Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Neurosci., 31 July 2014
Sec. Neuroendocrine Science
This article is part of the Research Topic From Sex Differences in Neuroscience to a Neuroscience of Sex Differences: New Directions and Perspectives View all 17 articles

Gender effects and sexual-orientation impact on androstadienone-evoked behavior and neural processing

  • 1Department of Biomedical Imaging and Image-Guided Therapy, Medical University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
  • 2Department of Biomedical Imaging and Image-Guided Therapy, Computational Image Analysis and Radiology Lab, Medical University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
  • 3Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden
  • 4Monell Chemical Senses Center, Philadelphia, PA, USA
  • 5Department of Psychology, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA

In humans, the most established and investigated substance acting as a chemosignal, i.e., a substance that is excreted from the body, is 4,16-androstadien-3-one (AND). AND, which is found in sweat and saliva, is known to be responsible for influencing several variables, such as psychophysiological status, behavior, as well as cortical processing. The aim of the present review is to give insight into the variety of AND effects, with special regard to specific cross-sexual characteristics of this putative human chemosignal, emphasizing the neural activation patterns and factors such as contextual conditions. This review highlights the importance of including those contributing factors into the analysis of behavioral as well as brain-related studies.

Introduction

There is evidence for a sexual dimorphism of the brain (e.g., Coffey et al., 1998; Sacher et al., 2013) and new medical theories point out the importance of gender specific medicine in terms of risk-factors, epidemiology and treatment outcomes (e.g., Mielke et al., 2014). In order to extract essential and comprehensive information from behavioral as well as functional imaging studies, including gender, as an essential factor is of great interest (Sacher et al., 2013). This review highlights this importance for chemosensory science in which chemosignals are known to evoke a gender specific effect themselves.

The term “pheromones” (so-called chemosignals) was coined in 1959 by Peter Karlson and Martin Lüscher, who defined them as “substances which are secreted to the outside by an individual and received by a second individual of the same species, in which they release a specific reaction, for example a definite behavior or a developmental process” (Karlson and Lüscher, 1959). The search for these semiochemicals is still an elusive goal of chemical ecology and communication studies. Today, it is clear that chemosignals play a significant role in social interaction and communication in various species. It is well known that animals are able to communicate affective states, such as stress, alarm, fear, anxiety, or sexual interest, by manipulating chemosignals produced from their skin (Kiyokawa, 2004; Kiyokawa et al., 2006). In animals, these signals are jointly processed by the vomeronasal organ (VNO) and by the accessory olfactory systems, as well as by the main olfactory system (Chamero et al., 2012; Petrulis, 2013). The wide range of literature addressing the VNO provides little consensus about the presence of the VNO in humans (for a review, see Meredith, 2001). While a structure similar to the VNO of animals was found in utero in humans (Knecht et al., 2003), the same structure is not continuously noticeable in adults (Trotier et al., 2000; Trotier, 2011). To date, it is not clear whether a human VNO exists and whether it plays a role in the perception of chemosignals (Frasnelli et al., 2011).

Several areas of the human body—such as feet, mouth, or, particularly, axillary regions—are known to produce odors that act as chemosignals (Pause, 2012). Recently, Gelstein et al. (2011) found that tears convey chemosignals, and even ear wax was proposed to be able to transport chemosensory information (Prokop-Prigge et al., 2014). Nevertheless, human sweat is the most extensively investigated conductor of chemosignals (Porter and Moore, 1981; Lundström et al., 2008; Zhou and Chen, 2008; Zernecke et al., 2010; Albrecht et al., 2011).

Although the human body odor cocktail can contain well over 200 individual components (Zeng et al., 1996), the most intensively studied component is the steroid 4,16-androstadien-3-one (AND). The chemical structure of the molecule AND is very similar to androstenon, a well-known animal pheromone (Melrose et al., 1971). AND's specific cross-sexual characteristics and its impact on human behavior and psychophysiological events, in particular, have drawn much attention in recent research.

The following sections will focus on research addressing the behavioral and psychophysiological effects as well as the neuronal processing of AND, emphasizing its gender-specific and cross-sexual characteristics.

Behavioral and Psychophysiological Effects of AND

AND is one of the substances known to modulate psychological and physiological states, as well as human behavior in a non-conscious manner (Lundström and Olsson, 2005). It is known that AND is associated with specific cross-sexual characteristics and has recently attracted much attention, particularly regarding its effect on women's psychophysiology (Jacob and McClintock, 2000; Jacob et al., 2001, 2002; Lundström and Olsson, 2005; Wyart et al., 2007). However, there is evidence that AND influences men's psychophysiology and behavior as well (Bensafi et al., 2003, 2004). These gender-specfic effects not only make AND particularly interesting, but also induce an interpretation bias of certain results due to mixed study groups and inextensive study description. Table 1 gives an overview of gender-specific studies and findings.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Behavioral and psychophysiological results induced by AND.

The data from previous behavioral and psychophysiological studies are provided in the following sections categorized according to the gender of the sample.

Studies Investigating AND in a Female Sample

Psychophysiological manifestations related to AND exposure in female subjects have been reported, based on AND detected in salivary cortisol levels (Wyart et al., 2007) and autonomic physiology levels showed a significant reduction in respiratory and cardiac frequency, as well as skin conductance and increased body temperature (Grosser et al., 2000). Beyond that, Lundström and colleagues suggested that AND enhanced women's feeling of being focused (Lundström et al., 2003) and induced an increased attentiveness, even outside the conscious detection of AND (Lundström and Olsson, 2005). In addition, analyses of chemosensory event-related potential (ERP) recordings revealed AND to be processed between 13 and 20 percent faster than odorants similar in hedonic and intensity ratings (Lundström et al., 2006b). With special regard to AND's generally proposed role in reproductive behavior, two studies (Thorne et al., 2002; Saxton et al., 2008) indicate that men's ratings of female attractiveness are modulated by this special chemosignal with higher evaluations of women in the AND condition, while another experiment (Lundström and Olsson, 2005) was not able to confirm this effect.

Saxton et al. (2008), who assessed the effect of AND in a speed-dating-event, argued that the suggested context-dependency of AND may occur only in the presence of a male person (Jacob et al., 2001; Lundström and Olsson, 2005). Furthermore, recent literature suggests that AND strenghtens intrasexual competition strategies in women (Parma et al., 2012).

The role of AND as modulator-chemosignal has also been discussed in the context of serving as a link between hormonal status and this special steroid: While women taking oral contraceptives are more sensitive to environmental odors, fertile women showed a higher sensitivity to chemosignals with reproductive relevance, like androstadienone (Lundström et al., 2006a). With regard to AND's influence on mood, AND was found to enhance a positive mood in women, with feelings of being more relaxed, calm, and free of negative feelings (Grosser et al., 2000; Preti et al., 2003). Another study reported that the setting, the manner, and by whom the experiment was conducted play a role in perception. Lundström and Olsson (2005) emphasized the impact of socioexperimental conditions in women who showed changes in self- reported mood only when experimental interactions were completed by a male experimenter, thus, again, proposing a context-dependent effect for AND.

Studies Investigating AND in a Male Sample

Studies using male samples are rare. The only study investigating the effect of AND in men came out recently, and demonstrated that AND directly affected mens' cooperative behavior by increasing such behavior (Huoviala and Rantala, 2013).

Studies Investigating AND in Mixed Samples

We defined studies with mixed samples as those in which the study design included men and women.

With regard to the activational effects in the sympathetic nervous system, a study by Jacob et al. (2001) was able to confirm the suggested calming effects of AND on women's physiology, as already suggested by a study using the female sample presented in the above section (see Section “Studies Investigating AND in a Female Sample”; Grosser et al., 2000). While AND administration led to raised skin temparature in men and lowered temperature in women, AND increased skin conductance in both sexes, with a significantly higher effect observed in women, indicating that the arousing effect was more prevalent in women than in men (Jacob et al., 2001). Interestingly, the activational effects of AND were dependent on the socioexperimental context, since women's reactions were observed only in sessions administered by a male investigator. Some other findings emphasized the context-dependency of AND in a similar fashion. While AND administered in a neutral context, or in a context with little social interaction, did not influence autonomic nervous system functions, it enabled increased sexual arousal in a sexually arousing context in a sex-independent manner (Bensafi et al., 2004; Hummer and McClintock, 2009). During the same sexually arousing context, respiration rates, especially in men, decreased, while skin temperature in both sexes rose (Bensafi et al., 2004).

Concerning psychological variables, AND was reported to have divergent effects in men and women, as shown for psychophysiological states; while AND administration led to increased negative emotions in men (Jacob and McClintock, 2000), especially in unpleasant settings (Bensafi et al., 2004), no negative effect was evoked in women. Focusing on the context-dependency of AND, positive feelings in women were found to be sustained during a sad time (Bensafi et al., 2004) and were increased in a neutral context (Jacob and McClintock, 2000).

Further, another study that analyzed the effect of AND on mood and pain perception concluded that exposure to this steroid led to an amelioration of mood state only in women (Villemure and Bushnell, 2007). Based on this finding, the authors further hypothesized that women would show lower pain sensation when exposed to AND. However, this assumption was not confirmed, as women, interestingly, showed increased percieved pain (Villemure and Bushnell, 2007).

An effect of the experimenters sex on the baseline response in an AND experiment has recently also been observed in rodents (Sorge et al., 2014). Regarding future research, preliminary findings about the sex-divergent effects of AND on psychophysiological as well as psychological variables should be considered, especially the context-dependency of AND. This context-dependency should implicitly be noted when planning experiments, as well as interpreting results to prevent an interpretation bias.

Neuronal Correlates of AND

In the past few years, a great number of neuroimaging studies have provided insight into the neuronal processing of common odorants by the olfactory pathway (for a review, see Lundström et al., 2011). Whereas common odors normally activate the temporal-frontal junction, the so-called piriform cortex, amygdala, insula, and the orbitofrontal cortex, body odors are commonly found to trigger a network located outside the main olfactory system, including the posterior cingulate cortex, the occipital gyrus, the angular gyrus, and the anterior cingulate cortex (for a review, see Lundström and Olsson, 2010). The following section aims to provide the reader with an overview about the neuronal processing of the chemosignal AND.

Neuroimaging studies have illustrated a gender-specific outcome, but another significant finding should be specifically noted—the sexual-orientation effect. As a result of this, the following sections will be segmented into gender-specific neuronal processing and the impact of sexual orientation.

To illustrate the findings discussed in the subsequent paragraphs more clearly, we provide the reader with an overview of neural activation patterns induced by AND, classified by gender and sexual orientation (see Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Axial mean anatomical images overlaid with neural activation induced by AND, resulting from different functional imaging studies (see Table 2). In order to enhance comparability, we included all positron emission tomography (PET) studies using the same tracer, with the special pheromone-like compound that induced neuronal activation (see Table 2). Since results from certain subject groups were re-utilized as controls throughout these studies, these activations are illustrated once. Voxels were highlighted with a 10 mm sphere. To illustrate the impact of sexual orientation, activations in heterosexual (green) and homosexual (red) men, in heterosexual (yellow) and homosexual (cyan) women, as well as in non-homosexual male-to-female transsexuals, are mapped separately (A). Sex-specific differences in activation patterns are shown in (B) (hetero- and homosexual males; blue) and (C) (hetero- and homosexual females, as well as male-to-female transsexuals; orange).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Overview of studies included in graphic design.

Gender-Specific Neuronal Processing of AND

Early positron emisson tomography (PET) studies exploring the neural correlates of AND perception suggested the presence of gender-specific neural activation in which the hypothalamic pathway was significantly activated in heterosexual women, but heterosexual men lacked this hypothalamic activation, and areas of the olfactory cortex were activated instead (Savic et al., 2001, 2005, 2009; Berglund et al., 2006, 2008; Hillert et al., 2007; Ciumas et al., 2009). However, contrary to these PET studies, a recent fMRI study found non-sex-specific hypothalamic activation (Burke et al., 2012). When the authors applied various concentrations of AND, significantly higher hypothalamic activation was demonstrated in women than in men when a higher concentration was used (10 mM). This corresponds with previous studies. However, when participants were exposed to medium concentrations (0.1 mM), men demonstrated a significantly stronger hypothalamic response than the participating women (Burke et al., 2012). These results led the authors to conclude that AND evokes hypothalamic responses in both sexes in a stimulus concentration-dependent manner. However, when comparing these results to the series of studies by Savic and co-workers (Savic et al., 2001, 2005, 2009; Berglund et al., 2006, 2008; Hillert et al., 2007; Ciumas et al., 2009), it should be noted that Burke et al. (2012) used other odor delivery methods, compound concentrations, as well as another imaging technique, all of which together affected our ability to directly compare results.

Impact of Sexual-Orientation on Neuronal Processing of AND

The last decade of brain imaging research has revealed that AND stimulation produces significant and localized group effects that are seemingly sexual orientation-dependent. There are several functional neuroimaging studies, which address cortical responses to pheromones that seem to be dependent on sexual orientation; homosexual men and male-to-female transsexuals were found to display the same activation pathway as heterosexual women, i.e., demonstrating hypothalamic responses upon exposure to AND (Savic et al., 2005; Berglund et al., 2008).

In contrast, Berglund and colleagues concluded that homosexual women process AND similar to heterosexual men, namely, by parts of the olfactory cortex (Berglund et al., 2006). Those findings (Savic et al., 2005; Berglund et al., 2006, 2008), are of great importance in order to underline existing evidence of sexual-orientation effects on neural processing in other research areas as well. Recently, behavioral and neuroimaging results from Perry et al. (2013) showed empathy to be related to gender, as well as sexual preference. Further, a study aiming to characterize regional homogeneity and functional connectivity during rest found significant differences between homo- and heterosexual men (Hu et al., 2013). Hence, it would be presumptuous not to consider the ability to use this variable as a modulating factor to achieve homogeneous subject groups.

AND-EST Inconsistency

As noted above, AND is processed in a gender-specific manner; however, thus far, few studies have dealt with the functional aspects beyond neural activity. Most published studies investigating the effect of AND on neural processes also included another potential human pheromone, namely estra-1,3,5(10),16-tetraen-3-ol (EST) (Savic et al., 2001, 2005, 2009; Berglund et al., 2006, 2008; Hillert et al., 2007; Ciumas et al., 2009). EST has, among others, been detected as a natural component of the urine in pregnant women (Thysen et al., 1968). Interestingly, the administration of this estrogen-like steroid causes an effect complementary to AND. Whereas AND application caused a hypothalamic activation in women and the activation of common olfactory areas in men, EST is processed in a diametrically opposite fashion, namely, via the hypothalamic pathway in heterosexual men and parts of the olfactory cortex in heterosexual women (Savic et al., 2001, 2005, 2009; Berglund et al., 2006, 2008; Hillert et al., 2007; Ciumas et al., 2009). This complementary relationship seems to be of great physiological relevance.

Compared to AND, specific psychophysiological measures of EST are largely unknown. However, while the results of Bensafi et al. (2003) revealed no effect of EST on physiological arousal, the same authors found EST to affect physiological arousal in a content-dependent way (Bensafi et al., 2004). Exploring the conscious odor perception of AND and EST in an animal model, a gender-specific effect was obtained by Laska et al. (2006), who detected olfactory sensitivity to AND in female, but not male, spider monkeys, while responses to the highest concentrations of EST were found in males, but not in female, monkeys. These data also highlight the gender-specific processing of these two pheromone-like compounds in non-human animal models. Finally, these results indicate that there is a considerable need for research on the psychophysiological effects of EST.

Final Remarks

As shown, non-conscious application of AND mediates human behavior, psychophysiology, as well as cortical processing, with different responses and activations in men and women. With regard to previous findings, the suggested influence of contextual condition should be considered in any further planning of a trial, as well as in the interpretation and reporting of results. In addition, the discrepancies in results between various studies further emphasize the need for further research in this area, especially in the field of sub- and suprathreshold application of AND to rule out potential concentration-dependent effects. Moreover, the advent of chemosensory imaging using fMRI allows a more stringent and temporally detailed investigation of the neural processing of AND. Finally, as demonstrated by the neuroimaging results presented above, linked to sexual orientation, care should be taken to either include homogeneous subject groups or to carefully control for demographic characteristics. The mediating mechanisms of these sex and sexual preference-specific effects on behavioral, psychophysiological, and neural processing of AND should form the basis of further research.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by the FWF (Veronika Schöpf, Karl-Heinz Nenning, Kathrin Kollndorfer, Jacqueline Krajnik: P23205-B09; Jacqueline Krajnik: KLI 252; Karl-Heinz Nenning: P22578-B19) and by the EU (Karl-Heinz Nenning: FP7-ICT-2009-5/257528). Johan N. Lundström is funded by the Knut and Alice Wallenberg Foundation (KAW 2012.0141).

References

Albrecht, J., Demmel, M., Schöpf, V., Kleemann, A. M., Kopietz, R., May, J., et al. (2011). Smelling chemosensory signals of males in anxious versus nonanxious condition increases state anxiety of female subjects. Chem. Senses 36, 19–27. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjq087

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bensafi, M., Brown, W. M., Khan, R., Levenson, B., and Sobel, N. (2004). Sniffing human sex-steroid derived compounds modulates mood, memory and autonomic nervous system function in specific behavioral contexts. Behav. Brain Res. 152, 11–22. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2003.09.009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bensafi, M., Brown, W. M., Tsutsui, T., Mainland, J. D., Johnson, B. N., Bremner, E. A., et al. (2003). Sex-steroid derived compounds induce sex-specific effects on autonomic nervous system function in humans. Behav. Neurosci. 117, 1125–1134. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.117.6.1125

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Berglund, H., Lindström, P., Dhejne-Helmy, C., and Savic, I. (2008). Male-to-female transsexuals show sex-atypical hypothalamus activation when smelling odorous steroids. Cereb. Cortex 18, 1900–1908. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm216

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Berglund, H., Lindström, P., and Savic, I. (2006). Brain response to putative pheromones in lesbian women. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 103, 8269–8274. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0600331103

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Burke, S. M., Veltman, D. J., Gerber, J., Hummel, T., and Bakker, J. (2012). Heterosexual men and women both show a hypothalamic response to the chemo-signal androstadienone. PLoS ONE 7:e40993. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0040993

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Chamero, P., Leinders-Zufall, T., and Zufall, F. (2012). From genes to social communication: molecular sensing by the vomeronasal organ. Trends Neurosci. 35, 597–606. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2012.04.011

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ciumas, C., Lindén Hirschberg, A., and Savic, I. (2009). High fetal testosterone and sexually dimorphic cerebral networks in females. Cereb. Cortex 19, 1167–1174. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn160

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Coffey, C. E., Lucke, J. F., Saxton, J. A., Ratcliff, G., Unitas, L. J., Billig, B., et al. (1998). Sex differences in brain aging: a quantitative magnetic resonance imaging study. Arch. Neurol. 55, 169–179. doi: 10.1001/archneur.55.2.169

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Frasnelli, J., Lundström, J. N., Boyle, J. A., Katsarkas, A., and Jones-Gotman, M. (2011). The vomeronasal organ is not involved in the perception of endogenous odors. Hum. Brain Mapp. 32, 450–460. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21035

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gelstein, S., Yeshurun, Y., Rozenkrantz, L., Shushan, S., Frumin, I., Roth, Y., et al. (2011). Human tears contain a chemosignal. Science 331, 226–230. doi: 10.1126/science.1198331

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Grosser, B. I., Monti-Bloch, L., Jennings-White, C., and Berliner, D. L. (2000). Behavioral and electrophysiological effects of androstadienone, a human pheromone. Psychoneuroendocrinology 25, 289–299. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4530(99)00056-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hillert, L., Musabasic, V., Berglund, H., Ciumas, C., and Savic, I. (2007). Odor processing in multiple chemical sensitivity. Hum. Brain Mapp. 28, 172–182. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20266

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hu, S., Xu, D., Peterson, B. S., Peterson, B., Wang, Q., He, X., et al. (2013). Association of cerebral networks in resting state with sexual preference of homosexual men: a study of regional homogeneity and functional connectivity. PLoS ONE 8:e59426. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0059426

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hummer, T. A., and McClintock, M. K. (2009). Putative human pheromone androstadienone attunes the mind specifically to emotional information. Horm. Behav. 55, 548–559. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2009.01.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Huoviala, P., and Rantala, M. J. (2013). A putative human pheromone, androstadienone, increases cooperation between men. PLoS ONE 8:e62499. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0062499

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jacob, S., Garcia, S., Hayreh, D., and McClintock, M. K. (2002). Psychological effects of musky compounds: comparison of androstadienone with androstenol and muscone. Horm. Behav. 42, 274–283. doi: 10.1006/hbeh.2002.1826

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jacob, S., Hayreh, D. J., and McClintock, M. K. (2001). Context-dependent effects of steroid chemosignals on human physiology and mood. Physiol. Behav. 74, 15–27. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(01)00537-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jacob, S., and McClintock, M. K. (2000). Psychological state and mood effects of steroidal chemosignals in women and men. Horm. Behav. 37, 57–78. doi: 10.1006/hbeh.1999.1559

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Karlson, P., and Lüscher, M. (1959). Pheromones: a new term for a class of biologically active substances. Nature 183, 55–56. doi: 10.1038/183055a0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kiyokawa, Y. (2004). Alarm pheromones with different functions are released from different regions of the body surface of male rats. Chem. Senses 29, 35–40. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjh004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kiyokawa, Y., Shimozuru, M., Kikusui, T., Takeuchi, Y., and Mori, Y. (2006). Alarm pheromone increases defensive and risk assessment behaviors in male rats. Physiol. Behav. 87, 383–387. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2005.11.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Knecht, M., Witt, M., Abolmaali, N., Hüttenbrink, K. B., and Hummel, T. (2003). The human vomeronasal organ. Nervenarzt 74, 858–862. doi: 10.1007/s00115-003-1573-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Laska, M., Wieser, A., and Salazar, L. T. H. (2006). Sex-specific differences in olfactory sensitivity for putative human pheromones in nonhuman primates. J. Comp. Psychol. 120, 106–112. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.120.2.106

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lundström, J. N., Boesveldt, S., and Albrecht, J. (2011). Central processing of the chemical senses: an overview. ACS Chem. Neurosci. 2, 5–16. doi: 10.1021/cn1000843

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lundström, J. N., Boyle, J. A., Zatorre, R. J., and Jones-Gotman, M. (2008). Functional neuronal processing of body odors differs from that of similar common odors. Cereb. Cortex 18, 1466–1474. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm178

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lundström, J. N., Goncalves, M., Esteves, F., and Olsson, M. J. (2003). Psychological effects of subthreshold exposure to the putative human pheromone 4,16-androstadien-3-one. Horm. Behav. 44, 395–401. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2003.06.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lundström, J. N., McClintock, M. K., and Olsson, M. J. (2006a). Effects of reproductive state on olfactory sensitivity suggest odor specificity. Biol. Psychol. 71, 244–247. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.07.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lundström, J. N., and Olsson, M. J. (2005). Subthreshold amounts of social odorant affect mood, but not behavior, in heterosexual women when tested by a male, but not a female, experimenter. Biol. Psychol. 70, 197–204. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.01.008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lundström, J. N., and Olsson, M. J. (2010). Functional neuronal processing of human body odors. Vitam. Horm. 83, 1–23. doi: 10.1016/S0083-6729(10)83001-8

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lundström, J. N., Olsson, M. J., Schaal, B., and Hummel, T. (2006b). A putative social chemosignal elicits faster cortical responses than perceptually similar odorants. Neuroimage 30, 1340–1346. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.10.040

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Melrose, D. R., Reed, H. C., and Patterson, R. L. (1971). Androgen steroids associated with boar odour as an aid to the detection of oestrus in pig artificial insemination. Br. Vet. J. 127, 497–502.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Meredith, M. (2001). Human vomeronasal organ function: a critical review of best and worst cases. Chem. Senses 26, 433–445. doi: 10.1093/chemse/26.4.433

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mielke, M. M., Vemuri, P., and Rocca, W. A. (2014). Clinical epidemiology of Alzheimer's disease: assessing sex and gender differences. Clin. Epidemiol. 6, 37–48. doi: 10.2147/CLEP.S37929

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Parma, V., Tirindelli, R., Bisazza, A., Massaccesi, S., and Castiello, U. (2012). Subliminally perceived odours modulate female intrasexual competition: an eye movement study. PLoS ONE 7:e30645. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0030645

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pause, B. M. (2012). Processing of body odor signals by the human brain. Chemosens. Percept. 5, 55–63. doi: 10.1007/s12078-011-9108-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Perry, D., Walder, K., Hendler, T., and Shamay-Tsoory, S. G. (2013). The gender you are and the gender you like: sexual preference and empathic neural responses. Brain Res. 1534, 66–75. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2013.08.040

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Petrulis, A. (2013). Chemosignals, hormones and mammalian reproduction. Horm. Behav. 63, 723–741. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2013.03.011

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Porter, R. H., and Moore, J. D. (1981). Human kin recognition by olfactory cues. Physiol. Behav. 27, 493–495. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(81)90337-1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Preti, G., Wysocki, C. J., Barnhart, K. T., Sondheimer, S. J., and Leyden, J. J. (2003). Male axillary extracts contain pheromones that affect pulsatile secretion of luteinizing hormone and mood in women recipients. Biol. Reprod. 68, 2107–2113. doi: 10.1095/biolreprod.102.008268

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Prokop-Prigge, K. A., Thaler, E., Wysocki, C. J., and Preti, G. (2014). Identification of volatile organic compounds in human cerumen. J. Chromatogr. B Analyt. Technol. Biomed. Life Sci. 953–954. doi: 10.1016/j.jchromb.2014.01.043

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sacher, J., Neumann, J., Okon-Singer, H., Gotowiec, S., and Villringer, A. (2013). Sexual dimorphism in the human brain: evidence from neuroimaging. Magn. Reson. Imaging 31, 366–375. doi: 10.1016/j.mri.2012.06.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Savic, I., Berglund, H., Gulyas, B., and Roland, P. (2001). Smelling of odorous sex hormone-like compounds causes sex-differentiated hypothalamic activations in humans. Neuron 31, 661–668. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00390-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Savic, I., Berglund, H., and Lindström, P. (2005). Brain response to putative pheromones in homosexual men. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 102, 7356–7361. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0407998102

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Savic, I., Hedén-Blomqvist, E., and Berglund, H. (2009). Pheromone signal transduction in humans: what can be learned from olfactory loss. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 3057–3065. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20727

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Saxton, T. K., Lyndon, A., Little, A. C., and Roberts, S. C. (2008). Evidence that androstadienone, a putative human chemosignal, modulates women's attributions of men's attractiveness. Horm. Behav. 54, 597–601. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2008.06.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sorge, R. E., Martin, L. J., Isbester, K. A., Sotocinal, S. G., Rosen, S., Tuttle, A. H., et al. (2014). Olfactory exposure to males, including men, causes stress and related analgesia in rodents. Nat. Methods 11, 629–632. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.2935

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Thorne, F., Neave, N., Scholey, A., Moss, M., and Fink, B. (2002). Effects of putative male pheromones on female ratings of male attractiveness: influence of oral contraceptives and the menstrual cycle. Neuro Endocrinol. Lett. 23, 291–297.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Thysen, B., Elliott, W. H., and Katzman, P. A. (1968). Identification of estra-1,3,5(10),16-tetraen-3-ol (estratetraenol) from the urine of pregnant women (1). Steroids 11, 73–87. doi: 10.1016/S0039-128X(68)80052-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Trotier, D. (2011). Vomeronasal organ and human pheromones. Eur. Ann. Otorhinolaryngol. Head Neck Dis. 128, 184–190. doi: 10.1016/j.anorl.2010.11.008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Trotier, D., Eloit, C., Wassef, M., Talmain, G., Bensimon, J. L., Døving, K. B., et al. (2000). The vomeronasal cavity in adult humans. Chem. Senses 25, 369–380. doi: 10.1093/chemse/25.4.369

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Villemure, C., and Bushnell, M. C. (2007). The effects of the steroid androstadienone and pleasant odorants on the mood and pain perception of men and women. Eur. J. Pain 11, 181–191. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpain.2006.02.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wyart, C., Webster, W. W., Chen, J. H., Wilson, S. R., McClary, A., Khan, R. M., et al. (2007). Smelling a single component of male sweat alters levels of cortisol in women. J. Neurosci. 27, 1261–1265. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4430-06.2007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Zeng, X. N., Leyden, J. J., Spielman, A. I., and Preti, G. (1996). Analysis of characteristic human female axillary odors: qualitative comparison to males. J. Chem. Ecol. 22, 237–257. doi: 10.1007/BF02055096

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Zernecke, R., Kleemann, A. M., Haegler, K., Albrecht, J., Vollmer, B., Linn, J., et al. (2010). Chemosensory properties of human sweat. Chem. Senses 35, 101–108. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjp087

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Zhou, W., and Chen, D. (2008). Encoding human sexual chemosensory cues in the orbitofrontal and fusiform cortices. J. Neurosci. 28, 14416–14421. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3148-08.2008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: androstadienone, chemosignals, gender-effect, sexual-orientation, neuronal processing, psychophysiological status, behavior

Citation: Krajnik J, Kollndorfer K, Nenning K-H, Lundström JN and Schöpf V (2014) Gender effects and sexual-orientation impact on androstadienone-evoked behavior and neural processing. Front. Neurosci. 8:195. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2014.00195

Received: 20 March 2014; Accepted: 22 June 2014;
Published online: 31 July 2014.

Edited by:

Belinda Pletzer, University of Salzburg, Austria

Reviewed by:

Bruno Bonaz, Grenoble Faculty of Medicine and Hospital, France
Markus J. Rantala, University of Turku, Finland

Copyright © 2014 Krajnik, Kollndorfer, Nenning, Lundström and Schöpf. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Veronika Schöpf, Department of Biomedical Image and Image-Guided Therapy, Medical University of Vienna, Waehringer Guertel 18-20, Vienna 1090, Austria e-mail: veronika.schoepf@meduniwien.ac.at

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.