Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Neurosci., 11 August 2020
Sec. Sleep and Circadian Rhythms
This article is part of the Research Topic Assessing Sleep Neuroplasticity in Pathological Conditions and in Extreme Environments Through Neurophysiological and Multi-faceted Daily Lifestyle Patterns View all 8 articles

Sleep in Isolated, Confined, and Extreme (ICE): A Review on the Different Factors Affecting Human Sleep in ICE

  • Department of Psychology, Sapienza University of Rome, Rome, Italy

The recently renewed focus on the human exploration of outer space has boosted the interest toward a variety of questions regarding health of astronauts and cosmonauts. Among the others, sleep has traditionally been considered a central issue. To extend the research chances, human sleep alterations have been investigated in several analog environments, called ICEs (Isolated, Confined, and Extreme). ICEs share different features with the spaceflight itself and have been implemented in natural facilities and artificial simulations. The current paper presents a systematic review of research findings on sleep disturbances in ICEs. We looked for evidence from studies run in polar settings (mostly Antarctica) during space missions, Head-Down Bed-Rest protocols, simulations, and in a few ICE-resembling settings such as caves and submarines. Even though research has shown that sleep can be widely affected in ICEs, mostly evidencing general and non-specific changes in REM and SWS sleep, results show a very blurred picture, often with contradictory findings. The variable coexistence of the many factors characterizing the ICE environments (such as isolation and confinement, microgravity, circadian disentrainment, hypoxia, noise levels, and radiations) does not provide a clear indication of what role is played by each factor per se or in association one with each other in determining the pattern observed, and how. Most importantly, a number of methodological limitations contribute immensely to the unclear pattern of results reported in the literature. Among them, small sample sizes, small effect sizes, and large variability among experimental conditions, protocols, and measurements make it difficult to draw hints about whether sleep alterations in ICEs do exist due to the specific environmental characteristics, and which of them plays a major role. More systematic and cross-settings research is needed to address the mechanisms underlying the sleep alterations in ICE environments and possibly develop appropriate countermeasures to be used during long-term space missions.

Introduction

Human factor research has always been a primary topic of interest in the space agencies’ programs. However, the recently renewed focus on the human exploration of outer space has boosted the interest toward a variety of questions regarding safety for astronauts. Traveling to another planet or being involved in a long duration space-mission requires individuals to deal with several exceptional conditions (e.g., microgravity, altered light-dark cycle, danger, autonomy, isolation, and confinement) whose effects on psychophysiological processes are still not well understood. Humans can adjust to these conditions, but in a way that it is still not well-known, making this knowledge strictly necessary for the development of effective countermeasures to support the success of a mission. Consequently, human adaptation in extreme conditions has been investigated in several analog (to space) environments, called ICEs (Isolated, Confined, and Extreme). ICEs share different features with the spaceflight itself and have been implemented in natural facilities and artificial simulations. The advantage of using ICE environments mostly relies upon the reduction in costs and the increase in practicability relative to space missions, and thus the possibility to program long-term experimental projects. However, they vary widely in both the degree and the nature of the challenges they present to individuals. Moreover, it is reasonable to argue that all the challenges can give different outcomes if combined or presented alone. In this picture, deciding which research focuses should be mainly adopted and developing a reliable theoretical framework are of primary importance.

Among others, sleep in space has traditionally been considered a central issue, and great attention is still being paid to it (e.g., Pandi-Perumal and Gonfalone, 2016). Generally, sleep in space has been found to be shorter in duration and worse in quality than on Earth (Evans-Flynn et al., 2016b; Wu et al., 2018), but the causes, the degree, and the functional role of this apparent impairment are still not clear.

Spaceflights and analogs are characterized by the presence of systemic (i.e., having more physical qualities, such as hypoxia, environmental noise, and low physical activity) or processive (i.e., having more psychological features, such as lack of privacy, boredom, fatigue, and interpersonal issues) stressors (Herman and Cullinan, 1997). The effects of stress on sleep have been characterized by neurogenic and somatic responses (Buguet et al., 1998), and often their interaction makes their individual effects quite indistinguishable. Moreover, the environmental circumstances put severe constraints on experimental control: in most studies, the sample sizes are small, and it is often necessary to rely on only subjective measurements.

Unfortunately, the small number of studies in each specific environment, the variety of experimental conditions, and the typically small sample sizes prevent any meta-analytic approach to be adopted. Thus, this review aims at summarizing the findings on human sleep in ICE environments by reducing, as much as possible, the different environments considered to the least common denominators.

This will be done by reviewing the results obtained in the most investigated ICEs (spaceflights, polar environments, HDBR, and Mars analogs) which made use of objective measures of sleep (polysomnography -PSG- and actigraphy). Thereafter, the principal findings of sleep studies investigating the effects of specific factors that individuals in ICEs might encounter will be described. We will consider: circadian disentrainment, as observed in caves, submarines, and forced desynchrony studies; noise, as experienced in Intensive Care Units and laboratory controlled exposures; altered levels of oxygen, such as in high-altitude environments and hypo/hyperbaric chambers; psychosocial stressors.

Finally, the findings reviewed will be discussed, together with the different possible strategies for studying sleep in ICEs, suggesting a possible enlargement of the spectra of relevant experimental conditions. Clarifying the incidence of specific factors rather than investigating the broader effect of the prolonged permanence in ICEs may represent a central issue in this field.

ICE Environments

Isolated, Confined, and Extremes are very complex environments in which the simultaneous presence of various type of stressors may hide their specific effects on humans’ physiology. The knowledge about the factors present in each, and their relative weight, is of paramount importance for the understanding of the outcomes observed in experimental studies. Hereafter, a brief description of the most investigated ICEs is presented.

Spaceflight

Human exploration of the outer space has never been a concrete objective of national governments and international space agencies as nowadays. Even though the previous presence of other space stations (such as the Mir, the Skylab, and the Salyut), the construction of the International Space Station (ISS) has definitely permitted the uninterrupted presence of human beings in space allowing for long-term multinational collaborations in orbit (Kanas and Manzey, 2008). The activities performed on this orbiting laboratory are precious for the astronauts training for manned exploration of space. For example, long-term microgravity may affect individuals’ physiological systems, the free-fall experience may affect sensorimotor abilities, and the multiple daily revolutions around the Earth of the ISS may have a strong impact over astronauts’ circadian rhythmicity (Frantzidis et al., 2019). Together with the presence of other stressors such as constant noise, radiations, and prolonged isolation, the astronauts’ health in orbit is constantly threatened. Increasing knowledges about the direction of changes in space will improve our understanding of how organisms cope with these stressors and prepare safe missions to planets, satellites, asteroids, and outer space.

Polar Environments

Since several decades, Antarctica has been the most productive place were analog studies have been conducted. The presence of dozens of research stations, the uncontaminated and harsh environment, the reproduction of long-term confinement due to the difficulty to reach almost all stations during the winter, and the circannual alteration of seasonality are just some of the features that make it an exceptional context to study for psychophysiological research. A key element in polar environments is the quantity of light to which individuals are exposed during a day. Beyond the 66°33′ parallel (North or South), there is at least one day in which the Sun stay above and one below the horizon for 24 h. Proceeding throughout latitudes, this continuous day/night may spread to six months at 90°.

Moreover, Antarctica is the coldest continent, where temperatures may be lower than −80°C. Furthermore, some stations are located at high elevation, in some cases above 4000 meters above the sea level, making hypoxia another important threatening condition. Besides coping with all these physical factors, crewmembers have to deal with the interpersonal issues that may arise in very small and often heterogeneous groups of individuals (Suedfeld and Weiss, 2000).

The opposite Pole presents similar conditions but is quite more reachable. Indeed, some of the lands above the Arctic Circle are inhabited by civil human communities even though remote research stations are also present in the harshest areas, such as in Nunavut (Canada), Greenland, Svalbard Islands, and in Yakutia (Russia).

Head-Down Bed Rest (HDBR)

Space agencies have developed HDBR protocols to investigate on Earth the physiological effects of weightlessness. In these protocols, individuals must rest for days, weeks, or months on a tilted-down bed (usually 6°) to reproduce the adaptation of human bodies as in space. HDBR protocols impose reduced physical activity and body fluid shift, possibly inducing adverse biomedical effects such as bone demineralization, cardio-vascular deconditioning, and muscle wasting, typical conditions experienced under exposure to microgravity.

Mars Analogs

Due to the programmed travel to Mars, several research projects have been implemented by space agencies as real mission simulation on Earth. Among these, the Mars-500 experiment has been one of the most productive.

Mars-500 has been a precious program to study sleep in a controlled condition of prolonged confinement. The experiment consisted of a pilot study (105 days) and a main study (520 days) in which an international crew of 6 men lived and worked inside an isolation facility located at the Institute of Biomedical Problems (IBMP, Moscow). The facility consisted of four hermetically sealed habitat modules, plus one external module simulating the Martian surface. During both the studies, crewmembers participated in several experiments wherein sleep was also investigated.

Up to now, other important simulation studies come from two NASA stations: the Mars Arctic Research Station in the Canadian Arctic and the Mars Desert Research Station in Utah.

Other ICEs

Several other ICEs exist on Earth, such as caves, submarines, and dry immersions, even though studies conducted in them are still very few. For instance, despite the early interest for circadian investigation in caves and subterranean environments, these contexts have just been recently included among the space analogs (Strapazzon et al., 2014). Together with submarines, caves are an example of exceptional environments wherein a circadian misalignment might appear due to the lack of sunlight exposure. In addition, caves and submarines provide total isolation from the outer world: individuals can’t use any natural time-cue and are forced to interact with few individuals in a restricted environment. For this reason, NASA’s project “NEEMO” aims at investigating underwater environments as space-analogs. In dry immersions, instead, volunteers stay in a sort of bathtub for several days in suspension to simulate the floating conditions of spaceflight.

Widening the spectra of ICEs is really important to make adequate comparisons between conditions. Specific factors must be analyzed alone and together with the others to investigate their peculiar and mixed effect on human health.

Literature Selection

A literature search for studies investigating sleep in ICE environments was conducted in the Scopus and PubMed databases and in the NASA Technical Report Server using the keywords sleep followed by ICE environment, extreme environment, spaceflight, Antarctic(a), Arctic, polar, head-down bed-rest, HDBR, Mars, and closely related terms. Additional articles were identified from reference lists. Studies that made no use of objective measures (polysomnography and actigraphy) were not included, as well as high-altitude studies in Antarctica (hypoxia has been treated as a separate specific factor).

ICEs and Sleep

The literature selection process resulted in 13 spaceflight, 18 polar, 6 HDBR, and 5 Mars analog studies. They will be reviewed in this paragraph evidencing common findings and limitations.

Short-Term Spaceflights

Astronauts’ sleep in space is threatened by many factors, the most prominent being confinement, space radiation, microgravity, noise, artificial light exposure, workload, and shifts in schedules. Gravity is the most studied as it is known, through animal and human research, to have a role in REM sleep: indeed, it has been proposed that the REM onset is associated with the reduced sensitivity to environmental parameters, such as the gravity force (see Gonfalone, 2016 for a review). Interestingly, research on animals has shown that aquatic mammals exhibit strongly reduced or absent REM sleep, whereas semi-aquatic mammals show a reduced REM sleep when in water, and a REM sleep comparable to most of the terrestrial mammals when out of water (Madan and Jha, 2012b). Those results on animals may help to understand the impact on REM sleep of spaceflight conditions, which have been linked to the presence/absence of gravity (Gonfalone and Jha, 2015).

Several subjective reports indicated that sleep duration and quality (Dijk et al., 2001; Kelly et al., 2005; Barger et al., 2014a) are affected during spaceflight.

In an early PSG study, Gundel et al. (1993) described, in a single astronaut in an 8-days space mission, a reduction of both REM latency and REM amount in the second sleep cycle, whereas SWS was redistributed from the first to the second cycle. This study firstly suggested a possible effect on the temporal organization of sleep rather than on the duration of sleep stages, even though it was conducted on just one astronaut.

Differently, Monk et al. (1998) recorded the sleep EEG activity in 4 astronauts who spent 17 days in space during a shuttle mission. The authors employed two 72h measurement blocks during the flight, from the second day in space (early), and from the 12th day (late); also, there were a pre-flight block in the week before the launch, and a post-flight block two weeks after the return. The pre-flight measure was used as the baseline in only one participant (because of the highly stressful week before the launch), whereas the post-flight measure was used as the baseline for the other three astronauts. The first sleep episode in each block was used as habituation. The authors reported no changes in REM sleep, but reduced sleep duration and slow-wave sleep (SWS) during the spaceflight, compared to the baseline. However, the authors did not run any statistical analyses, given the small sample size.

Dijk et al. (2001) reported in five astronauts during the STS-90 and the STS-95 shuttle missions that in-flight sleep was similar to pre-flight sleep for all the PSG parameters but the latency to REM that was shorter in-flight than pre-flight. However, a marked variability was found once sleep episodes were splitted into three segments, with significant effects mostly concerning the three post-flight recordings. Overall, they found that in the first post-flight session REM latency was markedly reduced compared to pre-flight measurements but returned to the in-flight levels in the second and third post-flight sessions. However, after the return to Earth the wake percentage decreased in the first segment, the SWS percentage decreased in the second segment, and the REM percentage increased in the first and the second segments compared to pre-flight. Even if not significantly, wake percentage decreased in the first segment also during in-flight, suggesting that sleep onset was not disturbed. Hence, the alterations observed in-flight consisted of a general decrease in REM latency, that prolonged to the post-flight sessions, a decrease in SWS, and an increase in wake during the third segment of sleep, compared to pre- and post-flight and to pre-flight only, respectively. Finally, sleep period time was found to be reduced during spaceflight compared to pre- and post- sessions. It must be noted that the very low statistical power in this study make it likely that at least some of the described effects are due to chance only.

Indeed, Chen et al. (2018) did not find any alteration on sleep-wake activity in astronauts, though sleep quality before and during the flight was lower than that after the space flight. They found a longer but less efficient sleep after the spaceflight than in the pre- and in the in-flight periods, and more efficient sleep and shorter sleep latency during the in-flight period compared to pre- and post-measurements.

Summarizing, the most relevant and surprising finding during and after short-term spaceflights was that the overall structure and temporal organization of sleep were not markedly altered (Table 1), besides some small changes. Overall, PSG sleep parameters during early flight, late flight, and post-flight recordings did not show any difference apart from a drastic decrease of REM latency in the first post-flight sleep. As reported elsewhere (Manzey and Lorenz, 1998), adaptation effects rather than stable systematic changes might occur more prominently in astronauts, especially in short-term exposure to microgravity, requiring more investigations.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Overview of studies investigating sleep through objective measures during space-flight missions.

Adaptation effects, indeed, have also been reported in subjective measures: debriefed astronauts reported that the duration of their sleep was shorter on the first and the last day of the mission (Santy et al., 1988). Other subjective reports indicated a trough also after three months compared to the first observation (Monk et al., 2001), even if other evidence reported that sleep quality worsened during the first part compared to the last part of the mission (Stuster, 2010). However, general impairments were not found (Whitmire et al., 2013).

It is not known if the reduced amount of sleep required by astronauts during spaceflight is a temporary effect of spaceflight stressors or a characteristic of the exceptional individuals involved (Evans-Flynn et al., 2016b). Microgravity per se, for instance, may affect sleep measures without impairing the ability of sleep to be restful: an HDBR study found that sleep quality was reduced, but the subjective feeling of refreshment was not (Boschert et al., 2019).

Results from sleep studies conducted during long-term missions or in microgravity analogs (such as HDBR) might be useful to disentangle the acute effects of microgravity from the adaptation/long-term effects of isolation and confinement.

Long-Term Space Missions

The psychophysiological literature aimed at investigating alterations due to the permanence in ICEs has broadly showed that short and long-term effects may coexist. Indeed, two different kind of adaptation processes can be outlined: at the outset, an acute adjustment to the environmental challenges followed by a progressive acclimatization of these first effects and, presumably, by the cumulative development of long-term psychological and physiological consequences. When possible, considering both the processes is essential for the understanding of all the possible outcomes exerted by the permanence in ICEs. Simultaneously, such adjustments might be investigated both pre- and post-permanence, to further disentangle specific changes space-related (such as microgravity) to unspecific adaptation effects.

For instance, due to the different aims that move human beings in space, spaceflights can be of different durations, making possible the distinction between short- and long-term effects on sleep in space, and the relative possible pre- and post- alterations.

Six studies during long-term spaceflights used PSG. Firstly, Frost et al. (1976) observed, during the Skylab mission, an increase/decrease in stage 3 sleep during/after the spaceflight period and a decreased stage 4 post-flight with some variability during the spaceflight. REM sleep increased, and REM latency decreased after the mission. However, the authors suggested that none of the observed changes could be expected to adversely affect performance capability.

Subsequent studies have been mostly conducted on the Russian space station MIR. Gundel et al. (1997) found no significant variations in TST and in sleep efficiency, but a shorter REM latency in the first and increased SWS in the second sleep cycle during in-flight compared to pre-flight baseline on 4 astronauts. However, differences in the time-points when the nights were recorded among the astronauts did not allow to observe possible adaptation effects.

With a different approach, Stoilova et al. (2000) compared short- and long-term astronauts in prolonged space missions, they found a longer SWS latency compared to the baseline and the short-term missions. Subsequently, Stoilova and Jordanova (2005) described a marked REM sleep and SWS decrease in the pre-flight period. During in-flight REM sleep remained low, while SWS increased (also compared to normative values). Both studies, however, remained essentially descriptive without any statistical comparison. Differently, Moldofsky et al. (2000) studied eight astronauts in the MIR space station and observed a reduction of SWS during spaceflight compared to the pre-flight, while Stickgold and Hobson (1999) found a 50% reduction of REM sleep in-flight, together with the decrease of total sleep time and sleep efficiency.

One recent study conducted on the ISS investigated sleep events during wake (Petit et al., 2019). EEG was not recorded during sleep, but, interestingly, they found that the astronauts involved in a docking simulation task presented increased global theta EEG oscillations while in space than on Earth, and that this increment was associated with slower reaction times. It is worth to note that this relationship was found despite the comparable amount of time asleep the night before and time awake before the recordings (even if it was not objectively measured). The investigation of circadian- and homeostatic-related changes in sleep pressure (i.e., subjective and objective measures of sleepiness) during wakefulness appears to have paramount importance for the safety of astronauts’ activities and the development of effective countermeasures.

Actigraphy studies also provided measures of circadian and adaptation changes. The incidence of misaligned sleep has been investigated by Evans-Flynn et al. (2016a) aboard the ISS on 21 astronauts between 2006 and 2011 (missions 14 to 26). They reported that compared to the pre-flight baseline, there were more misaligned sleep episodes relative to endogenous circadian temperature (the authors defined a misaligned sleep episode as the minimum core temperature falling out of the sleep episode) during the spaceflight. Moreover, the authors observed that sleep duration was shorter, and quality was lower during the misaligned than during the aligned sleep episodes. On the other hand, Barger et al. (2014a) compared 64 astronauts who participated in a shuttle mission to 21 astronauts who worked on the ISS. Both groups of astronauts were found to sleep significantly more after their return to Earth than during the pre-flight baseline (up to 3 months before the launch) and the in-flight period.

As for the short-term space studies, also the long-term ones revealed mixed pictures. The most representative findings obtained (Table 1) regard changes in REM and SWS sleep, even if the direction of these effects remains not clear, and the shortening of REM latency during and after the flight, eventually representing a response to adaptation, additive to the circadian adjustment.

Primary analogs on Earth where the additive effects of long-term isolation and circadian adjustment can be investigated are the polar environments. The lack of microgravity (and noise and radiations as well) in these contexts allows for a better understanding of specific and unspecific changes in sleep behavior due to the permanence in ICEs.

Head-Down Bed Rest Protocols

Head-Down Bed Rest is today the gold standard for the investigation on ground of physiological changes due to microgravity.

Five PSG studies investigated sleep changes due to simulated microgravity conditions, though they differ for the duration and the degree of head-down position. In one of them, 11 participants rested one night in the 12° head-down condition (Boschert et al., 2019). The authors observed a significant increase in light sleep and a significant decrease in SWS and REM sleep in HDBR compared to the horizontal control condition. In more prolonged and less tilted protocols, Komada et al. (2006) found in 7 participants, who stayed 6° tilted down for 7 days, a significant decrease in stage 4 sleep in the former half of sleep and an increase in the number of arousals compared to the pre-ambulatory recordings. In a 17-days study, without daylight exposure, 8 subjects showed a marked increase of sleep latency and percentage of night awakenings in the early phase of bedrest compared to baseline (Monk et al., 1997). However, the authors did not find any significant changes in sleep architecture. Instead, Samel and Gander (1995) studied the effects of 11 days of 6° degrees HDBR and the effects of a phase shift and a bright light illumination treatment on sleep. Their results showed that total sleep time and sleep efficiency were lower than pre-shift during the days of phase shift. Moreover, stage 2 sleep significantly decreased (by 20%), and SWS increased during the shift days and after the resynchronization to the new shift. Moreover, the HDBR group slept less and less efficiently than an ambulatory control group and presented a trend to more stage 1 and less stage 4 sleep. Accordingly, Frantzidis et al. (2020) found a degradation in stage 2 sleep in a 6° and 60 days-protocol. Total sleep time was not found to change in an actigraphy recording of 45-days of 6° head-down bed rest (Liang et al., 2014).

Summarizing, also in the case of HDBR studies, results are controversial, mostly because of the difference in protocols, their duration, and the severity of the tilt (Table 2). It is plausible to argue that more severe and acute exposure to head-down sleep (i.e., simulated microgravity) might exert a pattern of effects that can disappear or change when habituation occurs or when the restricted environment becomes uncomfortable.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Overview of Head-Down Bed-Rest studies investigating sleep through objective measures.

Polar Environments

Besides hypoxia in high-altitude Antarctic stations, the alteration of the normal circannual seasonality and the long-term confinement, together with the interpersonal issues related to isolation, are among the major challenges individuals who spend prolonged periods in polar environments face. Unfortunately, the lack of a systematic investigation of seasonal sleep changes through objective measures, in natural non-confined environments, does not permit a clear understanding of the specific role of confinement, and even if a specific role does exist.

In two early polysomnographic (PSG) studies with similar designs investigating an Antarctic crew for 9 and 8 months, respectively, opposite results were found. Paterson (1975), in a study carried out at the Halley station with 10 participants and monthly sessions, found a significant reduction of Slow Wave Sleep (SWS) in winter (August) compared to the first month (April). This decrease lasted until the end of the study, except for the September session. Also, stage-2 sleep, summer Total Sleep Time, and REM sleep were reported to increase, albeit without any supporting analysis. Comparing their results to a previous study with similar findings at the high-altitude South Pole station (Natani et al., 1970), Paterson suggested the incidence of altered light conditions as a major cause for SWS decrement. However, no significant changes in sleep efficiency were highlighted in a subsequent study (Buguet et al., 1987) at the Dumont D’Urville station in 8 (of the 28) crewmembers with usual good sleeping habits; indeed, the results showed an opposite trend: stage 3-4 sleep increased, while stage 1-2 sleep decreased. Buguet et al. (1987) argued that the crewmembers were physically active outside during the winter (whereas Paterson (1975) reported that the physical exercise was greater during the summer) and that acclimation to cold might have played a role. However, the two studies did not include any baseline measurements. The only one-year study (Bhattacharyya et al., 2008) recorded 3 consecutive nights per month for a total of 13 time-points (1 pre-departure) with six male crewmembers at the Indian Maitri station. Compared to the pre-departure baseline, total sleep time (TST), sleep efficiency (SE), and stages 3-4 decreased, whereas wake after sleep onset (WASO), and stage 1-2 increased in Antarctica. These effects appeared almost at all months, except for the November session, when the effects on stage 1-2 and 3-4 were opposed compared to the general trend (i.e., stages 1-2 decreased and stages 3-4 increased compared to the baseline) and recovered toward the end of the expedition. Further seasonal effects were observed in the winter months when the changes of the PSG measures were found to be more consistent, and REM sleep was also found to increase compared to the baseline. Differently, sleep latency exhibits a specific lengthening in January (i.e., summer, but also the first month in Antarctica) and in April.

Seasonal changes in sleep onset and sleep duration have also been reported through actigraphy.

In a large study conducted at the German Neumayer station, on the overwintering crews from 2008 to 2014 for a total of 63 participants (Steinach et al., 2016), both the permanence in confinement and the circannual photoperiod effect showed different effects on sleep-wake activity, depending on gender. Indeed, while changes in sleep efficiency fitted a linear and a quadratic trend, time in bed for all the crewmembers and women alone, sleep onset for all the crewmembers and men alone, and sleep offset for all the crewmembers and also for men and women separately, varied according to seasonal changes, fitting a quadratic trend with maxima in mid-winter. Differently, sleep time significantly decreased linearly throughout the year for all participants, while the number of arousals per night increased for all the crewmembers and women only, suggesting a cumulative effect of the permanence in Antarctica. Sleep duration was found to decrease significantly during midwinter on 13 crewmembers at the Argentinian Belgrano station (Folgueira et al., 2019) compared to other seasonal time-points. However, even in this case, the summertime point was the last month of the Antarctic period, and possible effects other than photoperiod might be present. Instead, total time slept, together with sleep efficiency and latency, did not change in 17 winter-over crewmembers at the Chinese Zhonshang station (Chen et al., 2016). The authors reported a delay of sleep onset, offset, a mid-sleep time in Antarctica compared to a pre-departure measurement, with a peak in midwinter for sleep onset and before and in mid-winter in the other time-points compared to the baseline and to the end of winter (Chen et al., 2016). Delay in sleep timing, and an increase in sleep fragmentation, were also found in winter by Francis et al. (2008). Winter delay for bed-, wake-, sleep start-, and sleep end-time were also observed by Kawasaki et al. (2018). In their study, two Antarctic stations, Halley and Concordia, were investigated, aiming at highlighting possible differences between the two altitudes. The inter-daily stability of the circadian rhythm (the stability over days) declined after two months without direct sunlight, and the 10-hours period with the maximum activity was delayed. The authors reported no relevant differences between the two stations in the circadian and sleep indices. Differently, Collet et al. (2015) found that at sea level (Dumont D’Urville station) individual sleep-time was longer, sleep-efficiency was higher, and wake after sleep onset was shorter than at Concordia, confirming the role of altitude in determining the pattern of results observed in most of the Antarctic studies (Pattyn et al., 2018). With regard to seasonality, their results point to a general worsening of the sleep quality during summer compared to winter, with higher fragmentation and night energy expenditure. The detrimental effect of summer was investigated and confirmed in a recent PSG study (Pattyn et al., 2017). In their study, after 3 weeks of acclimation in Antarctica, 9 subjects were compared to a control group of 12 good sleepers. They found a comparable Total Sleep Time, while sleep onset latency, sleep efficiency, SWS, REM latency, and light sleep were significantly lower, whereas REM sleep, wake after sleep onset, and the latency between the first REM episode and the first SWS episode were higher. Instead, an actigraphy study revealed no effects in summer (Weymouth and Steel, 2013). However, their study was conducted with 14 students on a 12-day field trip in Antarctica, where the expedition members spent 1 night at the Scott Base, 7 nights in a camp several kilometers away, and 3 nights at the Scott Base again. Thus, the design, the nature, and the short duration of the trip, as well as the sample, might have a role in the negative finding.

At the opposite Pole, PSG has been used by only two studies conducted during winter with the primary aim to assess the effects of cold. Buguet et al. (1976) found increased wakefulness, decreased SWS and REM, while Angus et al. (1979) found a 50% decrement in REM sleep in the first night in the Arctic and a 25% decrement in the subsequent nights as compared to the baseline sleep. This large REM reduction further worsened during the coldest nights.

Beyond some confirmations of Antarctic findings, these two studies suggest a specific effect of REM suppression by cold, which resembles the robust phenomenon in animal studies of REM deprivation by exposing animals to cold temperature (Amici et al., 1998, 2008). Thermal stress, due to the alteration of normal sympathetic/parasympathetic activity, exerts a homeostatic change in NREM sleep, with more light and fragmented episodes, and, consequently, an adaptive suppression of REM sleep (Szymusiak, 2018). However, given the technological development of polar stations and facilities, the impact of cold has now been dramatically reduced, if not eliminated.

Together with the Antarctic studies, where light interventions have been revealed effective in reducing total sleep time, advancing sleep start and wake up time (Corbett et al., 2012), reducing sleep onset and sleep latency (Mottram et al., 2011), and augmenting sleep efficiency (Francis et al., 2008), the effectiveness of countermeasures has also been studied in the Arctic. The Arctic studies were implemented in Alert (Paul et al., 2015a,b,c). In a first study (2015a), the authors compared two groups of individuals who spent at least one month in loco, one group in winter (January), and one group in summer (June). Results showed that participants in the winter group slept significantly more than those in the summer group. In another study (2015b), two summer groups (in 2012 and 2014) were compared, and melatonin was tested as a countermeasure. First, the authors showed that participants in the second trial slept more than their counterparts in the first trial. The authors reported that schedules and daily-light exposure changed between the two groups, with more conservative station rules during the second campaign, especially regarding light exposure restriction during the night. However, the melatonin treatment did not exert any macroscopic change in the objective sleep parameters. Differently, the last study was conducted during winter and involved individualized light-intervention based on melatonin circadian-rhythm (2015c). In the post-treatment measurement, sleep efficiency and percentage of sleep were higher, and wake after sleep onset and number of sleep episodes were lower than the pre-treatment session, with no effects on sleep quantity.

Summarizing, studies in polar environments mostly suggest a general worsening of sleep quality as indexed by decreases of SWS (i.e., the sleep stage more strictly associated to homeostatic need), TST and sleep efficiency, and a concomitant increase of sleep fragmentation (i.e., WASO) and lighter sleep stages (i.e., stages 1 and 2). Seasonal effects have also been shown.

Until now, it is not clear how a prolonged permanence in a confined polar environment affects sleep. Again, the findings reviewed (Table 3) might be attributable to seasonal changes, to the confinement itself, or to a mixture of the two. Unfortunately, very few studies aimed at investigating their differential role, for instance by methodologically disentangling linear from seasonal effects. Hence, more studies aimed at comparing sleep changes at different stations, at different altitudes and latitudes, are strongly needed. Furthermore, considering other laboratory-controlled studies aimed at investigating specific effects of circadian disentrainment on sleep stages, as well as investigations in natural but not confined polar or quasi-polar environments will be highly informative on how and how much sleep changes are dependent upon these specific factors.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Overview of studies investigating sleep through objective measures in polar environments.

The Mars Analogs

Several attempts have been made to investigate the biomedical and psychological effects of a simulated mission to Mars, especially focusing on the prolonged isolation and confinement, on the scheduling of activities and operations, and on the interpersonal issues that astronauts may encounter during their travel to outer space.

During the Mars-105 experiment, Barger et al. (2014b) observed that both crewmembers and mission controllers obtained inadequate sleep on the day of the nightshift and that sleep deprivation affected cognitive performance, highlighting the importance of adequate scheduling of activities to safeguard the level of performance from sleepiness and stress-like responses during operation of paramount importance. Following this line, Gemignani et al. (2014) found in Mars-105 that higher cortisol levels in the crewmembers were correlated with the reduction of REM latency, the decrease of sleep duration, the increase of arousals, and the reduction of delta power in stage 3 sleep. Instead, in the 520-days experiment, Zavalko et al. (2013) found an increase in sleep and SWS latency when the confinement started. Sleep latency persisted high even after the confinement ended, while SWS latency recovered to the baseline levels before the end of the confined period. In terms of stage percentages, the authors found an increase in stage 2 and a decrease in SWS after the confinement compared to the last part of the isolation period. Crewmembers slept more toward the end of the mission and exhibited less sleep activity in the third quarter of the mission compared to the others (Basner et al., 2013).

Apart from the Mars-500 experiment, sleep has been studied in other simulation of Mars expeditions. For instance, the Hawaii – Space Exploration Analog and Simulation was conducted for 365 days in a geodesic dome in the Mauna Loa volcano’s harsh environment in Hawaii. In a pilot study (Matsangas et al., 2017) evaluating 4 out of 6 crewmembers, high interindividual variability was observed. Whereas no differences in sleep bedtime patterns compared to pre-mission baselines were found at a general level, but the authors reported that the participant with less sleep obtained was more satisfied with his sleep quality, while the opposite was true for the participants who slept more. Moreover, the authors investigated if individual light exposure and physical activity may contribute to the variability observed. For instance, the participant who rested and woke up earlier than the other participants was more exposed to white light during the day, while the participant who slept less but more efficiently was exposed to blue light during the night and was the less physically active.

The astronauts supposed to spend time on Mars have also to deal with the Martian Sol, the daily timekeeper consisted of 24 h and about 39 min per day. With this aim, in Gríofa et al.’s (2011) study, 7 subjects spent 110 days during the Arctic summer at the FMARS station on Devon Island, Canada. After 51 days of confinement, the crew underwent the Martian Sol implementation with no other terrestrial timekeeper. They collected subjective and objective (cardiopulmonary) sleep data. Only non-significant minor variations in subjective sleep indices and Total Sleep Time were found throughout the mission, except for an increase of alertness in the early phase of the Martian Sol compared to the pre-Sol baseline measure. Cardiopulmonary data showed only a tendency to decreased sleep stability during the Sol.

In a short-term study conducted at the Mars Desert Research Station, Utah, during the “AustroMars” mission, Groemer et al. (2010) investigated sleep in 4 crewmembers during two weeks of Mars simulation activities. Since they found no significant changes in sleepiness (as assessed by the Karolinska Sleepiness Scale, KSS) or in vigilance (as assessed by Psychomotor Vigilance Task, PVT), findings that authors attributed to the high motivation showed by subjects during the study.

In conclusion, the few studies conducted (Table 4) until now do not significantly contribute to the already little knowledge produced in ICE environments. However, promising lines of research look possible.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Overview of studies investigating sleep in Mars analogs.

Analogs of the Analogs

Taken together, the results described above do not appear to highlight a consistent pattern of changes in sleep in ICE environments. The small sample sizes and the large differences existing in the experimental designs and environmental factors indeed represent one of the most prominent causes. The severe methodological limitations existing in this research field posit the question of how it could be possible to advance the knowledge on how sleep adjusts in these exceptional environments, where adequate control conditions are not often possible to implement. One possibility to overcome this weakness is to gather information from other conditions on Earth that might be taken as a reference for the sleep effects identified in ICEs. Even though some of them cannot inherently be qualifiable as ICE environments, environments such as submarines, caves, prisons, military contexts, offshore platforms, intensive care units, hypoxic environments, circadian shift studies, etc., may represent useful comparisons in the progressive construction of a theoretical framework for investigating sleep under a prolonged systemic and/or processive stressful condition.

One main objective of the comparison could be the replication of results obtained when subtracting one of two factors in the condition studies. The next section will describe results reported in various fields, mostly regarding the effects of circadian disruptions and other stressors on sleep.

Circadian Analogs

Given that crews living and working at polar stations must deal with a circannual change of the normal photoperiodicity and that on the ISS astronauts experience many sunset and sunrises each day, it is not surprising that circadian alterations may arise in individual physiology, as it has been highlighted in several chronobiological studies (Arendt and Middleton, 2018; Premkumar et al., 2013; Harris et al., 2011; Dijk et al., 2001). Indeed, the circadian rhythm is extremely sensitive to light exposure, and with the massive use of artificial lights and digital devices the physiological sleep/wake pattern related only to sunlight exposure has been blurred (Wright et al., 2013). For instance, in indoor workplaces, the light intensity is approximately 300–500 lux, the direct sunlight approximately 100000 lux and 1–100 lux when the sun is below the horizon (Arendt, 2012): low light intensity, even in controlled light-dark cycles, has found to delay sleep onset (Middleton et al., 2002). Though the nature of the possible misalignment seems to be very different in the several environments investigated (e.g., polar stations and space missions), considering other literature can help disentangle the circadian from the effects of other stressful factors on sleep.

For instance, in 19 marines involved in a 70-day mission aboard the French Strategic Submarine with Ballistic Nuclear missiles, Trousselard et al. (2015) did not find any significant difference in any parameters recorded between two PSG sessions (days 21 and 51 of the mission) nor in night-time cortisol levels. The authors concluded that the imposed scheduled activity and the control of light and temperature aboard were important factors for preventing any sleep impairment. In any case, because no control groups or baseline sessions were recorded, no significant conclusion can be drawn. In addition, together with but differently from Gemignani et al. (2014), a correlation between cortisol and sleep has been described: here, cortisol levels were positively correlated to REM sleep after 51 days of the mission. Several other studies conducted and reported by the U.S. Naval Postgraduate School (Miller et al., 2011) provide evidence that submariners sleep subjectively less while underway and that more compressed daily activity schedules resulted in a less amount of sleep, as measured by actigraphy. The findings that work schedules in submarines have an impact on sleep duration have been confirmed elsewhere (Duplessis et al., 2007): social and work times can represent strong zeitgeibers for sleep/wake rhythm, even in the absence of sunlight (Yoneyama et al., 1999). In the same report (Miller et al., 2011), other results have been summarized. For instance, on surface ships, mariners have been found to sleep less if working on topside compared to the ones that slept below decks, thus not seeing sunlight, and that rank position might have a role (senior personnel slept less than junior personnel). A recent paper reviewed subterranean studies (Mogilever et al., 2018), not only presenting evidence of the well-known circadian stabilization at more than 24h of several physiological measures (free-running), but also of changes in sleep architecture. Indeed, SWS and REM sleep were found to increase at the expense of light sleep: this happened when individuals were found to sleep desynchronized and to follow a bi-circadian rhythm (34/14h of wake/sleep) (Chouvet et al., 1974). In this case, given the homeostatic role of SWS, the increase in SWS sleep could be more related to the longer time spent awake rather than to the isolation itself.

Circadian misalignments per se have been mostly investigated through laboratory-controlled forced desynchrony studies. In two time-isolation studies (10 and 12 days, respectively), Monk et al. found that a progressive 2h phase delay (Monk et al., 2004) led to a significant linear decrease/increase of time spent asleep/WASO during and after the phase delay sessions and a decrement/increment in sleep latency during/after the isolation relative to the baseline, whereas a progressive 0.5h phase advance (Monk et al., 2006) led to a significant decrease/increase of time spent asleep and sleep latency in the last sleep episodes relative to the baseline. However, in both studies no differences were found in REM and SWS percentages. One concern can be advanced over the suited methodology, and over the number of EEG derivations used: in Monk et al.’s as in other space-mission studies (for technical purposes), EEG was only registered at C3 or C4, as reported by the authors. Indeed, it has been shown that SWS has a topographical distribution with dissociable sleep pressure-dependent and -independent components (Bersagliere et al., 2018), and with relevant changes might not be detected. For instance, Lazar et al. (2015) described how sleep-dependent and circadian-related changes of SWS can be topographically distinguished through a forced-desynchrony protocol: frontal areas seem to be most related to sleep- while circadian-related effects might be prominently found in central and posterior areas. In addition to SWS, a circadian role in REM sleep has also been documented (Czeisler et al., 1980).

Noise

A good example of an environment constituted by a multiplicity of factors that may affect sleep and psychological well-being is the Intensive Care Unit (ICU). High environmental noise, exposure to only artificial lights, low physical activity, social confinement, and lack of privacy are the most common ICU patients’ complaints. However, given the nature of the context, ICU studies have been conducted on clinical populations, in which the pathological condition represents a confounding variable. Keeping that in mind, patients in ICU showed decreased SWS and REM sleep and increased awakenings (Boyko et al., 2012). The only study investigating the effects on healthy participants (nurses and doctors) found similar results when participants were sleeping in the ICU compared to when they were sleeping at home or in an unknown environment (Reinke et al., 2019).

Noise has been considered one of the most influential detrimental factors for sleep in ICU. Given that it is also present during space-missions, attention must be paid to it. Changes in sleep structure have also been found due to laboratory-controlled environmental noise both in animals (Rabat et al., 2004) and humans (Basner and Samel, 2005; Griefahn et al., 2006). Basner and Samel (2005), investigated the effects of nocturnal aircraft noise on sleep. They polygraphically recorded 13 consecutive nights in 128 subjects. Subjects were exposed to aircraft noise events in 9 nights, preceded by an adaptation and a baseline night, and followed by two recovery nights. Noise exposure was related to sleep fragmentation, as stage 1 sleep and number of awakenings increased, whereas SWS decreased. Dose-response relationship between Sound Pressure Level and number of events per night were observed. Similarly, Griefahn et al. (2006) investigated the influence of different road, rail, and aircraft noise during three weeks in 24 participants. Compared to a quietly sleeping control group, participants exposed to noise obtained less sleep and spent more time awake after sleep onset. In addition, SWS and REM sleep decreased even though this effect was not found in the first sleep cycle. Strongest effects were found according to noise levels, while the type of noise exacerbated different changes in sleep structure. The impact of other noise sources has been largely documented: the effects of traffic noise on sleep have also been reported to reduce SWS (Myllyntausta et al., 2020) and influencing the hormonal stress reaction mechanisms (Thiesse et al., 2020). Similar effects were shown during night exposure to wind turbines (Ageborg Morsing et al., 2018). In general, transportation noises have been positively associated with the probability of stage transition to wake or Stage 1 (Basner and McGuire, 2018).

Hypoxia

A prominent physical stressor that has been widely investigated in relation to sleep is the availability of oxygen. Most of the studies have been aimed at investigating sleep apnea phenomena, but literature also exists on the investigation of natural and laboratory-controlled oxygen deprivation. In many circumstances (such as some Antarctic stations), the oxygen level might fall below normal values for human well-being. Environmental levels of oxygen are related to barometric pressure, which mainly depends upon altitude. The higher the altitude, the lower the barometric pressure, the lower the partial pressure of oxygen (PO2): this condition is named hypobaric hypoxia. People who work and live in high-altitude environments experience several physiological, neurological, and psychological effects (Wilson et al., 2009). Organism response to hypoxia acts as a compensatory mechanism. When PaO2 (arterial pressure of oxygen) falls below ∼60 mmHg, the carotid body, through chemoreceptors, starts a signaling cascade that involves the nucleus of the tractus solitarii, the ventrolateral medulla and the paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus. These structures are central regulating autonomic activity: the increase of the alveolar ventilation (HVR, hypoxic ventilator response) and the sympatho-adrenal activity (peripheral epinephrine levels and cardiovascular activity) are the first responses that allow the maintaining of the oxygen supply. Hyperventilation leads to periodic breathing during sleep, which is the hypothesized mechanisms through which sleep (mostly light sleep) is affected under hypoxic exposure (Ainslie et al., 2013). Cerebral blood flow increases due to vasodilation induced by catecholaminergic and non-catecholaminergic processes: central nervous system and cognition can be affected, as it is broadly described in the high-risk literature (Virués-Ortega et al., 2004; Taylor et al., 2015; McMorris et al., 2017). Several studies also suggest that acute hypoxia can determine an increase in cortisol levels (Heyes et al., 1982; Simeoni et al., 2011). Differently, chronic exposure to hypoxia may result in an attenuation of these symptoms, in a process known as acclimation or acclimatization. For instance, Strewe et al. (2017) found that chronic exposure to hypoxia (21 days) did not affect individual stress response. Moreover, because a high percentage of the studies conducted in Antarctica have been conducted in high-altitude internal stations (e.g., Concordia station), the effects of hypoxia on sleep might have been confounded or strengthened the effects due to the photoperiod alteration and to confinement (Pattyn et al., 2018). In these studies, the hypoxic environment was found to increase sleep fragmentation compared to sea-level (Collet et al., 2015). SWS was also found to be reduced (Natani et al., 1970; Shurley, 1974), together with REM sleep, and sleep onset latency (SOL) increased (Natani et al., 1970), but in these studies there was no comparison with a sea-level station. A recent study (Mairesse et al., 2019) reported that sleep parameters investigated through PSG were found to be stable throughout a one year-campaign at the Concordia station, with increased averaged SOL compared to literature standards. Accordingly, besides respiratory variables and positive effects of exercise, sleep has been found not to change in a long-term permanence at the Concordia Station, even if REM sleep has been found to increase in a compared short-term hypoxic exposure after 10 days in a normobaric chamber (Tellez et al., 2016). The increase in REM sleep has been suggested to be a regulatory mechanism helping the organism to maintain optimum levels of CO2 through the modulation of breathing response, as has been observed in the switch between NREM and REM sleep in normal sleep (Madan and Jha, 2012a).

Shorter TST was found in a real high-altitude environment and in a simulated environment compared to a control condition (Heinzer et al., 2016). Accordingly, in a simulated normobaric hypoxic experiment (Pramsohler et al., 2019) it was reported that the more severe the hypoxia, the lower the TST, together with reductions in REM sleep. Comparisons between bed rest and hypoxic conditions showed that SWS decreased while light sleep increased due to hypoxia (Rojc et al., 2014; Morrison et al., 2017). Coherently, studies in high-altitude treks revealed similar patterns (Nussbaumer-Ochsner et al., 2011).

Research on hyperbaric environments also shown similar results. For instance, Stoilova (1992) found that prolonged exposure to increased pressure in 40 aquanauts decreased SWS and REM sleep percentage, paralleling other results observed in space and analogs. Similar findings were provided by a precedent study (Rostain et al., 1988). Apart from changes in sleep stages, increased WASO and SOL, and decreased TST and SE were commonly observed in these studies (Seo et al., 2001; Nagashima et al., 2002a). However, similar results were also reported for a non-pressurized control group in a subsequent study (Nagashima et al., 2002b): the authors concluded that results were mostly due to a reaction to the psychological stress of confinement.

Lighter sleep and more fragmentation seem to be the principal findings for the hypoxic studies. However, as it has already been written, hypoxic effects in Antarctica might be confounded by other challenging conditions. Comparisons between high-altitude and sea-level investigations, together with simulated hypoxic environments, might be the right way to solve the puzzle. Moreover, problems in identifying the specific effects of hypoxia or, conversely, cut them off to isolate photoperiodicity and isolation, may also arise when considering acute and chronic exposure of oxygen deprivation and, thus, the counteractive impact of acclimation.

Psychosocial Stressors

With regards to purely psychological variables, few results have been produced. Comprehensively, psychosocial stress has also been showed to reduce SE, SWS, and REM, and to increase awakenings, even if the findings are not definitive given the large number of different stress conditions studied and the small number of studies conducted on sleep (for a review, Kim and Dimsdale, 2007). In their review, Kim and Dimsdale (2007) divided everyday stress from laboratory-induced stress. They observed that works studying the effects of everyday emotional stress lead to a reduction of SWS. They reported that a reduction of SWS was also found in laboratory-induced stress, both for mild stressor, such as using an indwelling venous catheter (Prinz et al., 2001) or the “first night effect” (Curcio et al., 2004), and emotional stress (Levin et al., 2002), whereas conflicting evidence for REM has been reported. However, high-risk environments can frequently be characterized by a lack of privacy and interpersonal conflicts, and a link between sleep disturbances and lack of privacy has been suggested (Jaksic et al., 2019). An early study conducted in prison found a reduction in TST, SL, and percentage of stage 2, and an increase in awakenings in prisoners compared to normal controls, but no changes in REM and in SWS (Karacan et al., 1974). Studies on rats on the effects of social defeat stress reported that the conflict itself caused an increase in SWS and a suppression of REM sleep in the first hours after the conflict (Kamphuis et al., 2015).

Despite the low number of studies and the variety of conditions investigated in the field, the results obtained have shown that sleep structure can also be altered by moderate psychological stressors. Astronauts and individuals who live and work in confined environments, in very close prolonged contact with other few individuals reported that factors regarding interpersonal relations and communication with the outside are often challenging, representing one of the most prominent stressors in ICE. According to current knowledge, linking those variables to sleep problems looks premature, and future research must be able to keep track, as possible, of personal and interpersonal stressful events during the permanence in ICE, to understand eventual variations in sleep indices better.

Discussion

This review aimed at drawing a current state of our knowledge on human sleep in ICE environments by comparing findings from studies run in different settings. Results show a very blurred and contrasting picture, with very few apparently consistent effects, and essentially no clear indication of which of the many factors characterizing the ICE environments play a role per se or in association with each other.

Regarding spaceflights, the few existing studies reported that during short-term space missions, astronauts show a decreased SWS, an increased REM, and reduced sleep efficiency (e.g., Monk et al., 1998; Dijk et al., 2001). Albeit these alterations mimic those reported in Antarctica, contradictory findings have also been reported (e.g., Chen et al., 2018). Specific, albeit small, changes in REM and SWS also occur during long-term space missions, even if the direction of these effects remains unclear.

Some hints about the factors determining the sleep alteration in ICEs might come from the studies performed in polar environments (mostly, Antarctica), that share the isolation and confinement condition with space-flight missions. The reviewed studies suggest, but not univocally, a general worsening of sleep quality as indexed by the decreased slow wave sleep (SWS) (i.e., the sleep stage more strictly associated with homeostatic need) and sleep efficiency, and the concomitant increase of sleep fragmentation (i.e., WASO) and lighter sleep stages (i.e., stages 1 and 2) (see Pattyn et al., 2018, for a recent review). However, it is not clear how a prolonged permanence in a confined polar environment determines these changes and if these changes are a specific or aspecific (e.g., acting as a stressor) consequence of polar confinement. Indeed, they might be due to seasonal changes, the confinement itself, a mixture of the two, or any other feature of the polar environment. Unfortunately, very few studies aimed at investigating their differential role, for instance by methodologically disentangling linear from seasonal effects.

Bed-rest studies, simulating microgravity’s effects on physiological and psychological processes, are hard to interpret because of differences in protocols and duration. However, even in this condition, studies exist that report decreased SWS and increased sleep fragmentation (e.g., Komada et al., 2006). It is worth noting that in bed-rest studies, isolation and confinement are markedly less pronounced than during space missions, and this makes the attribution of the sleep alterations found in the latter to the isolation and confinement conditions.

The MARS-500 program that more directly explored the effects of isolation and confinement gave mixed results. Indeed, during the first, 105-day isolation study, it has been reported that higher cortisol levels were correlated with the reduction of REM latency, the decrease of sleep duration, and the reduction of delta power in stage 3 sleep (Gemignani et al., 2014). However, during the second, 520-day study, different findings have been reported, with a decreased SWS appearing only after the confinement (Zavalko et al., 2013). In other Mars analog studies, no PSG techniques have been implemented, making it difficult to get informative results.

Data from other settings that share some features with the polar environments and the space missions also contribute to blur the general picture. For instance, studies in underwater settings and Intensive Care Units substantially failed to highlight significant alterations of sleep or of the sleep/wake cycle (e.g., Trousselard et al., 2015; Reinke et al., 2019), whereas studies in caves showed an increased SWS (Mogilever et al., 2018), contrarily to what has been reported in polar settings. Instead, the decrease in SWS has been extensively observed during the exposure to a systemic stressor such as noise (Basner and Samel, 2005; Griefahn et al., 2006), or hypoxia (Rojc et al., 2014; Morrison et al., 2017), even if not conclusive. Although few studies exist, psychosocial stressors have also been found to induce similar changes (Kim and Dimsdale, 2007).

Moreover, several methodological limitations plague the research on this topic. They are probably unavoidable, since the very nature of most of the studies, still they contribute immensely to the unclear pattern of results reported in the literature. Firstly, sample sizes are very small in most of the reviewed studies. This, of course, has a negative impact on the studies’ statistical power, but might also lead to false effects and exacerbation of the impact of how individuals cope with the exceptional situation they live in. Secondly, the effect sizes in most of the studies are also typically small. Again, this might suggest non-specific effects of ICE environments on sleep, or that better experimental control over the multiple variables that might affect the human sleep in ICE environments is mandatory. Thirdly, a huge variety of experimental conditions, protocols, measurements do exist, making it difficult to compare results obtained in different studies and settings.

Most of the constraints that limit the degree of experimental control researchers have over their studies cannot be easily avoided. However, a more systematic and coordinated approach, developing international collaborations (Suedfeld and Weiss, 2000), might be useful. For instance, collecting data in multiple missions/experiments with the same methodology in the same ICE setting would increase the sample size and likely the statistical power, and would reduce the role of all the factors inherent to a specific mission or experiment (e.g., the composition of the crew in terms of individual differences). Also, planning experiments in different and selected ICE conditions to investigate the role of the specific factor under investigation would help to compare the results across different settings. Again, this would help to disentangle the specific role of the several features that define each ICE environment. Future research is required to employ such a more systematic and collaborative approach to solve the several methodological and theoretical limits of the field. Until now, most of the studies on human sleep in ICE environments have only addressed the issue of whether any sleep alteration exists in ICE. It might be time to move toward addressing what causes sleep alterations in ICE environments.

Conclusion

Overall, the scientific literature on sleep in ICE suggests that sleep alterations in isolated and confined environments are rather non-specific. The general changes in SWS and REM sleep often described due to the prolonged permanence in ICEs are not a specific condition of the prolonged isolation and confinement. These changes appear to be non-specific and generalized responses to various types of stressors. Both systemic and processive stressors that exist in these environments might independently or in a combined way induce changes in sleep architecture and quality, and the current findings do not provide enough information about the direction of these changes. A deeper understanding of the mechanisms underlying such factors is required: until now, circadian and hypoxic effects on sleep represent the most investigated. Nevertheless, the knowledge produced until now seems insufficient to disentangle the spectrum of effects that individuals in chronic stress conditions might experience.

Author Contributions

All authors listed have made a substantial, direct and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication. PZ retrieved most of the sources and drafted the initial version of the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Ageborg Morsing, J., Smith, M. G., Ögren, M., Thorsson, P., Pedersen, E., Forssén, J., et al. (2018). Wind turbine noise and sleep: pilot studies on the influence of noise characteristics. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 15:2573. doi: 10.3390/ijerph15112573

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ainslie, P. N., Lucas, S. J., and Burgess, K. R. (2013). Breathing and sleep at high altitude. Respir. Physiol. Neurobiol. 188, 233–256. doi: 10.1016/j.resp.2013.05.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Amici, R., Cerri, M., Ocampo-Garcés, A., Baracchi, F., Dentico, D., Jones, C. A., et al. (2008). Cold exposure and sleep in the rat: REM sleep homeostasis and body size. Sleep 31, 708–715. doi: 10.1093/sleep/31.5.708

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Amici, R., Zamboni, G., Perez, E., Jones, C. A., and Parmeggiani, P. L. (1998). The influence of a heavy thermal load on REM sleep in the rat. Brain Res. 781, 252–258. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(97)01242-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Angus, R. G., Pearce, D. G., Buguet, A. G., and Olsen, L. (1979). Vigilance performance of men sleeping under arctic conditions. Aviat. Space Environ. Med. 50, 692–696.

Google Scholar

Arendt, J. (2012). Biological rhythms during residence in polar regions. Chronobiol. Int. 29, 379–394. doi: 10.3109/07420528.2012.668997

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arendt, J., and Middleton, B. (2018). Human seasonal and circadian studies in Antarctica (Halley, 75 S). Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 258, 250–258. doi: 10.1016/j.ygcen.2017.05.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barger, L. K., Evans-Flynn, E. E., Kubey, A., Walsh, L., Ronda, J. M., Wang, W., et al. (2014a). Prevalence of sleep deficiency and use of hypnotic drugs in astronauts before, during, and after spaceflight: an observational study. Lancet Neurol. 13, 904–912. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(14)70122-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barger, L. K., Wright, K. P. Jr., Burke, T. M., Chinoy, E. D., Ronda, J. M., Lockley, S. W., et al. (2014b). Sleep and cognitive function of crewmembers and mission controllers working 24-h shifts during a simulated 105-day spaceflight mission. Acta Astronaut. 93, 230–242. doi: 10.1016/j.actaastro.2013.07.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Basner, M., Dinges, D. F., Mollicone, D., Ecker, A., Jones, C. W., Hyder, E. C., et al. (2013). Mars 520-d mission simulation reveals protracted crew hypokinesis and alterations of sleep duration and timing. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 110, 2635–2640. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1212646110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Basner, M., and McGuire, S. (2018). WHO environmental noise guidelines for the European region: a systematic review on environmental noise and effects on sleep. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 15:519. doi: 10.3390/ijerph15030519

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Basner, M., and Samel, A. (2005). Effects of nocturnal aircraft noise on sleep structure. Somnologie. 9, 84–95. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-054X.2005.00051.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bersagliere, A., Pascual-Marqui, R. D., Tarokh, L., and Achermann, P. (2018). Mapping slow waves by EEG topography and source localization: effects of sleep deprivation. Brain Topogr. 31, 257–269. doi: 10.1007/s10548-017-0595-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bhattacharyya, M., Pal, M. S., Sharma, Y. K., and Majumdar, D. (2008). Changes in sleep patterns during prolonged stays in Antarctica. Int. J. Biometeorol. 52, 869–879. doi: 10.1007/s00484-008-0183-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boschert, A. L., Elmenhorst, D., Gauger, P., Li, Z., Garcia-Gutierrez, M. T., Gerlach, D., et al. (2019). Sleep is compromised in- 12° head down tilt position. Front. Physiol. 10:397. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2019.00397

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boyko, Y., Ørding, H., and Jennum, P. (2012). Sleep disturbances in critically ill patients in ICU: How much do we know? Acta Anaesthesiol. Scand. 56, 950–958. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-6576.2012.02672.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buguet, A., Cespuglio, R., and Radomski, M. W. (1998). Sleep and stress in man: an approach through exercise and exposure to extreme environments. Can. J. Physiol. Pharmacol. 76, 553–561. doi: 10.1139/cjpp-76-5-553

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buguet, A., Rivolier, J., and Jouvet, M. (1987). Human sleep patterns in Antarctica. Sleep 10, 374–382. doi: 10.1093/sleep/10.4.374

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buguet, A. G. C., Livingstone, S. D., Reed, L. D., and Limmer, R. E. (1976). EEG patterns and body temperatures in man during sleep in Arctic winter nights. Int. J. Biometeorol. 20, 61–69. doi: 10.1007/bf01553173

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, H., Lv, K., Ji, G., Liu, Z., Guo, J., Wan, Y., et al. (2018). Characterization of sleep-wake patterns in crew members under a short-duration spaceflight. Biol. Rhythm Res. 51, 392–407. doi: 10.1080/09291016.2018.1533730

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, N., Wu, Q., Xiong, Y., Chen, G., Song, D., and Xu, C. (2016). Circadian rhythm and sleep during prolonged Antarctic residence at Chinese Zhongshan station. Wilderness Environ. Med. 27, 458–467. doi: 10.1016/j.wem.2016.07.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chouvet, G., Mouret, J., Coindet, J., Siffre, M., and Jouvet, M. (1974). Periodicite bicircadienne du cycle veille-sommeil dans des conditions hors du temps. Etude polygraphique. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 37, 367–380. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(74)90112-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Collet, G., Mairesse, O., Cortoos, A., Tellez, H. F., Neyt, X., Peigneux, P., et al. (2015). Altitude and seasonality impact on sleep in Antarctica. Aerosp. Med. Hum. Perform. 86, 392–396. doi: 10.3357/AMHP.4159.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Corbett, R. W., Middleton, B., and Arendt, J. (2012). An hour of bright white light in the early morning improves performance and advances sleep and circadian phase during the Antarctic winter. Neurosci. Lett. 525, 146–151. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2012.06.046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Curcio, G., Ferrara, M., Piergianni, A., Fratello, F., and De Gennaro, L. (2004). Paradoxes of the first-night effect: a quantitative analysis of antero-posterior EEG topography. Clin. Neurophysiol. 115, 1178–1188. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2003.12.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Czeisler, C. A., Zimmermann, J. C., Ronda, J. M., Moore-Ede, M. C., and Wietzman, E. D. (1980). Timing of REM sleep is coupled to the circadian rhythm of body temperature in man. Sleep. 2, 329–346. doi: 10.1093/sleep/2.3.329

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dijk, D. J., Neri, D. F., Wyatt, J. K., Ronda, J. M., Riel, E., Ritz-De Cecco, A., et al. (2001). Sleep, performance, circadian rhythms, and light-dark cycles during two space shuttle flights. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 281, R1647–R1664. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.2001.281.5.R1647

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duplessis, C. A., Miller, J. C., Crepeau, L. J., Osborn, C. M., and Dyche, J. (2007). Submarine watch schedules: underway evaluation of rotating (contemporary) and compressed (alternative) schedules. J. Undersea Hyperb. Med. Soc. 34, 21–33.

Google Scholar

Evans-Flynn, E. E., Barger, L. K., Kubey, A. A., Sullivan, J. P., and Czeisler, C. A. (2016a). Circadian misalignment affects sleep and medication use before and during spaceflight. NPJ Microgravity 2:15019. doi: 10.1038/npjmgrav.2015.19

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Evans-Flynn, E. E., Gregory, K., Arsintescu, L., and Whitmire, A. (2016b). Risk of Performance Decrements and Adverse Health Outcomes Resulting from Sleep Loss, Circadian Desynchronization, and Work Overload: Evidence Report. Houston, TX: Johnson Space Center.

Google Scholar

Folgueira, A., Simonelli, G., Plano, S., Tortello, C., Cuiuli, J. M., Blanchard, A., et al. (2019). Sleep, napping and alertness during an overwintering mission at Belgrano II Argentine Antarctic station. Sci. Rep. 9:10875. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-46900-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Francis, G., Bishop, L., Luke, C., Middleton, B., Williams, P., and Arendt, J. (2008). Sleep during the Antarctic winter: preliminary observations on changing the spectral composition of artificial light. J. Sleep Res. 17, 354–360. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2008.00664.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frantzidis, C. A., Dimitriadou, C. K., Chriskos, P., Gilou, S. C., Plomariti, C. S., Gkivogkli, P., et al. (2020). Cortical connectivity analysis for assessing the impact of microgravity and the efficacy of reactive sledge jumps countermeasure to NREM 2 sleep. Acta Astronaut. 166, 579–589. doi: 10.1016/j.actaastro.2018.11.043

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frantzidis, C. A., Kontana, E., Karkala, A., Nigdelis, V., Karagianni, M., Nday, C. M., et al. (2019). Current trends and future perspectives of space neuroscience towards preparation for interplanetary missions. Neurol. India 67:182. doi: 10.4103/0028-3886.259124

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frost, J. J., Shumate, W. H., Salamy, J. G., and Booher, C. R. (1976). Sleep monitoring: the second manned Skylab mission. Aviat. Space Environ. Med. 47, 372–382.

Google Scholar

Gemignani, A., Piarulli, A., Menicucci, D., Laurino, M., Rota, G., Mastorci, F., et al. (2014). How stressful are 105 days of isolation? Sleep EEG patterns and tonic cortisol in healthy volunteers simulating manned flight to Mars. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 93, 211–219. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2014.04.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gonfalone, A. A. (2016). Sleep on manned space flights: zero gravity reduces sleep duration. Pathophysiology 23, 259–263. doi: 10.1016/j.pathophys.2016.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gonfalone, A. A., and Jha, S. K. (2015). The influence of gravity on REM sleep. Open Access Anim. Physiol. 7, 65–72. doi: 10.2147/OAAP.S80731

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Griefahn, B., Marks, A., and Robens, S. (2006). Noise emitted from road, rail and air traffic and their effects on sleep. J. Sound Vib. 295, 129–140. doi: 10.3390/ijerph16061073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gríofa, M. Ó., Blue, R. S., Cohen, K. D., and O’Keeffe, D. T. (2011). Sleep stability and cognitive function in an Arctic Martian analogue. Aviat. Space Environ. Med. 82, 434–441. doi: 10.3357/ASEM.2570.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Groemer, G., Gruber, V., Bishop, S., Peham, D., Wolf, L., and Högl, B. (2010). Human performance data in a high workload environment during the simulated Mars expedition “AustroMars”. Acta Astronaut. 66, 780–787. doi: 10.1016/j.actaastro.2009.08.017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gundel, A., Nalishiti, V., Reucher, E., Vejvoda, M., and Zulley, J. (1993). Sleep and circadian rhythm during a short space mission. Clin. Invest. 71, 718–724. doi: 10.1007/bf00209726

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gundel, A., Polyakov, V. V., and Zulley, J. (1997). The alteration of human sleep and circadian rhythms during spaceflight. J. Sleep Res. 6, 1–8. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2869.1997.00028.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harris, A., Waage, S., Ursin, H., and Eriksen, H. R. (2011). Saliva cortisol levels in construction workers in the Arctic (78 N). Int. J. Circumpolar Health 70, 542–551. doi: 10.3402/ijch.v70i5.17857

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heinzer, R., Saugy, J. J., Rupp, T., Tobback, N., Faiss, R., Bourdillon, N., et al. (2016). Comparison of sleep disorders between real and simulated 3,450-m altitude. Sleep 39, 1517–1523. doi: 10.5665/sleep.6010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herman, J. P., and Cullinan, W. E. (1997). Neurocircuitry of stress: central control of the hypothalamo–pituitary–adrenocortical axis. Trends Neurosci. 20, 78–84. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(96)10069-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heyes, M. P., Farber, M. O., Manfredi, F., Robertshaw, D., Weinberger, M., Fineberg, N., et al. (1982). Acute effects of hypoxia on renal and endocrine function in normal humans. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 243, R265–R270. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.1982.243.3.R265

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jaksic, C., Steel, G., Stewart, E., and Moore, K. (2019). Antarctic stations as workplaces: adjustment of winter-over crew members. Polar Sci. 22:100484. doi: 10.1016/j.polar.2019.100484

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kamphuis, J., Lancel, M., Koolhaas, J. M., and Meerlo, P. (2015). Deep sleep after social stress: NREM sleep slow-wave activity is enhanced in both winners and losers of a conflict. Brain Behav. Immun. 47, 149–154. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2014.12.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kanas, N., and Manzey, D. (2008). Space Psychology and Psychiatry, Vol. 22. Berlin: Springer.

Google Scholar

Karacan, I., Williams, R. L., Guerrero, M. W., Salis, P. J., Thornby, J. I., and Hursch, C. J. (1974). Nocturnal penile tumescence and sleep of convicted rapists and other prisoners. Arch. Sex. Behav. 3, 19–26. doi: 10.1007/bf01541039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kawasaki, A., Wisniewski, S., Healey, B., Pattyn, N., Kunz, D., Basner, M., et al. (2018). Impact of long-term daylight deprivation on retinal light sensitivity, circadian rhythms and sleep during the Antarctic winter. Sci. Rep. 8:16185. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-33450-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kelly, T. H., Hienz, R. D., Zarcone, T. J., Wurster, R. M., and Brady, J. V. (2005). Crewmember performance before, during, and after spaceflight. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 84, 227–241. doi: 10.1901/jeab.2005.77-04

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, E. J., and Dimsdale, J. E. (2007). The effect of psychosocial stress on sleep: a review of polysomnographic evidence. Behav. Sleep Med. 5, 256–278. doi: 10.1080/15402000701557383

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Komada, Y., Mizuno, K., Mishima, K., Sato, H., Inoue, Y., Tanaka, H., et al. (2006). Effects of acute simulated microgravity on nocturnal sleep, daytime vigilance, and psychomotor performance: comparison of horizontal and 6 head-down bed rest. Percept. Mot. Skills 103, 307–317. doi: 10.2466/pms.103.2.307-317

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lazar, A. S., Lazar, Z. I., and Dijk, D. J. (2015). Circadian regulation of slow waves in human sleep: topographical aspects. Neuroimage 116, 123–134. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.05.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levin, Y. I., Strygin, K. N., and Korabel’nikova, E. A. (2002). Effect of personality on changes of sleep structure caused by emotional stress. Hum. Physiol. 28, 282–286.

Google Scholar

Liang, X., Zhang, L., Shen, H., Chen, X., Wan, Y., Li, L., et al. (2014). Effects of a 45-day head-down bed rest on the diurnal rhythms of activity, sleep, and heart rate. Biol. Rhythm Res. 45, 591–601. doi: 10.1080/09291016.2014.882093

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Madan, V., and Jha, S. K. (2012a). A moderate increase of physiological CO2 in a critical range during stable NREM sleep episode: a potential gateway to REM sleep. Front. Neurol. 3:19. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2012.00019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Madan, V., and Jha, S. K. (2012b). Sleep alterations in mammals: Did aquatic conditions inhibit rapid eye movement sleep? Neurosci. Bull. 28, 746–758. doi: 10.1007/s12264-012-1285-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mairesse, O., MacDonald-Nethercott, E., Neu, D., Tellez, H. F., Dessy, E., Neyt, X., et al. (2019). Preparing for Mars: human sleep and performance during a 13 month stay in Antarctica. Sleep 42:zsy206. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsy206

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manzey, D., and Lorenz, B. (1998). Mental performance during short-term and long-term spaceflight. Brain Res. Rev. 28, 215–221. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0173(98)00041-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matsangas, P., Lewis Shattuck, N., Heinicke, C., and Dunn, J. (2017). Sleep patterns of crewmembers in mission IV of the Hawaii space exploration analog and simulation (HI-SEAS): a pilot study. Proc. Hum. Fact. Ergon. Soc. Annu. Meet. 61, 73–77. doi: 10.1177/1541931213601484

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McMorris, T., Hale, B. J., Barwood, M., Costello, J., and Corbett, J. (2017). Effect of acute hypoxia on cognition: a systematic review and meta-regression analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 74, 225–232. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.01.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Middleton, B., Stone, B. M., and Arendt, J. (2002). Human circadian phase in 12: 12 h, 200< 8 lux and 1000< 8 lux light-dark cycles, without scheduled sleep or activity. Neurosci. Lett. 329, 41–44. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(02)00574-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, N. L., Matsangas, P., and Kenney, A. (2011). “The role of sleep in the military: implications for training and operational effectiveness”, in The Oxford Handbook of Military Psychology, eds J. H. Laurence and M. D. Matthews (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 262–281.

Google Scholar

Mogilever, N. B., Zuccarelli, L., Burles, F., Iaria, G., Strapazzon, G., Bessone, L., et al. (2018). Expedition cognition: a review and prospective of subterranean neuroscience with spaceflight applications. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 12:407. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00407

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moldofsky, H., Lue, F., MacFarlane, J., Jiang, C., Poplonski, L., Ponomoreva, I., et al. (2000). Long term effects of microgravity on human sleep, cytokine, and endocrines. Gravitational Space Biol. Bull. 14:41.

Google Scholar

Monk, T. H., Buysse, D. J., and Billy, B. D. (2006). Using daily 30-min phase advances to achieve a 6-hour advance: circadian rhythm, sleep, and alertness. Aviat. Space Environ. Med. 77, 677–686.

Google Scholar

Monk, T. H., Buysse, D. J., Billy, B. D., and DeGrazia, J. M. (2004). Using nine 2-h delays to achieve a 6-h advance disrupts sleep, alertness, and circadian rhythm. Aviat. Space Environ. Med. 75, 1049–1057.

Google Scholar

Monk, T. H., Buysse, D. J., Billy, B. D., Kennedy, K. S., and Kupfer, D. J. (1997). The effects on human sleep and circadian rhythms of 17 days of continuous bedrest in the absence of daylight. Sleep 20, 858–864. doi: 10.1093/sleep/20.10.858

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Monk, T. H., Buysse, D. J., Billy, B. D., Kennedy, K. S., and Willrich, L. M. (1998). Sleep and circadian rhythms in four orbiting astronauts. J. Biol. Rhythms 13, 188–201. doi: 10.1177/074873098129000039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Monk, T. H., Kennedy, K. S., Rose, L. R., and Linenger, J. M. (2001). Decreased human circadian pacemaker influence after 100 days in space: a case study. Psychosom. Med. 63, 881–885. doi: 10.1097/00006842-200111000-00005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morrison, S. A., Mirnik, D., Korsic, S., Eiken, O., Mekjavic, I. B., and Dolenc-Groselj, L. (2017). Bed rest and hypoxic exposure affect sleep architecture and breathing stability. Front. Physiol. 8:410. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2017.00410

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mottram, V., Middleton, B., Williams, P., and Arendt, J. (2011). The impact of bright artificial white and ‘blue-enriched’ light on sleep and circadian phase during the polar winter. J. Sleep Res. 20, 154–161. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2010.00875.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Myllyntausta, S., Virkkala, J., Salo, P., Varjo, J., Rekola, L., and Hongisto, V. (2020). Effect of the frequency spectrum of road traffic noise on sleep: a polysomnographic study. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 147, 2139–2149. doi: 10.1121/10.0000985

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nagashima, H., Matsumoto, K., Seo, Y. J., Mohri, M., Naraki, N., and Matsuoka, S. (2002a). Changes in sleep patterns during simulated nitrox saturation diving to 20 and 30 meters. Percept. Mot. Skills 94, 753–766. doi: 10.2466/pms.2002.94.3.753

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nagashima, H., Matsumoto, K., Seo, Y., Mohri, M., Naraki, N., and Matsuoka, S. (2002b). Sleep patterns during 30-m nitrox saturation dives and in a confined atmospheric environment. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 56, 267–268. doi: 10.1046/j.1440-1819.2002.01021.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Natani, K., Shurley, J. T., Pierce, C. M., and Brooks, R. E. (1970). Long-term changes in sleep patterns in men on the South Polar Plateau. Arch. Intern. Med. 125, 655–659. doi: 10.1001/archinte.125.4.655

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nussbaumer-Ochsner, Y., Schuepfer, N., Siebenmann, C., Maggiorini, M., and Bloch, K. E. (2011). High altitude sleep disturbances monitored by actigraphy and polysomnography. High Alt. Med. Biol. 12, 229–236. doi: 10.1089/ham.2010.1073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pandi-Perumal, S. R., and Gonfalone, A. A. (2016). Sleep in space as a new medical frontier: the challenge of preserving normal sleep in the abnormal environment of space missions. Sleep Sci. 9, 1–4. doi: 10.1016/j.slsci.2016.01.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paterson, R. A. (1975). Seasonal reduction of slow-wave sleep at an Antarctic coastal station. Lancet 305, 468–469. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(75)91552-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pattyn, N., Mairesse, O., Cortoos, A., Marcoen, N., Neyt, X., and Meeusen, R. (2017). Sleep during an Antarctic summer expedition: new light on “polar insomnia”. J. Appl. Physiol. 122, 788–794. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00606.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pattyn, N., Van Puyvelde, M., Fernandez-Tellez, H., Roelands, B., and Mairesse, O. (2018). From the midnight sun to the longest night: sleep in Antarctica. Sleep Med. Rev. 37, 159–172. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2017.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paul, M. A., Love, R. J., Hawton, A., and Arendt, J. (2015a). Sleep and the endogenous melatonin rhythm of high arctic residents during the summer and winter. Physiol. Behav. 141, 199–206. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.01.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paul, M. A., Love, R. J., Hawton, A., Brett, K., McCreary, D. R., and Arendt, J. (2015b). Light treatment improves sleep quality and negative affectiveness in high arctic residents during winter. Photochem. Photobiol. 91, 567–573. doi: 10.1111/php.12418

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paul, M. A., Love, R. J., Hawton, A., Brett, K., McCreary, D. R., and Arendt, J. (2015c). Sleep deficits in the High Arctic summer in relation to light exposure and behaviour: use of melatonin as a countermeasure. Sleep Med. 16, 406–413. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2014.12.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petit, G., Cebolla, A. M., Fattinger, S., Petieau, M., Summerer, L., Cheron, G., et al. (2019). Local sleep-like events during wakefulness and long-term space travel. NPJ Microgravity 5:10. doi: 10.1038/s41526-019-0069-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pramsohler, S., Schilz, R., Patzak, A., Rausch, L., and Netzer, N. C. (2019). Periodic breathing in healthy young adults in normobaric hypoxia equivalent to 3500 m, 4500 m, and 5500 m altitude. Sleep Breath. 23, 703–709. doi: 10.1007/s11325-019-01829-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Premkumar, M., Sable, T., Dhanwal, D., and Dewan, R. (2013). Circadian levels of serum melatonin and cortisol in relation to changes in mood, sleep, and neurocognitive performance, spanning a year of residence in Antarctica. Neurosci. J. 2013:254090. doi: 10.1155/2013/254090

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prinz, P., Bailey, S., Moe, K., Wilkinson, C., and Scanlan, J. (2001). Urinary free cortisol and sleep under baseline and stressed conditions in healthy senior women: effects of estrogen replacement therapy. J. Sleep Res. 10, 19–26. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2869.2001.00236.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rabat, A., Bouyer, J. J., Aran, J. M., Courtiere, A., Mayo, W., and Le Moal, M. (2004). Deleterious effects of an environmental noise on sleep and contribution of its physical components in a rat model. Brain Res. 1009, 88–97. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2004.02.046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reinke, L., Haveman, M., Horsten, S., Falck, T., van der Heide, E. M., Pastoor, S., et al. (2019). The importance of the intensive care unit environment in sleep—A study with healthy participants. J. Sleep Res. 29:e12959. doi: 10.1111/jsr.12959

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rojc, B., Morrison, S. A., Eiken, O., Mekjavic, I. B., and Dolenc-Grošelj, L. (2014). The separate and combined effects of hypoxia and sustained recumbency/inactivity on sleep architecture. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 114, 1973–1981. doi: 10.1007/s00421-014-2909-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rostain, J. C., Gardette-Chauffour, M. C., Gourret, J. P., and Naquet, R. (1988). Sleep disturbances in man during different compression profiles up to 62 bars in helium-oxygen mixture. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 69, 127–135. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(88)90208-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Samel, A., and Gander, P. (1995). Bright light as a chronobiological countermeasure for shiftwork in space. Acta Astronaut. 36, 669–683. doi: 10.1016/0094-5765(95)00157-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Santy, P. A., Kapanka, H., Davis, J. R., and Stewart, D. F. (1988). Analysis of sleep on Shuttle missions. Aviat. Space Environ. Med. 59(11 Pt 1), 1094– 1097.

Google Scholar

Seo, Y. J., Matsumoto, K., Park, Y. M., Mohri, M., and Matsuoka, S. (2001). Changes in sleep patterns during simulated heliox saturation dive at 16, 19, and 24 ATA. Int. J. Ind. Ergon. 28, 203–218. doi: 10.1016/S0169-8141(01)00034-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shurley, J. T. (1974). “Physiological research at U.S. stations in Antarctica,” in Antarctic Research Series: Human Adaptability to Antarctic Conditions, Vol. 22, ed. E. K. E. Gunderson (Washington, DC: American Geophysical Union), 71–87. doi: 10.1029/ar022p0071

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simeoni, S., Biselli, R., D’Amelio, R., Rocca, B., Lattanzio, S., Mucci, L., et al. (2011). Stress-induced salivary cortisol secretion during hypobaric hypoxia challenge and in vivo urinary thromboxane production in healthy male subjects. Stress 14, 282–289. doi: 10.3109/10253890.2010.545458

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steinach, M., Kohlberg, E., Maggioni, M. A., Mendt, S., Opatz, O., Stahn, A., et al. (2016). Sleep quality changes during overwintering at the German Antarctic Stations Neumayer II and III: the gender factor. PLoS One 11:e0150099. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0150099

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stickgold, R., and Hobson, J. A. (1999). REM sleep and sleep efficiency are reduced during space flight. Sleep 22(Suppl.):S82.

Google Scholar

Stoilova, I. (1992). “Sleep as a restorative process under extreme exposure conditions. Cited in: Chokroverty S. (2017). “Sleep in Extreme Environment”,” in Sleep Disorders Medicine, ed. S. Chokroverty (New York, NY: Springer), 1239.

Google Scholar

Stoilova, I., Zdravev, T., and Yanev, T. (2000). Evaluation of sleep in space flight. C. R. Acad. Bulg. Sci. 53, 6–59.

Google Scholar

Stoilova, I. M., and Jordanova, M. M. (2005). “Sleep in microgravity,” in Proceedings of 2nd International Conference on Recent Advances in Space Technologies. RAST, 2005, Istanbul, 744–748. doi: 10.1109/RAST.2005.1512666

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strapazzon, G., Pilo, L., Bessone, L., and Barratt, M. R. (2014). CAVES as an environment for astronaut training. Wilderness Environ. Med. 25, 244–245. doi: 10.1016/j.wem.2013.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strewe, C., Zeller, R., Feuerecker, M., Hoerl, M., Kumprej, I., Crispin, A., et al. (2017). PlanHab study: assessment of psycho-neuroendocrine function in male subjects during 21 d of normobaric hypoxia and bed rest. Stress. 20, 131–139. doi: 10.1080/10253890.2017.1292246

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stuster, J. (2010). Behavioral Issues Associated with Long Duration Space Expedition: Review and Analysis of Astronaut Journals, Experiment 01/E104 (Journals): Final Report. NASA/TM-2010-216130. Houston, TX: Johnson Space Center.

Google Scholar

Suedfeld, P., and Weiss, K. (2000). Antarctica: natural laboratory and space analogue for psychological research. Environ. Behav. 32, 7–17. doi: 10.1177/00139160021972405

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Szymusiak, R. (2018). Body temperature and sleep. Handb. Clin. Neurol. 156, 341–351. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-63912-7.00020-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, L., Watkins, S. L., Marshall, H., Dascombe, B. J., and Foster, J. (2015). The impact of different environmental conditions on cognitive function: a focused review. Front. Physiol. 6:372. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2015.00372

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tellez, H. F., Morrison, S. A., Neyt, X., Mairesse, O., Piacentini, M. F., Macdonald-Nethercott, E., et al. (2016). Exercise during short-term and long-term continuous exposure to hypoxia exacerbates sleep-related periodic breathing. Sleep 39, 773–783. doi: 10.5665/sleep.5626

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thiesse, L., Rudzik, F., Kraemer, J. F., Spiegel, K., Leproult, R., Wessel, N., et al. (2020). Transportation noise impairs cardiovascular function without altering sleep: the importance of autonomic arousals. Environ. Res. 182:109086. doi: 10.1016/j.envres.2019.109086

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trousselard, M., Leger, D., van Beers, P., Coste, O., Vicard, A., Pontis, J., et al. (2015). Sleeping under the ocean: despite total isolation, nuclear submariners maintain their sleep and wake patterns throughout their under-sea mission. PLoS One 10:e0126721. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0126721

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Virués-Ortega, J., Buela-Casal, G., Garrido, E., and Alcázar, B. (2004). Neuropsychological functioning associated with high-altitude exposure. Neuropsychol. Rev. 14, 197–224. doi: 10.1007/s11065-004-8159-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weymouth, W., and Steel, G. D. (2013). Sleep patterns during an Antarctic field expedition. Mil. Med. 178, 438–444. doi: 10.7205/MILMED-D-12-00447

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whitmire, A., Slack, K., Locke, J., Keeton, K., Patterson, H., Faulk, J., et al. (2013). Sleep Quality Questionnaire Short-Duration Flyers. NASA/TM-2013-217378. Houston, TX: Johnson Space Center.

Google Scholar

Wilson, M. H., Newman, S., and Imray, C. H. (2009). The cerebral effects of ascent to high altitudes. Lancet Neurol. 8, 175–191. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(09)70014-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wright, K. P. Jr., McHill, A. W., Birks, B. R., Griffin, B. R., Rusterholz, T., and Chinoy, E. D. (2013). Entrainment of the human circadian clock to the natural light-dark cycle. Curr. Biol. 23, 1554–1558. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.06.039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, B., Wang, Y., Wu, X., Liu, D., Xu, D., and Wang, F. (2018). On-orbit sleep problems of astronauts and countermeasures. Mil. Med. Res. 5:17. doi: 10.1186/s40779-018-0165-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yoneyama, S., Hashimoto, S., and Honma, K. (1999). Seasonal changes of human circadian rhythms in Antarctica. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 277, R1091–R1097. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.1999.277.4.R1091

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zavalko, I. M., Rasskazova, E. I., Gordeev, S. A., Palatov, S. U., and Kovrov, G. V. (2013). Effects of long-term isolation and anticipation of significant event on sleep: results of the project “Mars-520”. Hum. Physiol. 39, 602–607. doi: 10.1134/S0362119713060133

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: sleep, ICE, circadian rhythms, stress, polar environments, hypoxia, microgravity, spaceflights

Citation: Zivi P, De Gennaro L and Ferlazzo F (2020) Sleep in Isolated, Confined, and Extreme (ICE): A Review on the Different Factors Affecting Human Sleep in ICE. Front. Neurosci. 14:851. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2020.00851

Received: 13 April 2020; Accepted: 21 July 2020;
Published: 11 August 2020.

Edited by:

Christiane M. Nday, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, Greece

Reviewed by:

Sushil K. Jha, Jawaharlal Nehru University, India
Giulio Bernardi, IMT School for Advanced Studies Lucca, Italy
Michele Bellesi, University of Bristol, United Kingdom
Anna Chernikova, Russian Academy of Sciences, Russia

Copyright © 2020 Zivi, De Gennaro and Ferlazzo. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Fabio Ferlazzo, fabio.ferlazzo@uniroma1.it

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.