Skip to main content

GENERAL COMMENTARY article

Front. Neurosci., 12 January 2015
Sec. Auditory Cognitive Neuroscience
This article is part of the Research Topic How and Why Does Spatial-Hearing Ability Differ among Listeners? What Is the Role of Learning and Multisensory Interactions? View all 24 articles

Cross-modal and multisensory training may distinctively shape restored senses

  • 1Neuroscience Graduate Program, Vanderbilt University Medical Center, Nashville, TN, USA
  • 2Vanderbilt University Medical Center, Vanderbilt Brain Institute, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN, USA

A commentary on
Multisensory training improves auditory spatial processing following bilateral cochlear implantation

by Isaiah, A., Vongpaisal, T., King, A. J., and Hartley, D. E. H. (2014). J. Neurosci. 34, 11119–11130. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4767-13.2014

The coupling of recent technological advances and conceptual understandings within the field of systems neuroscience, and in particular in the study of cross- and multisensory systems, has given rise to the development of a host of sensory substitution and restorative devices. These either leverage a particular sensory modality in order to compensate for loss in another or at least partly rely on a secondary sensory system in order to compensate for missing information. It is under this context that the study of cross-modal (i.e., transfer between sensory modalities) and multisensory (i.e., integration across different sensory modalities) training paradigms has provided information of vital importance (see Sharma et al., 2014). Isaiah et al.s' (2014) contribution in the Journal of Neuroscience describing the impact of audio-visual training on auditory localization in ferrets with cochlear implants (CIs) is one of the most recent examples of these efforts.

Ferrets were deafened either around the onset of hearing or as adults and submitted to either unilateral or bilateral cochlear implantation (UniCI and BiCI, respectively). Following a period of auditory and/or interleaved auditory and visual localization training, approach-to-target accuracy and head orienting responses were examined. In addition, various aspects of neuronal response in primary auditory cortex (A1) were measured as a function of time of hearing loss onset (early vs. late) and sensory training (none, auditory, or audio-visual).

Behaviorally, animals in the UniCI group were unable to localize auditory stimuli regardless of the duration of deafness and training provided. In contrast, late-onset hearing loss BiCI animals performed significantly above chance after auditory training, both in terms of approach-to-target behavior and initial head-orienting responses. Early-deafened BiCI ferrets could not localize sounds beyond chance, and unisensory auditory training did not improve target localization even after repeated sessions. Subsequently, these animals (both UniCI and BiCI) were trained on an interleaved auditory and visual paradigm in an attempt to achieve more accurate auditory localization. After cross-modal training early-deafened BiCI ferrets' auditory localization improved significantly. Importantly, this facilitation was sustained in ensuing unisensory auditory-only localization sessions.

Electrophysiological findings suggested that the behavioral improvements were likely a consequence of increased responsiveness and selectivity of neurons in A1. After interleaved visual and auditory training, neurons in ferret's A1 responded more vigorously and selectively to stimulation provided by the CI. This suggests a putative mechanism underpinning the behavioral improvements. However, the work also raises a number of interesting questions.

First, Isaiah et al. (2014) did not directly investigate the impact of a “classic” multisensory training paradigm (one in which the auditory and visual stimuli would be aligned in space and in time), but rather employed a training paradigm in which information was provided in an interleaved fashion. This raises an interesting question with regard to the brain circuits mediating the changes in A1 responsiveness and the associated behavioral benefits. Are these changes driven by activation differences in multisensory areas (e.g., temporal/parietal) or in reward-related regions (e.g., prefrontal)? In fact, prior research has repeatedly shown that multisensory training can improve unisensory performance through engagement of a wide spread cerebral network (Cappe et al., 2009; Shams and Kim, 2010). Furthermore, Isaiah et al. (2014) findings are in line with a model where cross-modal transfer is mediated by frontal areas since audition and vision were never conjointly activated, and therefore there is no reason to postulate that multisensory areas alone serve as a fundamental node in the computation leading to a facilitated auditory localization (for a review see Ettlinger and Wilson, 1990). Indeed, the authors propose that perhaps it is the prefrontal cortices that are driving cross-modal localization training and the enhanced responsiveness and selectivity exhibited by A1.

Similarly, human psychophysical and neuroimaging literature has repeatedly shown remapping effects to occur for auditory spatial representations after both cross-modal and multisensory training (for a review see Chen and Vroomen, 2013). The spatial ventriloquist aftereffects (Radeau and Bertelson, 1977) are a behavioral example of such an auditory spatial remapping due to vision. Further, evidence from human neuroimaging studies suggests the contribution of a fronto-temporo-parietal network in cross-modal and multisensory spatial cognition (for a review see Koelewijn et al., 2010). Nonetheless, a more mechanistic understanding of how this network comes to modulate A1 responsiveness and selectivity after interleaved visual trials and in the lack of spatiotemporal congruency remains unanswered. When framed from the perspective of sensory substitution devices, the overarching question for these experiments is whether genuine cross-modal plasticity occurred in multisensory networks, or whether reward networks mediated the perceptual learning?

Sensory loss leads to extensive cross-modal plasticity (Bavelier et al., 2006). In the case of congenitally deaf individuals, for instance, neural substrates in the auditory cortex might be recruited by other sensory modalities. Finney and MDobkins (2001) showed responses to visual motion in auditory cortex of deaf individuals. In addition, this plasticity seems to be the basis for the behavioral benefit auditory deprived individuals show in processing visual motion in the peripheral visual field (Bavelier et al., 2006). On the other hand, cross-modal reorganization of the deprived cortex can also be deleterious. By supporting processes grounded in another sensory modality, cross-modal plasticity might hinder cortical recruitment by the native sensory system. That is, electrical input to the auditory cortex after cochlear implantation might be inefficient if the cortical structure has been functionally reorganized by the spared sensory modalities. Accordingly, Lee et al. (2001), reported that deaf individuals in whom cross-modal plasticity was the most extensive were the least likely to benefit from CIs. An open question is whether a training paradigm based on invoking changes in prefrontal networks such as the cross-modal approaches employed here would be more or less effective than approaches founded on invoking changes in multisensory cortical networks derived from direct multisensory training methods.

The question becomes, could cross-modal training have long-term detrimental effects, as well as the short-term beneficial effects Isaiah et al. (2014) demonstrate? A key issue remains whether one type of training (e.g., cross-modal) would incite cortical plasticity more readily than the other (e.g., multisensory), and even whether the nature of this putative neuroplasticity would be akin in both conditions? Likely cross-modal and multisensory training will both result in cortical changes—the nature of which could be very different and which may be used in different ways when thinking about sensory substitution and restoration.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Dr. Wallace and the Multisensory Lab at Vanderbilt University for valuable input in the discussion leading to the present manuscript. Antonia Thelen is supported by Swiss National Science Foundation.

References

Bavelier, D., Dye, M. W. G., and Hauser, P. C. (2006). Do deaf individuals see better? Trends Cogn. Sci. 10, 512–518. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.09.006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cappe, C., Rouiller, E. M., and Barone, P. (2009). Multisensory anatomical pathways. Hear. Res. 258, 28–36. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2009.04.017

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, L., and Vroomen, J. (2013). Intersensory binding across space and time: a tutorial review. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 75, 790–811. doi: 10.3758/s13414-013-0475-4

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ettlinger, G., and Wilson, W. A. (1990). Cross-modal performance: behavioural processes, phylogenetic considerations and neural mechanisms. Behav. Brain Res. 40, 169–192. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(90)90075-P

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Finney, E. M, and Dobkins, K. R. (2001). Visual contrast sensitivity in deaf versus hearing populations: exploring the perceptual consequences of auditory deprivation and experience with a visual language. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 11, 171–183. doi: 10.1016/S0926-6410(00)00082-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Isaiah, A., Vongpaisal, T., King, A. J., and Hartley, D. E. H. (2014). Multisensory training improves auditory spatial processing following bilateral cochlear implantation. J. Neurosci. 34, 11119–11130. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4767-13.2014

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koelewijn, T., Bronkhorst, A., and Theeuwes, J. (2010). Attention and the multiple stages of multisensory integration: a review of audiovisual studies. Acta Psychol. (Amst.) 134, 372–384. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.03.010

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, D. S., Lee, J. S., Oh, S. H., Kim, S. K., Kim, J. W., Chung, J. K., et al. (2001). Cross-modal plasticity and cochlear implants. Nature 409, 149–150. doi: 10.1038/35051653

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Radeau, M., and Bertelson, P. (1977). Adaptation to auditory-visual discordance and ventriloquism in semirealistic situations. Percept. Psychophys. 22, 137–146. doi: 10.3758/BF03198746

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shams, L., and Kim, R. (2010). Crossmodal influences on visual perception. Phys. Life Rev. 7, 269–284. doi: 10.1016/j.plrev.2010.04.006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sharma, A., Campbell, J., and Cardon, G. (2014). Developmental and cross-plasticity in deafness: evidence from the P1 and the N1 event related potentials in cochlear implanted children. Int. J. Psychophysiol. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2014.04.007. [Epub ahead of print].

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: multisensory, cross-modal, cochlear implant, spatial localization

Citation: Noel J-P and Thelen A (2015) Cross-modal and multisensory training may distinctively shape restored senses. Front. Neurosci. 8:450. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2014.00450

Received: 11 November 2014; Accepted: 22 December 2014;
Published online: 12 January 2015.

Edited by:

Guillaume Andeol, Institut de Recherche Biomédicale des Armées, France

Reviewed by:

Pascal Barone, Université Paul Sabatier, France

Copyright © 2015 Noel and Thelen. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: thelen.antonia@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.