10-year period. Am. J. Epidemiol. 165, 1364–1371. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwm036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Massee, L. A., Ried, K., Pase, M., Travica, N., Yoganathan, J., Scholey, A., et al. (2015). The acute and sub-chronic effects of cocoa flavanols on mood, cognitive and cardiovascular health in young healthy adults: a randomized, controlled trial. Front. Pharmacol. 6:93. doi: 10.3389/fphar.2015.00093

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moher, D., Shamseer, L., Clarke, M., Ghersi, D., Liberati, A., Petticrew, M., et al. (2015). Preferred reporting items for systematic review and meta-analysis protocols (prisma-p) 2015 statement. Syst. Rev. 4:1. doi: 10.1186/2046-4053-4-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nehlig, A. (2010). Is caffeine a cognitive enhancer? J. Alzheimers Dis. 20, 85–94. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2010-091315

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Philip, P., Sagaspe, P., Taillard, J., Mandon, C., Constans, J., Pourtau, L., et al. (2019). Acute intake of a grape and blueberry polyphenol-rich extract ameliorates cognitive performance in healthy young adults during a sustained cognitive effort. Antioxidants 8:12. doi: 10.3390/antiox8120650

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Potì, F., Santi, D., Spaggiari, G., Zimetti, F., and Zanotti, I. (2019). Polyphenol health effects on cardiovascular and neurodegenerative disorders: a review and meta-analysis. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 20:351. doi: 10.3390/ijms20020351

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rajaram, S., Jones, J., and Lee, G. J. (2019). Plant-based dietary patterns, plant foods, and age-related cognitive decline. Adv. Nutr. 10, 422–436. doi: 10.1093/advances/nmz081

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scarmeas, N., Anastasiou, C. A., and Yannakoulia, M. (2018). Nutrition and prevention of cognitive impairment. Lancet Neurol. 17, 1006–1015. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(18)30338-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schaffer, S., and Halliwell, B. (2012). Do polyphenols enter the brain and does it matter? Some theoretical and practical considerations. Genes Nutr. 7, 99–109. doi: 10.1007/s12263-011-0255-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scholey, A. B., French, S. J., Morris, P. J., Kennedy, D. O., Milne, A. L., and Haskell, C. F. (2010). Consumption of cocoa flavanols results in acute improvements in mood and cognitive performance during sustained mental effort. J. Psychopharmacol. 24, 1505–1514. doi: 10.1177/0269881109106923

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shamseer, L., Moher, D., Clarke, M., Ghersi, D., Liberati, A., Petticrew, M., et al. (2015). Preferred reporting items for systematic review and meta-analysis protocols (prisma-p) 2015: elaboration and explanation. Br. Med. J. 350:g7647. doi: 10.1136/bmj.g7647

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shishtar, E., Rogers, G. T., Blumberg, J. B., Au, R., and Jacques, P. F. (2020). Long-term dietary flavonoid intake and change in cognitive function in the framingham offspring cohort. Public Health Nutr. 23, 1576–1588. doi: 10.1017/S136898001900394X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Solfrizzi, V., Agosti, P., Lozupone, M., Custodero, C., Schilardi, A., Valiani, V., et al. (2018). Nutritional intervention as a preventive approach for cognitive-related outcomes in cognitively healthy older adults: a systematic review. J. Alzheimer's Dis. 64, S229–S254. doi: 10.3233/JAD-179940

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

The Jamovi Project (2020). Jamovi (Version 1.2.27). Computer Software.

Google Scholar

Van Duynhoven, J., Van Der Hooft, J. J., Van Dorsten, F. A., Peters, S., Foltz, M., Gomez-Roldan, V., et al. (2014). Rapid and sustained systemic circulation of conjugated gut microbial catabolites after single-dose black tea extract consumption. J. Proteome Res. 13, 2668–2678. doi: 10.1021/pr5001253

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vauzour, D. (2017). Polyphenols and brain health. OCL 24:2. doi: 10.1051/ocl/2017008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watson, A. W., Haskell-Ramsay, C. F., Kennedy, D. O., Cooney, J. M., Trower, T., and Scheepens, A. (2015). Acute supplementation with blackcurrant extracts modulates cognitive functioning and inhibits monoamine oxidase-b in healthy young adults. J. Func. Foods 17, 524–539. doi: 10.1016/j.jff.2015.06.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watson, A. W., Okello, E. J., Brooker, H. J., Lester, S., Mcdougall, G. J., and Wesnes, K. A. (2019). The impact of blackcurrant juice on attention, mood and brain wave spectral activity in young healthy volunteers. Nutr. Neurosci. 22, 596–606. doi: 10.1080/1028415X.2017.1420539

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wightman, E. L., Haskell, C. F., Forster, J. S., Veasey, R. C., and Kennedy, D. O. (2012). Epigallocatechin gallate, cerebral blood flow parameters, cognitive performance and mood in healthy humans: a double-blind, placebo-controlled, crossover investigation. Hum. Psychopharmacol. 27, 177–186. doi: 10.1002/hup.1263

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wightman, E. L., Jackson, P. A., Khan, J., Forster, J., Heiner, F., Feistel, B., et al. (2018). The acute and chronic cognitive and cerebral blood flow effects of a sideritis scardica (greek mountain tea) extract: a double blind, randomized, placebo controlled, parallel groups study in healthy humans. Nutrients 10:955. doi: 10.3390/nu10080955

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wightman, E. L., Reay, J. L., Haskell, C. F., Williamson, G., Dew, T. P., and Kennedy, D. O. (2014). Effects of resveratrol alone or in combination with piperine on cerebral blood flow parameters and cognitive performance in human subjects: a randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled, cross-over investigation. Br. J. Nutr. 112, 203–213. doi: 10.1017/S0007114514000737

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Young, L. M., Gauci, S., Scholey, A., White, D. J., and Pipingas, A. (2020). Self-selection bias: an essential design consideration for nutrition trials in healthy populations. Front. Nutr. 7:587983. doi: 10.3389/fnut.2020.587983

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: cognition, nutrition, simple RT, choice RT, vigilance, rapid visual information processing, brain, flavonoids

Citation: Hepsomali P, Greyling A, Scholey A and Vauzour D (2021) Acute Effects of Polyphenols on Human Attentional Processes: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Front. Neurosci. 15:678769. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2021.678769

Received: 10 March 2021; Accepted: 26 April 2021;
Published: 24 May 2021.

Edited by:

Zoltan Sarnyai, James Cook University, Australia

Reviewed by:

Ondine Van De Rest, Decision Research, United States
Reza Rastmanesh, Independent Researcher, Tehran, Iran

Copyright © 2021 Hepsomali, Greyling, Scholey and Vauzour. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Piril Hepsomali, p.hepsomali@roehampton.ac.uk; Arno Greyling, arno.greyling@unilever.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.