- 1Department of Management Science and Engineering, School of Management, Zhejiang University, Hangzhou, China
- 2Neuromanagement Lab, Zhejiang University, Hangzhou, China
To investigate the time course of the neural processing of facial attractiveness and its influence on fairness consideration during social interactions, event-related potentials (ERP) were recorded from 21 male subjects performing a two-person Ultimatum Game (UG). During this bargaining game, the male subjects played responders who decided whether to accept offers from female proposers, whose facial images (grouped as “attractive” and “unattractive”) were presented prior to the offer presentation. The behavioral data demonstrated that the acceptance ratio increased with the fairness level of the offers and, more importantly, the subjects were more likely to accept unfair offers when presented with the attractive-face condition compared with the unattractive-face condition. The reaction times (RTs) for five offers (1:9, 2:8, 3:7, 4:6, and 5:5) in the unattractive-face condition were not significantly different. In contrast, the subjects reacted slower to the attractive proposers' unfair offers and quicker to fair offers. The ERP analysis of the face presentation demonstrated a decreased early negativity (N2) and enhanced late positive potentials (LPPs) elicited by the attractive faces compared with the unattractive faces. In addition, the feedback-related negativity (FRN) in response to an offer presentation was not significantly different for the unfair (1:9 and 2:8) and fair (4:6 and 5:5) offers in the attractive-face condition. However, the unfair offers generated larger FRNs compared with the fair offers in the unattractive-face condition (consistent with prior studies). A similar effect was identified for P300. The present study demonstrated an undermining effect of proposer facial attractiveness on responder consideration of offer fairness during the UG.
Introduction
Classic economic theory expects individuals to be rational and self-interest driven. For example, in a typical Ultimatum Game (UG), recipients were assumed to accept an amount of money to maximize gains. However, in a real bargaining situation, unsatisfying offers were often rejected by responders in favor of some notions of fairness. Empirical studies have demonstrated that proposers typically offered approximately 40% (fair offers) of the total money, and responders were more likely to reject 20% or less of unfair offers (Güth et al., 1982; Camerer, 2003; Sanfey et al., 2003; Oosterbeek et al., 2004). In addition to irrationality within humanity, several social factors influence individuals' fairness considerations, such as social distance (Campanhã et al., 2011; Wu et al., 2011a), initial ownership (Wu et al., 2012), self-contribution to income (Guo et al., 2014), social exclusion (Qu et al., 2013), and social comparison (Wu et al., 2011b, 2012).
Attractive faces can be perceived as informative or evolutionarily valuable in strategic games. Evolutionary theory states that facially attractive individuals are perceived to be physically healthier (Shackelford and Larsen, 1999), predict longevity (Henderson and Anglin, 2003), or have increased fertility (Thornhill and Gangestad, 1999). Zeng et al. (2012) performed a novel task of deciding whether to view an attractive female's picture or gain a certain amount of money while recording the male participants' brain activities. The results demonstrated stronger neural responses in the posterior regions related to the evaluation processes, which were induced by the attractive female pictures compared with the cash equivalents; these findings suggest that the attractive females were perceived as socially rewarding. An increasing body of evidence from functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) has also demonstrated that attractive faces activate the nucleus accumbens, orbitofrontal cortex and ventral striatum (Aharon et al., 2001; O'Doherty et al., 2003; Ishai, 2007; Winston et al., 2007; Cloutier et al., 2008; Tsukiura and Cabeza, 2011), which are all dedicated to the reward system, and thereby indicates a momentary equivalent role of facial attractiveness.
Our first concern in this study was how the brain responds to attractive faces. As we previously discussed, brain imaging studies have demonstrated that several brain regions are differentially responsive to attractive and unattractive faces (Aharon et al., 2001; O'Doherty et al., 2003). Evidence from electrophysiological experiments has illustrated the time course of the neural processes of facial attractiveness. An early posterior negativity (EPN) is more sensitive to attractive faces (Werheid et al., 2007) in the frontal brain region (N300, Zhang et al., 2011). Werheid et al. (2007) reported enhanced late positive potential (LPP) amplitudes in response to attractive compared with unattractive faces during an attractiveness classification task, which was consistent with previous findings (Johnston and Oliver-Rodriguez, 1997; Oliver-Rodriguez et al., 1999). A similar LPP effect was identified in other experiments (Schacht et al., 2008; Zhang et al., 2011). Chen et al. (2012) investigated the same two temporal stages of processing attractive and unattractive faces, including an early negativity (N2) and late LPP. However, the authors identified a contrary LPP pattern. Given the various findings regarding how facial attractiveness is processed, we continue to explore the temporal features of facial attractiveness processing using the high time resolutions of event-related potentials (ERPs).
In studies of labor markets, the theory of “beauty premium” and “plain penalty” has been noted by Hamermesh and Biddle (1993) in which attractive individuals were more likely to be hired and promoted, and workers of above average beauty earned approximately 10–15 percent more than workers of below average beauty. To date, several behavioral studies have investigated the effects of facial attractiveness on adherence to social norms, such as fairness (Solnick and Schweitzer, 1999) and cooperation (Mulford et al., 1998; Eckel and Wilson, 2006; Eckel and Petrie, 2011). However, the temporal dynamics of this process are rarely discussed. In this study, we investigated the influence of facial attractiveness on individuals' consideration of fairness during the UG. More specifically, we explored the “beauty premium” effect on fairness and its underlying neural mechanisms.
The UG is a stylized bargaining situation that has been widely used to study behaviors in social interactions. In this task, one player (proposer/allocator) proposes how to split a certain amount of money to another player (responder/recipient). The responder subsequently observes the offer and decides whether to object or accept it. If the offer is accepted, the money is divided as the proposer offered; otherwise, both individuals receive nothing. Several ERP publications have examined the brain responses to fair and unfair offers during the UG, which benefits from its high resolution time and is indexed by two ERP components, the feedback-related negativity (FRN) and P300 (Polezzi et al., 2008; Boksem and De Cremer, 2010; Hewig et al., 2011; Wu et al., 2011a, 2012; Alexopoulos et al., 2012; Luo et al., 2014).
FRN is a frontal-central negative deflection that peaks at 200–350 ms following the outcome presentation. ERP source localization and fMRI evidence suggest that the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) is primarily responsible for negative feedback processing and FRN generation. According to reinforcement learning theory (RL-theory), the ACC activity is modulated by dopamine signals from the midbrain, where positive and negative prediction errors are coded. Negative prediction errors induced by unfavorable outcomes initiate phasic decreases in dopamine inputs and therefore result in increased ACC activity, which is reflected as a more negative FRN component (Gehring and Willoughby, 2002; Yeung and Sanfey, 2004; Ma et al., 2014). Studies regarding game theory and economic interactions have demonstrated that a violation of social norm could also generate an enhanced FRN response. For example, when responders encountered an unequal offer during an asset division, a more negative FRN was elicited compared with equal offers (Polezzi et al., 2008; Boksem and De Cremer, 2010; Hewig et al., 2011; Wu et al., 2011a).
Based on these studies, we expected a larger FRN for the unfair offers in our experiment. More importantly, this FRN pattern was predicted to be modulated by the attractiveness of proposers. Multiple researches (Campanhã et al., 2011; Wu et al., 2011a; Qu et al., 2013) have shown that allocator condition can differentiate individuals' brain responses to fair/unfair offers. For example, recipients became more sensitive to individuals who previously excluded them compared with an includer and a stranger and generated a more negative FRN in response to unfair offers (Qu et al., 2013). The modulation of social distance between recipients and responders on the recipients' fairness consideration was also discussed and found to influence both FRN and P300 components during the evaluation process of offers (Wu et al., 2011a). In this experiment, face images of proposers were classified into two groups based on level of attractiveness and presented to subjects who played as the recipients. As previous studies suggested that men are more sensitive and vulnerable to facial beauty (Buss, 1989; Cloutier et al., 2008; van Hooff et al., 2011), we recruited 24 male participants and presented them with 300 photographs of female allocators (half attractive, half unattractive), which enabled us to manipulate a one-shot UG across the experiment. Considering the reward equivalent function and theory of “beauty premium,” recipients' fairness toward attractive allocators may be attenuated and bring changes to FRN waveforms.
We also examined whether another ERP component, P300, would be influenced by the “beauty premium” effect. The P300 is the most positive peak in the 300–600 ms period after the presentation of feedback; it typically increases its magnitude from the frontal to parietal sites. Prior studies have demonstrated that P300 was sensitive to the magnitude and valence of reward and was more positive to a larger reward compared with a smaller reward (Yeung and Sanfey, 2004; Wu and Zhou, 2009), as well as to positive feedback compared with negative feedback (Hajcak et al., 2005, 2007; Wu and Zhou, 2009). In its extension to game theory, P300 was more positive during fair offers compared with unfair offers (Wu et al., 2011a, 2012; Qu et al., 2013). The similar pattern of P300 was expected to be observed in our experiment.
Material and Methods
Participants
Twenty-four right-handed male subjects participated in this study. The participants were all students of Zhejiang University aged 18–26 years (M = 22.19 years, SD = 1.78 years). They were all native Chinese speakers with normal or corrected-to-normal vision and had no history of neurological disorders or mental disease. Informed consent was obtained from all subjects prior to the ERP experiment. The data from three subjects were discarded for excessive recoding artifacts and misunderstanding the rules of the bargaining task; thus, 21 valid subjects were included in the final data analysis. This experiment was approved by the Ethical Committee of Neuromanagement Lab of Zhejiang University.
Experimental Procedure
All facial images were obtained from the CAS-PEAL-R1 Face Database, photo pools of PKU and CAS and the Internet; the images were unfamiliar to the subjects (no movie stars, singers or other celebrities). All faces were gray processed by Photoshop software and edited to a uniform size (4.5 by 4 cm, 220 by 200 pixels). Three hundred images of Chinese female faces were rated for their attractiveness (from 1 = “not attractive at all” to 7 = “extremely attractive”) by 20 male students prior to the ERP experiment. The ratings of the two facial categories were compared with a paired t-test, and the attractiveness was significantly different [Mattractive = 5.13, Munattractive = 2.07; t(19) = 14.531, p < 0.001]. All 300 images were categorized into two groups: attractive-face and unattractive-face (each group had 150 female faces paired with 150 offers).
Another group of male subjects were seated comfortably in a dim, sound-attenuated, electrically shielded room. An introduction of the modified version of the UG was presented on written paper. The experimental stimuli were presented in the center of a computer screen at a distance of 100 cm. A keypad was provided to the subjects to make choices. The experiment consisted of three blocks, and each block contained 100 trials that featured 50 attractive and 50 unattractive facial images. Prior to the initiation of the formal experiment, each subject practiced 20 trials to familiarize themselves with the procedure.
A single trial is illustrated in Figure 1. A fixation appeared at the beginning of each trial for 400–600 ms on a black screen. A photo of the allocator was subsequently presented for 2000 ms followed by her proposal of how to split ¥10 between herself and the responder. The subject (responder) had sufficient time to decide whether to accept or reject the offer by pressing the keypad. If the subject chose to accept the offer, then he and the female allocator would receive the amount of money she suggested; otherwise, they received nothing. Once he made the decision, the final income from this trial was presented on the screen for 2000 ms prior to continuing to another trial. The subjects were told that the photos and offers were collected in a previous experiment, and they were going to bargain with different female allocators who really existed. The payment for their participation was ¥30 (approximately $4.8) plus the income of two randomly selected trials. There were five different offer conditions (¥1, ¥2, ¥3, ¥4, and ¥5), and each offer was repeated 30 times. An average analysis was based on the trials that represented unfair offers (¥1 and ¥2) and fair offers (¥4 and ¥5). The counterbalance was manipulated among the subjects. The E-prime 2.0 software package (Psychology Software tools, Pittsburgh, PA, USA) was adopted for the stimuli presentation, triggers and response recording.
Figure 1. A single trial of the experimental procedure. The participants first saw either an attractive face or an unattractive face prior to the offer presentation. The participants made their choices by pressing the keypad with an unlimited time to make a decision. The screen subsequently displayed the final payoffs.
EEG Recordings
Scalp voltages were recorded (band-pass 0.05–70 Hz, sampling rate 500 Hz) with the Neuroscan Synamp2 Amplifier (Scan 4.3.1, Neurosoft Labs, Inc., VA, USA) using a 64-channel electro cap with Ag/AgCl electrodes according to the standard international 10–20 system. A cephalic (forehead) location was connected as the ground. The left mastoid was chosen as a reference, and the recorded EEGs were off-line re-referenced to the average of the left and right mastoids. An electro-oculogram (EOG) was recorded from electrodes placed 10 mm from the lateral canthi of both eyes (horizontal EOG) and above and below the left eye (vertical EOG). The electrode impedance was maintained below 5 kΩ during the recording.
Data Analysis
For the behavioral data analysis, repeated measures ANOVA was used to compare the acceptance ratios and reaction times (RTs) across 10 conditions: 2 (attractiveness: attractive and unattractive) × 5 (offer: 1:9, 2:8, 3:7, 4:6, 5:5). The post-hoc analyses were conducted using the Bonferroni correction.
The EEG data were analyzed using Neuroscan 4.3.1. The EOG artifacts were corrected followed by digital filtering through a zero phase shift (low pass at 30 Hz, 24 dB/octave). The EEGs were segmented for 1000 ms into epochs initiated at 200 ms before and 800 ms after the stimulus onset. The whole epoch was subsequently baseline-corrected by the 200 ms interval prior to stimulus onset. Trials that contained amplifier clipping, bursts of electromyography activity, or peak-to-peak deflections that exceeded ±80 μV were excluded from the final average. During the photo presentation, the EEG epochs were averaged for the attractive and unattractive faces. During the offer presentation, the EEG epochs were separately averaged for attractiveness (attractive/unattractive face) × valence (fair/unfair offer). Therefore, there were four conditions: attractive face-fair, attractive face-unfair, unattractive face-fair, and unattractive face-unfair.
Based on visual observations of the grand average waveforms and previous ERP studies (Wu et al., 2011b; Ullsperger et al., 2014) regarding outcome processing, two ERP components (frontal FRN and parietal P300) were analyzed. We averaged the ERP amplitude from the time range 270–340 ms and 350–550 ms post-offer presentation for the FRN and P300 analyses, respectively. According to the scalp distribution and previous reports, we selected nine electrode sites (F3, Fz, F4, FC3, FCz, FC4, C3, Cz, and C4) in the frontal and central areas for the FRN analysis and six electrode sites (CP3, CPz, CP4, P3, Pz, and P4) in the central-parietal areas for the P300 analysis. To investigate the effect across groups, a 2 (attractiveness: attractive and unattractive) × 2 (valence: fair and unfair offers) × 9 (electrodes) repeated measures ANOVA was conducted for FRN. A similar analysis was performed to examine the P300 amplitudes. A simple effect analysis was conducted when the interaction effect was significant. The Greenhouse-Geisser correction was applied in all statistical analyses when necessary.
To examine the neural processing of attractiveness of female allocators, the mean amplitudes of the frontal-central N2 (F3, Fz, F4, FC3, FCz, FC4, C3, Cz, and C4) and the central-parietal LPP (CP3, CPZ, CP4, P3, Pz, and P4) were further analyzed in the ranges of 240–280 and 350–550 ms post-onset of face presentation, respectively. Two-Way ANOVAs were conducted regarding the N2 and LPP components. The ANOVA factors were face (attractive and unattractive faces) and electrode sites. The degrees of freedom of the F-ratio were corrected according to the Greenhouse-Geisser method.
Results
Acceptance Ratios
The ANOVA analysis for the acceptance ratio (Figure 2) identified main effects of attractiveness [F(1,20) = 16.709, p = 0.001] and offers [F(1,20) = 43.893, p < 0.001]. The subjects accepted more offers in the attractive-face condition (mean ± SE, 68.6 ± 4.2%) compared with the unattractive-face condition (mean ± SE, 55.6 ± 5.5%). The acceptance ratio of offer (5:5) was significantly increased than offer (3:7) (p = 0.003), offer (2:8) (p < 0.001) and offer (1:9) (p < 0.001). However, a post-hoc comparison indicated there was no significant difference (p = 0.129) between offer (5:5) and offer (4:6). Offer (3:7) was accepted more than offer (2:8) (p = 0.002) and offer (1:9) (p < 0.001). A comparison between the two least fair offers was also significant (p = 0.022). The interaction effect was also significant [F(1,20) = 3.426, p = 0.027]. All five offers were accepted more in the attractive-face conditions: for offer (1:9), p = 0.023; for offer (2:8), p = 0.009; for offer (3:7), p = 0.001; for offer (4:6), p = 0.007; for offer (5:5), p = 0.041. The acceptance ratios of each condition are reported in Table 1.
Figure 2. The reaction time and acceptance ratio of five offers in two face conditions. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01.
Response Time
The ANOVA analysis for the acceptance ratios (Figure 2) identified a main effect of offer [F(1,20) = 3.636, p = 0.041]. The subjects typically responded the quickest to offer (5:5) (mean ± SE, 888.27 ± 99.31 ms), which was significantly faster than offer (2:8) (p = 0.007) and offer (3:7) (p = 0.001); however, the main effect of attractiveness was not significant [F(1,20) = 0.128, p = 0.725]. The interaction effect exhibited a marginally significant difference [F(1, 20) = 3.453, p = 0.072] with longer RTs for offer (1:9) (p = 0.043) and shorter times for offer (4:6) (p = 0.02) in the attractive-face condition compared with the unattractive-face condition. Other comparisons between the two face conditions produced statistical differences. In the unattractive-face condition, the RTs of the five offers were not significantly different (Table 1). However, in the attractive-face condition, the results indicated significant differences between the five offer conditions: offer (1:9) to offer (4:6), p = 0.001; offer (1:9) to offer (5:5), p = 0.002; offer(2:8) to offer (4:6), p < 0.001; offer (2:8) to offer (5:5), p = 0.003; offer (3:7) to offer (4:6), p < 0.001; and offer (3:7) to offer (5:5), p = 0.006.
N2 and LPP
ANOVA analysis for N2 and parietal LPP (Figure 3) identified main effects of attractiveness with a more negative [F(1, 20) = 8.253, p = 0.009] N2 elicited by the unattractive faces (mean ± SE, 0.954 ± 0.632 μV) compared with the attractive faces (mean ± SE, 1.875 ± 0.6 μV). A more positive [F(1, 20) = 7.12, p = 0.015] LPP was elicited by the attractive faces (mean ± SE, 4.673 ± 0.618 μV) compared with the unattractive faces (mean ± SE, 3.581 ± 0.479 μV). The interaction between attractiveness and electrode for N2 was not significant [F(1, 20) = 0.666, p = 0.612], but it was significant for LPP [F(1, 20) = 6.209, p < 0.001].
Figure 3. The ERP grand-average waveforms and topographical maps of N2 and LPP at the midline Fz, FCz, Cz, CPz, and Pz for the attractive and unattractive face conditions. The shaded 240–280 ms time window was used for the mean N2 amplitude. The shaded 350–550 ms time window at CPz and Pz was used for the mean LPP amplitude.
FRN
As presented in Figure 4, the ANOVA analysis for the FRN identified a marginal main effect of attractiveness [F(1, 20) = 4.299, p = 0.051] and a main effect of valence [F(1, 20) = 11.073, p = 0.003]. Moreover, the interaction effect between attractiveness and valence was also significant [F(1, 20) = 5.596, p = 0.028] with a more negative FRN for the unfair offers (mean ± SE, −2.576 ± 0.473 μV) compared with the fair offers (mean ± SE, −1.064 ± 0.529 μV) in the unattractive-face condition (p = 0.002) but not in the attractive-face condition (p = 0.115, unfair offers: −2.798 ± 0.608 μV, fair offers: −2.293 ± 0.636 μV).
Figure 4. The ERP grand-average waveforms of FRN at Fz, FCz, and Cz. The time window for FRN was 270–340 ms.
P300
For the central-parietal component P300 (Figure 5), we identified a significant main effect for valence in which the fair offers (mean ± SE, 4.383 ± 0.689 μv) elicited a more positive [F(1, 20) = 11.951, p = 0.002] P300 compared with the unfair offers (mean ±i SE, 3.153 ± 0.697 μV). A comparison between the two face conditions (attractive face: 3.731 ± 0.713 μV, unattractive face: 3.805 ± 0.685 μV) indicated no significant effect [F(1, 20) = 0.853, p = 0.853]. The interaction effect between attractiveness and valence approached significance [F(1, 20) = 3.956, p = 0.061], and the P300 was more positive for the fair offers (mean ± SE, 4.823 ± 0.724 μV) compared with the unfair offers (mean ± SE, 2.788 ± 0.758 μV) in the unattractive-face condition (p = 0.002) but not in the attractive-face condition (p = 0.413, unfair offers: 3.518 ± 0.768 μV, fair offers: 3.944 ± 0.745 μV). Additionally, the two face conditions produced similar P300 amplitudes in response to the unfair (p = 0.253) and fair (p = 0.099) offers.
Figure 5. The ERP grand-average waveforms of P300 for fair and unfair offers in the two face conditions at CPz and Pz. The time window for LPP was 350–550 ms.
Discussion
The primary aim of this study was to elucidate how facial attractiveness modulated the response to unfair/fair offers during an UG. The behavioral data indicated stable RT results in the unattractive-face condition when the subjects responded to five offers, which suggests they followed a clearly defined strategy when bargaining with the unattractive-face group (similar to a previous study of fair and unfair offers Polezzi et al., 2008). However, a remarkable fluctuation in the RT was identified during the attractive-face condition, and the subjects delayed their decision making in the unfair conditions (1:9, 2:8, and 3:7) and responded quicker to the fair offers (4:6 and 5:5). More importantly, the subjects hesitated more when they received an offer (1:9) from an attractive allocator compared with when an unattractive female proposed an offer (4:6). Together with the findings of varied RTs in the attractive-face condition, this pattern implied that offers from proposers with increased physical attractiveness with mid-value offers, as presented in Polezzi et al.'s study (Polezzi et al., 2008), involved more complex decision making mechanisms with less predictable outcomes. The perceived attractiveness of the proposers could facilitate the subjects' reaction process toward a fair offer (4:6) and could also induce a struggle as to whether to accept an unfair offer (1:9).
A comparison of the acceptance ratio indicated that male recipients were more prone to accept an unfair offer if it was provided by an attractive female proposer. Solnick and Schweitzer (1999) first discussed the influence of physical appearance on decisions in an UG in which attractive females were often offered more than ordinary-looking individuals. The behavioral data in the current study were consistent with the notion of the “beauty premium.” (Campanhã et al., 2011) also reported a decreased rejection rate during the UG when participants bargained with a friend compared with a stranger. The authors attributed this effect to a fundamental bias caused by the friendship's effect on the subjective perception of fairness. In the present study, similar to the friendship effect, facial attractiveness can also be perceived as a type of subjective value (Cloutier et al., 2008; Zeng et al., 2012) that aroused reward-related neural circuitries (Aharon et al., 2001; O'Doherty et al., 2003; Senior, 2003). Furthermore, the questionnaires obtained after the experiment demonstrated that the subjects were more willing and looked forward to seeing the appearance of attractive faces. The impact of affections on the decision process during the UG has been demonstrated in multiple studies (Harlé and Sanfey, 2007; Andrade and Ariely, 2009; Kirk et al., 2011), and Andrade and Ariely (2009) pointed out the happy responder rejection rate (40%) of unfair offers during the UG was significantly lower than that of angry receivers (73%). Based on this evidence, the enjoyment of the perceived beauty could relieve the subjects' dissatisfaction with an unequal money distribution and attenuate their intention to decline unequal offers to punish the selfish proposers.
The ERP data first illustrated how the brain responds to facial attractiveness. A more positive LPP was generated between 350 and 550 ms in response to attractive faces compared with unattractive faces, which was consistent with previous findings (Johnston and Oliver-Rodriguez, 1997; Oliver-Rodriguez et al., 1999; Werheid et al., 2007; van Hooff et al., 2011; Zhang et al., 2011; Lu et al., 2014). LPP may reflect deliberative evaluation, and attractiveness could be perceived as a more emotionally significant stimulus, which would therefore enlarge the LPP amplitude. Additionally, considering the rewarding value of beauty (Aharon et al., 2001), attractive faces could draw more motivational attention from male subjects, which could evoke increased posterior positivity. An early negativity, N2, was also elicited from the frontal area to the parietal area, and it was larger in the unattractive-face condition. In a judgment task to categorize faces as “attractive” and “unattractive” (Zhang et al., 2011), an early negative component (250–350 ms) enhanced the response to unattractive faces. In a face recognition task (Grasso et al., 2009), mother generated a smaller N2 when perceived the picture of her son and much larger N2 when saw pictures of others, which was explained as a mismatch effect that mothers were always looking forward to seeing their children's faces and when they could not, cognitive control elicited an enhanced negativity. Similarly in our study, male subjects always anticipated to see beauty faces and when disfavored unattractive faces showed, elicited a more intense mental conflict and resulted in a more negative N2. Our findings further support the idea that the processing of facial attractiveness is initiated as early as 200 ms after the stimuli.
The FRN reflects the affective appraisal of negative events, such as a loss of money or a worse outcome than expected. An enlarged FRN in the frontal-central areas was identified when the participants received unfair offers (1:9 and 2:8) compared with fair offers (4:6 and 5:5), which is consistent with previous findings (Boksem and De Cremer, 2010; van der Veen and Sahibdin, 2011). The differential FRN responses to unfair and fair offers in the unattractive-face condition reflects the detection of a violation from the subjects' expectations (even asset distribution is an expected social norm) (Fehr and Gächter, 2002; Fehr and Fischbacher, 2004).
Surprisingly, we further analyzed the FRN in the attractive-face condition and demonstrated that this FRN effect disappeared. A similar pattern was observed in a dictator game (Wu et al., 2011a) when participants played with strangers or with one of their friends. Friendship was thought to be a factor that promoted an egalitarian distribution of assets; consequently, subjects' feelings and judgments of the strangers were weakened. In our experiment, the differentiated FRN patterns identified in the two face conditions suggested that male responders' sensitivity and dissatisfaction with inequality were stronger in the unattractive-face than in the attractive-face condition. Sanfey et al. (2003) demonstrated an activation of both emotional and cognitional-related brain regions when subjects performed an UG. Then involvement of emotional brain system in the UG was also supported by the evidences from participants' EEG, skin conductance responses (SCR) and subjective ratings of emotional valence to monetary offers (van't Wout et al., 2006; Hewig et al., 2011). Based on the correlation between mood change and FRN amplitude previously described (Qu et al., 2013), when subjects experiences a negative emotion in exclusion, they were subsequently more sensitive to the excluders' offers in the UG. Previous studies on social function of beauty showed that male subjects are willing to exert effort (Aharon et al., 2001) or sacrifice a certain amount of money (Eckel and Petrie, 2011; Zeng et al., 2012) to gain access to attractive female faces. The neural mechanism of the preference for beauty was outlined above that the perception of an attractive face could activate the reward circuitry of the brain and result in money-equivalent pleasure. Considering the rewarding effect of the beauty, when responders felt hurt by unequal treatment, the perceived attractiveness compensated for the negative emotions and distracted their fairness sensitivity. Therefore, we posited that it was subjects' enjoyment of facial attractiveness that weakened their consideration of fairness, decreased FRN responses for the comparative processes and resulted in the null effect of FRN.
Additionally, an increased P300 was identified in the fair condition in response to an increased amount of monetary allocation. In general, P300 is thought to represent the motivational significance (Yeung and Sanfey, 2004; Nieuwenhuis et al., 2005) and attentional allocation (Gray et al., 2004; Linden, 2005) of the outcome. The main effect of the valence of the offer with more positive responses to fair offers compared with unfair offers was consistent with previous studies (Wu et al., 2011a, 2012; Qu et al., 2013; Moser et al., 2014). Stronger P300 responses to fair offers indicated that the subjects attached more attention to larger momentary income and placed more value on equal divisions compared with unequal divisions.
In addition, a comparison of P300 for unfair and fair offers followed this general pattern in the unattractive-face condition; however, no valence effect was identified in the attractive-face condition. LPP, with a similar function in social evaluation as P300, was modulated by social comparison in an UG (Wu et al., 2011b; Moser et al., 2014). In Wu et al.'s study (2011b) social comparison was evoked when average level information was provided and highlighted to the subjects, and the subjects denoted more attention to the average comparison than their direct proposers. Due to this setting, the offer effect on LPP that moderately unfair offers elicited more positive-going responses compared with highly unfair offers only predominately appeared in an upward comparison when the subjects received more than average and vanished in other comparative conditions when the subjects were equally treated or received less. In a recent experimental setting, the offers not only included momentary meaning but also conveyed social information, such as the evaluation of perceived fairness and the proposers' looks, which could also evoke subject outcome comparisons with attractive or unattractive partners. Because the attractive-face condition also produced a lack of difference in P300 with respect to fair and unfair offers, this result can be explained as the offer effect on P300 was overshadowed by proposers' facial attractiveness that subjects attached no differential attentional resources to the two types of offers that originated from attractive proposers. In the unattractive-face condition, unfair offers were highly disfavored and captured less attention compared with fair offers, which therefore generated a smaller P300.
Conclusion
To summarize, the “beauty premium” influenced responder fairness during the UG. Unfair offers from attractive female allocators were more acceptable to the male subjects, and the males presented with fluctuating RTs to the five offers in comparison with a stable reaction pattern in the unattractive-face condition. The ERP data supported the behavioral findings. In the early (FRN) and late (P300) stages of outcome evaluation, the subjects' fairness consideration was undermined by the “beauty premium,” which resulted in null FRN and P300 effects in the attractive-face condition. Additionally, the time course of brain responses to facial attractiveness illustrated that attractive faces elicited a decreased N2 and an enhanced LPP compared with unattractive faces.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
We thank Guan-xiong Pei for his help with the figure construction. This work was supported by Grant No. 71371167 from the National Natural Science Foundation of China. LM was funded by Philosophy and Social Science Planning Project of Zhejiang Province (No. 14NDJC011Z).
References
Aharon, I., Etcoff, N., Ariely, D., Chabris, C. F., O'Connor, E., and Breiter, H. C. (2001). Beautiful faces have variable reward value: fMRI and behavioral evidence. Neuron 32, 537–551. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00491-3
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Alexopoulos, J., Pfabigan, D. M., Lamm, C., Bauer, H., and Fischmeister, F. P. (2012). Do we care about the powerless third? An ERP study of the three-person ultimatum game. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:59. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00059
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Andrade, E. B., and Ariely, D. (2009). The enduring impact of transient emotions on decision making. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 109, 1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2009.02.003
Boksem, M. A., and De Cremer, D. (2010). Fairness concerns predict medial frontal negativity amplitude in ultimatum bargaining. Soc. Neurosci. 5, 118–128. doi: 10.1080/17470910903202666
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Buss, D. M. (1989). Sex differences in human mate preferences: evolutionary hypotheses tested in 37 cultures. Behav. Brain Sci. 12, 1–14. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00023992
Camerer, C. (2003). Behavioral Game Theory: Experiments in Strategic Interaction. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Campanhã, C., Minati, L., Fregni, F., and Boggio, P. S. (2011). Responding to unfair offers made by a friend: neuroelectrical activity changes in the anterior medial prefrontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 31, 15569–15574. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1253-11.2011
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Chen, J., Zhong, J., Zhang, Y., Li, P., Zhang, A., Tan, Q., et al. (2012). Electrophysiological correlates of processing facial attractiveness and its influence on cooperative behavior. Neurosci. Lett. 517, 65–70. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2012.02.082
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Cloutier, J., Heatherton, T. F., Whalen, P. J., and Kelley, W. M. (2008). Are attractive people rewarding? Sex differences in the neural substrates of facial attractiveness. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 20, 941–951. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20062
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Eckel, C. C., and Petrie, R. (2011). Face value. Am. Econ. Rev. 101, 1497–1513. doi: 10.1257/aer.101.4.1497
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Eckel, C., and Wilson, R. (2006). Judging a book by its cover: beauty and expectations in the trust game. Polit. Res. Q. 59, 189–202. doi: 10.1177/106591290605900202
Fehr, E., and Fischbacher, U. (2004). Third-party punishment and social norms. Evol. Hum. Behav. 25, 63–87. doi: 10.1016/S1090-5138(04)00005-4
Fehr, E., and Gächter, S. (2002). Altruistic punishment in humans. Nature 415, 137–140. doi: 10.1038/415137a
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Gehring, W. J., and Willoughby, A. R. (2002). The medial frontal cortex and the rapid processing of monetary gains and losses. Science 295, 2279–2282. doi: 10.1126/science.1066893
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Grasso, D. J., Moser, J. S., Dozier, M., and Simons, R. (2009). ERP correlates of attention allocation in mothers processing faces of their children. Biol. Psychol. 81, 95–102. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2009.03.001
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Gray, H. M., Ambady, N., Lowenthal, W. T., and Deldin, P. (2004). P300 as an index of attention to self-relevant stimuli. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 40, 216–224. doi: 10.1016/S0022-1031(03)00092-1
Guo, X., Zheng, L., Cheng, X., Chen, M., Zhu, L., Li, J., et al. (2014). Neural responses to unfairness and fairness depend on self-contribution to the income. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 9, 1498–1505. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst131
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Güth, W., Schmittberger, R., and Schwarze, B. (1982). An experimental analysis of ultimatum bargaining. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 3, 367–388. doi: 10.1016/0167-2681(82)90011-7
Hajcak, G., Holroyd, C. B., Moser, J. S., and Simons, R. F. (2005). Brain potentials associated with expected and unexpected good and bad outcomes. Psychophysiology 42, 161–170. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2005.00278.x
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Hajcak, G., Moser, J. S., Holroyd, C. B., and Simons, R. F. (2007). It's worse than you thought: the feedback negativity and violations of reward prediction in gambling tasks. Psychophysiology 44, 905–912. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00567.x
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Hamermesh, D. S., and Biddle, J. E. (1993). Beauty and the labor market (No. w4518). Natl. Bur. Econ. Res. 84, 1174–1194. doi: 10.3386/w4518
Harlé, K. M., and Sanfey, A. G. (2007). Incidental sadness biases social economic decisions in the Ultimatum Game. Emotion 7:876. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.4.876
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Henderson, J. J., and Anglin, J. M. (2003). Facial attractiveness predicts longevity. Evol. Hum. Behav. 24, 351–356. doi: 10.1016/S1090-5138(03)00036-9
Hewig, J., Kretschmer, N., Trippe, R. H., Hecht, H., Coles, M. G., Holroyd, C. B., et al. (2011). Why humans deviate from rational choice. Psychophysiology 48, 507–514. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.01081.x
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Ishai, A. (2007). Sex, beauty and the orbitofrontal cortex. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 63, 181–185. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2006.03.010
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Johnston, V. S., and Oliver-Rodriguez, J. C. (1997). Facial beauty and the late positive component of event-related potentials. J. Sex Res. 34, 188–198. doi: 10.1080/00224499709551884
Kirk, U., Downar, J., and Montague, P. R. (2011). Interoception drives increased rational decision-making in meditators playing the ultimatum game. Front. Neurosci. 5:49. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2011.00049
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Linden, D. E. (2005). The P300: where in the brain is it produced and what does it tell us? Neuroscientist 11, 563–576. doi: 10.1177/1073858405280524
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Lu, Y., Wang, J., Wang, L., Wang, J., and Qin, J. (2014). Neural responses to cartoon facial attractiveness: an event-related potential study. Neurosci. Bull. 30, 441–450. doi: 10.1007/s12264-013-1401-4
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Luo, Y., Wu, T., Broster, L. S., Feng, C., Zhang, D., Gu, R., et al. (2014). The temporal course of the influence of anxiety on fairness considerations. Psychophysiology 51, 834–842. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12235
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Ma, Q., Meng, L., Wang, L., and Shen, Q. (2014). I endeavor to make it: effort increases valuation of subsequent monetary reward. Behav. Brain Res. 261, 1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2013.11.045
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Moser, A., Gaertig, C., and Ruz, M. (2014). Social information and personal interests modulate neural activity during economic decision-making. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:31 doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00031
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Mulford, M., Orbell, J., Shatto, C., and Stockard, J. (1998). Physical attractiveness, opportunity, and success in everyday exchange. Am. J. Soc. 103, 1565–1592. doi: 10.1086/231401
Nieuwenhuis, S., Aston-Jones, G., and Cohen, J. D. (2005). Decision making, the P3, and the locus coeruleus–norepinephrine system. Psychol. Bull. 131:510. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.4.510
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
O'Doherty, J., Winston, J., Critchley, H., Perrett, D., Burt, D. M., and Dolan, R. J. (2003). Beauty in a smile: the role of medial orbitofrontal cortex in facial attractiveness. Neuropsychologia 41, 147–155. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00145-8
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Oliver-Rodriguez, J. C., Guan, Z., and Johnston, V. S. (1999). Gender differences in late positive components evoked by human faces. Psychophysiology 36, 176–185. doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.3620176
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Oosterbeek, H., Sloof, R., and van de Kuilen, G. (2004). Cultural differences in ultimatum game experiments: evidence from a meta-analysis. Exp. Econ. 7, 171–188. doi: 10.1023/B:EXEC.0000026978.14316.74
Polezzi, D., Daum, I., Rubaltelli, E., Lotto, L., Civai, C., Sartori, G., et al. (2008). Mentalizing in economic decision-making. Behav. Brain Res. 190, 218–223. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2008.03.003
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Qu, C., Wang, Y., and Huang, Y. (2013). Social exclusion modulates fairness consideration in the ultimatum game: an ERP study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:505. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00505
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Sanfey, A. G., Rilling, J. K., Aronson, J. A., Nystrom, L. E., and Cohen, J. D. (2003). The neural basis of economic decision-making in the ultimatum game. Science 300, 1755–1758. doi: 10.1126/science.1082976
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Schacht, A., Werheid, K., and Sommer, W. (2008). The appraisal of facial beauty is rapid but not mandatory. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 8, 132–142. doi: 10.3758/CABN.8.2.132
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Senior, C. (2003). Beauty in the brain of the beholder. Neuron 38, 525–528. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00293-9
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Shackelford, T. K., and Larsen, R. J. (1999). Facial attractiveness and physical health. Evol. Hum. Behav. 20, 71–76. doi: 10.1016/S1090-5138(98)00036-1
Solnick, S. J., and Schweitzer, M. E. (1999). The influence of physical attractiveness and gender on ultimatum game decisions. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 79, 199–215. doi: 10.1006/obhd.1999.2843
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Thornhill, R., and Gangestad, S. W. (1999). Facial attractiveness. Trends Cogn. Sci. 3, 452–460. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01403-5
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Tsukiura, T., and Cabeza, R. (2011). Remembering beauty: roles of orbitofrontal and hippocampal regions in successful memory encoding of attractive faces. Neuroimage 54, 653–660. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.07.046
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Ullsperger, M., Fischer, A. G., Nigbur, R., and Endrass, T. (2014). Neural mechanisms and temporal dynamics of performance monitoring. Trends Cogn. Sci. 18, 259–267. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2014.02.009
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
van't Wout, M., Kahn, R. S., Sanfey, A. G., and Aleman, A. (2006). Affective state and decision-making in the ultimatum game. Exp. Brain Res. 169, 564–568. doi: 10.1007/s00221-006-0346-5
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
van der Veen, F. M., and Sahibdin, P. P. (2011). Dissociation between medial frontal negativity and cardiac responses in the ultimatum game: effects of offer size and fairness. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 11, 516–525. doi: 10.3758/s13415-011-0050-1
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
van Hooff, J. C., Crawford, H., and van Vugt, M. (2011). The wandering mind of men: ERP evidence for gender differences in attention bias towards attractive opposite sex faces. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 6, 477–485. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsq066
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Werheid, K., Schacht, A., and Sommer, W. (2007). Facial attractiveness modulates early and late event-related brain potentials. Biol. Psychol. 76, 100–108. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.06.008
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Winston, J. S., O'Doherty, J., Kilner, J. M., Perrett, D. I., and Dolan, R. J. (2007). Brain systems for assessing facial attractiveness. Neuropsychologia 45, 195–206. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.05.009
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Wu, Y., Hu, J., van Dijk, E., Leliveld, M. C., and Zhou, X. (2012). Brain activity in fairness consideration during asset distribution: does the initial ownership play a role? PLoS ONE 7:e39627. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0039627
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Wu, Y., Leliveld, M. C., and Zhou, X. (2011a). Social distance modulates recipient's fairness consideration in the dictator game: an ERP study. Biol. Psychol. 88, 253–262. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.08.009
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Wu, Y., and Zhou, X. (2009). The P300 and reward valence, magnitude, and expectancy in outcome evaluation. Brain Res. 1286, 114–122. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.06.032
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Wu, Y., Zhou, Y., van Dijk, E., Leliveld, M. C., and Zhou, X. (2011b). Social comparison affects brain responses to fairness in asset division: an ERP study with the ultimatum game. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 5:131. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00131
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Yeung, N., and Sanfey, A. G. (2004). Independent coding of reward magnitude and valence in the human brain. J. Neurosci. 24, 6258–6264. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4537-03.2004
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Zeng, J., Wang, Y., and Zhang, Q. (2012). An ERP study on decisions between attractive females and money. PLoS ONE 7:e45945. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0045945
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Zhang, Y., Kong, F., Chen, H., Jackson, T., Han, L., Meng, J., et al. (2011). Identifying cognitive preferences for attractive female faces: an event−related potential experiment using a study−test paradigm. J. Neurosci. Res. 89, 1887–1893. doi: 10.1002/jnr.22724
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Keywords: facial attractiveness, Ultimatum Game, fairness, FRN, P300, decision neuroscience
Citation: Ma Q, Hu Y, Jiang S and Meng L (2015) The undermining effect of facial attractiveness on brain responses to fairness in the Ultimatum Game: an ERP study. Front. Neurosci. 9:77. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2015.00077
Received: 21 August 2014; Accepted: 22 February 2015;
Published: 10 March 2015.
Edited by:
Sacha Bourgeois-Gironde, Université de Provence, FranceReviewed by:
Lasana Harris, New York University, USAJavier De La Asuncion, University of Antwerp, Belgium
Copyright © 2015 Ma, Hu, Jiang and Meng. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Qingguo Ma, Neuromanagement Lab, School of Management, Zhejiang University, Zheda Road 38, Hangzhou 310027, China maqingguo3669@zju.edu.cn
†These authors have contributed equally to this work.