Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurosci., 03 December 2020
Sec. Neural Technology

Reduced Information Transmission of Medial Prefrontal Cortex to Basolateral Amygdala Inhibits Exploratory Behavior in Depressed Rats

  • School of Biomedical Engineering and Technology, Tianjin Medical University, Tianjin, China

Depression is a mental and neurological disease that reduces the desire for exploration. Dysregulation of the information transmission between medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) and basolateral amygdala (BLA) is associated with depression. However, which direction of information transmission (mPFC-BLA or BLA-mPFC) related to the decline of exploratory interests in depression is unclear. Therefore, it is important to determine what specific changes occur in mPFC and BLA information transmission in depressed rats during exploratory behavior. In the present study, local field potentials (LFPs) were recorded via multi-electrodes implanted in the mPFC and BLA for the control and depression groups of rats when they were exploring in an open field. The theta band was determined to be the characteristic band of exploratory behavior. The direct transfer function (DTF) was used to calculate the mPFC and BLA bidirectional information flow (IF) to measure information transmission. Compared with the control group, the theta IF of mPFC-BLA in the depression group was significantly reduced, and there was no significant difference in theta IF of BLA-mPFC between the two groups. Our results indicated that the reduction of mPFC-BLA information transmission can inhibit the exploratory behavior of depressed rats.

Introduction

The lifetime prevalence (∼ 17%) and financial burden ($ 100 billion per year) associated with depression make it one of the most common and debilitating mental and neurological diseases worldwide (Ashtari et al., 1999; Pezawas et al., 2005). Depression is characterized by several psychophysiological symptoms, such as feelings of hopelessness, low energy and decreased interest in exploration (Nestler et al., 2002).

The medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) is involved in the expression and regulation of depression-like behaviors (Nikulina et al., 2004). Neurons in the mPFC are sensitive to stress, and as a central hub, they can shape activities in a distributed network of output structures, including regulating behaviors and autonomous responses to stress (Liston et al., 2006; Czeh et al., 2007; Dias-Ferreira et al., 2009). Rodent exposure to stress can lead to mPFC dendritic spines loss and decreased activity (Covington et al., 2010). The amygdala is commonly involved in regulating negative memory and stimuli (Hendler et al., 2003), and the basolateral amygdala (BLA) is thought to be related to fear, anxiety and depression (Tye et al., 2011; Felix-Ortiz et al., 2013). Repeated stress will increase the firing rate of BLA neurons in rats, and increased membrane excitability can be observed between the main neurons of the BLA (Padival et al., 2015). The projection from the mPFC to the BLA forms a circuit that involves many cognitive and emotional processes. Although the mPFC sends excitability prediction signals to the BLA, in vivo electrical stimulation of the mPFC in rats have been shown to cause suppression of BLA output (Shansky et al., 2009). Under normal conditions, the mPFC exerts inhibitory control on the activity of the BLA, limiting its output and thereby preventing negative expression of emotions (Quirk et al., 2003; Motzkin et al., 2015). Electrophysiological records in the brains of depressed mice indicate that the glutamate projection synapses between the mPFC and BLA are reduced (McGarry and Carter, 2017). These findings suggest that the directional interaction between mPFC and BLA may regulate BLA activity, and the appearance of depression-like behaviors may be related to the reduction in directional activity between mPFC and BLA. Here, we employ a novel informatics approach to test this hypothesis.

Exploration is one of the basic behavioral responses of animals in the process of adapting to the environment. It can be used to obtain information about the surrounding environment and can optimize the escaping from predators (Alvarez and Alvarez, 2008; Magalhaes et al., 2008). The mPFC and BLA also play crucial roles in the exploration behavior of animals. Research indicates that the theta power of the mPFC and BLA in the safe state is higher than that in the fear state (Adhikari et al., 2010). Evidence shows that the mPFC and BLA use theta oscillations to communicate during and after fear regulation (Likhtik et al., 2014). There is evidence that the mPFC and BLA are more synchronized in the theta band in animals that distinguish between aversion and safety (Adhikari et al., 2010). Exploratory activity declines in rodents exposed to chronic stress, and theta power and synchronization of the mPFC and BLA decrease (Jacinto et al., 2013). Given the essential role of the mPFC and BLA in depression and exploratory behavior, it is important to identify what specific changes occur in mPFC and BLA information transmission in depressed rats during exploratory behavior.

To address this issue, we recorded the local field potentials (LFPs) from the mPFC and BLA in freely moving rats in the open field test (OFT). LFP has a high spatiotemporal resolution and is a powerful tool for analyzing networks in the brain structure (Marceglia et al., 2007). The OFT is commonly used to detect rodent exploratory behavior and motor activity; it can quantitatively and qualitatively detect animal activity and is widely used to detect animal models of depression (Belovicova et al., 2017). The bidirectional information flow (IF) between the mPFC and BLA network was calculated during the exploratory behavior. Furthermore, the difference in information transmission in the mPFC and BLA network between the two groups of rats was quantitatively compared.

Materials and Methods

All experimental procedures described here were conducted in accordance with the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals and approved by the Tianjin Medical University Animal Care and Use Committee.

Subjects and Chronic Unpredictable Mild Stress (CUMS) Procedure

The experimental animals were 12 specific pathogen-free (SPF) male Sprague Dawley (SD) rats, 10–12 weeks, weighing 300–350 g, and randomly divided into the control and depression groups, with 6 animals in each group. The animals were provided by the Experimental Animal Center of the Chinese Academy of Military Medical Sciences, kept in the experimental animal department of Tianjin Medical University with 12 h of light (started at 7:00 am), 12 h of darkness, a room temperature of 25 ± 2°C and humidity ranging from 50 to 55%. Rats were divided into groups with 4 rats per cage. Food and water were available ad libitum. Based on the experimental methods of Willner (1998), the CUMS procedure was conducted on the depression group for 21 days. Six kinds of stressors, including ice water swimming (4°C, 5 min), water and food deprivation (24 h), tail pinch (1 min), reversed light/dark cycle (24 h), cage tilt (12 h), and wet padding (12 h), were applied in random order, with each stressor performed once a week. The control group did not receive any intervention.

We examined CUMS depression in rats by the sucrose preference test (SPT) and forced-swim test (FST). Compared with the control group, the sucrose preference rate of the depression group decreased, and the FST immobility time increased significantly. These results suggested that CUMS-depressed rats were successfully prepared.

Surgery

The surgical procedures were performed according to Bai et al. (2012). The rats were deeply anesthetized with pentobarbital sodium (40 mg/kg, i. p.). The coordinates were determined according to the rat brain atlas in stereotaxic coordinates (Paxinos and Watson, 2007). Then two 16-channel microelectrode arrays (arranged in 2 × 8 configuration, 50 μm diameter nickel-chromium wires with formvar insulation (California Fine Wire Co., Grover Beach, CA, United States), 200 μm inter-electrode spacing, impedance <1 MΩ tested in Omega-Tip-Z (World Precision Instruments, Sarasota, FL, United States) were, respectively, implanted into mPFC (2.5–4.5 mm anterior to bregma, 0.2–1.0 mm lateral to midline, 2.5–3.0 mm from dura) and BLA (1.56–3.36 mm posterior to bregma, 4.4–5.2 mm lateral to midline, 8.6–9.1 mm from dura) (Supplementary Figure 1) of rat. Iodophor was used to prevent infection after surgery. Rats were allowed to recover from surgery for 1 week, then the electrophysiological signals were recorded from mPFC and BLA during OFT.

Exploratory Behavior in Open Field

The open field test (OFT) is based on the rodent locomotion that drove by exploratory (curiosity) and they have a tendency to spend the majority of their time in close proximity to the walls (Hall and Ballachey, 1932; Hall, 1934). As shown in Figure 1, the open field we used in this research was square (100 cm × 100 cm), a square area (60 cm × 60 cm) in the center of the open field was defined as “central zone,” the area between the wall and “central zone” was defined as “peripheral zone.” Testing was recorded by a video tracking system (Sony, Japan) for 5 min under normal illumination. The moment when the four paws of the rat entering the central zone is defined as the reference point (RP, “0 s”), and the entry of the rats from the peripheral zone into the central zone during OFT was defined as a trial of exploratory behavior (including 5 s in the peripheral zone and 5 s in the central zone). EthoVision XT 8.5 software (Noldus, Netherlands) was used to detect the position of the rat in the open field. After the test, the open field was thoroughly cleaned with 75% alcohol to eliminate odors. Behavioral parameters, including the movement distance, the rearing times, the entries of the central zone and the time spent in the central zone, were measured.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Schematic representation of exploratory behavior in OFT. The black square refers to the four walls of the open field, and the red square in the middle is the central area (only for video analysis). The area between the two squares is the peripheral zone. The blue one-way arrow indicates that the rat moves from the peripheral to the central zone, and the black two-way arrow is used to describe the size of the open field.

Neurophysiological Recording

The electrophysiological signals were collected by a neurophysiological recording system (Plexon, United States), and the rat’s position was simultaneously recorded by the overhead camera. The LFPs (16 channels each in the mPFC and BLA) were amplified (gain: 5000), bandpass filtered (0.3–120 Hz) and acquired at a frequency of 2 kHz (Tiaotiao et al., 2014). A 50-Hz notch filter was applied to eliminate noise from the main power line and correct the baseline drift by polynomial fitting. Then, the delta (1–4 Hz), theta (4–12 Hz), beta (12–30 Hz), low-gamma (30–70 Hz) and high-gamma (70–120 Hz) bands were obtained by bandpass filtering. To ensure the availability of data, the recorded data that the rat stayed in the central zone for less than 5 s were excluded from analysis.

Information Transmission Analysis

The IF was estimated according to the directed transform function (DTF) to measure information transmission. The DTF was defined in the framework of multiple auto-regressive (MVAR) models and was used to analyze causality in the frequency domain (Seth, 2010). Based on the multivariate Granger causality connectivity analysis, the nodes of the causal network were defined as electrodes, and the edges of the causal network were elements of the DTF matrix, which were used to describe the network connectivity of the LFP. The DTF value was estimated in a window with a width of 1000 ms and an interval of 250 ms throughout the task.

According to the MVAR model, a multichannel LFPs can be represented as a data vector X of N source signals:

X ( t ) = [ x 1 ( t ) , x 2 ( t ) , , x N ( t ) ] T (1)

The MVAR model can then be expressed as:

X ( t ) = n = 1 p A n X ( t - n ) + E ( t ) (2)

Where p is the model order, calculated by the Bayesian information criterion. An is the coefficient matrix of the MVAR model. E(t) is the vector of multivariate zero mean uncorrelated white noise at time t.

X ( f ) = A - 1 ( f ) E ( f ) = H ( f ) E ( f ) (3)
H ( f ) = A - 1 ( f ) = ( i = 0 p A ( i ) e - j 2 π f i Δ t ) (4)

f denotes a specific frequency, H(f) is the transfer function matrix, and A(0) = −I, I is an identity matrix.

γ i j ( f ) = | H ( f ) | 2 m = 1 k | H i m ( f ) | 2 (5)

γij(f) represents the ratio between the effect from channel j to channel i and the combined effect from all other nodes to node j, k is the number of nodes, and H is the transfer matrix of the system.

D T F = 1 N ( N - 1 ) i j G D T F i j (6)

DTFij refers to the connection strength from channel i to channel j. N is the number of channels. G is the set of channels in the causal network.

The mPFC-BLA causal network was constructed based on the connection strength matrix, and the bidirectional information flow (IF) from mPFC to BLA and BLA to mPFC was calculated, respectively:

I F P B = 1 N P × N B i N B j N P D T F i j (7)
I F B P = 1 N B × N P i N P j N B D T F j i (8)

IFB→P refers to the IF from the mPFC to the BLA, and IFB→P indicates the IF from the BLA to the mPFC. NP and NB are the number of channels in the mPFC and BLA, respectively (Xia et al., 2019).

Histology

At the completion of all behavioral and electrophysiological studies, the rats were deeply anesthetized, then transcardially fused with phosphate-buffered saline (PBS) and 4% paraformaldehyde solution. The brain was dissected and cryoprotected (30% sucrose in PBS, 4°C). Brain slices (150 μm thick) were obtained on a vibratome (Vibratome, United States), and tracks caused by electrode tips were observed on an optical microscope (Olympus, Japan). The recorded sites were verified histologically by referring to the rat brain atlas (Paxinos and Watson, 2007).

Statistical Analysis

We collected a total of 240 trials (120 trials with the control group and 120 trials with the depression group; the distribution of trials in control and depression groups for per subject were shown in Supplementary Table 1). SPSS 22.0 was used for statistical analysis. The data in the text and figures are expressed as the mean ± SEM. The data involved in this study were all normally distributed, and t-test (paired sample t-test/independent sample t-test) was used to assess significant differences. Specifically, comparisons of the behavioral parameters of the two groups of rats in the OFT were performed using independent sample t-tests. The average theta band DTF/IF in the peripheral and central zone were compared by using paired sample t-test. The average theta band DTF/IF in the control and depression groups were compared by using independent sample t-test. p-values are marked statistically significant as follows: ns p > 0.05 no significance, p < 0.05, ∗∗p < 0.01, ∗∗∗p < 0.001.

Results

Behavioral Performance in OFT

During the 5-min open field test, the behavioral parameters in the two groups of rats showed significant differences, and the average movement distance (t = 4.423, p < 0.01; Figure 2A), rearing times (t = 7.400, p < 0.001, Figure 2B), entries of the central zone (t = 4.676, p < 0.01; Figure 2C) and time spent in the central zone (t = 5.742, p < 0.001; Figure 2D) of the depression group were significantly lower than those of the control group.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Comparison of behavioral performance in the OFT between the control and depression groups. (A) Comparison of the averaged movement distance. (B) Comparison of the averaged rearing times. (C) Comparison of the averaged entries of the central zone. (D) Comparison of the averaged time spent in the central zone. Error bars indicate SEM. Independent sample t-test, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

Information Transmission Between the mPFC and BLA

Connection Strength of the mPFC and BLA LFP Network

To compare mPFC and BLA information transmission between the control group and the depression group during exploratory behavior, the connection strength of mPFC and BLA LFP networks in two groups were calculated. We recorded mPFC and BLA (16 channels each) LFPs in the two groups during the OFT. LFPs from the mPFC and BLA were used to calculate and obtain the DTF of different frequencies of the mPFC-BLA network in the peripheral and central zone (Figures 3A,B). The results showed that the theta band (4–12 Hz) DTF of the mPFC and BLA changed most sharply in the peripheral and central zone (Control: Delta: t = 2.953, p < 0.01; Theta: t = 6.341, p < 0.001; Beta: t = 3.662, p < 0.05; Low-gamma: t = 3.369, p < 0.05; High-gamma: t = 3.522, p < 0.05; Figure 3C) (Depression: Delta: t = 3.126, p < 0.01; Theta: t = 7.255, p < 0.001; Beta: t = 2.912, p < 0.05; Low-gamma: t = 3.431, p < 0.05; High-gamma: t = 3.563, p < 0.05; Figure 3D) in the open field, and the theta band in the mPFC and BLA was determined to be the characteristic band of exploratory behavior.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Direct transfer function (DTF) at different frequencies of the mPFC-BLA network in the peripheral and central zone. (A) DTF in the peripheral and central zone of the control group. (B) DTF in the peripheral and central zone of the depression group. The bandwidth calculated for DTF ranged from 1 to 120 Hz. (C) DTF of the control group in different frequency bands in the peripheral and central zone. (D) DTF of the depression group in different frequency bands in the peripheral and central zone. Error bars indicate SEM. Paired sample t-test, *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

Then, we analyzed the theta band connection strength of the mPFC and BLA during exploratory behavior. As shown in Figure 4A, DTF in the theta band declined before rats left the peripheral zone. The theta band DTF in the peripheral zone was significantly higher than that in the central zone (Control-PFC: t = 3.083, p < 0.01; Depression-PFC: t = 3.235, p < 0.01; Control-BLA: t = 3.151, p < 0.01; Depression-BLA: t = 5.745, p < 0.001; Figure 4B), suggesting that the theta band connection strength of mPFC and BLA was stronger in the peripheral zone than in the central zone. The average theta DTF of mPFC in depression group decreased significantly compared with the control group (Peripheral zone: t = 6.446, p < 0.001; Central zone: t = 6.628, p < 0.001; Total: t = 6.401, p < 0.001; Figures 5A–C), but the average theta DTF of BLA in the depression group increased significantly in both the peripheral and central zones (Peripheral zone: t = 4.569, p < 0.01; Central zone: t = 4.073, p < 0.01; Total: t = 4.358, p < 0.01; Figures 5D–F). This indicates that the difference in the theta band connection strength of the mPFC and BLA between the control group and the depression group may be related to the abnormal information transmission between the mPFC and BLA.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Direct transfer function (DTF) in the mPFC and BLA of the control and depression groups in the peripheral and central zones. (A) Averaged DTF in the theta band from the peripheral to the central zone. The red line represents the RP. (B) Comparison of averaged theta band DTF in peripheral and central zone. Error bars indicate SEM. Paired sample t-test, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Comparison of the average theta band DTF of the mPFC and BLA in exploratory behavior between the control and depression groups. (A) Comparison of the average theta DTF of the mPFC in the peripheral zone. (B) Comparison of the average theta band DTF of the mPFC in the central zone. (C) Comparison of the average theta band DTF of the mPFC from the peripheral to the central zone. (D) Comparison of the average theta band DTF of the BLA in the peripheral zone. (E) Comparison of the average theta band DTF of the BLA in the central zone. (F) Comparison of the average theta band DTF of the BLA from the peripheral to the central zone. Error bars indicate SEM. Independent sample t-test, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

Abnormal mPFC-BLA Information Transmission in Depression

The IF was estimated based on the DTF to measure mPFC and BLA information transmission. The bidirectional IF in the theta band of the mPFC and BLA rose to a peak in the peripheral zone and then gradually decreased (Figure 6A). The average IF of the mPFC-BLA and BLA-mPFC in the theta band in the peripheral zone was significantly higher than that in the central zone (Control-PFC-BLA: t = 6.271; p < 0.001; Depression-PFC-BLA: t = 5.991, p < 0.001; Control-BLA-PFC: t = 4.754, p < 0.01; Depression-BLA-PFC: t = 4.498, p < 0.01; Figure 6B). The average theta IF of the mPFC-BLA in the depression group was significantly lower than that in the control group (Peripheral zone: t = 5.011, p < 0.001; Central zone: t = 4.545, p < 0.001; Total: t = 4.762, p < 0.001; Figures 7A–C), while the average theta IF of the BLA-mPFC in the two groups was not significantly different (Peripheral zone: t = 0.759, p > 0.05; Central zone: t = 0.333, p > 0.05; Total: t = 0.589, p > 0.05; Figures 7D–F). This indicates that mPFC-BLA information transmission is necessary for exploratory behavior, and the reduction of mPFC-BLA information transmission may inhibit exploratory behavior in depressed rats. The BLA-mPFC is not necessary for the exploratory behavior of rats.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Information flow (IF) of the mPFC-BLA and BLA-mPFC between the control and depression groups in the peripheral and central zones. (A) Averaged IF in the theta band from the peripheral zone to the central zone. The red line represents the RP. (B) Comparison of averaged theta IF in the peripheral and central zone. Error bars indicate SEM. Paired sample t-test, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Comparison of the average theta IF of the mPFC-BLA and BLA-mPFC in exploratory behaviors between the control and depression groups. (A) Comparison of the average theta IF of mPFC-BLA in the peripheral zone. (B) Comparison of the average theta IF of the mPFC-BLA in the central zone. (C) Comparison of the average theta IF of the mPFC-BLA from the peripheral to the central zone. (D) Comparison of the average theta IF of the BLA-mPFC in the peripheral zone. (E) Comparison of the average theta IF of the BLA-mPFC in the central zone. (F) Comparison of the average theta IF of the BLA-mPFC from the peripheral to the central zone. Error bars indicate SEM. Independent sample t-test, ns p > 0.05 no significance, ***p < 0.001.

Histology

Extracellular electrophysiological recordings were performed from the mPFC and BLA of rats. Figure 8 shows representative histological sections of the recording sites (the scar caused by the electrode tip) of the mPFC and BLA.

FIGURE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 8. Histological verification of the recording sites in the mPFC and BLA. The superimposed schematics show the coronal brain sections at 3.24 mm anterior and 2.28 mm posterior to the bregma. The red square refers to the track of the electrode tips. (A) The track of the recording electrode in the mPFC. (B) The track of the recording electrode in BLA.

Correlation Between DTF/IF and Movement Speed

To detect whether the movement speed of rats affects DTF/IF, the Pearson Correlation Coefficient (r) was calculated between the movement speed and each animal’s DTF/IF, and the results showed that the dynamic changes in DTF/IF had no significance with the movement speed of rats (Figures 9, 10).

FIGURE 9
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 9. Correlation between moving speed and DTF in the OFT. (A,B) There is no significant correlation between moving speed and DTF of the mPFC in the (A) control and (B) depression group. (C,D) There is no significant correlation between moving speed and DTF of the BLA in the (C) control and (D) depression group.

FIGURE 10
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 10. Correlation between moving speed and IF in the OFT. (A,B) There is no significant correlation between moving speed and IF of the mPFC-BLA in the (A) control and (B) depression group. (C,D) There is no significant correlation between moving speed and IF of the BLA-mPFC in the (C) control and (D) depression group.

Discussion

Our main finding in this study was that the reduction of the theta band information transmission of mPFC-BLA in depressed rats caused a decline in mPFC network connection strength and a rise in BLA network connection strength compared to normal rats, which led to disorders of exploring behavior in depressive rats. It also shows that chronic stress not only leads to depression-like behavior but also inhibits exploratory behavior. Our results were based on the OFT behavioral parameters in two groups of rats, consistent with previous reports (Zhan, 2015; Song et al., 2018). These results quantitatively reveal the mPFC and BLA information transmission and may provide a new perspective for the study of depression.

In most individuals, the circuit of the mPFC-BLA may be specially protected from the harmful effects of stress, but when there is chronic stress, the pathway is easily affected by stress. The neuronal structure of the entire mPFC can be altered by stress, including the anterior cingulate gyrus, lower limbs and forelimb regions (Radley et al., 2004; Izquierdo et al., 2006; Shansky and Morrison, 2009). BLA projection neurons selectively avoid stress-induced remodeling, and this circuit may be prepared for increased input and possible excessive stimulation, which may lead to dysfunction (Dilgen et al., 2013). The mPFC-BLA circuit is essential for depression-like and exploratory behavior. The activation of this circuit in mice by optogenetic technology can reduce the immobility time of the FST and improve exploratory behavior in the OFT but has no effect on the sucrose preference of the SPT (Hare et al., 2019). According to the connection type between the BLA and mPFC, the BLA projection neurons are divided into two clusters: one cluster is interconnected with the mPFC (mPFC ↔ BLA), and the other only receives mPFC input (mPFC → BLA) (Zhang et al., 2019). However, chronic stress selectively shifts the mPFC-driven excitatory-inhibitory balance toward excitement in the mPFC → BLA, rather than excitement in the mPFC ↔ BLA, which is due to presynaptic glutamate release selectively increasing the results of the mPFC → BLA. Our results quantify this conclusion and explore the influence of chronic stress on mPFC-PrL and BLA during exploratory behaviors.

BLA projects glutamate into multiple subregions of the mPFC, including the anterior cingulate cortex, anterior marginal cortex, and marginal subcortex. In vivo electrophysiological analysis of this pathway shows that BLA activation can regulate the activity of different subpopulations of mPFC neurons (Pérez-Jaranay and Vives, 1991; Ishikawa and Nakamura, 2003). This circuit is also related to many cognitive and emotional processes, such as the acquisition and elimination of organized fear (Maren and Quirk, 2004). The circuit of the BLA-mPFC is considered to be the basis of emotional and cognitive disorders in diseases such as schizophrenia, depression and drug addiction (Shenton et al., 2001; Drevets, 2003; Bechara, 2005). The BLA-mPFC is also involved in the regulation of anxiety-like behavior and social interaction. Activating this circuit will lead to increased anxiety and social disorders in rodents, and inhibition of this circuit will produce anti-anxiety effects and promote social interaction (Felix-Ortiz et al., 2016).

In addition to the mPFC and BLA, the hippocampus (HPC) is also considered to be a key brain region for depression and exploratory behavior (Voss et al., 2011; Lu et al., 2019). The circuit of the HPC-mPFC originates from the ventral hippocampus (vHPC) CA1 and the lower support, projecting to PrL and MO of the mPFC (Thierry et al., 2000). Research has shown that activation of HPC-PFC can produce antidepressant effects (Carreno et al., 2016). Due to the development of experimental technology, more brain regions and circuits have become potential targets for studying emotion (Camporeze et al., 2018). The dorsal nucleus raphe, globus pallidus and various circuits and networks composed of the above brain regions are considered potential research directions for regulating emotions and cognition (Chang and Grace, 2014).

Taken together, our results indicate that mPFC-BLA information transmission is necessary for exploratory behavior and provide new ideas and potential mechanisms for depression research. Future research will involve more brain regions, more signal processing algorithms, and other behavioral tasks.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets presented in this article are not readily available because the raw/generated data required to reproduce these findings cannot be shared at this time as the data also forms part of an ongoing study. Requests to access the datasets should be directed to XZ.

Ethics Statement

The animal study was reviewed and approved by the Tianjin Medical University Animal Care and Use Committee.

Author Contributions

XZ and TL designed the experiments. CQ, WB, and ZW carried out the experiments and analyzed the data. CQ and XZ wrote the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by the Training Program of School of Biomedical Engineering and Technology of Tianjin Medical University and National Natural Science Foundation of China (61976157 and 61971311).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank Prof. Xin Tian for her helpful comments.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnins.2020.608587/full#supplementary-material

References

Adhikari, A., Topiwala, M. A., and Gordon, J. A. (2010). Synchronized activity between the ventral hippocampus and the medial prefrontal cortex during anxiety. Neuron 65, 257–269. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alvarez, E. O., and Alvarez, P. A. (2008). Motivated exploratory behaviour in the rat: the role of hippocampus and the histaminergic neurotransmission. Behav. Brain Res. 186, 118–125. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2007.07.038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ashtari, M., Greenwald, B., Kramer-Ginsberg, E., Hu, J., Wu, H., Patel, M., et al. (1999). Hippocampal/amygdala volumes in geriatric depression. Psychol. Med. 29, 629–638. doi: 10.1017/S0033291799008405

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bai, W., Liu, T., Yi, H., Li, S., and Tian, X. (2012). Anticipatory activity in rat medial prefrontal cortex during a working memory task. Neurosci. Bull. 28, 693–703. doi: 10.1007/s12264-012-1291-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bechara, A. (2005). Decision making, impulse control and loss of willpower to resist drugs: a neurocognitive perspective. Nat. Neurosci. 8, 1458–1463. doi: 10.1038/nn1584

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belovicova, K., Bogi, E., Csatlosova, K., and Dubovicky, M. (2017). Animal tests for anxiety-like and depression-like behavior in rats. Interdiscip. Toxicol. 10, 40–43. doi: 10.1515/intox-2017-0006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Camporeze, B., Manica, B. A., Bonafé, G. A., Ferreira, J. J. C., Diniz, A. L., de Oliveira, C. T. P., et al. (2018). Optogenetics: the new molecular approach to control functions of neural cells in epilepsy, depression and tumors of the central nervous system. Am. J. Cancer Res. 8, 1900–1918.

Google Scholar

Carreno, F. R., Donegan, J. J., Boley, A. M., Shah, A., DeGuzman, M., Frazer, A., et al. (2016). Activation of a ventral hippocampus-medial prefrontal cortex pathway is both necessary and sufficient for an antidepressant response to ketamine. Mol. Psychiatry 21, 1298–1308. doi: 10.1038/mp.2015.176

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, C. H., and Grace, A. A. (2014). Amygdala-ventral pallidum pathway decreases dopamine activity after chronic mild stress in rats. Biol. Psychiatry 76, 223–230. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.09.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Covington, H. E. III, Lobo, M. K., Maze, I., Vialou, V., Hyman, J. M., Zaman, S., et al. (2010). Antidepressant effect of optogenetic stimulation of the medial prefrontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 30, 16082–16090. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1731-10.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Czeh, B., Muller-Keuker, J. I., Rygula, R., Abumaria, N., Hiemke, C., Domenici, E., et al. (2007). Chronic social stress inhibits cell proliferation in the adult medial prefrontal cortex: hemispheric asymmetry and reversal by fluoxetine treatment. Neuropsychopharmacology 32, 1490–1503. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301275

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dias-Ferreira, E., Sousa, J. C., Melo, I., Morgado, P., Mesquita, A. R., Cerqueira, J. J., et al. (2009). Chronic stress causes frontostriatal reorganization and affects decision-making. Science 325, 621–625. doi: 10.1126/science.1171203

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dilgen, J., Tejeda, H. A., and O’Donnell, P. (2013). Amygdala inputs drive feedforward inhibition in the medial prefrontal cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 110, 221–229. doi: 10.1152/jn.00531.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Drevets, W. (2003). Neuroimaging abnormalities in the amygdala in mood disorders. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 985, 420–444. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2003.tb07098.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Felix-Ortiz, A. C., Beyeler, A., Seo, C., Leppla, C. A., Wildes, C. P., and Tye, K. M. (2013). BLA to vHPC inputs modulate anxiety-related behaviors. Neuron 79, 658–664. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.06.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Felix-Ortiz, A. C., Burgos-Robles, A., Bhagat, N. D., Leppla, C. A., and Tye, K. M. (2016). Bidirectional modulation of anxiety-related and social behaviors by amygdala projections to the medial prefrontal cortex. Neuroscience 321, 197–209. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.07.041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hall, C. S. (1934). Drive and emotionality: factors associated with adjustment in the rat. J. Comp. Psychol. 17, 89–108. doi: 10.1037/h0073676

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hall, C. S., and Ballachey, E. L. (1932). A Study of the Rat’s Behavior in a Field. A Contribution to Method in Comparative Psychology, Vol. 6. Berkeley, CA: University of California Publications in Psychology, 1–12.

Google Scholar

Hare, B. D., Shinohara, R., Liu, R. J., Pothula, S., DiLeone, R. J., and Duman, R. S. (2019). Optogenetic stimulation of medial prefrontal cortex Drd1 neurons produces rapid and long-lasting antidepressant effects. Nat. Commun. 10:223. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-08168-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hendler, T., Rotshtein, P., Yeshurun, Y., Weizmann, T., Kahn, I., Ben-Bashat, D., et al. (2003). Sensing the invisible: differential sensitivity of visual cortex and amygdala to traumatic context. Neuroimage 19, 587–600. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00141-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ishikawa, A., and Nakamura, S. (2003). Convergence and interaction of hippocampal and amygdalar projections within the prefrontal cortex in the rat. J. Neurosci. 23, 9987–9995. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-31-09987.2003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Izquierdo, A., Wellman, C., and Holmes, A. (2006). Brief uncontrollable stress causes dendritic retraction in infralimbic cortex and resistance to fear extinction in mice. J. Neurosci. 26, 5733–5738. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0474-06.2006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jacinto, L. R., Reis, J. S., Dias, N. S., Cerqueira, J. J., Correia, J. H., and Sousa, N. (2013). Stress affects theta activity in limbic networks and impairs novelty-induced exploration and familiarization. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 7:127. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00127

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Likhtik, E., Stujenske, J. M., Topiwala, M. A., Harris, A. Z., and Gordon, J. A. (2014). Prefrontal entrainment of amygdala activity signals safety in learned fear and innate anxiety. Nat. Neurosci. 17, 106–113. doi: 10.1038/nn.3582

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liston, C., Miller, M. M., Goldwater, D. S., Radley, J. J., Rocher, A. B., Hof, P. R., et al. (2006). Stress-induced alterations in prefrontal cortical dendritic morphology predict selective impairments in perceptual attentional set-shifting. J. Neurosci. 26, 7870–7874. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1184-06.2006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lu, Q., Mouri, A., Yang, Y., Kunisawa, K., Teshigawara, T., Hirakawa, M., et al. (2019). Chronic unpredictable mild stress-induced behavioral changes are coupled with dopaminergic hyperfunction and serotonergic hypofunction in mouse models of depression. Behav. Brain Res. 372:112053. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2019.112053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Magalhaes, A., Melo, P., Alves, C. J., Tavares, M. A., de Sousa, L., and Summavielle, T. (2008). Exploratory behavior in rats postnatally exposed to cocaine and housed in an enriched environment. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1139, 358–365. doi: 10.1196/annals.1432.046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marceglia, S., Rossi, L., Foffani, G., Bianchi, A., Cerutti, S., and Priori, A. (2007). Basal ganglia local field potentials: applications in the development of new deep brain stimulation devices for movement disorders. Expert Rev. Med. Devices 4, 605–614. doi: 10.1586/17434440.4.5.605

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maren, S., and Quirk, G. (2004). Neuronal signalling of fear memory. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 5, 844–852. doi: 10.1038/nrn1535

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McGarry, L. M., and Carter, A. G. (2017). Prefrontal cortex drives distinct projection neurons in the basolateral amygdala. Cell Rep. 21, 1426–1433. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2017.10.046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Motzkin, J. C., Philippi, C. L., Wolf, R. C., Baskaya, M. K., and Koenigs, M. (2015). Ventromedial prefrontal cortex is critical for the regulation of amygdala activity in humans. Biol. Psychiatry 77, 276–284. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.02.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nestler, E. J., Barrot, M., DiLeone, R. J., Eisch, A. J., Gold, S. J., and Monteggia, L. M. (2002). Neurobiology of depression. Neuron 34, 13–25. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00653-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nikulina, E. M., Covington, H. E. III, Ganschow, L., Hammer, R. P. Jr., and Miczek, K. A. (2004). Long-term behavioral and neuronal cross-sensitization to amphetamine induced by repeated brief social defeat stress: fos in the ventral tegmental area and amygdala. Neuroscience 123, 857–865. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2003.10.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Padival, M. A., Blume, S. R., Vantrease, J. E., and Rosenkranz, J. A. (2015). Qualitatively different effect of repeated stress during adolescence on principal neuron morphology across lateral and basal nuclei of the rat amygdala. Neuroscience 291, 128–145. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.02.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paxinos, G., and Watson, C. (2007). The Rat Brain Stereotaxic Co-Ordinates. Cambridge, MA: Academic Press.

Google Scholar

Pérez-Jaranay, J. M., and Vives, F. (1991). Electrophysiological study of the response of medial prefrontal cortex neurons to stimulation of the basolateral nucleus of the amygdala in the rat. Brain Res. 564, 97–101. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(91)91357-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pezawas, L., Meyer-Lindenberg, A., Drabant, E. M., Verchinski, B. A., Munoz, K. E., Kolachana, B. S., et al. (2005). 5-HTTLPR polymorphism impacts human cingulate-amygdala interactions: a genetic susceptibility mechanism for depression. Nat. Neurosci. 8, 828–834. doi: 10.1038/nn1463

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Quirk, G. J., Likhtik, E., Pelletier, J. G., and Paré, D. (2003). Stimulation of medial prefrontal cortex decreases the responsiveness of central amygdala output neurons. J. Neurosci. 23:8800. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-25-08800.2003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Radley, J., Sisti, H., Hao, J., Rocher, A. B., McCall, T., Hof, P., et al. (2004). Chronic behavioral stress induces apical dendritic reorganization in pyramidal neurons of the medial prefrontal cortex. Neuroscience 125, 1–6. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.01.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seth, A. K. (2010). A MATLAB toolbox for Granger causal connectivity analysis. J. Neurosci. Methods 186, 262–273. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2009.11.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shansky, R. M., Hamo, C., Hof, P. R., McEwen, B. S., and Morrison, J. H. (2009). Stress-induced dendritic remodeling in the prefrontal cortex is circuit specific. Cereb. Cortex 19, 2479–2484. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shansky, R. M., and Morrison, J. H. (2009). Stress-induced dendritic remodeling in the medial prefrontal cortex: effects of circuit, hormones and rest. Brain Res. 1293, 108–113. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.03.062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shenton, M. E., Dickey, C. C., Frumin, M., and McCarley, R. W. (2001). A review of MRI findings in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 49, 1–52. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(01)00163-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Song, Y., Sun, R., Ji, Z., Li, X., Fu, Q., and Ma, S. (2018). Perilla aldehyde attenuates CUMS-induced depressive-like behaviors via regulating TXNIP/TRX/NLRP3 pathway in rats. Life Sci. 206, 117–124. doi: 10.1016/j.lfs.2018.05.038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thierry, A. M., Gioanni, Y., Dégénétais, E., and Glowinski, J. (2000). Hippocampo-prefrontal cortex pathway: anatomical and electrophysiological characteristics. Hippocampus 10, 411–419. doi: 10.1002/1098-1063(2000)10:4<411::aid-hipo7>3.0.co;2-a

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tiaotiao, L., Wenwen, B., Hu, Y., Tao, T., Jing, W., Ju, W., et al. (2014). Functional connectivity in a rat model of Alzheimer’s Disease during a working memory task. Curr. Alzheimer Res. 11, 981–991. doi: 10.2174/1567205011666141107125912

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tye, K. M., Prakash, R., Kim, S. Y., Fenno, L. E., Grosenick, L., Zarabi, H., et al. (2011). Amygdala circuitry mediating reversible and bidirectional control of anxiety. Nature 471, 358–362. doi: 10.1038/nature09820

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Voss, J., Gonsalves, B., Federmeier, K., Tranel, D., and Cohen, N. (2011). Hippocampal brain-network coordination during volitional exploratory behavior enhances learning. Nat. Neurosci. 14, 115–120. doi: 10.1038/nn.2693

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Willner, P. (1998). Validity, reliability and utility of the chronic mild stress model of depression: a 10-year review and evaluation. Psychopharmacology 134, 319–329. doi: 10.1007/s002130050456

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xia, M., Liu, T., Bai, W., Zheng, X., and Tian, X. (2019). Information transmission in HPC-PFC network for spatial working memory in rat. Behav. Brain Res. 356, 170–178. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2018.08.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhan, Y. (2015). Theta frequency prefrontal-hippocampal driving relationship during free exploration in mice. Neuroscience 300, 554–565. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.05.063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, W. H., Liu, W. Z., He, Y., You, W. J., Zhang, J. Y., Xu, H., et al. (2019). Chronic stress causes projection-specific adaptation of amygdala neurons via small-conductance calcium-activated potassium channel downregulation. Biol. Psychiatry 85, 812–828. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2018.12.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: depression, medial prefrontal cortex, basolateral amygdala, local field potentials, information flow

Citation: Qi C, Wang Z, Bai W, Liu T and Zheng X (2020) Reduced Information Transmission of Medial Prefrontal Cortex to Basolateral Amygdala Inhibits Exploratory Behavior in Depressed Rats. Front. Neurosci. 14:608587. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2020.608587

Received: 21 September 2020; Accepted: 16 November 2020;
Published: 03 December 2020.

Edited by:

Hari S. Sharma, Uppsala University, Sweden

Reviewed by:

Ignacio Delgado Martinez, Autonomous University of Barcelona, Spain
Aritra Kundu, University of Texas at Austin, United States

Copyright © 2020 Qi, Wang, Bai, Liu and Zheng. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Tiaotiao Liu, liutt@tmu.edu.cn; Xuyuan Zheng, zhengxuyuan@tmu.edu.cn

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.