Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurosci., 09 March 2018
Sec. Neurogenomics

Neurogenomic Profiling Reveals Distinct Gene Expression Profiles Between Brain Parts That Are Consistent in Ophthalmotilapia Cichlids

\r\nSofie Derycke,*Sofie Derycke1,2*Loic Kver,Loic Kéver3,4Koen HertenKoen Herten5Koen Van den Berge,Koen Van den Berge6,7Maarten Van Steenberge,Maarten Van Steenberge1,8Jeroen Van HoudtJeroen Van Houdt5Lieven Clement,Lieven Clement6,7Pascal PoncinPascal Poncin4Eric ParmentierEric Parmentier3Erik VerheyenErik Verheyen1
  • 1Operational Direction Taxonomy and Phylogeny, Royal Belgian Institute for Natural Sciences, Brussels, Belgium
  • 2Department of Biology, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium
  • 3Laboratory of Functional and Evolutionary Morphology, University of Liège, Liège, Belgium
  • 4Behavioural Biology Unit, Ethology and Animal Psychology, University of Liège, Liège, Belgium
  • 5Department of Human Genetics, Genomics Core Facility, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
  • 6Department of Applied Mathematics, Computer Science and Statistics, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium
  • 7Bioinformatics Institute Ghent, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium
  • 8Section Vertebrates, Ichthyology, Royal Museum for Central Africa, Tervuren, Belgium

The detection of external and internal cues alters gene expression in the brain which in turn may affect neural networks that underly behavioral responses. Previous studies have shown that gene expression profiles differ between major brain regions within individuals and between species with different morphologies, cognitive abilities and/or behaviors. A detailed description of gene expression in all macroanatomical brain regions and in species with similar morphologies and behaviors is however lacking. Here, we dissected the brain of two cichlid species into six macroanatomical regions. Ophthalmotilapia nasuta and O. ventralis have similar morphology and behavior and occasionally hybridize in the wild. We use 3′ mRNA sequencing and a stage-wise statistical testing procedure to identify differential gene expression between females that were kept in a social setting with other females. Our results show that gene expression differs substantially between all six brain parts within species: out of 11,577 assessed genes, 8,748 are differentially expressed (DE) in at least one brain part compared to the average expression of the other brain parts. At most 16% of these DE genes have |log2FC| significantly higher than two. Functional differences between brain parts were consistent between species. The majority (61–79%) of genes that are DE in a particular brain part were shared between both species. Only 32 genes show significant differences in fold change across brain parts between species. These genes are mainly linked to transport, transmembrane transport, transcription (and its regulation) and signal transduction. Moreover, statistical equivalence testing reveals that within each comparison, on average 89% of the genes show an equivalent fold change between both species. The pronounced differences in gene expression between brain parts and the conserved patterns between closely related species with similar morphologies and behavior suggest that unraveling the interactions between genes and behavior will benefit from neurogenomic profiling of distinct brain regions.

Introduction

Behavioral responses to external and internal cues are essential for organismal survival and reproductive success, because they allow organisms to find food, flee from predators and find mating partners, amongst others. The detection of external cues is mediated by sensory structures (e.g., eye, ear, olfactory membranes, taste, and pain receptors) which transduce the information to the central nervous system via neurons and synaptic junctions. There is a tight interaction between electrical activity, hormones and gene expression in neural networks underlying behavior (Newman, 1999; Robinson et al., 2008; Oliveira, 2012) and physiological responses (O'Connell and Hofmann, 2011; Albert et al., 2012; Heyne et al., 2014; Di Poi et al., 2016).

The vertebrate brain is divided in morphologically distinct but interconnected structures that are well conserved across different taxa (Northcutt, 2002). These brain parts are functionally distinct: the nuclei of two behavioral neural networks (the social behavior network and mesolimbic reward system) are mainly located in the diencephalon and in the telencephalon, respectively (O'Connell and Hofmann, 2011; Bshary et al., 2014). Neurons and receptors mediating the production of hormones related to reproduction are located in the forebrain (diencephalon, telencephalon, and olfactory bulbs) (Zohar et al., 2010; Gopurappilly et al., 2013; Prasad et al., 2015). The telecephalon is mostly involved in learning, memory and social behavior and the diencephalon regulates hormone production and signaling. The cerebellum has an important role in implementing motory programs (Roberts et al., 1992) and spatial and emotional learning (Yoshida et al., 2004). These functional differences are reflected in brain region specific gene expression profiles in different model organisms (Khaitovich et al., 2004; Lein et al., 2007; Myers et al., 2015).

At the molecular level, temporal and spatial variation in gene expression change the structure of the neural network by rewiring or biochemically switching nodes of the neural network (Cardoso et al., 2015). Different neurogenomic states correspond to different behavioral states and the switches between states are mediated by signaling pathways that interface the environment and the genotype (Oliveira, 2012). Immediate early genes (IEG) represent the earliest genomic response to environmental cues and have proven valuable as candidate genes for understanding behavioral responses (Burmeister et al., 2005; Wood et al., 2011; Cummings, 2015). Yet, many behaviors are generated through a network of genes with most of them only showing minor changes in gene expression (Sih et al., 2004). Moreover, reponses to social stimuli can be massive, involving thousands of genes potentially in many brain regions at once (Robinson et al., 2008). As such, behavioral responses are probably better linked to changes in brain neurogenomic states than to candidate genes. Comparison of differential gene expression in the brain within and between individuals with different behaviors has pointed to the genetic basis of tameness/agressiveness in rats (Heyne et al., 2014). Comparisons between species have shown that genes underlying domestication of dogs, pigs and rabbits are species-specific (Albert et al., 2012) and that behavioral differences between humans and other primates are probably linked to species-specific gene expression in the brain (Enard et al., 2002). In contrast, neurogenomic profiles between sister species that are similar in morphology and behavior are, to our knowledge, not yet available.

The same neural circuits, or the same genes within neural circuits, can be involved in conflicting behaviors (Sih et al., 2004), sometimes with opposing gene expression levels (Cummings et al., 2008; Wong and Hofmann, 2010; Sanogo et al., 2012; Sanogo and Bell, 2016). Moreover, gene expression can be cell type- and brain region specific (Sanogo et al., 2012) and extensive variation in gene expression between tissues, individuals and populations occurs (Whitehead and Crawford, 2005). Adequately linking gene expression in the brain to behavior therefore requires accurate profiling of the expression of all genes in each of the brain regions and in several individuals. At the same time, statistical models need to account for correlation between the different brain regions from the same individual. In addition, detecting differential expression between and across treatments requires methods that allow gene-level FDR control as often complex experimental designs typically involve many research hypotheses that have to be assessed for every individual gene (Van den Berge et al., 2017).

Here we assess differential expression in six morphologically defined brain regions of two congeneric cichlid fish species from Lake Tanganyika using 3′ mRNA sequencing (Moll et al., 2014) and a stage-wise statistical data analysis procedure (Hanssens et al., 1999; Nevado et al., 2011; Van den Berge et al., 2017). We study differential gene expression in the brain of mature female Ophthalmotilapia nasuta and O. ventralis that have been kept in a social setting with conspecific females under controlled laboratory conditions. The two species have similar morphology (Hanssens et al., 1999), similar behavior (Kéver et al., 2017) and occasionally hybridize in the field (Nevado et al., 2011). They are maternal mouthbrooders and females take care of the fry (Konings, 2014). Mating behavior is therefore assumed to be largely controlled by the female. We hypothesize that (1) gene expression profiles between brain parts within each species will be highly different because neural circuits, receptors for neurotransmitters and hormone production are located in specific brain regions and that (2) gene expression profiles across brain parts between species will reflect their similar morphology, behavior, and cognitive abilities. We also use the 3′ mRNAseq data to investigate differential expression between brain parts of 36 genes that have been linked in previous studies to fish behavior and physiology.

Materials and Methods

Specimen Collection

Wild-caught female fish of O. ventralis (OV) and O. nasuta (ON) were bought from Cichlidenstadl (Allerheim, Germany) and arrived at the lab on February 24, 2015. The individuals of O. ventralis and O. nasuta were collected at Ulwile Island and Mtosi, respectively.

Experimental Setup

Five female fish from each species were kept in aquaria containing aerated freshwater of 28°C, and received a light-dark cycle of 12:12 h for 51 days after arrival in the lab. The O. nasuta females were kept in an aquarium with a water volume of 270 L (W40 × H50 × L135 cm), while O. ventralis females were kept in a slightly smaller aquarium containing a water volume of 216 L (W50 × H39 × L111 cm). The two aquaria were located in the same room, and recieved the same maintenance throughout the 51 day period. All experimental procedures were approved by the University of Liège Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (protocol number 1759).

RNA Extraction

Five females per species were caught, tapped on the neurocranium to knock them out and killed by cutting the spine just after the neurocranium. All ten females were killed on the same day (April 16th, 2015) between 11.30 h and 16 h. The brain atlas from Oreochromis mossambicus (Simões et al., 2012) was used to locate the main brain regions. For each specimen, the cerebellum (CE) was dissected first, after which the telencephalon (TE) + olfactory bulbs (OB) were removed and then separated on a glass slide and stored in RNA later. Subsequently, the two lobes of the optic tectum (OT), the brain stem (BS) and the diencephalon (DI, including the preoptic area, hypothalamus and pituitary) were removed and stored in RNA later Stabilization Solution (Invitrogen). Time between catching the fish and dissection of the last brain part varied between 14 and 21 min. The five specimens of O. nasuta were dissected first, after which the five specimens of O. ventralis were dissected. The parts (6 parts × 10 specimens = 60 samples in total) were stored in RNA later at room temperature for one night, after which they were transferred to −80°C until further processing. After dissection of the brain, the specimen was dissected further to verify the presence of ovaria with eggs to ensure all females were mature.

RNA extraction was performed using the RNeasy Lipid Tissue Mini Kit from Qiagen following the manufacturer's protocol. Brain tissues were homogenized using pestles and a cordless motor (Sigma Aldrich). RNA extractions were performed in batches of 12 samples that were randomly taken from the total collection of 60 samples. Hence, samples from different specimens and different brain parts were extracted per batch. Quality and quantity of the RNA samples were checked using the Bioanalyzer (Agilent Technologies). Out of the 60 samples, 50 samples had RIN values above nine and six samples had RIN values between 7.6 and 8.9. All 56 samples showed two clear peaks corresponding to the 18S and 28S ribosomal RNA, indicating a low level of degradation. Four OB samples had too low amount of RNA (<10 ng/μl).

RNA Library Preparation and Sequencing

Library preparation for next generation sequencing was performed by the Genomics Core facility of KULeuven. The 60 RNA samples were prepared using the QuantSeq 3′ mRNA-Seq Library Prep Kit for Illumina (Lexogen). The method has high strand specificity (>99.9%) and most sequences are generated from the last exon and the 3′ untranslated region. The method generates only one fragment per transcript and the number of reads mapped to a given gene is proportional to its expression. Fewer reads than classical RNAseq are needed to determine unambiguous gene expression levels, allowing a high level of multiplexing (Moll et al., 2014). Library preparation involved reverse transcription of RNA with oligodT primers, followed by removal of RNA and second strand cDNA synthesis with random primers. The resulting fragments containing both linker fragments were PCR amplified with primers that also contain the Illumina adaptors and sample specific barcodes. All 60 libraries were pooled and sequenced (single-end 50 bp) on one lane of the Illumina Hiseq 2500.

Data Analysis

Read Trimming and Mapping

Using Trimmomatic 0.36 (Bolger et al., 2014), we trimmed the first 10 bp to remove possible introduced errors due to the second strand synthesis based on random priming and removed poly A tails. Only trimmed reads with a length >20 bp were retained. The trimmed reads were mapped to the Oreochromis niloticus genome version ASM185804v2 using default settings in STAR 2.5.2b (Dobin et al., 2013). Mapped reads were then processed with SAMtools 1.1 (Li et al., 2009; Li, 2011) and Picard tools 2.2.2 (cite site: https://broadinstitute.github.io/picard/), resulting in a position-sorted bam file containing all read metadata information. Read counting was done using HTSeq-count 0.6.1p1 (Anders et al., 2015) using gene features defined in ensemble annotation version 103. Counting was done using a strict strategy, which uses the strand information (reads had to be on the same strand as the feature). The mapping quality was set to 10. Reads were discarded when they overlapped two features.

Statistical Analysis

One sample (Co1Na_OB_5) contained a substantially lower number of reads (62,681) compared to all other samples (min 325 869; 57 out of the 60 samples had > 500,000 reads) and was removed. Genes with low overall counts (threshold of at least 15 cpm in at least four samples) were removed from the analysis. We corrected for differences in sequencing depth and RNA population using a weighted trimmed mean of the log expression ratios (TMM) normalization (Robinson and Oshlack, 2010). A multidimensional scaling (MDS) plot of Euclidean distances based on the gene expression profiles of the top 500 genes was created using the limma package (Ritchie et al., 2015).

We fitted gene-wise negative binomial generalized linear models (GLMs) implemented in edgeR v3.12.1 (Robinson et al., 2010; Lund et al., 2012) with fixed effects for individual, species (O. nasuta and O. ventralis), brain parts (BS, OB, OT, CE, TE, DI) and species x brain part interactions. The individual effects are necessary to account for the clustered design, i.e., different brain parts are sampled within each individual. The interaction effect is required to accomodate for species-specific expression in different brain regions. The quasi-likelihood framework was used for parameter estimation and statistical inference (Lund et al., 2012). We assessed two sets of hypotheses: (1) gene expression does not differ between brain parts within each species and (2) gene expression differences between brain parts do not differ between species (involving differences between species × brain part interaction terms). Hypothesis tests were assessed using a stage-wise testing procedure implemented in the R package stageR to allow for gene-level false discovery rate (FDR) control when assessing multiple research hypotheses for each gene (Van den Berge et al., 2017). In the screening stage, the global null hypothesis (i.e., testing whether any of the null hypotheses of interest are false) was tested on a 5% FDR level using the Benjamini-Hochberg method (Benjamini and Hochberg, 1995). In the second stage, all hypotheses were tested separately only for the significant genes from the screening stage and the within-gene family-wise error rate (FWER) was controlled on the adjusted FDR level from the screening stage using Holm's method (Holm, 1979). The procedure guarantees to control the gene-level FDR at 5% (Heller et al., 2009). In addition, using the DE genes from the screening stage we tested for |log2FC| significantly larger than 2 using a test for differential expression relative to a threshold (McCarthy and Smyth, 2009) on a 5% gene-level FDR. For the first set of hypotheses, differential expression between brain parts was tested by comparing the gene expression in a particular brain part with the average gene expression across the remaining five brain parts within each species. We defined 12 contrasts: BS-avg_Na, CE-avg_Na, DI-avg_Na, OB-avg_Na, OT-avg_Na, TE-avg_Na, BS-avg_Ve, CE-avg_Ve, DI-avg_Ve, OB-avg_Ve, OT-avg_Ve, TE-avg_Ve. For the second set of hypotheses, differential expression between brain parts across species was investigated by defining 15 contrasts (BSvCE_Na-BSvCE_Ve, BSvDI_Na-BSvDI_Ve, BSvOB_Na-BSvOB_Ve, BSvOT_Na-BSvOT_Ve, BSvTE_Na-BSvTE_Ve, CEvDI_Na-CEvDI_Ve, CEvOB_Na-CEvOB_Ve, CEvOT_Na-CEvOT_Ve, CEvTE_Na-CEvTE_Ve, DIvOB_Na-DIvOB_Ve, DIvOT_Na-DIvOT_Ve, DIvTE_Na-DIvTE_Ve, OBvOT_Na-OBvOT_Ve, OBvTE_Na-OBvTE_Ve, OTvTE_Na-OTvTE_Ve). We did not consider differential expression between species within the same brain part because statistical tests implemented in state-of-the-art RNA-seq data analysis tools cannot correct for variability within and between individuals, moreover, main effects between species are also confounded with the aquarium effect. The 200 genes with lowest screening test adjusted p-values were used to generate a heatmap in the R library gplots (Warnes et al., 2016).

Finally, we performed an equivalence test to investigate whether differences in gene expression profiles between both species are conserved. We consider an equivalence interval of log2FC from −2 to 2. The design matrix is reparametrized such that one contrast corresponds to one coefficient and perform two one-side tests (TOST) by adjusting the offset for the coefficient of interest by −2 and 2. The equivalence test p-value then corresponds to the maximum p-value from the two one-sided tests (Schuirmann, 1987). We test for equivalence within each contrast by controlling the FDR at each contrast at a 5% level. Moreover, we also test for genes that are equivalent across all 15 contrasts by controlling the FDR on the within-gene maximum p-value across all 15 contrasts.

Gene ontology (GO) terms of all DE genes after the confirmation step were obtained using the Oreochromis niloticus dataset in BiomaRt (Durinck et al., 2005). GO terms related to “Biological Processes” were selected for further analysis. Enriched GO terms for each brain part were determined using a competitive gene set test implemented in CAMERA (Wu and Smyth, 2012). This test determines whether genes in the set are highly ranked in terms of differential expression (DE) relative to genes not in the set. The enrichment analysis was performed using genes that were DE in only one brain part in both species (these genes are likely to contribute most to the functional differences between brain parts) relative to all DE genes in the dataset. Two-tailed p-values were corrected on a 5% FDR level using the Benjamini-Hochberg method (Benjamini and Hochberg, 1995). GO terms that are significant in both species were visualized with the GOBubble and GOHeat function of R package GOplot (Wencke et al., 2015).

Expression of Genes Linked to Fish Behavior and Physiology

We screened the 3′ mRNA sequencing data for the presence of five immediate early genes, 15 behavioral genes and 16 receptor genes that have been linked to fish behavior and physiological networks in previous studies (full list of genes and the studies are presented in Table S4). This allows to investigate their expression in the six brain regions. The log2 fold changes and adjusted p-values from the statistical test of hypothesis 1 were used to assess differential expression between brain parts.

Results

Data Exploration

The number of trimmed reads per sample ranged between 62,681 and 3,249,822. On average, 53.4% of the reads uniquely mapped against the O. niloticus genome and only a small fraction (2.5–5.3%) of the reads were mapped to multiple loci (Figure S1).

After filtering and normalization, 11,577 genes were kept in the dataset, the bulk of which were expressed in all 59 samples (7,224 genes, 62.4%). The total number of genes expressed in each sample was very similar and ranged between 9,565 and 11,499 (median 11,324). Not a single gene was uniquely expressed in one brain part.

The MDS plot revealed a clear clustering based on brain part for both species. The cerebellum was the most distinct brain part (Figure 1). The diencephalon was also separated from all other brain parts, while the telencephalon and olfactory bulbs on the one hand, and the optic tectum and brain stem on the other hand, were more similar in gene expression to each other than to the other brain parts.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. MDS plot of all 59 samples used for analysis. Distances between samples reflect log2fold changes between the 500 most variable genes. The MDS plot clearly shows a separation based on brain part for both species. BS, brain stem; CE, cerebellum; DI, diencephalon; OB, olfactory bulb; OT, optic tectum; TE, telencephalon.

Differential Gene Expression Between Brain Parts Within Species

The two-stage testing procedure yielded 8748 genes that show differential expression between at least one brain part and the average of the remaining brain parts on a 5% gene-level FDR. These DE patterns are consistent between species (Figure 2): the cerebellum contains the highest number of DE genes (average 4,455 DE genes), followed by the telencephalon (average 3,193). The majority (61–88%) of genes that were DE in a particular brain part were shared between both species (Figure 2). The heatmap with gene expression values of the 200 most significant genes clusters the samples from both species according to brain part and shows a pronounced difference in gene expression in the cerebellum compared to all other brain parts (Figure S2). The heatmap also illustrates the high similarity in gene expression between telencephalon and olfactory bulbs (Figure S2).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Percentage of DE genes (black bars), percentage of DE genes with |log2 FC| >2 (gray bars), percentage of DE genes that are unique for each brain part within each species (white bars) and percentage of DE genes that are unique for each species and brain part (striped bar) (BS, brainstem; CE, cerebellum; DI, diencephalon; OB, olfactory bulb; OT, optic tectum; TE, telencephalon; Na: O. nasuta; Ve: O. ventralis). The percentage of DE genes was calculated as the number of DE genes divided by the average number of expressed genes across the biological replicates (four for OB_Na, five for the other brain parts) for each brain part.

The number of DE genes with |log2 FC| above 2 (i.e., at least a fourfold increase or decrease in gene expression) was below 8% in most brain parts for both species, except for the cerebellum, where 16% of the DE genes had a fourfold higher expression compared to the average expression in the other brain parts (Figure 2). The cerebellum also contained the highest proportion of DE genes that were only significant for the cerebellum (19 and 21% for O. nasuta and O. ventralis, respectively; Figure 2). DE genes with |log2 FC| > 2 are always upregulated in the brainstem compared to the average expression in the other brain parts (Figure S2, Table S1). A detailed list of the DE genes that are substantially up- or downregulated (i.e., |log2 FC| significantly > 2) in a particular brain part for both species along with their GO annotation is presented in Table S1.

For functional analysis, we focus on those genes that were uniquely DE in one brain part across both species since these are the genes most likely to contribute to the functional differences between brain parts. In total, genes that were uniquely DE in one brain part yielded 1,720 and 1,734 GO terms for O. nasuta and O. ventralis respectively. The brain stem and optic tectum contained a high number of significantly enriched GO terms (BS: 75 and 64 for O. nasuta and O. ventralis, respectively; OT: 69 and 72 for O. nasuta and O. ventralis, respectively, Figure 3). A large proportion (54–94%) of the significantly enriched GO terms were shared between both species (Figure 3). The direction of regulation, number of genes involved and the significance level of the enriched GO terms were highly similar between the two species, but differed substantially between brain parts (Figure S3). A small number of GO terms that were significantly enriched in both species were enriched in more than one brain part (BS: 31, CE: 4, DI: 26, OB: 3, OT: 49, TE: 14).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Number of significantly enriched GO terms for each brain part and each species determined by CAMERA. The height of the bar shows the total number significantly enriched GO terms. (BS, brainstem; CE, cerebellum; DI, diencephalon; OB, olfactory bulb; OT, optic tectum; TE, telencephalon; Na: O. nasuta; Ve: O. ventralis). The number of significantly enriched GO terms that are shared between the two species for each brain part are indicated in black. A large proportion (54–94%) of the significantly enriched GO terms was shared between both species.

We subsequently looked at the top 5 upregulated biological processes that were significantly enriched in both species (Table 1). The brain stem and optic tectum contained biological processes that were mediated by a large number of genes (Table 1). For the brain stem, these were multicellular organism development and lipid metabolic process. For the optic tectum, these were cell adhesion, homophilic cell adhesion via plasma membrane adhesion molecules and ionotropic glutamate receptor signaling pathway. For the diencephalon, the ephrin receptor signaling pathway contained 17 DE genes, while the other enriched processes contained much less DE genes. The cerebellum, olfactory bulbs and telencephalon contained only enriched processes with a low number of DE genes (Table 1). The full list of biological processes enriched in each brain part for both species can be found in Table S2.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Top 5 upregulated GO terms that were identified by CAMERA as enriched in both species.

Species Specific Regulation Between Brain Parts

The stage-wise testing procedure identified 32 genes with a differential FC between two brain parts between the species (i.e., the difference in brain part x species interactions). At most seven genes were significant in the respective contrasts. Of the 32 genes, 14 genes were not annotated, and the remaining genes are involved in 18 biological processes that are mainly linked to basic mechanisms such as transport, transmembrane transport, transcription (and its regulation) and signal transduction (Table S3). Gene LOC100695791 is involved in the steroid hormone mediated signaling pathway. The expression patterns of each of the 32 genes across the brain parts in both species are presented in Figure S4.

Conservative Regulation Between Brainparts

Above we have shown that there are minor differences in specific brain part effects between species. Here, we test whether the expression differences are indeed equivalent between species. We consider an equivalence interval of [−2, 2] for the difference in log fold change between two brain parts between the species. Assessing the same contrasts as above, we find that on average 89% of the genes show equivalent expression between species, with a range of 85% to 94% of equivalent fold changes across the contrasts on a 5% FDR level. Moreover, we find evidence for 71% of the genes to be equivalent across all between-species comparisons. These results provide strong evidence for conserved expression differences between the species.

Gene Expression of Genes Linked to Fish Behavior and Physiology

Of the five IEG that have been linked to behavior in previous studies (Table S4), expression of two genes was detected in the brain of Ophthalmotilapia under our control conditions: egr1 was expressed in all brain parts (Figure S5) and significantly more so in the telencephalon (Table S4) while bdnf was nearly absent in the cerebellum (at most five reads, Figure S5) and was significantly more expressed in the telencephalon and the olfactory bulbs in both species (Table S4). We did not detect cfos, but two other IEG from the fos family were expressed in all brain regions: fosb and fosl2 (Figure S5). No significant differences between brain parts were found for these two genes.

Of the 16 behavioral and reproductive genes, the expression of nine genes was detected in the female brain (Figure S5): gnrh1 and gnrh3 were consistently expressed in the olfactory bulbs and telencephalon, and were significantly more expressed in the olfactory bulbs compared to the average expression in the other brain parts in both species (Table S4); Vip was expressed in the brainstem, diencephalon and the olfactory bulbs and was significantly more expressed in the brainstem and the diencephalon compared to the average expression in the other brain parts in both species (Table S4); oxt was consistently and significantly overexpressed in the brainstem (Table S4). The remaining five genes were expressed in all brain parts (Figure S5): avpi1 was significantly more expressed in the cerebellum, serpini1 in the diencephalon, telencephalon and olfactory bulbs and gabarap in the telencephalon compared to the average expression in the other brain parts in both species (Table S4). Expression of the kpna1 and nlgn3 genes were similar in all brain parts (Figure S5, Table S4).

Of the 17 receptor genes that we screened, only four were expressed in the brain of female Ophthalmotilapia under our control setting (Figure S5): the adrenergic receptor adrb1 was highly expressed in the cerebellum (Figure S5, Table S4). The serotonergic receptor htr1a was significantly more expressed in the optic tectum and htr2a was significantly more expressed in the telencephalon, optic tectum and olfactory bulbs compared to the average gene expression in the remaining brain parts in both species (Table S4). The dopaminergic receptor drd2 was significantly more expressed in the brainstem and the olfactory bulbs compared to the average gene expression in the remaining brain parts in both species (Table S4).

Discussion

The Quantseq approach (Moll et al., 2014) and the two-stage statistical testing procedure (Van den Berge et al., 2017) allow to simultaneously investigate expression of more than 11,500 genes in a large number of samples (here: 60) and revealed more than 8,000 genes that were differentially expressed between brain parts while providing false positive control on the level of the gene. The Quantseq method generates a single sequence for each transcript of the last exon thereby eliminating the need to correct for exon number and transcript length differences between genes and greatly reduces sequencing depth per sample (Moll et al., 2014). Consequently, Quantseq is more cost-effective than classical RNAseq when only differential gene expression analysis is of interest; it does not allow to investigate differential splicing of exons or to build a de novo transcriptome. On average 53% of our data was uniquely mapped against the O. niloticus reference genome, and on average 84% of the uniquely mapped data was used for differential expression analyses.

All Six Brain Parts Show Pronounced Differences in Gene Expression

The MDS plot showed a clear difference in gene expression between all six brain parts (Figure 1) and our statistical analysis showed that 75% of the genes are differentially expressed between brain parts. We argue that these differences are linked to functional differences of the brain parts rather than to their dissection order for the following reasons: (1) RNA quality of all samples, including the diencephalon which was dissected last, was high (RIN values ranged between 9.3 and 10 for DI samples) implying that mRNA degradation was limited. Even in metabolic highly active tissue such as liver, RNA quality did not decline at room temperature within the first 2 h (van Maldegem et al., 2008); (2) the prefixation time (i.e. the time between the death of the individual and placing the tissue in RNAlater) may significantly alter gene expression due to anoxia, pH changes and other biochemical changes in the tissues (Srinivasan et al., 2002). The prefixation time of our tissues was at most 5 min which is within the maximum prefixation time of 10 min suggested by Srinivasan et al. (2002); (3) none of the genes with |log2FC| > 2 had GO terms linked to catabolic processes, mRNA decay, mRNA stability, apoptosis or regulation of pH (acidification) (Table S1). In addition, the enriched GO terms for each brain part also did not point to mRNA decay (Table S2); (4) we did not observe a decrease of gene expression levels of the 200 most DE genes from the cerebellum, optic tectum, telencephalon, olfactory bulbs, brain stem to the diencephalon (Figure S2).

Similar to our results, pronounced differences in gene expression between four human brain regions (Myers et al., 2015), between forebrain, midbrain and hindbrain regions in female sticklebacks in a social context (Greenwood and Peichel, 2015) and in the telencephalon, diencephalon, cerebellum and brainstem of male sticklebacks in response to a territorial intrusion (Sanogo et al., 2012) have been observed. Our results demonstrate that also the olfactory bulbs and the optic tectum show clear differences in gene expression and suggest that subdividing the brain in macroanatomical regions is preferable over whole brain gene expression when a characterisation of gene networks underlying behavioral responses is of interest. Moreover, the macroanatomical division applied here may provide a framework to define microanatomical subdivisions and the component cell types for future more fine scale resolution of gene expression patterns related to behavior.

Brain Part Specific Functional Annotation

The cerebellum is the most distinct brain part in terms of gene expression (Figure 1, Figure S2), contained the highest number of DE genes and 70% of the genes that showed a substantial DE (|log2 FC| > 2) in both species were uniquely DE in the cerebellum. Yet, the 1512 DE genes in the cerebellum in both species yielded only seven significantly enriched GO terms which were related to the development of cardiac muscle fibers, development of the lateral line and development of the neural tube, and to digestion and self-proteolysis (Table S2). This brain part is known to be involved in motory responses and emotions, and the development of cardiac muscle fibers and the lateral line can be linked to motory movement in fish. Our results also point to a role of the cerebellum in the physical, chemical, and biochemical processes to break down ingested nutrients into components that may be easily absorbed and directed into metabolism. Digestion of glycine, glutamine, glycerol and beta-hydroxybutyrate for energie production in the cerebellum has been documented in rats (Rotta et al., 2002).

The telencephalon is a region involved in learning and memory, and the molecular mechanism underlying these functions typically involve rewiring of the neuronal connections through changes in signaling pathways and cell communication, and the development of new and apoptosis of old cells (Cardoso et al., 2015). Five out of the 14 enriched GO terms in the telecephalon were linked to signaling pathways, and four were linked to cell formation (including clathrin coat assembly and endoderm formation). These four GO terms were uniquely enriched in the telencephalon.

The olfactory bulbs in Ophthalmotilapia are very small which complicated their dissection and yielded small quantities of RNA for sequencing. This technical issue may explain the larger variation across the replicates for this brain part (Figure 1). Nevertheless, the most upregulated enriched GO terms in the olfactory bulbs are involved in nose development, the cannabinoid signaling pathway and the positive regulation of feeding behavior which point to an important role of this brain part in detecting environmental cues related to obtaining food.

The brainstem, optic tectum and diencephalon contained a large number of enriched GO terms typically with a large number of DE genes (Table 1). The optic tectum plays an important role in sensory detection, and we found four GO terms related to placode formation, two GO terms related to neuromast deposition and two GO terms related to retina morphogenesis (Table S2). The optic tectum also contains many GO terms linked to signaling pathways, cell formation, communication and apoptosis suggesting that the machinery for rewiring is also highly active in this brain part under a social setting. The GO term neuromast deposition was also present in the diencephalon, and the GO term formation of eye tissue was also detected in the brain stem, suggesting that both brain parts are also involved in sensory perception. Amongst the most highly DE genes in the brainstem were the hox genes (Table S1) which play an important role during the formation of the bodyplan in embryos. These genes remain expressed in the hindbrain of adult mouse (Zapala et al., 2005) and were also the most upregulated genes in the cerebellum/hindbrain of sticklebacks under a social setting (Greenwood and Peichel, 2015). We show that the hox genes are 70–191 times upregulated in the brain stem compared to the average expression in the remaining brain parts (Table S1) while no significant differential regulation of these genes is present in the cerebellum. Moreover, the number of reads mapped to these genes was between 0 and 2 in the non brain stem samples, which shows that they are mainly expressed in the brain stem and not in the cerebellum.

Gene Expression Differences Between Brain Parts Are Conserved Across Species

Our analyses show concordant patterns between the two species in terms of differences in fold changes (Figure 1, Figure S2), number of DE genes (Figure 2), and enriched biological processes (Figure 3, Figure S3). Our statistical analyses identified only 32 genes with a significant difference in fold change between species. These genes were involved in basic biological processes related to transcription and transport of molecules between cells (Table S3) and indicate that the neural networks and functioning of the brain in both species are consistent. In addition to the small number of significant interaction genes, we found a large proportion of genes (on average 86% genes) with an equivalent logFC in each contrast in both species, showing that gene expression differences between brain parts are conserved across species.

Expression of Genes Linked to Fish Behavior Across the Female Brain Under a Social Setting

Immediate early genes (IEG) play an important role in gene x environment interactions because they rapidly respond to environmental cues and provide the possibility of lasting adaptation to new environmental conditions through the regulation of genes involved in neuronal activity and plasticity (Herdegen and Leah, 1998). IEG show a basal expression in many brain regions, and particularly so where neurons recieve ongoing synaptic input (Herdegen and Leah, 1998). Our results indeed show that egr1, bdnf, fosb, and fosl2 are expressed throughout the female brain and obtain high gene expression in the olfactory bulbs (Figure S5). Egr1 is a transcription factor encoding gene that allows a rapid response to social opportunities in birds (Mello et al., 1992) and cichlids. In addition, egr1 and bdfn have been linked to learning and performing a spatial task and were significantly more expressed in the telencephalon in the cichlid A. burtoni (Wood et al., 2011) which agrees with our results. egr1 mediates the transcription of the gnrh1 gene in the gonadotropin-releasing-containing neurons located in the hypothalamic–preoptic area of the brain (Fernald and Maruska, 2012). Our results show that gnrh1 and gnrh3 are expressed in the olfactory bulbs and to a lesser extent in the telencephalon. This is in agreement with the forebrain localisation of both hormones in sea bass (Gonzalez-Martinez et al., 2001).

Two hormones are known to play an important role in social behavior in fishes and other vertebrates: arginine vasotocin (avpi1) and isotocin (oxt). These are both expressed in the preoptic area of the anterior hypothalamus (located in the diencephalon) in fish (Godwin and Thompson, 2012) and in the hypothalamus of human (Sukhov et al., 1993). Our results show that the expression of vasotocin (avpi1) in Ophthalmotilapia occurs at low levels in all brain parts, and significantly more in the cerebellum than in the other brain parts (Figure S5). Isotocin on the other hand was highly and consistently expressed only in the brainstem of both species (Figure S5). This is different than gene expression of oxytocin (the vertebrate homolog of fish isotocin) in the homolog of the telencephalon and diencephalon in rodents (Bosch, 2013) and in the homolog of the diencephalon in humans (Sukhov et al., 1993; Bosch, 2013). Vip (vasoactive intestinal peptide) has shown differential expression in the cerebellum of stickleback males and its expression is regulated in opposite directions during courtship and a territorial challenge (Sanogo and Bell, 2016). In contrast, Vip was not expressed in the cerebellum of chick but was detected in several other brain parts, with highest expression levels in the diencephalon (Kuenzel et al., 1997). Our results show that Vip was also not expressed in the cerebellum of Ophthalmotilapia females, and was significantly more expressed in the brainstem and the diencephalon. Neuroserpin and neuroligin modulate synaptic plasticity and synaptogenesis in the human brain (Dean and Dresbach, 2006; Miranda and Lomas, 2006), and play a role in female mate choice of swordtails (Cummings et al., 2008). Neuroserpin is especially expressed in the human homologs of the fish telencephalon, diencephalon and cerebellum (Miranda and Lomas, 2006) and is believed to play a key role in memory and learning. We find that both genes are expressed in all brain regions of the two ciclid species, and that neuroserpin was significantly more expressed in the telencephalon, diencephalon and olfactory bulbs.

Since behavioral patterns and adaptation to new environments may be linked to receptor activity in the brain rather than to signaling activity (Di Poi et al., 2016), we screened the expression of 16 receptor genes involved in four physiological regulatory networks. Two serotonergic receptors (htr1a and htr2a), one dopaminergic receptor (drd2) and one adrenergic receptor (adrb1) are expressed in all parts of the female brain, except for the cerebellum. In this brain part, there was a strong expression of the adrb1 gene, but the other receptors were not expressed (Figure S4). The adrenergic system regulates anxiety and social behavior through adrenergic receptors, which form the bridge between the sympathic nervous system and the cardivascular system and with many endocrine and parenchymal tissues in animal in vitro systems (Hein and Kobilka, 1997). In agreement with our results, a high density of the adrenergic beta receptors have been documented in the cerebellum and diencephalon of the zebrafish (Ampatzis and Dermon, 2016).

Conclusion

The results of our study demonstrate substantial differential gene expression between all six brain parts which are consistent between two closely related cichlid species that have similar morphology and behavior. The detection of differential gene expression in the different brain parts of closely related species greatly improves our understanding of the genetic basis of the functional differences of different brain regions. We also detected several genes linked to fish behavior under our control setting. The pronounced differences between brain regions indicate that understanding the interactions between genes and behavioral responses in Ophthalmotilapia will be enhanced by neurogenomic profiling of the separate brain regions.

Data Accesibility

RNA sequences: the data presented in this publication have been deposited in NCBI's Gene Expression Omnibus (Edgar et al., 2002) and are accessible through GEO Series accession number GSE109106 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/geo/query/acc.cgi?acc=GSE106109). R script: see Datasheet 1 in online Supporting Information.

Author Contributions

EV, SD, JV, KH, EP, PP, LK. and MV concieved the idea, SD, KH, JV, and LK optimized the methodology, JV and KH generated the sequencing data and optimized the bioinformatic pipeline for filtering and mapping the reads, SD, KV, and LC analyzed the data, KV and LC performed the statistical analyses, SD wrote the manuscript. All authors contributed critically to the drafts and gave final approval for publication.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

Financial support was provided by the Belgian science policy office (contract BR/132/A1/GENBAS; https://www.belspo.be/belspo/brain-be/projects/GENBAS_en.pdf). KV is an SB Ph.D. fellow at FWO, grant number 1S41816N.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnins.2018.00136/full#supplementary-material

Table S1. List of genes that are DE in both species with a |log2FC| > 2. Both the locus name (Locus) and gene ID (GeneID) from Oreochromis niloticus of each gene is provided, along with the gene ontology ID (GoID) and Biological Process. LogFC and adjusted p-values after the stage-wise statistical analyses for O. nasuta (Na) and O. ventralis (Ve) are provided. ‘Brainpart’ refers to the brainpart in which the DE of the gene was found.

Table S2. GO terms for each brainpart identified by CAMERA as significantly enriched. For each brainpart (BS, CE, TE, OT, OB, DI), the GO term, Biological process, adjusted P-value for O. nasuta (adj Pvalue ON) and for O. ventralis (adj Pvalue OV) are provided. GO terms and Biological processes in bold are uniquely enriched in that particular brainpart.

Table S3. Genes that are significantly different for the interaction brain part * species after the stage wise testing procedure. Gene, Locus name Oreochromis niloticus; GeneID, GeneID Oreochromis niloticus; log2 FC, log fold chance; adj P value, adjusted P value according to Holm; contrast, contrast compared between O. nasuta and O. ventralis; go_id, go_id retrieved using Biomart; Biological Process, biological process the gene is involved in; NA, not available.

Table S4. Log2FC represent the gene expression in each brainpart compared to the average gene expression in the remaining 5 brainparts. Genes that are significantly differentially expressed in both species are indicated with LogFC and AdjP values in bold. Values in bold indicate genes that are significantly more expressed compared to the average gene expression in the other brainparts in both species. not expressed' means that the genes were not detected at all in the current dataset. blank lines indicate that the gene was not DE or was not expressed in that particular brainpart/species.

Figure S1. STAR alignment scores of the reads for each of the 60 samples. All reads were mapped against the Oreochromis nilo-cus genome version ASM185804v2. Green: % of reads uniquely mapped; Blue: % of reads mapped to mulJple loci; orange: % of reads not mapped because the read was too short; Red: % of reads not mapped for other reasons (eg because they were too different from the reference genome). On average, 53.4% of the reads uniquely mapped against the O. nilo-cus genome and only a small fracJon (2.5 – 5.3 %) of the reads were mapped to mulJple loci.

Figure S2. Heatmap of log2 values of cpm from the top 200 genes that are differentially expressed in one brain part compared to the average expression in the other five brain parts for O. nasuta and O. ventralis. Rows represent genes, columns represent brain parts. (Na: O. nasuta; Ve: O. ventralis; BS, brainstem; CE, cerebellum; DI, diencephalon; OB, olfactory bulb; OT, optic tectum; TE, telencephalon; 1-5: biological replicates).

Figure S3. Bubble plots of significant GO terms determined by CAMERA. Plots for each brainpart (BS, OB, CE, OT, DI and TE) are side by side for the two species (Na or Ve). Only significant GO terms are labelled. The Y-axis represents the significance of the GO term (the higher the value in the plot, the more significant the term is), the X-axis shows the Z-score, a measure that indicates whether the biological process is more likely to be decreased (negaSve values) or increased (posiSve values), circles represent GO terms and the size of the circle illustrates the number of genes included in the GO term. The yellow line demarcates the 5% significance level.

Figure S4. Log2 cpm values of the 32 interaction genes in each brain part for O. nasuta (Black) and O. ventralis (Red).

Figure S5. Number of reads in each of the 59 sample safter quality filtering for four immediate early genes (bdnf, egr1, fosb, fosl2), nine behavioral genes (avpi1, gabarap, gnrh1, gnrh3, kpna1, nlgn3, oxt, serpini1, vip) and four receptor genes (adrb1, drd2, htr1a, htr2a) reported in previous studies (see references in Table S4). Note that the Y-axis has different limits between genes to enhance visualisation.

References

Albert, F. W., Somel, M., Carneiro, M., Aximu-Petri, A., Halbwax, M., Thalmann, O., et al. (2012). A comparison of brain gene expression levels in domesticated and wild animals. PLoS Genet. 8:e1002962. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1002962

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ampatzis, K., and Dermon, C. R. (2016). Sexual dimorphisms in swimming behavior, cerebral metabolic activity and adrenoceptors in adult zebrafish (Danio rerio). Behav. Brain Res. 312, 385–393. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2016.06.047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anders, S., Pyl, P. T., and Huber, W. (2015). HTSeq-a python framework to work with high-throughput sequencing data. Bioinformatics 31, 166–169. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btu638

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benjamini, Y., and Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate - a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing J. R. Stat. Soc. Ser. B-Methodol. 57, 289–300.

Google Scholar

Bolger, A. M., Lohse, M., and Usadel, B. (2014). Trimmomatic: a flexible trimmer for Illumina sequence data. Bioinformatics 30, 2114–2120. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btu170

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bosch, O. J. (2013). Maternal aggression in rodents: brain oxytocin and vasopressin mediate pup defence. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 368:20130085. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2013.0085

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bshary, R., Gingins, S., and Vail, A. L. (2014). Social cognition in fishes. Trends Cogn. Sci. 18, 465–471. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2014.04.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burmeister, S. S., Jarvis, E. D., and Fernald, R. D. (2005). Rapid behavioral and genomic responses to social opportunity. PLoS Biol. 3:e363. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0030363

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cardoso, S. D., Teles, M. C., and Oliveira, R. F. (2015). Neurogenomic mechanisms of social plasticity. J. Exp. Biol. 218, 140–149. doi: 10.1242/jeb.106997

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cummings, M. E. (2015). The mate choice mind: studying mate preference, aversion and social cognition in the female poeciliid brain. Anim. Behav. 103, 249–258. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.02.021

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cummings, M. E., Larkins-Ford, J., Reilly, C. R., Wong, R. Y., Ramsey, M., and Hofmann, H. A. (2008). Sexual and social stimuli elicit rapid and contrasting genomic responses. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 275, 393–402. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2007.1454

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dean, C., and Dresbach, T. (2006). Neuroligins and neurexins: linking cell adhesion, synapse formation and cognitive function. Trends Neurosci. 29, 21–29. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2005.11.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Di Poi, C., Bélanger, D., Amyot, M., Rogers, S., and Aubin-Horth, N. (2016). Receptors rather than signals change in expression in four physiological regulatory networks during evolutionary divergence in threespine stickleback. Mol. Ecol. 25, 3416–3427. doi: 10.1111/mec.13690

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dobin, A., Davis, C. A., Schlesinger, F., Drenkow, J., Zaleski, C., Jha, S., et al. (2013). STAR: ultrafast universal RNA-seq aligner. Bioinformatics 29, 15–21. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/bts635

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Durinck, S., Moreau, Y., Kasprzyk, A., Davis, S., De Moor, B., Brazma, A., et al. (2005). BioMart and Bioconductor: a powerful link between biological databases and microarray data analysis. Bioinformatics 21, 3439–3440. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/bti525

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Edgar, R., Domrachev, M., and Lash, A. E. (2002). Gene expression omnibus: NCBI gene expression and hybridization array data repository. Nucleic Acids Res. 30, 207–210. doi: 10.1093/nar/30.1.207

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Enard, W., Khaitovich, P., Klose, J., Zollner, S., Heissig, F., Giavalisco, P., et al. (2002). Intra- and interspecific variation in primate gene expression patterns. Science 296, 340–343. doi: 10.1126/science.1068996

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernald, R. D., and Maruska, K. P. (2012). Social information changes the brain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 109, 17194–17199. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1202552109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Godwin, J., and Thompson, R. (2012). Nonapeptides and social behavior in fishes. Horm. Behav. 61, 230–238. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.12.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gonzalez-Martinez, D., Madigou, T., Zmora, N., Anglade, I., Zanuy, S., Zohar, Y., et al. (2001). Differential expression of three different prepro-GnRH (gonadotrophin-releasing hormone) messengers in the brain of the European sea bass (Dicentrarchus labrax). J. Comp. Neurol. 429, 144–155. doi: 10.1002/1096-9861(20000101)429:1<144::AID-CNE11>3.0.CO;2-B

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gopurappilly, R., Ogawa, S., and Parhar, I. S. (2013). Functional significance of GnRH and kisspeptin, and their cognate receptors in teleost reproduction. Front. Endocrinol. 4:24. doi: 10.3389/fendo.2013.00024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenwood, A. K., and Peichel, C. L. (2015). Social regulation of gene expression in threespine sticklebacks. PLoS ONE 10:e0137726. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0137726

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hanssens, M., Snoeks, J., and Verheyen, E. (1999). A morphometric revision of the genus Ophthalmotilapia (Teleostei, Cichlidae) from Lake Tanganyika (East Africa). Zool. J. Linn. Soc. 125, 487–512. doi: 10.1111/j.1096-3642.1999.tb00602.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hein, L., and Kobilka, B. K. (1997). Adrenergic receptors - from molecular structure to in vivo function. Trends Cardiovasc. Med. 7, 137–145. doi: 10.1016/S1050-1738(97)00034-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heller, R., Manduchi, E., Grant, G. R., and Ewens, W. J. (2009). A flexible two-stage procedure for identifying gene sets that are differentially expressed. Bioinformatics 25, 1019–1025. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btp076

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herdegen, T., and Leah, J. D. (1998). Inducible and constitutive transcription factors in the mammalian nervous system: control of gene expression by Jun, Fos and Krox, and CREB/ATF proteins. Brain Res. Rev. 28, 370–490. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0173(98)00018-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heyne, H. O., Lautenschlager, S., Nelson, R., Besnier, F., Rotival, M., Cagan, A., et al. (2014). Genetic influences on brain gene expression in rats selected for tameness and aggression. Genetics 198, 1277–1290. doi: 10.1534/genetics.114.168948

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holm, S. (1979). A simple sequentially rejective mulitple test procedure. Scand. J. Stat. 6, 65–70.

Google Scholar

Kéver, L., Parmentier, E., Derycke, S., Verheyen, E., Snoeks, J., and Van Steenberge, M. (2017). Limited possibilities for prezygotic barriers in the reproductive behaviour of sympatric Ophthalmotilapia species (Teleostei, Cichlidae). Zoology. doi: 10.1016/j.zool.2017.12.001. [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Khaitovich, P., Muetzel, B., She, X. W., Lachmann, M., Hellmann, I., Dietzsch, J., et al. (2004). Regional patterns of gene expression in human and chimpanzee brains. Genome Res. 14, 1462–1473. doi: 10.1101/gr.2538704

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Konings, A. (2014). Featherfins in their Natural Habitat. Cichlid Press.

Kuenzel, W. J., McCune, S. K., Talbot, R. T., Sharp, P. J., and Hill, J. M. (1997). Sites of gene expression for vasoactive intestinal polypeptide throughout the brain of the chick (Gallus domesticus). J. Comp. Neurol. 381, 101–118. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(19970428)381:1<101::AID-CNE8>3.0.CO;2-5;2-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lein, E. S., Hawrylycz, M. J., Ao, N., Ayres, M., Bensinger, A., Bernard, A., et al. (2007). Genome-wide atlas of gene expression in the adult mouse brain. Nature 445, 168–176. doi: 10.1038/nature05453

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, H. (2011). A statistical framework for SNP calling, mutation discovery, association mapping and population genetical parameter estimation from sequencing data. Bioinformatics 27, 2987–2993. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btr509

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, H., Handsaker, B., Wysoker, A., Fennell, T., Ruan, J., Homer, N., et al. (2009). The Sequence Alignment/Map format and SAMtools. Bioinformatics 25, 2078–2079. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btp352

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lund, S. P., Nettleton, D., McCarthy, D. J., and Smyth, G. K. (2012). Detecting differential expression in RNA-sequence data using Quasi-likelihood with shrunken dispersion Estimates. Stat. Appl. Genet. Mol. Biol. 11. doi: 10.1515/1544-6115.1826

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCarthy, D. J., and Smyth, G. K. (2009). Testing significance relative to a fold-change threshold is a TREAT. Bioinformatics 25, 765–771. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btp053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mello, C. V., Vicario, D. S., and Clayton, D. F. (1992). Song presentation induces gene-expression in the songbird forebrain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 89, 6818–6822. doi: 10.1073/pnas.89.15.6818

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miranda, E., and Lomas, D. A. (2006). Neuroserpin: a serpin to think about. Cell. Mol. Life Sci. 63, 709–722. doi: 10.1007/s00018-005-5077-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moll, P., Ante, M., Seitz, A., and Reda, T. (2014). QuantSeq 3′ mRNA sequencing for RNA quantification. Nat. Meth. 11. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.f.376

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Myers, E. M., Bartlett, C. W., Machiraju, R., and Bohland, J. W. (2015). An integrative analysis of regional gene expression profiles in the human brain. Methods 73, 54–70. doi: 10.1016/j.ymeth.2014.12.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nevado, B., Fazalova, V., Backeljau, T., Hanssens, M., and Verheyen, E. (2011). Repeated unidirectional introgression of nuclear and mitochondrial DNA between four congeneric Tanganyikan Cichlids. Mol. Biol. Evol. 28, 2253–2267. doi: 10.1093/molbev/msr043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Newman, S. W. (1999). The Medial Extended Amygdala in Male Reproductive Behavior A Node in the Mammalian Social Behavior Network. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 877, 242–257. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1999.tb09271.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Northcutt, R. G. (2002). Understanding vertebrate brain evolution. Integr. Comp. Biol. 42, 743–756. doi: 10.1093/icb/42.4.743

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O'Connell, L. A., and Hofmann, H. A. (2011). The vertebrate mesolimbic reward system and social behavior network: a comparative synthesis. J. Comp. Neurol. 519, 3599–3639. doi: 10.1002/cne.22735

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oliveira, R. F. (2012). Social plasticity in fish: integrating mechanisms and function. J. Fish Biol. 81, 2127–2150. doi: 10.1111/j.1095-8649.2012.03477.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prasad, P., Ogawa, S., and Parhar, I. S. (2015). Role of serotonin in fish reproduction. Front. Neurosci. 9:195. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2015.00195

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ritchie, M. E., Phipson, B., Wu, D., Hu, Y. F., Law, C. W., Shi, W., et al. (2015). limma powers differential expression analyses for RNA-sequencing and microarray studies. Nucleic Acids Res. 43:e47. doi: 10.1093/nar/gkv007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roberts, B. L., Vanrossem, A., and Dejager, S. (1992). The influence of cerebellar lesions on the swimming performance of the trout. J. Exp. Biol. 167, 171–178.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Robinson, G. E., Fernald, R. D., and Clayton, D. F. (2008). Genes and social behavior. Science 322, 896–900. doi: 10.1126/science.1159277

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, M. D., McCarthy, D. J., and Smyth, G. K. (2010). Edger: a bioconductor package for differential expression analysis of digital gene expression data. Bioinformatics 26, 139–140. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btp616

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, M. D., and Oshlack, A. (2010). A scaling normalization method for differential expression analysis of RNA-seq data. Genome Biol. 11:R25. doi: 10.1186/gb-2010-11-3-r25

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rotta, L. N., Valle, S. C., Schweigert, I., Dei Ricardi, L., Ferronatto, M. E., da Silva, L., et al. (2002). Utilization of energy nutrients by cerebellar slices. Neurochem. Res. 27, 201–206. doi: 10.1023/A:1014828419900

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sanogo, Y. O., Band, M., Blatti, C., Sinha, S., and Bell, A. M. (2012). Transcriptional regulation of brain gene expression in response to a territorial intrusion. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 279, 4929–4938. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2012.2087

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sanogo, Y. O., and Bell, A. M. (2016). Molecular mechanisms and the conflict between courtship and aggression in three-spined sticklebacks. Mol. Ecol. 25, 4368–4376. doi: 10.1111/mec.13766

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schuirmann, D. J. (1987). A comparison of the two one-sided tests procedure and the power approach for assessing equivalence of average bioavailability. J. Pharmacokin. Biopharm. 15, 657–680. doi: 10.1007/BF01068419

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sih, A., Bell, A., and Johnson, J. C. (2004). Behavioral syndromes: an ecological and evolutionary overview. Trends Ecol. Evol. (Amst). 19, 372–378. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2004.04.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simões, J. M., Teles, M. C., Oliveira, R. F., Van der Linden, A., and Verhoye, M. (2012). A three-dimensional stereotaxic MRI brain atlas of the cichlid fish Oreochromis mossambicus. PLoS ONE 7:e44086. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0044086

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Srinivasan, M., Sedmak, D., and Jewell, S. (2002). Effect of fixatives and tissue processing on the content and integrity of nucleic acids. Am. J. Pathol. 161, 1961–1971. doi: 10.1016/S0002-9440(10)64472-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sukhov, R. R., Walker, L. C., Rance, N. E., Price, D. L., and Young, W. S. (1993). Vasopressin and oxytocin gene-expression in the human hypothalamus. J. Comp. Neurol. 337, 295–306. doi: 10.1002/cne.903370210

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van den Berge, K., Soneson, C., Robinson, M. D., and Clement, L. (2017). stageR: a general stage-wise method for controlling the gene-level false discovery rate in differential expression and differential transcript usage. Genome Biol. 18:151. doi: 10.1186/s13059-017-1277-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Maldegem, F., de Wit, M., Morsink, F., Musler, A., Weegenaar, J., and van Noesel, C. J. M. (2008). Effects of processing delay, formalin fixation, and immunohistochemistry on RNA recovery from formalin-fixed paraffin-embedded tissue sections. Diagn. Mol. Pathol. 17, 51–58. doi: 10.1097/PDM.0b013e31814b8866

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Warnes, G. R., Bolker, B., Bonebakker, L., Gentleman, R., Liaw, W. H. A., Lumley, T., et al. (2016). Gplots: Various R Programming Tools for Plotting Data. R package version 3.0.1. (Online). Available online at: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=gplots

Wencke, W., Sanchez-Cabo, F., and Ricote, M. (2015). GOplot: an R package for visually combining expression data with functional analysis. Bioinformatics 31, 2912–2914. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btv300

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whitehead, A., and Crawford, D. L. (2005). Variation in tissue-specific gene expression among natural populations. Genome Biol. 6:R13. doi: 10.1186/gb-2005-6-2-r13

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wong, R. Y., and Hofmann, H. A. (2010). “Behavioural Genomics: an organismic perspective,” in Encyclopedia of Life Sciences. (Chichester: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.).

Google Scholar

Wood, L. S., Desjardins, J. K., and Fernald, R. D. (2011). Effects of stress and motivation on performing a spatial task. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 95, 277–285. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2010.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, D., and Smyth, G. (2012). Camera: a competitive gene set test accounting for inter-gene correlation. Nucleic Acids Res. 40:e133. doi: 10.1093/nar/gks461

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yoshida, M., Okamura, I., and Uematsu, K. (2004). Involvement of the cerebellum in classical fear conditioning in goldfish. Behav. Brain Res. 153, 143–148. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2003.11.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zapala, M. A., Hovatta, I., Ellison, J. A., Wodicka, L., Del Rio, J. A., Tennant, R., et al. (2005). Adult mouse brain gene expression patterns bear an embryologic imprint. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 102, 10357–10362. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0503357102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zohar, Y., Munoz-Cueto, J. A., Elizur, A., and Kah, O. (2010). Neuroendocrinology of reproduction in teleost fish. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 165, 438–455. doi: 10.1016/j.ygcen.2009.04.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: cerebellum, social behavior, cichlid fish, equivalence testing, gene-level FDR, neurogenomics

Citation: Derycke S, Kéver L, Herten K, Van den Berge K, Van Steenberge M, Van Houdt J, Clement L, Poncin P, Parmentier E and Verheyen E (2018) Neurogenomic Profiling Reveals Distinct Gene Expression Profiles Between Brain Parts That Are Consistent in Ophthalmotilapia Cichlids. Front. Neurosci. 12:136. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2018.00136

Received: 08 September 2017; Accepted: 20 February 2018;
Published: 09 March 2018.

Edited by:

David F. Clayton, Queen Mary University of London, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Hans A. Hofmann, University of Texas at Austin, United States
Christopher N. Balakrishnan, East Carolina University, United States
Alison Bell, University of Illinois at Urbana–Champaign, United States

Copyright © 2018 Derycke, Kéver, Herten, Van den Berge, Van Steenberge, Van Houdt, Clement, Poncin, Parmentier and Verheyen. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Sofie Derycke, s.derycke@ugent.be

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.