Skip to main content

HYPOTHESIS AND THEORY article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 15 May 2024
Sec. Speech and Language
This article is part of the Research Topic Beyond the speech motor disorders in Parkinson’s disease: mechanisms, neural substrates, and potential therapeutic interventions View all 4 articles

A neurocomputational view of the effects of Parkinson’s disease on speech production

  • 1Department of Speech, Language, and Hearing Sciences, Boston University, Boston, MA, United States
  • 2Department of Communicative Disorders and Sciences, University at Buffalo, Buffalo, NY, United States
  • 3Program in Speech and Hearing Bioscience and Technology, Division of Medical Sciences, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, United States
  • 4Department of Neurosurgery, Massachusetts General Hospital, Boston, MA, United States
  • 5Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, United States
  • 6Department of Neurobiology, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, United States
  • 7Aligning Science Across Parkinson’s (ASAP) Collaborative Research Network, Chevy Chase, MD, United States
  • 8Department of Biomedical Engineering, Boston University, Boston, MA, United States
  • 9Picower Institute for Learning and Memory, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA, United States

The purpose of this article is to review the scientific literature concerning speech in Parkinson’s disease (PD) with reference to the DIVA/GODIVA neurocomputational modeling framework. Within this theoretical view, the basal ganglia (BG) contribute to several different aspects of speech motor learning and execution. First, the BG are posited to play a role in the initiation and scaling of speech movements. Within the DIVA/GODIVA framework, initiation and scaling are carried out by initiation map nodes in the supplementary motor area acting in concert with the BG. Reduced support of the initiation map from the BG in PD would result in reduced movement intensity as well as susceptibility to early termination of movement. A second proposed role concerns the learning of common speech sequences, such as phoneme sequences comprising words; this view receives support from the animal literature as well as studies identifying speech sequence learning deficits in PD. Third, the BG may play a role in the temporary buffering and sequencing of longer speech utterances such as phrases during conversational speech. Although the literature does not support a critical role for the BG in representing sequence order (since incorrectly ordered speech is not characteristic of PD), the BG are posited to contribute to the scaling of individual movements in the sequence, including increasing movement intensity for emphatic stress on key words. Therapeutic interventions for PD have inconsistent effects on speech. In contrast to dopaminergic treatments, which typically either leave speech unchanged or lead to minor improvements, deep brain stimulation (DBS) can degrade speech in some cases and improve it in others. However, cases of degradation may be due to unintended stimulation of efferent motor projections to the speech articulators. Findings of spared speech after bilateral pallidotomy appear to indicate that any role played by the BG in adult speech must be supplementary rather than mandatory, with the sequential order of well-learned sequences apparently represented elsewhere (e.g., in cortico-cortical projections).

1 Introduction

The aim of this paper is to provide a neurocomputational view of the planning and execution of speech in Parkinson’s disease (PD) by reviewing the scientific literature within the theoretical framework of two well-developed computational models of speech production: the DIVA and GODIVA models (see Guenther, 2016 for a detailed treatment). PD is the second most common neurodegenerative disorder in the world (Feigin et al., 2020), affecting approximately 8.5 million adults worldwide (World Health Organization, 2023). As the disease progresses, markers of neuropathology (e.g., Lewy bodies) are observed in different areas of the nervous system, proceeding through characteristic Braak stages (Braak et al., 2004). This process begins in the lower brainstem and olfactory system (Stage 1); followed by the raphe nucleus and locus coeruleus (Stage 2); the substantia nigra pars compacta (SNc) and basal nucleus of Mynert (Stage 3); the thalamus, amygdala, mesocortex, and allocortex (Stage 4); and finally, neocortex (Stages 5–6). Clinically, PD is characterized by a set of classic motor signs, including bradykinesia, tremor, rigidity, postural instability, and gait disturbances. Speech and voice symptoms are highly prevalent in people with PD (Sapir et al., 2008). Perceptually, the characteristics of speech in PD include monopitch, monoloudness, reduced stress, imprecise consonants, inappropriate silences, short rushes of speech, harsh voice, breathy voice, low pitch, and variable rate (Duffy, 2019), with voice changes typically presenting as the earliest and most prevalent speech symptoms (Logemann et al., 1978; Ho et al., 1998; Harel et al., 2004; Rusz et al., 2011). The constellation of motor speech symptoms of PD reflects a complex interplay of sensory, motor, cognitive, and affective mechanisms (Sapir, 2014).

The most well-characterized and devastating effects on motor control in PD arise from pathophysiological changes in the basal ganglia (BG). The BG are functionally organized into sensorimotor, oculomotor, associative, and limbic loops based on their projections to relevant regions of the cerebral cortex (Alexander et al., 1990). Figure 1 shows the classic firing-rate based model of the cortico-BG-thalamo-cortical (hereafter cortico-BG) motor loop in a healthy individual (panel A) and in a person with PD (panel B). In PD, the presence of Lewy body pathology and destruction of cells within the substantia nigra pars compacta (SNc) occurring during Braak Stage 3 result in the loss of dopaminergic input to the putamen and caudate nucleus of the striatum. These striatal nuclei form the main input structures of the basal ganglia, receiving projections from a wide range of sensory, motor, and cognitive regions of the cerebral cortex. Without sufficient dopaminergic input, cortico-BG loops become dysregulated, leading to changes in the firing rates and firing patterns of neurons within these pathways.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Schematic of the cortico-basal ganglia-thalamo-cortical motor loop in a neurologically normal adult (A) and an adult with Parkinson’s disease (B). Adapted from Guenther (2016).

Dopamine has an excitatory effect on striatal neurons in the direct pathway (indicated in green in Figure 1) that send inhibitory projections to the internal segment of the globus pallidus (GPi) and/or the substantia nigra pars reticulata (SNr), which together form the output portion of the basal ganglia. In contrast, dopamine has an inhibitory effect on striatal neurons in the indirect pathway (indicated in red in Figure 1). Striatal neurons in the indirect pathway send inhibitory projections to the external segment of the globus pallidus (GPe), which in turn inhibits GPi, both directly as well as through inhibiting the subthalamic nucleus (STN). GPi neurons project through inhibitory pathways to the thalamus, in particular the ventral anterior (VA) and ventral lateral (VL) thalamic nuclei, which send excitatory projections to the motor and premotor cortical areas, respectively. The direct pathway is often characterized as pro-kinetic since activating a striatal neuron in the direct pathway is thought to have a net excitatory effect on motor cortex: the striatal neuron inhibits GPi, reducing inhibitory GPi output to thalamus and increasing excitatory input to cortex via thalamus. Because the indirect pathway includes an additional inhibitory stage, activating a striatal neuron in the indirect pathway is thought to have a net inhibitory (anti-kinetic) effect on motor cortex. A third pathway (not shown in Figure 1) from cortex to STN called the hyperdirect pathway has been hypothesized to inhibit large areas of cortex representing possible motor programs before the correct motor program is activated via the direct pathway (Mink, 1996; Nambu et al., 2002; see Turner and Desmurget, 2010 for an alternative view).

Early basal ganglia models of PD focused on the impacts of basal ganglia dysregulation on cortical firing rates (e.g., Albin et al., 1989; Delong, 1990). Within the classic rate model of PD, deficient dopaminergic input results in overactivation of the anti-kinetic indirect pathway and underactivation of the pro-kinetic direct pathway, leading to a net decrease of excitatory input to cerebral cortex from the thalamus (Figure 1B). More recent models have focused on changes to neuronal firing patterns and the synchronization of firing rates across neuronal populations (e.g., Levy et al., 2000; Brown, 2003; Magnusson and Leventhal, 2021). These pattern-based models instead describe a pathological increase in synchronized firing between neighboring neurons in the basal ganglia nuclei and their output structures, which interferes with the reliable transmission of signals between thalamus and cortex.

It is well-established that speech production heavily involves the cortico-BG motor loop. Functional neuroimaging studies of speech and vocal tract movements commonly report activity in the putamen (Brown et al., 2009; Chang et al., 2009; Parkinson et al., 2012; Simonyan et al., 2013). Moreover, lesions of the putamen and caudate result in marked changes in speech production and organization (Pickett et al., 1998; Gronholm et al., 2016). STN and GPi are also functionally connected to multiple cortical and subcortical regions within the speech network (Manes et al., 2014).

Intracranial recordings from humans undergoing deep brain stimulation (DBS) lead placement surgery have provided valuable evidence for STN involvement in speech production. Watson and Montgomery (2006) were the first to report basal ganglia recordings during speech production. In line with firing rate models of the basal ganglia, the authors reported drastic reduction in firing rate in the STN for the duration of an utterance during a sentence repetition task. However, more recent studies have painted a far more complex picture of STN and GPi function during speech. Neurons both increase and decrease firing rate in the STN in response to visual task-related cues and to speech production (Lipski et al., 2018; Tankus and Fried, 2019; Tankus et al., 2021; Johari et al., 2023), though a robust finding across reports and tasks is that most neurons increase firing rate at speech onset in STN (Johari et al., 2023). Studies of non-human primates (NHPs) also report that a majority of neurons increase firing rates during motor tasks (Delong, 1972; Mink and Thach, 1991; Turner and Anderson, 1997). These findings are somewhat surprising given the net inhibitory effect on movement expected from increased STN and GPi activity in the classic rate model. The increased firing rates have been interpreted as evidence of the suppression of unwanted or inappropriate movements that might compete with the desired movement (Mink, 1996; Nambu, 2004; Hikosaka et al., 2006), though Turner and Desmurget (2010) cast doubt on this interpretation due to the relatively late onset of activity in GPi relative to movement onset. We will return to this issue in a later section.

Although the primary focus of studies on speech deficits in PD is the basal ganglia, it should be noted that the neurodegenerative and pathophysiological effects of PD impact additional anatomical structures involved in speech motor control, including the cranial nuclei/nerves and cerebral cortex. Sapir (2014) proposed that early manifestations of speech and voice changes in PD may be the result of pathology within the dorsal motor nucleus of the vagus and glossopharyngeal nerves (corresponding to Braak Stage 1–2). Post-mortem studies of PD patients have reported Lewy-type pathology in the peripheral sensory neurons of the upper airway, correlating with dysphagia symptoms (Mu et al., 2013a,b; 2015). Still, it is unclear whether primary pathology in the cranial nerves and their nuclei has a meaningful impact of speech or voice function in PD.

Cortical pathology in PD is observed primarily in the later disease stages (Braak Stage 4–6) and is associated with cognitive decline (Hurtig et al., 2000; Mattila et al., 2000; Kovari et al., 2003; Smith et al., 2019); however, it is unknown whether cortical pathology contributes to PD speech symptoms. Linking speech changes to cortical pathology is especially challenging since definitive measures of brain pathology can only be collected post-mortem. Using non-invasive structural brain imaging, researchers have demonstrated multiple regions of cortical thinning in PD associated with disease progression (Zarei et al., 2013; Tremblay et al., 2021) and poorer cognitive function (e.g., Hanganu et al., 2013; Laansma et al., 2021). Applying this approach to the study of speech in PD, a study by Chen et al. (2020) found that the severity of hypokinetic dysarthria in PD (measured by the voice handicap index) was associated with cortical thinning in the right precentral gyrus and right fusiform cortex, suggesting a possible role of cortical atrophy in the presentation of PD speech symptoms. However, more research is needed to establish whether speech changes in PD can be linked to pathology in cerebral cortex, and it is important to bear in mind that pathological functioning in cortex can arise even in healthy cortical tissue due to impaired input from the thalamus and/or other brain regions, further complicating the issue.

2 The DIVA/GODIVA neurocomputational modeling framework

Figure 2 provides a schematic of the Directions Into Velocities of Articulators (DIVA) model of speech motor control (e.g., Guenther et al., 2006; Guenther, 2016), which provides a neurocomputational account of the brain mechanisms involved in producing single words. The large boxes in the model each represent a different cortical map containing a set of model neurons (nodes), with each node given a precise anatomical location (specified in Montreal Neurological Institute stereotactic coordinates). The smaller boxes represent subcortical regions, and arrows indicate excitatory projections while lines with circular heads indicate inhibitory axonal projections. The DIVA model is defined mathematically, and all model components are given precise locations in a standard stereotactic space. The current article will not address the mathematical formulation of the model; we refer the interested reader to the cited papers above for further details.

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. The DIVA model of speech motor control.

DIVA divides speech motor control into three sub-processes carried out by largely distinct neural regions: auditory feedback control, somatosensory feedback control, and feedforward control. The feedforward control subsystem is the most important of the three subsystems for fluent speech production as it generates the predominant portion of the motor outflow commands. Feedback control mechanisms play only a minor role in the control of an ongoing movement by producing small corrections to errors detected through audition and somatic sensation; the more substantial role for feedback control mechanisms is in helping tune the feedforward commands for future utterances based on errors detected on the current utterance.

The feedforward control subsystem is responsible for the readout of learned motor commands corresponding to motor chunk/words,1 without regard for auditory or somatosensory feedback arising from the movements. According to the DIVA model, feedforward commands are stored in synaptic projections from a speech sound map (whose nodes each represent a different word’s motor program) in left ventral premotor cortex (vPMC) to bilateral2 primary motor cortex (MC) both directly and via a loop through the pons, cerebellum (Cb) and ventrolateral nucleus of the thalamus (VL). Together, these regions constitute the articulation circuit portion of the feedforward control system, so-named because it is responsible for generating the highly coordinated muscle commands that are required for fluent speech.

The second component of the feedforward control subsystem, the initiation circuit, (indicated by red dotted outline in Figure 2) heavily involves the supplementary motor area (SMA) and BG—including the striatum, globus pallidus (GP), and sub-thalamic nucleus (STN) within the cortico-BG motor loop. The nodes in this network do not project directly to the motor periphery; instead, nodes in the SMA’s initiation map effectively determine which motor programs get generated by activating the corresponding nodes in the articulation circuit.

The auditory feedback control subsystem is responsible for detecting and correcting differences between the desired auditory signal for a speech sound and the current auditory feedback. According to the DIVA model, speech sound map nodes project to an auditory target map in the higher-order auditory cortical areas of the posterior superior temporal gyrus (pSTG) via both cortico-cortical projections and a cortico-cerebellar loop involving the pons, Cb, and medial geniculate (MG) nucleus of the thalamus. These projections encode the expected auditory signal for the speech sound currently being produced. Activity in the auditory target map thus represents the auditory feedback that should arise when the speaker hears himself/herself producing the current sound. The auditory target for the current sound is compared to incoming auditory information from the auditory periphery; this information projects to cortex via MG and is represented in the model’s auditory state map. If the current auditory feedback is outside the target region, auditory error map nodes become active. Like the auditory target map, the auditory state and error maps are hypothesized to lie in pSTG. Auditory error map activities are transformed into corrective motor commands through projections from the auditory error map nodes to the feedback control map in right vPMC, which in turn projects to the articulator map in vMC both directly and via a loop through the pons, Cb, and VL. Notably, the BG are not part of the auditory feedback control circuit in the DIVA model, in keeping with findings that eliminating BG output by inactivating GPi has no impact on feedback control mechanisms in non-human primates (e.g., Desmurget and Turner, 2008).

The DIVA model posits a somatosensory feedback control subsystem operating alongside the auditory feedback control subsystem described above. The main components of the somatosensory feedback control subsystem are hypothesized to reside in ventral somatosensory cortex (vSC), including the ventral postcentral gyrus and the supramarginal gyrus. Projections from the speech sound map to the somatosensory target map, including cortico-cortical as well as cortico-cerebellar loop projections via the ventral posterior medial (VPM) nucleus of the thalamus, encode the expected somatosensory feedback (i.e., tactile and proprioceptive feedback arising from mechanoreceptors and muscle spindles in the vocal tract) during sound production. The model’s somatosensory state map represents tactile and proprioceptive feedback from the speech articulators, which arrives from cranial nerve nuclei in the brain stem via VPM. Nodes in the somatosensory error map become active during speech if the speaker’s somatosensory state deviates from the somatosensory target region for the sound being produced. The output of the somatosensory error map then propagates to the feedback control map in right vPMC to transform somatosensory errors into motor commands that correct those errors. As with auditory feedback control, the somatosensory feedback control circuit does not involve BG and thus is not expected to be substantially impaired in PD.

The GODIVA model (Figure 3) extends the DIVA model by adding two prefrontal cortical regions hypothesized to contain working memory (WM) representations of the words in multiple word utterances, such as a phrase or sentence—preSMA and the left posterior inferior frontal sulcus (pIFS)—as well as basal ganglia and thalamic components of the model’s cortico-BG planning loop. Briefly, the phonological material (words) for the utterance is stored in a phonological content buffer in left pIFS; the proper ordering of these items is maintained through interactions (both direct and via the cortico-BG planning loop) between nodes representing rank order in pre-SMA and the corresponding phonological items in pIFS.

Figure 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. The GODIVA model of speech sequencing.

Within the DIVA/GODIVA framework, speech impairments in PD primarily arise from impairment to the initiation circuit of the feedforward control subsystem, as we will detail in the following section. Subsequent sections will address data bearing on whether additional control mechanisms, beyond the initiation circuit, are impaired in PD. Further detail regarding the DIVA/GODIVA models will be provided as needed in these sections.

3 Impaired performance in the initiation circuit

Figure 4 schematizes the brain regions and connections in the DIVA/GODIVA framework (planning and motor loops combined).3 It is helpful to think of this circuit as an adaptive pattern recognizer that monitors the current cognitive, sensory, and motor context as represented in cerebral cortex.4 For example, if a talker is in the midst of saying the first word in the utterance “test set,” the cognitive context indicates that the current word is “test” and the next word is “set,” the sensory context consists of incoming sensory feedback (filtered according to motor expectations) from the auditory and somatosensory systems, and the motor context consists of the currently active motor cortical neurons sending motor commands to the periphery. When the proper context for releasing the word “set” is recognized (i.e., the current motor commands and sensory feedback indicate the end of the word “test”), the loop effectively terminates the current motor program and initiates the motor program for “set” by deactivating the “test” node in the SMA initiation map and activating the “set” node.

Figure 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Schematic of the full cortico-basal ganglia-thalamo-cortical loop for speech sequencing and initiation in the DIVA/GODIVA framework.

There is substantial evidence that the BG participate actively in learning of a new motor sequence, but as the sequence becomes well-learned, the sequence becomes encoded elsewhere in the brain, e.g., in cortico-cortical projections, without the need for BG involvement in sequence readout (see Turner and Desmurget, 2010 for a review). Prior versions of the DIVA and GODIVA models have not addressed this transfer of performance from BG to cortex; modification of the model to address these findings is ongoing.

GPi inactivation studies indicate that the BG do maintain a role in movement gain (or vigor5) even after the sequence is learned; inactivation of GPi results in a general decrease in movement amplitudes, as commonly seen in PD (e.g., Desmurget and Turner, 2008). In the DIVA model this gain is embodied by a GO signal which multiplicatively scales movement amplitude (cf. Bullock and Grossberg, 1988).

Within our modeling framework, one major functional implication of dopaminergic depletion is a net decrease in excitatory support for the current word’s motor program in the SMA initiation map due to an imbalance in the direct and indirect pathways in which the anti-kinetic indirect pathway dominates. In DIVA, production of the current word starts with activation of the word’s initiation map node and continues as long as that node remains active. Initiation map nodes for upcoming words in the utterance are competing with the current word’s node through inhibitory interactions within SMA. The cortico-BG motor loop is posited to bias this competition in favor of the current word until the sensory-motor context indicating completion of this word is “recognized” by the striatum, at which time the excitatory support for the current word’s initiation map node via the cortico-BG motor loop ends, causing the node to lose the competition to the next word’s initiation map node.

According to this view, reduced support for the current motor program’s initiation map node via the cortico-BG motor loop as a result of PD should have at least two effects. First, the initiation map node for the current word will be less active, thereby producing movements with less gain (long known to be a prominent feature of movements in PD, termed hypokinesia) and, in extreme cases, inability to initiate movement. Second, the current word’s node will be more susceptible to competition from initiation map nodes for upcoming words in the utterance, possibly causing it to prematurely “lose out” to the next word, thereby truncating production of the current word. A possible third effect, erroneous selection of the proper motor program amongst competing alternatives (cf. Mink, 1996), would result in improperly ordered movement sequences. However, speech sequencing errors of this type are not characteristic of disorders of the BG such as PD (Duffy, 2019; Camerino et al., 2022) nor after pallidotomy (e.g., Green et al., 2002; Troster et al., 2003), indicating that the order of individual movements in well-learned sequences is represented elsewhere, e.g., in cortico-cortical connections. We will return to this topic in a later section.

Figure 5 provides a schematized depiction of the situation when producing a four-word utterance, with the motor program for each word represented with a box. The top plot shows the intensity (box height) and duration (box width) for each word in the 4-word utterance for a neurotypical speaker, while the bottom row depicts a speaker with PD. The first word is produced with normal intensity and duration by the PD speaker since it does not depend on the cortico-BG motor loop for activation of the SMA initiation map node for this word. However, for subsequent words, decreased support from the cortico-BG motor loop leads to decreased movement intensity as well as premature motor program termination. A further hypothesis—not directly derived from the GODIVA model but motivated by findings described below—is that, in speakers with PD, the reduced support from the cortico-BG motor loop gets progressively worse across the production of extended speech utterances – similar to the common observation in PD of progressively smaller handwriting as a patient continues to write (micrographia; McLennan et al., 1972). Note that, in this account, the overall speaking rate (measured in number of words, syllables or phonemes per unit time), can actually be faster in PD than in controls; experimental studies have produced mixed results, with some showing faster rates in PD (Skodda and Schlegel, 2008), others showing slower rates (Dworkin and Aronson, 1986; Ludlow et al., 1987), and still others showing no differences (Lowit et al., 2006).6 Despite variable reports of speaking rate, one common feature in PD is the acceleration of speech rate (i.e., increased speech rate over time; Tjaden, 2000; Moreau et al., 2007; Skodda and Schlegel, 2008; Skodda et al., 2010; Flasskamp et al., 2012; Rusz et al., 2015). This acceleration of speech may be due to an impaired ability to keep motor programs active that worsens over the course of a long utterance, as schematized in Figure 5.

Figure 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Schematic of accelerating speech resulting from an impaired ability to maintain activation of ongoing motor programs’ representations in the initiation map.

A number of additional behavioral observations concerning speech in PD support this account. (Table 1 provides a summary of experimental findings described in this and subsequent sections.) Reduced movement intensity/scaling is a hallmark of speech deficits in PD, including reduced vocal intensity (hypophonia) and reduced articulatory movement extent (hypokinetic dysarthria; Sapir, 2014; Duffy, 2019). In line with our theoretical view, it has been hypothesized that reduced scaling of speech movements results, at least in part, from impaired internal cueing mechanisms (Sapir, 2014), which are mediated by preparatory activity in SMA (Mushiake et al., 1991; Halsband et al., 1994). In fact, one key goal of the Lee-Silverman Voice Treatment (LSVT LOUD®) is to recalibrate internal cueing mechanisms for voice amplitude scaling through intensive voice therapy (Ramig et al., 2011). Although our theoretical view attributes this reduced intensity to reduced support of SMA initiation map nodes from the cortico-BG motor loop, alternative accounts have been put forward. For example, it has been hypothesized that decreased movement gain in PD results from increased rigidity of the speech musculature (Hunker et al., 1982; Pellat et al., 1983; Hanson et al., 1984; Gallena et al., 2001) and respiratory musculature (Solomon and Hixon, 1993; Sadagopan and Huber, 2007). However, a causative role of rigidity on reduced amplitude scaling has been called into question by others (Netsell et al., 1975; Caligiuri, 1987; Connor and Abbs, 1991; Sapir, 2014).

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Summary of findings concerning speech production in PD patients.

Additional support for this theoretical view can be found in the neuroscience literature. A common finding in single unit studies of GPi in non-human primates is that GPi neurons are typically sensitive to specific characteristics of a movement, such that different neurons are active in different motor contexts (Turner and Anderson, 2005); this finding accords well with the idea of the cortico-BG loop as a pattern recognizer. Activity of many GPi neurons scales monotonically with movement gain/peak velocity/force (Turner and Anderson, 1997), as represented by the GO signal in the DIVA model; this property has also been found in functional neuroimaging studies of human limb movements (Turner et al., 2003; Desmurget et al., 2004; Spraker et al., 2007; Thobois et al., 2007; Wasson et al., 2010).

Neuroimaging studies of speech production in PD have reported conflicting findings concerning the level of activation in SMA for PD patients compared to controls, with some studies reporting hypoactivation of SMA (Baumann et al., 2018; Narayana et al., 2020), some reporting hyperactivation of SMA (Liotti et al., 2003; Pinto et al., 2004; Rektorova et al., 2007), and others reporting no group differences in SMA activity between PD and controls (Pinto et al., 2011; Arnold et al., 2014; Klobusiakova et al., 2021; Manes et al., 2023). Regarding voice intensity, Liotti et al. (2003) found no differences in BG or SMA activity when comparing loud versus habitual overt speech in participants with PD. In contrast, a study of PD and HC participants performing covert (silent) speech in “loud” (as if shouting on a windy beach) and normal conditions found that SMA was hypoactive for PD participants compared to controls in the normal intensity condition but not the “loud” condition prior to voice therapy; group differences in SMA activity were no longer present after voice therapy (Baumann et al., 2018). Hypoactivity of right premotor cortex, but not SMA, was found during sustained phonation in PD participants with hypophonia compared to older healthy controls, suggesting that right premotor cortex may be linked to phonatory drive in PD (Manes et al., 2023). The discrepancy of findings between neuroimaging studies may be in part due to the limited temporal resolution of fMRI, the type of speech task employed (e.g., overt vs. covert speech), presentation of speech symptoms, and medication state. Taken as a whole, however, the existing literature suggests that overall SMA activation may not differ substantially between PD and controls during motor acts such as speech; in terms of our theoretical framework, this could occur because decreased activation of the correct SMA node is offset by increased activation in other (possibly competing) nodes, e.g., due to reduced inhibition from the chosen node, thereby resulting in little or no net activity as measured by fMRI.

Although many findings are in accord with our account, a few commonly reported experimental findings pose difficulties for the DIVA/GODIVA account of BG involvement in initiating speech. As noted in Section 1, activity in STN (Georgopoulos et al., 1983; Wichmann et al., 1994) and GPi (Mitchell et al., 1987; Turner and Anderson, 1997; Doyon et al., 2009; Jin and Costa, 2015) increases during peri-movement periods, which according to the classic rate model should inhibit movement in cortex, whereas in DIVA the BG facilitate production of the current motor program. A possible explanation for this is that the bulk of BG output acts to inhibit movements other than the correct movement such that the decrease in inhibition for the correct movement is outweighed (on average) by the increased inhibition of other movements (Mink, 1996; Nambu, 2004). Another possibility is that it is the temporal pattern of BG output that is crucial rather than the overall amount of activity (e.g., Levy et al., 2000; Brown, 2003). Within this view, a pathological increase in synchronized oscillatory firing between the basal ganglia nuclei and their output structures in PD can interfere with the ability to reliably transmit thalamic signals to cortex.

Perhaps a more troubling issue for the DIVA/GODIVA view is the frequently noted finding in the NHP literature that, for well-learned movements, activation in striatum and GPi typically lags activity in cortex and thalamus at movement onset in reaction time experiments, and furthermore reaction times are not negatively impacted by GPi inactivation in NHPs nor in PD pallidotomy recipients (Schwab et al., 2020; see Turner and Desmurget, 2010, for a review). On the surface these findings seem to contradict with the DIVA model’s inclusion of a role in movement initiation for the BG, at least for well-learned words produced in isolation.

To further consider this issue, it is helpful to recognize that speech consists of (at least) two levels of sequencing: (1) sequencing of phonemes within well-learned words (whose production can be considered “overlearned”), and (2) sequencing of words in larger utterances which are not typically well-learned since each utterance can contain a unique combination of words. The later mean onset of GPi activity compared to cortical activity found in reaction time experiments as described above suggests that the initiation of a well-learned word in isolation may not require BG involvement; however, Lipski et al. (2018) report that a subset of units in STN fire well before speech onset, leaving open the possibility of BG involvement in utterance initiation. Furthermore, even if initiation of the first phoneme in a well-learned word is primarily carried out by cortex, transitions from the first to subsequent phonemes might still involve the BG, which is ideally situated to detect the cognitive, sensory, and motor context indicating the completion of an ongoing movement given its widespread input from the relevant cortical areas. Such a role (if it exists) would appear to be supplementary rather than central given that well-learned sequences can be produced without error after GPi inactivation or pallidotomy (discussed further in Section 6.3), but the BG may contribute under more demanding conditions such as fast speech or when producing long or infrequent words that are not overlearned. Further study is needed to address these issues.

Although a central role for the BG in single word production by mature speakers is questionable, the BG very likely play a role in speech motor learning. Prior electrophysiological studies have indicated a role for the BG in a wide range of motor learning tasks (e.g., Miyachi et al., 1997; Sage et al., 2003; Ashby et al., 2010; Piron et al., 2016), and pallidotomy has been shown to negatively impact the learning of new motor sequences (Brown et al., 2003; Obeso et al., 2009). Turner and Desmurget (2010) posit that the BG are heavily involved in early learning stages, particularly for reward-based tasks (with learning modulated by phasic striatal dopamine training signals), based in part on findings that the striatum exhibits rapid changes in an associative learning task, whereas changes in cortex occur on a much slower time scale (Pasupathy and Miller, 2005). With regard to speech in PD, this view predicts that individuals with PD will be impaired in learning to speak a second language, or learning to produce words that are new to them in their native language(s); these hypotheses are supported by a several studies on speech motor learning in PD patients (Schulz et al., 2000; Kaipa et al., 2016; Whitfield and Goberman, 2017). Collectively, the literature strongly suggests that the BG are involved in word learning during speech development as well as learning of new words later in life.

There are also few studies we are aware of directly bearing on the question of whether the initiation of words within a larger utterance, such as a sentence or phrase, critically involves the cortico-BG loop, though Lipski et al. (2023) report speech sequence-related activity in STN in a task involving production of consonant-vowel syllable triplets. Given that naturally occurring speech does not typically involve repeated production of the same sentence nor over-learning of a given word sequence (except possibly for very commonly used sequences, such as one’s phone number), it seems unlikely that a reward-based learning mechanism would be necessary for word initiation in conversational speech, but experimental study of this possibility is still needed before a strong conclusion can be drawn. Turner and Desmurget (2010) also propose a role for the BG in adjusting movement vigor based on motivational factors such as reward contingencies. One can imagine a role for such a mechanism in emphatic stress during running speech if one considers the desire to emphasize a particular word a form of motivational signal; indeed, reduced stress is an established feature of dysarthria in PD (Darley et al., 1969a,b; Pell et al., 2006).

4 Audiomotor impairments in PD

The degree to which PD impacts auditory processing is unclear. In line with non-human primate studies showing responses in STN and GPi to passive movements (Delong et al., 1985; Wichmann et al., 1994), pure auditory responses have been reported in the STN during speech (Tankus and Fried, 2019; Tankus et al., 2021). Unpublished data collected during deep brain stimulation surgery from our group support this idea; STN neurons respond to auditory speech stimuli and even show selectivity for certain speech sounds, suggesting a possible role in sensorimotor processes.

Although some studies have found no difference in pure auditory perceptual task performance between PD patients and age-matched controls (e.g., Di Mauro et al., 2017; De Keyser et al., 2019) others have found a worsening of age-related high-frequency hearing loss in PD (Yylmaz et al., 2009; Vitale et al., 2012; Pisani et al., 2015; Vitale et al., 2016; Folmer et al., 2017; Shalash et al., 2017; Scarpa et al., 2020). With respect to speech, people with PD tend to perform comparably to older healthy controls during phoneme discrimination tasks (Lloyd, 1999; Graber et al., 2002; Ravizza, 2003; Lima et al., 2013); however, impairments in prosodic processing have been noted by Lloyd (1999) and Lima et al. (2013). Furthermore, there is some evidence for abnormal pitch and formant perception in PD. Mollaei et al. (2019) found that discrimination for first formants was significantly lower in PD compared to age and sex-matched healthy controls during passive listening. Meanwhile, PD participants showed better pitch discrimination compared to controls during active voice production. Based on a comprehensive review of the research literature on auditory deficits in PD, De Groote et al. (2020) concluded that speech and pitch perception appear normal in PD, whereas perception of loudness and duration are impaired, with PD patients overestimating intensity of less intense speech and underestimating intensity of more intense speech. Interestingly, loudness and duration are also impaired in the speech output of individuals with PD, suggesting the possibility that impairments in perception of these acoustic parameters may result from an impaired ability to “internally simulate” them. However, the inverse conclusion, that impaired perception leads to impaired production of these parameters, cannot be excluded based on the current evidence.

Within the DIVA/GODIVA framework, the BG do not play a direct role in auditory feedback control. However, impairment of auditory processing would be expected to affect both feedback and feedforward control mechanisms since an impaired ability to detect mismatches between the auditory target and current auditory state would impair both the ability to perform online corrections via feedback control as well as the ability to tune feedforward commands based on auditory feedback. Another possibility is that auditory feedback control may be normal, with only an impairment in updating feedforward commands. These possibilities are discussed in the following paragraphs.

Auditory feedback control of speech in PD has been probed using unexpected auditory perturbations applied to a speaker’s own speech in real-time; such perturbations induce reflexive compensatory responses involving auditory cortex and right inferior frontal cortex (Tourville et al., 2008). If the perturbation is consistent rather than unexpected, neurotypical speakers will also adjust their feedforward commands (sensorimotor adaptation), as evidenced by after-effects once the perturbation is removed.

Although impaired adaptation in response to sustained perturbations of pitch and F1 have been demonstrated in PD (Mollaei et al., 2013; Abur et al., 2018), reflexive responses to unpredicted perturbations of pitch and loudness appear to be either normal (Abur et al., 2021, who tested participants on medication) or larger than normal (Liu et al., 2012; Chen et al., 2013; Huang et al., 2016; 2019; Mollaei et al., 2016, 2019; these studies tested PD participants off medication). Deep brain stimulation applied to STN has been shown to normalize the pitch shift reflex, implicating the basal ganglia in the abnormally large pitch shift reflex in PD (Behroozmand et al., 2019). The lack of impairment in reflexive responses to unexpected pitch perturbations indicates that individuals with PD properly detect pitch errors (see also Mollaei et al., 2019; Abur and Stepp, 2020) which in turn indicates that impaired auditory processing is not the primary cause of impaired audio-motor adaptation to pitch perturbations in PD. In sum, the literature on pitch perturbation responses in PD is consistent with the lack of a BG component to auditory feedback control in the DIVA/GODIVA framework.

The literature on F1 perturbations in PD is somewhat less clear; whereas Abur et al. (2021) find normal F1 perceptual acuity as well as reflexive and adaptive response to F1 perturbations in PD patients who are on medication, reduced reflexive responses (Mollaei et al., 2016, 2019) and adaptive responses (Mollaei et al., 2013) to F1 perturbations have been demonstrated in PD patients tested while off medication, perhaps due to a reduced ability to discriminate small F1 differences (Mollaei et al., 2019). The finding of reduced F1 reflexive responses in PD poses a challenge to the DIVA/GODIVA framework as it suggests a role of the BG in auditory processing and/or auditory feedback control, neither of which are currently represented in the model. One possible explanation for differences between f0 and F1 responses in PD is that the sensorimotor control of speech involves different mechanisms for the phonatory and articulatory systems. As Mollaei et al. (2016) note, the suprasegmental aspects of speech (e.g., pitch and loudness) are more rapidly affected by changes in hearing status (Svirsky et al., 1992; Lane et al., 1997; Perkell et al., 2007), while phonemic parameters, such as vowel formants, are more resistant to changes in hearing status (Cowie and Douglas-Cowie, 1992; Perkell et al., 1992).

According to the DIVA model, reflexive responses to auditory perturbations are mediated by the auditory feedback control system as follows. Auditory errors induced by the perturbation are detected by an auditory error map in auditory cortex (specifically, bilateral posterior superior temporal gyrus) that compares incoming auditory signals with the auditory target for the current sound. Detected errors are then transmitted to right hemisphere premotor cortical regions, which transform the error into a corrective movement command. The size of this corrective command is scaled by an auditory feedback control gain parameter. Neuroimaging studies of reflexive auditory perturbations have verified the existence of auditory error maps in pSTG (Tourville et al., 2008; Parkinson et al., 2012).

Within the DIVA model, exaggerated reflexive responses can result in several ways. First, an abnormally large auditory feedback control gain would lead to a larger motor response for auditorily perceived errors. Second, since the overall motor command in DIVA is composed of both feedforward and feedback control components, a reduction in the gain of the feedforward control component results in an increased influence of feedback control mechanisms on the overall motor command, which in turn can result in increased reflexive responses to auditory perturbations. Reflexive responses in PD are reduced when the speaker volitionally increases loudness, either as part of therapy (Li et al., 2021) or external cuing (Huang et al., 2019), adding further support for interpreting exaggerated reflexive responses in PD as a secondary consequence of reduced feedforward control gain. A third possible mechanism is a reduction in the somatosensory feedback control gain, which, like a reduction of the feedforward command gain, has the effect of increasing the influence of auditory feedback control mechanisms on the overall motor command. This possibility is supported by the somatosensory deficits in PD described in the next section. Collectively, the observations described in this section suggest that anomalies in auditory feedback control-mediated responses are likely due to impairments in the motor and/or somatosensory systems rather than impairments in auditory processing or auditory feedback control mechanisms per se. Consistent with this view, auditory cues can be used to entrain movements in acoustic therapies (Leuk et al., 2020).

The finding of reduced adaptation to sustained auditory perturbations in PD (despite normal or larger-than-normal reflexive responses) is indicative of impaired updating of feedforward commands, captured by a learning rate parameter in the DIVA model. The DIVA model posits a central role for the cerebellum in the updating of feedforward commands, but currently the model does not explicitly implicate the basal ganglia in this process. The findings of impaired sensorimotor adaptation in PD reported here, combined with several reports of reduced adaptation in individuals who stutter (another disorder often associated with basal ganglia malfunction), seem to indicate that dopamine and/or the cortico-BG loop contribute to audio-motor adaptation in a way that is not currently captured by the DIVA/GODIVA framework.

Impaired auditory processing of important acoustic cues such as F1 could also impair the ability of the striatum to identify the proper sensorimotor context for terminating the current motor program and launching the next one in the sequence. This could lead to problems in initiating and maintaining motor programs, as described in Section 3. Some support for this possibility has been found using fMRI-based resting state functional connectivity; Manes et al. (2018) found reduced functional connectivity between left putamen and left superior temporal gyrus in PD participants with speech impairment compared to PD participants with no speech impairment.

5 Somatomotor impairments in PD

Motor action and somatic sensation are intimately linked. For example, the gamma motor neuron system uses efferent motor signals to adjust the sensitivity of muscle spindles for detecting changes in muscle length, a process referred to as proprioception. Though not as directly linked to motor activity as proprioception, tactile feedback from mechanoreceptors in the skin also depends heavily on self-generated movement for its interpretation; e.g., contact between the tongue and palate is expected if the tongue muscles have positioned it against the palate, but is indicative of a movement error if it occurs when the tongue has been commanded to a position that should not involve palatal contact. Indeed, somatosensory deficits have been well documented in PD across a number of nonspeech effectors (Konczak et al., 2009; Conte et al., 2013; Lee et al., 2018; Halperin et al., 2021) as well as along the vocal tract, including the airway (Hammer et al., 2013; Troche et al., 2014; Hegland et al., 2019), larynx (Hammer, 2009; Hammer and Barlow, 2010), jaw (Schneider et al., 1986), and tongue (Chen and Watson, 2017). Changes in vocal tract somatosensation have been shown to correlate with functional measures of speech, voice, and swallowing in people with PD. For example, Hammer and Barlow (2010) reported that larynx mechanosensory detection thresholds correlated with voice intensity, respiratory driving pressure, laryngeal resistance, and lung volume expended per syllable, and Chen and Watson (2017) demonstrated that reduced tongue tip acuity in PD was linked to poorer sibilant contrasts.

Signs of impaired somatosensory processing in PD can be found at multiple levels of the nervous system. Diminished neural activity compared to controls in somatosensory cortex of PD patients in response to passively delivered tactile stimulation of the digits has been shown using PET (Boecker et al., 1999) and fMRI (Nelson et al., 2018). Seemingly paradoxically, PD patients commonly show larger-than-normal reflexive responses to tactile or electrical stimulation of the lips (e.g., Caligiuri and Abbs, 1987; Alfonsi et al., 1993). However, these increased reflexive responses are typically interpreted as the result of decreased cortical inhibition of the subcortical reflex loop, thereby accounting for both decreased cortical activity under somatosensory stimulation and increased reflexive responses in PD.

Another technique used to probe somatosensory processing in the brain involves application of transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) over motor cortex to induce motor responses (e.g., finger muscle twitches) while tactile stimulation is simultaneously applied (e.g., mechanical stimulation of the finger). Neurotypicals exhibit afferent inhibition, evidenced by reduced motor response to TMS in the tactile stimulation condition compared to TMS applied in the absence of tactile stimulation. Several additional studies have demonstrated reduced afferent inhibition in PD compared to neurotypical controls (Sailer et al., 2003; Tamburin et al., 2003; Nelson et al., 2018); furthermore, Sailer et al. (2007) report improved afferent inhibition in PD patients when DBS stimulation is on compared to when it is off.

Conte et al. (2013) posit that dopaminergic denervation of the BG in PD may result in decreased response specificity of incoming sensory information. Neurophysiological studies in animal models have shown distinct representations of effectors in the STN and GP during passive movements (i.e., neurons responding selectively to movement of a single upper or lower limb joint); however, after selective lesions of the SNc are used to induce a parkinsonian state, the effector representation becomes less distinct—activity becomes spatially distributed throughout larger portions of the nuclei and neurons more frequently respond to passive movements of multiple joints (Filion et al., 1988; Boraud et al., 2000). Extrapolating to speech production in PD, it is possible that deficient somatosensory function of the vocal tract is the result of blurred sensory representations of speech effectors within the motor system (cf. Nambu, 2011).

An additional possible source of speech-related somatosensory deficits is neuropathology within the peripheral nerve fibers. Mu et al. (2013b, 2015) conducted post-mortem analyses of sensory nerves in the pharynx and upper airway and found evidence of Lewy-type pathology in people with PD. Furthermore, PD participants with dysphagia had more markers of pathology in regions critical for swallowing reflexes (Mu et al., 2013b, 2015). To date, no studies have investigated whether sensory nerve pathology along the vocal tract correlates with voice or speech symptoms in PD. However, the observations of Lewy-type pathology within the sensory nerves of the upper airway suggest that peripheral sensory mechanisms of voice and speech dysfunction merit further investigation.

In the DIVA/GODIVA framework, the BG are not directly involved in somatosensory feedback control of speech movements. However, the somatosensory feedback controller depends on accurate somatosensory feedback for proper operations, and deficits in somatosensory feedback processing will thus impact somatosensory feedback control (e.g., responses to externally applied forces). The larger-than-normal reflexive responses to tactile stimulation suggest an increased gain of the somatosensory feedback control system in PD, possibly due (at least in part) to reduced cortical inhibition of subcortical feedback loops, or to reduced “competition” with feedforward commands as posited above for exaggerated reflexive responses to auditory perturbations. It is also possible that these two accounts are two sides of the same coin; i.e., reduced cortical inhibition of subcortical feedback loops may be (part of) the mechanism that reduces the influence of feedforward commands on the somatosensory feedback controller’s responses to perturbations.

An alternative interpretation of increased reflexive responses in PD is that the brain may be “under-sensing” the size of the corrective responses, perhaps due to impaired proprioception. This view is compatible with the findings of decreased cortical activity in response to tactile stimulation noted above, and it also accords with the finding of Mefferd and Bissmeyer (2016), who noted an anomalous increase in vowel contrasts in PD when speaking with a bite block. In the absence of any compensation, the blocking of jaw movement imposed by a bite block would have the effect of reducing vowel contrasts. Neurotypical speakers compensate by increasing tongue and lip movements just enough to overcome the jaw-limiting effect of the bite block. In contrast, speakers with PD continue the compensatory movements beyond their normal productions, thereby producing larger vowel contrasts than they produce in the absence of the bite block. In the DIVA model, an increase in the gain of the somatosensory feedback controller would speed up compensatory movements, but these movements would not overshoot the targets for normal (unperturbed) speech; a sustained overshoot would only occur if the system was underestimating the effects of the compensatory movements generated by the somatosensory feedback control system.

6 Effects of pharmacological and surgical treatments for PD on speech

6.1 Pharmacological treatments

In PD, the primary medical approach is to correct for dopaminergic deficiencies using dopamine replacement therapy (DRT; i.e., levodopa) or dopamine agonists. DRT significantly improves clinical motor signs of PD (Katzenschlager and Lees, 2002). Studies of oral motor function suggest that levodopa may improve the strength (Cahill et al., 1998; De Letter et al., 2003; Lechien et al., 2019) and physiology (Robertson and Hammerstad, 1996; Gallena et al., 2001; Tawadros et al., 2012) of the speech articulators. However, the effects of levodopa on speech production are less straightforward.

Despite providing significant clinical improvement for motor signs in PD, DRT appears to have limited efficacy for improving functional speech outcomes. The majority of studies examining the acute effects of dopaminergic therapy found no significant group-level treatment effects on measures of speech and voice function (Kompoliti et al., 2000; Ho et al., 2008; Plowman-Prine et al., 2009; Skodda et al., 2010, 2011; Fabbri et al., 2017; Whitfield et al., 2018; Cavallieri et al., 2021; Tykalova et al., 2022). However, some studies have reported levodopa-related improvements in acoustic measures of vocal pitch and quality (Sanabria et al., 2001; Lechien et al., 2019; Pah et al., 2021). Ho et al. (2008) reported a trend towards improved vocal intensity when comparing medication ON and OFF states. Improvements in voice onset times (Fischer and Goberman, 2010), percent pause times (Goberman et al., 2005) and intelligibility (De Letter et al., 2005, 2007) have also been reported. However, in general the effects of dopaminergic medication on speech outcome measures do not appear to be robust or consistent across studies.

The inconsistency of dopaminergic effects on speech outcomes may be in part related to the variability of speech characteristics among participants with PD. While the evidence for a generalized effect of DRT on speech is limited, some studies have noted treatment related improvements for individuals with specific speech profiles as well as groups characterized by more severe speech characteristics. For example, Skodda et al. (2010) reported that a subset of participants with vowel articulation difficulties experienced a significant improvement in vowel articulation following DRT. Within the phonatory system, Cushnie-Sparrow et al. (2018) found that DRT improved voice quality for a subset of participants who had poor voice quality in the off-medication state. Gallena et al. (2001) found that levodopa helped to reduce overactive laryngeal EMG activity for individual participants and noted that these changes correlated with improved speech outcomes. Im et al. (2019) found that PD participants who were categorized as having dysfluencies had a significant effect of DRT for improving fluency. Interestingly, Cushnie-Sparrow et al. (2018), Gallena et al. (2001), and Im et al. (2019) found that levodopa responsiveness was associated with greater symptom severity. Using a longitudinal approach, Rusz et al. (2021) investigated the long-term effects of DRT on speech outcomes for de novo PD participants with different subtypes of speech characteristics—phonatory-prosodic, articulatory-prosodic, and prosodic. The study showed that after 1-year of DRT overall speech impairment improved in the phonatory-prosodic group and remained stable in the articulatory-prosodic and prosodic groups, while those who did not initiate DRT showed a decline in speech function. Taken together, these studies suggest that DRT can improve speech symptoms in those with significant speech impairment, while also highlighting the need to better understand how DRT affects the speech of PD patients with different speech profiles and different levels of speech severity.

Within the DIVA/GODIVA framework, dopaminergic depletion results in a net decrease in excitatory support for feedforward control processes in the SMA initiation map, speech sound map, and articulator map. Restoring dopaminergic input to the striatum via pharmacological treatment could alter speech sensorimotor control in a few ways. First, increasing excitatory support for the SMA initiation map would help facilitate the initiation of speech motor programs in PD and would allow the speaker to produce speech movements with higher gain. Second, restoring dopaminergic input to the striatum allows the initiation map to more effectively monitor the correct sensorimotor context for initiating the next motor program in a sequence. In doing so, dopaminergic therapy would help to strengthen the activation of the current motor program while reducing competition with subsequent motor programs. This could reduce the truncation and scaling decay of subsequent words in a sequence, thereby reducing the accelerated speech rate that is seen in PD. Third, the therapeutic effects of dopamine replacement on feedforward control would reduce the need for a compensatory reliance on auditory feedback. This view posits that PD patients who are off-medication state rely more heavily on sensory feedback control to compensate for feedforward control deficits. Meanwhile, patients in the on-medication state may rely less on sensory mechanisms due to the positive effects of levodopa on feedforward motor control. As a result, we would expect over-exaggerated compensatory responses to reflexive pitch perturbations in the off-medication state (Liu et al., 2012; Chen et al., 2013; Huang et al., 2016; Mollaei et al., 2016, 2019), but not in the on-medication state (Abur et al., 2021), consistent with the existing literature on auditory perturbation responses in PD.

6.2 Deep brain stimulation

Deep brain stimulation (DBS) is the gold-standard treatment for moderate- to late-stage PD for which motor symptoms are not well-controlled by DRT. DBS for PD delivers electrical impulses to either the STN or GPi,7 providing relief from the most debilitating PD motor symptoms (Starr et al., 1998). DBS induces physiological changes at multiple spatial (e.g., cellular, microcircuit, and network) and temporal (milliseconds to years) scales (Neumann et al., 2023). Although its precise therapeutic mechanisms remain unknown, there is a growing consensus that DBS works by dampening transmission of the pathologic patterns of neuronal activity associated with PD (synchronized oscillatory spiking) while not affecting the overall firing rate of BG output neurons (Bar-Gad et al., 2004; Agnesi et al., 2013; Rosenbaum et al., 2014; Wichmann et al., 2018; Neumann et al., 2023). Despite DBS’s efficacy for some limb motor symptoms (tremor and rigidity), speech deterioration is a concern for patients considering DBS for movement disorders (Bronstein et al., 2011; Aldridge et al., 2016). Because recent comprehensive reviews of DBS’s effects on speech have already been conducted (Skodda, 2012; Aldridge et al., 2016; Baudouin et al., 2023), we will only summarize the themes here and comment on what we can infer about healthy speech and PD speech from studies of DBS.

Using DBS outcome studies as a circuit dissection tool to understand the role of the BG in speech can be problematic. In addition to which nucleus is targeted, the following factors must be considered: behavioral measure (acoustic vs. intelligibility vs. self-perception outcomes); medication state (OFF vs. ON); electrode location within the target nucleus (e.g., anterior vs. posterior STN); stimulation parameters (low vs. high intensity and frequency); volume of neural tissue affected by the electrode current; and timescale of effects (immediate vs. gradual). The heterogeneity of behavioral measures across studies alone makes interpreting the effects of DBS challenging; some studies report acoustic parameters (D'Alatri et al., 2008; Van Lancker Sidtis et al., 2010; Karlsson et al., 2013; Martel-Sauvageau and Tjaden, 2017) while others focus on intelligibility ratings (Tornqvist et al., 2005; Tripoliti et al., 2011; Chiu et al., 2020) or self-perceived speech ratings (Miller et al., 2006; Wertheimer et al., 2014; Kopf et al., 2022). Furthermore, two different time windows of effects of stimulation should not be conflated: (a) acute intraoperative microelectrode stimulation mapping or DBS lead stimulation after it’s turned on in the weeks after surgery, and (b) long-term (months to years) cumulative effects of brain stimulation. Distinguishing between these time windows has important implications for the interpretation of the computational contribution of the BG to speech. Gradual pre- vs. post-DBS voice and speech changes are an important trend to highlight for patients’ quality of life (Tripoliti et al., 2011, 2014; Aviles-Olmos et al., 2014; Wertheimer et al., 2014; Tanaka et al., 2020; Gessani et al., 2023), but these gradual changes inform neurocomputational understanding of BG only indirectly because the neural substrates of such changes may result both from neuroplastic and neurodegenerative processes. We will thus focus primarily on short-term effects herein.

When considering the effects of STN-DBS on speech, a major confounding factor (from the perspective of understanding STN function) is that the STN is enveloped by axonal fibers, including motor efferents to the cranial nerves responsible for movements of the vocal tract and larynx residing within the internal capsule and ascending sensory fibers of the trigeminothalamic tract (Petersen et al., 2019). Given the thin morphology of the STN, it is difficult to avoid stimulating these fibers with DBS. GPi-DBS is less prone to this issue because GPi has a larger volume and therefore its borders are typically further from the internal capsule, and furthermore the trigeminothalamic tract is not nearby. The possibility of off-target stimulation highlights the need for caution when interpreting studies of STN-DBS that do not carefully control for electrode location (cf. Astrom et al., 2010; Jorge et al., 2020). In many cases speech impairment with STN-DBS may be due to disruption of signals in the internal capsule rather than (or in addition to) disruption of STN function. Furthermore, PD subtyping might elucidate why some patients experience a deterioration of speech after DBS while others experience an amelioration. Similarly, speech symptom subtyping may be a promising route to clarifying DBS’s effect on speech and making single-subject predictions about the risk of speech decline (Tsuboi et al., 2015; Tanaka et al., 2020; Gessani et al., 2023). A clear advantage of modern DBS is its programmability and dynamicity; closed-loop, responsive DBS systems will likely continue to improve DBS’s influence on speech (Little et al., 2016; Pina-Fuentes et al., 2020).

Setting these complications aside, the most pertinent studies for understanding the computational contribution of STN to speech are those that report effects on speech in a post-operative DBS-ON vs. DBS-OFF condition because this contrast reveals acute effects of DBS. One trend in these studies is that vocal parameters like loudness and pitch variability improve with DBS-ON (Dromey et al., 2000; Lundgren et al., 2011; Moreau et al., 2011; Karlsson et al., 2013; Skodda et al., 2014; Behroozmand et al., 2019) whereas speech intelligibility declines (Tornqvist et al., 2005; Klostermann et al., 2008; Tripoliti et al., 2008; Dromey and Bjarnason, 2011; Skodda et al., 2014). It is important to highlight the variability of patient outcomes in these studies; while the cohort statistics suggest one trend, many patients (~20% or more) will contradict the trend (Tripoliti et al., 2011; Tanaka et al., 2020; Gessani et al., 2023). Indeed, it is uncontroversial that STN-DBS increases the variability of speech outcomes (in both the short-term and long-term) relative to medication-only treatment.

Recently, Bobin et al. (2024) found that right STN stimulation was superior to left STN stimulation in improving dysphonia. They also report that with careful titration of stimulation parameters, both left and right STN stimulation could improve dysarthria acutely, with the effects of left STN stimulation limited to improvements in voice intensity measures. The authors also mapped stimulation location within the STN, finding posteromedial contacts had strong positive effects on voice parameters. This contrasts with a previous study reporting that anterior STN stimulation was associated with improved perceptual and acoustic-aerodynamic outcomes (Jorge et al., 2020), and another study reporting that posteromedial stimulation was associated with dysarthria (Astrom et al., 2010). Future studies reporting stimulation locations in greater quantity and with higher accuracy are needed to elucidate the extent to which STN topography and laterality contribute to voice and speech control.

Compared to STN-DBS, the influence of GPi-DBS on voice and speech has been less studied. A recent study comparing speech acoustic and perceptual measures from the speech of 10 STN-DBS and 8 GPi-DBS recipients found no differences between the two stimulation locations (van Brenk et al., 2024). A recent review article suggested that GPi-DBS may be favorable to STN-DBS along voice dimensions (Baudouin et al., 2023). Concordant with this idea, Finger et al. (2020) reported improved voice and speech intelligibility in patients undergoing bilateral GPi-DBS for dystonia. However, Chiu et al. (2020) looked retrospectively at 20 PD patients after bilateral GPi-DBS and found that laryngeal components along with velopharyngeal resonance worsened in the DBS-ON, medication-ON condition relative to the pre-operative medication-ON condition. While speech intelligibility remained unchanged in the short term (six-month follow-up vs. baseline), it trended downward after one year. These results are difficult to interpret, given the lack of a non-DBS comparison group to control for the expected effects of disease progression.

Thus far we have focused on the interaction between DBS and motor execution of speech. Another angle to consider is DBS and speech motor learning, motivated by the extensive evidence for the role of basal ganglia in songbird vocal learning (Haesler et al., 2007; Kosubek-Langer and Scharff, 2020), human and NHP non-speech motor learning (Doyon et al., 2009), and human speech acquisition (Lai et al., 2003; Vargha-Khadem et al., 2005; Ziegler and Ackermann, 2017). To our knowledge, no study has directly tested the effect of DBS-ON vs. DBS-OFF on speech motor learning in a laboratory setting, and results are mixed regarding whether PD patients with DBS realized greater gains from speech therapy than those without DBS (Spielman et al., 2011; Tripoliti et al., 2011). However, substantial evidence indicates a facilitative effect on short-term non-speech motor learning occurring during DBS-ON over DBS-OFF states. STN-DBS stimulation facilitates motor learning in tasks involving sequential cursor movements (Carbon and Eidelberg, 2006; Mure et al., 2012), sequential finger tapping (Muehlberg et al., 2023), single-target cursor movements (de Almeida Marcelino et al., 2019), and visuomotor perturbation adaptation (Singh et al., 2019). One study which measured regional cerebral blood flow during task performance found that STN-DBS-facilitated learning is associated with increased activity in lateral cerebellum and dorsal premotor cortex, coupled with reduced activity in supplementary motor area (Mure et al., 2012). This finding aligns with the prediction of the DIVA/GODIVA framework that successful motor sequence learning involves the chaining together of sequence elements via a cortico-cerebellar loop, along with reduced reliance on the SMA to control individual-element timing in well-learned sequences. GPi-DBS stimulation has been shown to improve both performance and learning of a sequential cursor movement task (Fukuda et al., 2002; Carbon and Eidelberg, 2006). Concurrent PET imaging revealed that GPi DBS-facilitated learning gains were associated with increased activity in a similar cortical network connected to STN DBS-facilitated learning, including bilateral premotor cortex (Fukuda et al., 2002). This network activity enhancement was not correlated with GPi-DBS facilitation of a motor reference task, indicating its specificity to motor learning. This learning-related network activity was subsequently shown to be associated with learning gains in the same task in healthy controls and PD patients without DBS treatment (Carbon et al., 2003). STN stimulation might additionally facilitate speech motor learning through an enhancement of verbal working memory (Pillon et al., 2000). The possibility of these effects holds clinical significance due to the widespread need of these patients for speech therapy to address disease-related speech and voice impairments (Ramig et al., 2008; Dressler et al., 2016; Barkmeier-Kraemer and Clark, 2017). It is noteworthy that DBS stimulation does not increase motor learning or performance to above the level of controls (Singh et al., 2019; Muehlberg et al., 2023), and the learning-facilitative effects of DBS may depend on disease progression being at an advanced stage (Meissner et al., 2016).

6.3 Pallidotomy

Stereotactic lesions to the basal ganglia have been used in various eras of neurosurgical treatment of PD (Guridi and Lozano, 1997). Popularized after a serendipitous surgical error, lesions to the globus pallidus (“pallidotomy”) have been shown to treat some of the most debilitating symptoms of PD (Cooper, 1959). Invasive vascular occlusion and chemical lesions (Guiot and Brion, 1953; Narabayashi et al., 1956; Obrador, 1957; Cooper, 1959), invasive thermal lesions (Svennilson et al., 1960; Laitinen et al., 1992), and most recently non-invasive focused ultrasound lesions (Krishna et al., 2023) share the same goal of destroying tissue in part of the pallidum. After the realization that dopamine-replacing therapies result in dyskinesias, thermal lesion unilateral and bilateral posteroventral pallidotomies (PVPs) were performed regularly in the 1990s and 2000s after Laitinen et al. (1992) reintroduced the procedure, integrating neuroimaging methods and anatomical knowledge (Laitinen et al., 1992; Iacono et al., 1995; Alterman and Kelly, 1998; Barlow et al., 1998; Scott et al., 1998; Ghika et al., 1999; Favre et al., 2000).

Given the many hypotheses of basal ganglia contributions to motor control (e.g., action selection, movement gain/vigor, habit formation, and behavioral automation), it is at first blush somewhat surprising that lesioning the primary output nucleus of the BG is not more detrimental to motor execution (Marsden and Obeso, 1994). As one of the most complex behaviors controlled by the human motor system, speech should, in theory, rely heavily on BG mechanism’s. However, speech production appears to remain largely intact in many patients even after bilateral PVP. In a rare study reporting acoustics and aerodynamics measurements before and after bilateral PVP in 14 patients, Barlow et al. (1998) found subtle changes along certain measures but overall “patients… were 100% intelligible before and after pallidotomy.” Favre et al. (2000) reported on both staged and simultaneous bilateral pallidotomies in a cohort of 17 patients. They found that 50–60% of patients experienced major speech deterioration after their procedure, though they speculate that speech problems involved the internal capsule rather than the GPi itself. De Bie et al. (2002) reported on 13 patients who underwent staged bilateral pallidotomy. Five of 13 patients “had problems with speech,” one of which had severe dysarthria. In a larger study with 115 patients in the UK and United states, Parkin et al. (2002) reported that “speech was adversely affected” after bilateral posteroventral pallidotomy but that “the change was small in most cases.” Two in three patients experienced a worsening of speech after pallidotomy, but only 8% of patients experienced major deterioration. Higuchi and Iacono (2003) looked retrospectively at 796 patients who underwent pallidotomy over a 7-year period. Of these, 272 received simultaneous bilateral pallidotomies while 88 received staged bilateral pallidotomies. They report that speech disturbances were observed in only 2.6% of patients after surgery, and that simultaneous bilateral pallidotomy was not significantly more associated with speech and swallowing disturbances than unilateral pallidotomy. However, some caveats are in order regarding older studies: the quality of imaging in these studies is generally lower that those performed with modern MRI sequences, the exact location and extent of the lesions are less precisely reported, and the durability of the behavioral effect (an indirect measure of lesion permanence) may be unknown. In a more recent study, York et al. (2007) reported speech declines in 9 of 15 patients who underwent bilateral staged pallidotomy at 3–12 months after their second procedure, with speech reported as the most prevalent complication at long-term follow-up as 79% of patients experienced hypophonia at >2 years post-procedure.

Pallidotomy fell out of favor with the rise of DBS in the late 2000s, but it has seen a resurgence with novel non-invasive technology. Focused ultrasound ablation of the globus pallidus is now in clinical trial for PD patients averse to invasive, open-brain surgery. Krishna et al. (2023) conducted a double-blind clinical trial of 65 PD patients who underwent unilateral focused ultrasound pallidal ablation, and 22 patients assigned to a sham procedure. Although just 2 of 65 patients experienced noticeable dysarthria after the procedure, no other speech measures were assessed.

Although rates of speech complications vary greatly across surgical centers after PVP (Favre et al., 2000), the relatively mild impact on speech from bilateral pallidotomy in some patients appears to indicate that the role(s) of the basal ganglia in speech may be supportive rather than essential, though one cannot completely rule out the possibility that spared portions of the BG output nuclei (e.g., the non-lesioned portion of GPi) or the substantia nigra pars reticulata (SNr) carry out speech-related roles after bilateral PVP. One possible explanation is that the BG are heavily involved in the learning of motor sequences, but their role diminishes after sequence learning, at which point they may still play a role in movement gain but are no longer needed for sequencing or action selection (Turner and Desmurget, 2010).

7 Summary

In this article we reviewed the scientific literature as it concerns hypothesized roles of the basal ganglia in speech production and how these roles may be impacted by Parkinson’s disease. The possible roles, summarized in the following paragraphs, include modulating the gain of speech movements, contributing to the learning of frequently used speech movement sequences, and contributing to the fluent sequencing of words within longer utterances.

Regarding gain modulation, widespread reports of reduced movement amplitudes and vocal output in PD, along with reduced movement gains found with pallidotomy, indicate a BG role in movement gain. Within the DIVA/GODIVA framework, this reduced gain is attributed to reduced support from the cortico-BG loop for the current word’s node in an SMA-based initiation map, which in turn reduces the gain of the motor commands constituting the word’s motor program. According to this account, this reduced support can also result in early termination of the word’s motor program, thus providing an explanation for common reports of accelerating speech in PD patients, a finding that might otherwise seem at odds with reduced movement gains.

A BG role in the learning of motor sequences has been firmly established in animal studies of non-speech motor sequencing. Because speech motor sequence learning (in the form of word learning) primarily occurs well before the onset of PD, an impairment in this capacity may go unnoticed unless the patient is learning a second language. The DIVA/GODIVA framework predicts that PD patients will show significant deficits relative to age-matched controls in learning to produce new words or word sequences; this prediction is supported by several experimental findings concerning the learning of novel speech sequences by individuals with PD.

A third possible role of the BG in speech concerns the temporary buffering and sequential readout of phonological material for longer utterances such as sentences. Speech sequence-related activity has been observed in STN during production of novel syllable sequences in a task that did not involve motor learning, suggesting some role for the BG in sequencing of words within longer utterances. However, bilateral pallidotomy does not typically result in a significant increase in sequencing errors, suggesting that any such role is supplementary rather than central. Numerous studies have reported reduced word stress in PD; these findings are compatible with the view that the BG are heavily involved in controlling movement intensity based in part on motivational context.

Although the primary roles of the BG within the DIVA/GODIVA framework concern the feedforward control mechanisms described above, a number of studies have investigated sensory-motor processes, specifically auditory and somatosensory feedback control, in PD. A number of studies report little or no impairment in pure auditory processing (with the exception of loudness and duration, two aspects of speech that are also impaired in the motor output of speakers with PD) nor in auditory feedback control of speech as measured by reflexive responses to unpredictable auditory perturbations which invoke auditory feedback control mechanisms, though reduced responses to F1 perturbations in the medication OFF state have been reported. Instead, PD patients often show exaggerated reflexive responses, possibly due to a reduced gain in the feedforward control subsystem that would otherwise dampen the auditory feedback control response. These findings are consistent with the DIVA/GODIVA framework, which posits that the BG are not heavily involved in sensory feedback-based control of speech movements. The frequently reported finding of reduced audiomotor adaptation in PD indicates a role for dopamine and/or cortico-BG loops in stimulating learning of feedforward commands; this role is not yet explicitly included in the DIVA/GODIVA framework.

Studies utilizing somatosensory perturbations, such as speaking with a bite block, also report exaggerated responses in PD. Unlike auditory feedback, which does not depend heavily on motor commands, somatosensory feedback processing requires integration of (possibly impaired) motor information with tactile and proprioceptive information from mechanoreceptors in the vocal tract articulators. One possible explanation for increased reflexive responses to tactile stimulation in PD is that the somatosensory feedback control system acts with a higher-than-normal gain. However, this account cannot explain experimental data indicating that PD speakers actually increase their vowel contrasts (compared to unperturbed speech) when speaking with a bite block; in other words, they overshoot their normal vowel targets. This finding may indicate that PD speakers underestimate the size of their corrective movements under perturbed conditions, providing an alternative account of the increased reflexive responses in response to auditory and somatosensory perturbations in PD.

The literature on therapeutic effects of drug and surgical treatments provides further clues to BG involvement in speech. Dopaminergic replacement therapy does not appear to consistently affect speech, though a few studies show improvement in some acoustic measures of speech. The literature on deep brain stimulation is even more mixed, with some studies showing improvement (particularly in loudness and pitch variability), but many studies noting deterioration of speech with DBS. In many cases deterioration of speech with DBS likely arises due to unintended stimulation of descending motor tracts in the internal capsule, or in the case of the STN, ascending sensory tracts in the medial lemniscus. In some cases, deterioration of speech occurs immediately, while in others it occurs only over a period of months where the effects of disease progression are difficult to disentangle. The literature on pallidotomy suggests relatively little deterioration of speech even with bilateral pallidotomy; this finding strongly suggests that any role played by the BG in executing previously learned movements is supplementary rather than mandatory, perhaps limited to adjustment of movement gains and durations based (at least in part) on motivational factors.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author contributions

JM: Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. LB: Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. AM: Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. RT: Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. RR: Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. FG: Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This study was supported in part by grants U01 NS117836 (RR, PI), R01 DC016270 (C. Stepp and FG, PIs), R01 DC07683 (FG, PI), T32 DC013017 (C. Moore and C. Stepp, PIs), T32 DC000038 (B. Delgutte, PI), and F31 DC022153 (LB, PI), from the National Institutes of Health, and by Aligning Science Across Parkinson’s through the Michael J. Fox Foundation for Parkinson’s Research (ASAP-020519 to RT; https://parkinsonsroadmap.org/, https://www.michaeljfox.org/).

Acknowledgments

We thank Barbara Holland for editing assistance.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Footnotes

1. ^For the remainder of this article, we will assume word-sized chunks and use the term “words” for the sake of readability, with the understanding that other chunk sizes (e.g., syllables, phonological words) may be represented in addition to, or instead of, word-sized chunks.

2. ^Hereafter, hemispheric laterality will only be mentioned in cases where function is significantly lateralized.

3. ^Although the cortical areas depicted in Figure 4 all project to the striatum, they do not all converge on the same portion of the striatum. For example, the putamen targets of prefrontal areas such as preSMA and pIFS are located dorsomedially relative to the more “motoric” regions vMC, SMA, and vPMC (Nambu, 2011). The theoretical account described herein is not dependent on any topographic organization within BG structures; however, it is certainly possible that such an organization may confer performance advantages over non-topographic (e.g., randomly distributed target neurons in the striatum) representations, and disruption of this topography may contribute to motor symptoms in PD (Nambu, 2011).

4. ^Not shown in Figure 4 are limbic and paralimbic areas that also contribute to what can be termed the motivational context (cf. Mazzoni et al., 2007); these will not be addressed in detail here since these aspects of the circuit are not elaborated in the DIVA or GODIVA model at present.

5. ^Turner and Desmurget (2010) use the term vigor to indicate an interaction between a motivational signal and movement gain. Since the DIVA/GODIVA framework does not address the motivational component, we will use the word gain in the remainder of the article in place of the term vigor.

6. ^It is important to consider that the variability of speech rate presentations in PD may reflect different neural mechanisms. For example, slow speaking rate can be the result of cognitive decline rather than purely motor processes (Lowit et al., 2006). Variability in speech rate symptoms may also reflect broader impairments in temporal processing (Jones and Jahanshahi, 2014), internal monitoring of speech rate (Ackermann et al., 1997), or an impaired ability to control the duration of speech events (Ludlow et al., 1987).

7. ^The other common target of DBS for movement disorders is ventral intermediate nucleus (Vim) of the thalamus (Neudorfer et al., 2024). Vim is targeted for treatment of essential tremor. Vim receives projections primarily from the cerebellum rather than the basal ganglia (Ilinsky et al., 2018); it will therefore not be treated here.

References

Abur, D., Lester-Smith, R. A., Daliri, A., Lupiani, A. A., Guenther, F. H., and Stepp, C. E. (2018). Sensorimotor adaptation of voice fundamental frequency in Parkinson's disease. PLoS One 13:e0191839. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0191839

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Abur, D., and Stepp, C. E. (2020). Acuity to changes in self-generated vocal pitch in Parkinson’s disease. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 63, 3208–3214. doi: 10.1044/2020_JSLHR-20-00003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Abur, D., Subaciute, A., Daliri, A., Lester-Smith, R. A., Lupiani, A. A., Cilento, D., et al. (2021). Feedback and feedforward auditory-motor processes for voice and articulation in Parkinson’s disease. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 64, 4682–4694. doi: 10.1044/2021_JSLHR-21-00153

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ackermann, H., Konczak, J., and Hertrich, I. (1997). The temporal control of repetitive articulatory movements in Parkinson's disease. Brain Lang. 56, 312–319. doi: 10.1006/brln.1997.1851

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Agnesi, F., Connolly, A. T., Baker, K. B., Vitek, J. L., and Johnson, M. D. (2013). Deep brain stimulation imposes complex informational lesions. PLoS One 8:e74462. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0074462

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Albin, R. L., Young, A. B., and Penney, J. B. (1989). The functional anatomy of basal ganglia disorders. Trends Neurosci. 12, 366–375. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(89)90074-X

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Aldridge, D., Theodoros, D., Angwin, A., and Vogel, A. P. (2016). Speech outcomes in Parkinson's disease after subthalamic nucleus deep brain stimulation: a systematic review. Parkinsonism Relat. Disord. 33, 3–11. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2016.09.022

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Alexander, G. E., Crutcher, M. D., and Delong, M. R. (1990). Basal ganglia-thalamocortical circuits: parallel substrates for motor, oculomotor, "prefrontal" and "limbic" functions. Prog. Brain Res. 85, 119–146

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Alfonsi, E., Nappi, G., Pacchetti, C., Martignoni, E., Conti, R., Sandrini, G., et al. (1993). Changes in motoneuron excitability of masseter muscle following exteroceptive stimuli in Parkinson's disease. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 89, 29–34. doi: 10.1016/0168-5597(93)90081-Y

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Alterman, R. L., and Kelly, P. J. (1998). Pallidotomy technique and results: the New York University experience. Neurosurg. Clin. N. Am. 9, 337–344. doi: 10.1016/S1042-3680(18)30269-9

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Arnold, C., Gehrig, J., Gispert, S., Seifried, C., and Kell, C. A. (2014). Pathomechanisms and compensatory efforts related to parkinsonian speech. Neuroimage Clin. 4, 82–97. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2013.10.016

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ashby, F. G., Turner, B. O., and Horvitz, J. C. (2010). Cortical and basal ganglia contributions to habit learning and automaticity. Trends Cogn. Sci. 14, 208–215. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.02.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Astrom, M., Tripoliti, E., Hariz, M. I., Zrinzo, L. U., Martinez-Torres, I., Limousin, P., et al. (2010). Patient-specific model-based investigation of speech intelligibility and movement during deep brain stimulation. Stereotact. Funct. Neurosurg. 88, 224–233. doi: 10.1159/000314357

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Aviles-Olmos, I., Kefalopoulou, Z., Tripoliti, E., Candelario, J., Akram, H., Martinez-Torres, I., et al. (2014). Long-term outcome of subthalamic nucleus deep brain stimulation for Parkinson's disease using an Mri-guided and Mri-verified approach. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 85, 1419–1425. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2013-306907

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bar-Gad, I., Elias, S., Vaadia, E., and Bergman, H. (2004). Complex locking rather than complete cessation of neuronal activity in the globus pallidus of a 1-methyl-4-phenyl-1,2,3,6-tetrahydropyridine-treated primate in response to pallidal microstimulation. J. Neurosci. 24, 7410–7419. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1691-04.2004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Barkmeier-Kraemer, J. M., and Clark, H. M. (2017). Speech-language pathology evaluation and Management of Hyperkinetic Disorders Affecting Speech and Swallowing Function. Tremor Other Hyperkinet. Mov. (N Y) 7:489. doi: 10.5334/tohm.381

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Barlow, S., Iacono, R., Paseman, L., Biswas, A., and D’antonio, L. (1998). “The effects of posteroventral pallidotomy on force and speech aerodynamics in Parkinson’s disease” in Neuromotor speech disorders: Nature, assessment and management. eds. M. Cannito, K. Yorkston, and D. Beukelman (Baltimore, MD: Brookes), 117–156.

Google Scholar

Baudouin, R., Lechien, J. R. M. R., Carpentier, L., Gurruchaga, J.-M., Lisan, Q., and Hans, S. P. (2023). Deep brain stimulation impact on voice and speech quality in Parkinson's disease: a systematic review. Otolaryngol. Head Neck Surg. 168, 307–318. doi: 10.1177/01945998221120189

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Baumann, A., Nebel, A., Granert, O., Giehl, K., Wolff, S., Schmidt, W., et al. (2018). Neural correlates of hypokinetic dysarthria and mechanisms of effective voice treatment in Parkinson Disease. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair 32, 1055–1066. doi: 10.1177/1545968318812726

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Behroozmand, R., Johari, K., Kelley, R. M., Kapnoula, E. C., Narayanan, N. S., and Greenlee, J. D. W. (2019). Effect of deep brain stimulation on vocal motor control mechanisms in Parkinson's disease. Parkinsonism Relat. Disord. 63, 46–53. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2019.03.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bobin, M., Sulzer, N., Brundler, G., Staib, M., Imbach, L. L., Stieglitz, L. H., et al. (2024). Direct subthalamic nucleus stimulation influences speech and voice quality in Parkinson's disease patients. Brain Stimul. 17, 112–124. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2024.01.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Boecker, H., Ceballos-Baumann, A., Bartenstein, P., Weindl, A., Siebner, H. R., Fassbender, T., et al. (1999). Sensory processing in Parkinson's and Huntington's disease: investigations with 3D H(2)(15)O-pet. Brain 122, 1651–1665. doi: 10.1093/brain/122.9.1651

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Boraud, T., Bezard, E., Bioulac, B., and Gross, C. E. (2000). Ratio of inhibited-to-activated pallidal neurons decreases dramatically during passive limb movement in the Mptp-treated monkey. J. Neurophysiol. 83, 1760–1763. doi: 10.1152/jn.2000.83.3.1760

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Braak, H., Ghebremedhin, E., Rub, U., Bratzke, H., and Del Tredici, K. (2004). Stages in the development of Parkinson's disease-related pathology. Cell Tissue Res. 318, 121–134. doi: 10.1007/s00441-004-0956-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bronstein, J. M., Tagliati, M., Alterman, R. L., Lozano, A. M., Volkmann, J., Stefani, A., et al. (2011). Deep brain stimulation for Parkinson disease: an expert consensus and review of key issues. Arch. Neurol. 68:165, doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2010.260

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, P. (2003). Oscillatory nature of human basal ganglia activity: relationship to the pathophysiology of Parkinson's disease. Mov. Disord. 18, 357–363. doi: 10.1002/mds.10358

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, R. G., Jahanshahi, M., Limousin-Dowsey, P., Thomas, D., Quinn, N. P., and Rothwell, J. C. (2003). Pallidotomy and incidental sequence learning in Parkinson's disease. Neuroreport 14, 21–24. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200301200-00004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, S., Laird, A. R., Pfordresher, P. Q., Thelen, S. M., Turkeltaub, P., and Liotti, M. (2009). The somatotopy of speech: phonation and articulation in the human motor cortex. Brain Cogn. 70, 31–41. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2008.12.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bullock, D., and Grossberg, S. (1988). Neural dynamics of planned arm movements: emergent invariants and speed-accuracy properties during trajectory formation. Psychol. Rev. 95, 49–90. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.95.1.49

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cahill, L. M., Murdoch, B. E., Theodoros, D. G., Triggs, E. J., Charles, B. G., and Yao, A. A. (1998). Effect of oral levodopa treatment on articulatory function in Parkinson's disease: preliminary results. Mot. Control. 2, 161–172. doi: 10.1123/mcj.2.2.161

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Caligiuri, M. P. (1987). Labial kinematics during speech in patients with parkinsonian rigidity. Brain 110, 1033–1044. doi: 10.1093/brain/110.4.1033

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Caligiuri, M. P., and Abbs, J. H. (1987). Response properties of the perioral reflex in Parkinson's disease. Exp. Neurol. 98, 563–572. doi: 10.1016/0014-4886(87)90265-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Camerino, I., Ferreira, J. O., Vonk, J. M., Kessels, R. P. C., De Leeuw, F.-E., Roelofs, A., et al. (2022). Systematic review and Meta-analyses of word production abilities in dysfunction of the basal ganglia: stroke, small vessel disease, Parkinson’s disease, and Huntington's disease. Neuropsychol. Rev. 34, 1–26. doi: 10.1007/s11065-022-09570-3

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Carbon, M., and Eidelberg, D. (2006). Functional imaging of sequence learning in Parkinson's disease. J. Neurol. Sci. 248, 72–77. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2006.05.005

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Carbon, M., Ghilardi, M. F., Feigin, A., Fukuda, M., Silvestri, G., Mentis, M. J., et al. (2003). Learning networks in health and Parkinson's disease: reproducibility and treatment effects. Hum. Brain Mapp. 19, 197–211. doi: 10.1002/hbm.10115

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cavallieri, F., Budriesi, C., Gessani, A., Contardi, S., Fioravanti, V., Menozzi, E., et al. (2021). Dopaminergic treatment effects on Dysarthric speech: acoustic analysis in a cohort of patients with advanced Parkinson's disease. Front. Neurol. 11:616062. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2020.616062

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, S. E., Kenney, M. K., Loucks, T. M., Poletto, C. J., and Ludlow, C. L. (2009). Common neural substrates support speech and non-speech vocal tract gestures. NeuroImage 47, 314–325. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.03.032

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, Y. W., and Watson, P. J. (2017). Speech production and sensory impairment in mild Parkinson's disease. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 141, 3030–3041. doi: 10.1121/1.4980138

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, Y., Zhu, G., Liu, D., Liu, Y., Yuan, T., Zhang, X., et al. (2020). Brain morphological changes in hypokinetic dysarthria of Parkinson's disease and use of machine learning to predict severity. CNS Neurosci. Ther. 26, 711–719. doi: 10.1111/cns.13304

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, X., Zhu, X., Wang, E. Q., Chen, L., Li, W., Chen, Z., et al. (2013). Sensorimotor control of vocal pitch production in Parkinson's disease. Brain Res. 1527, 99–107. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2013.06.030

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chiu, S. Y., Tsuboi, T., Hegland, K. W., Herndon, N. E., Shukla, A. W., Patterson, A., et al. (2020). Dysarthria and speech intelligibility following Parkinson’s disease Globus pallidus internus deep brain stimulation. J. Parkinsons Dis. 10, 1493–1502. doi: 10.3233/JPD-202246

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Connor, N. P., and Abbs, J. H. (1991). Task-dependent variations in parkinsonian motor impairments. Brain 114, 321–332,

Google Scholar

Conte, A., Khan, N., Defazio, G., Rothwell, J. C., and Berardelli, A. (2013). Pathophysiology of somatosensory abnormalities in Parkinson disease. Nat. Rev. Neurol. 9, 687–697. doi: 10.1038/nrneurol.2013.224

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cooper, I. S. (1959). Chemopallidectomy and Chemothalamectomy for Parkinsonism and dystonia. Proc. R. Soc. Med. 52, 47–60,

Google Scholar

Cowie, R., and Douglas-Cowie, E. (1992). Postlingually acquired deafness: speech deterioration and the wider consequences. New York: Mouton de Gruyter.

Google Scholar

Cushnie-Sparrow, D., Adams, S., Abeyesekera, A., Pieterman, M., Gilmore, G., and Jog, M. (2018). Voice quality severity and responsiveness to levodopa in Parkinson's disease. J. Commun. Disord. 76, 1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2018.07.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

D'alatri, L., Paludetti, G., Contarino, M. F., Galla, S., Marchese, M. R., and Bentivoglio, A. R. (2008). Effects of bilateral subthalamic nucleus stimulation and medication on parkinsonian speech impairment. J. Voice 22, 365–372. doi: 10.1016/j.jvoice.2006.10.010

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Darley, F. L., Aronson, A. E., and Brown, J. R. (1969a). Clusters of deviant speech dimensions in the dysarthrias. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 12, 462–496. doi: 10.1044/jshr.1203.462

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Darley, F. L., Aronson, A. E., and Brown, J. R. (1969b). Differential diagnostic patterns of dysarthria. J. Speech Hear. Res. 12, 246–269. doi: 10.1044/jshr.1202.246

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

De Almeida Marcelino, A. L., Horn, A., Krause, P., Kuhn, A. A., and Neumann, W. J. (2019). Subthalamic neuromodulation improves short-term motor learning in Parkinson's disease. Brain 142, 2198–2206. doi: 10.1093/brain/awz152

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

De Bie, R. M. A., Schuurman, P. R., Esselink, R. A. J., Bosch, D. A., and Speelman, J. D. (2002). Bilateral pallidotomy in Parkinson's disease: a retrospective study. Mov. Disord. 17, 533–538. doi: 10.1002/mds.10090

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

De Groote, E., De Keyser, K., Bockstael, A., Botteldooren, D., Santens, P., and De Letter, M. (2020). Central auditory processing in parkinsonian disorders: a systematic review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 113, 111–132. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.03.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

De Keyser, K., De Letter, M., De Groote, E., Santens, P., Talsma, D., Botteldooren, D., et al. (2019). Systematic audiological assessment of auditory functioning in patients with Parkinson’s disease. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 62, 4564–4577. doi: 10.1044/2019_JSLHR-H-19-0097

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

De Letter, M., Santens, P., De Bodt, M., Van Maele, G., Van Borsel, J., and Boon, P. (2007). The effect of levodopa on respiration and word intelligibility in people with advanced Parkinson's disease. Clin. Neurol. Neurosurg. 109, 495–500. doi: 10.1016/j.clineuro.2007.04.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

De Letter, M., Santens, P., and Van Borsel, J. (2003). The effects of levodopa on tongue strength and endurance in patients with Parkinson's disease. Acta Neurol. Belg. 103, 35–38

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

De Letter, M., Santens, P., and Van Borsel, J. (2005). The effects of levodopa on word intelligibility in Parkinson's disease. J. Commun. Disord. 38, 187–196. doi: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2004.09.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Delong, M. R. (1972). Activity of basal ganglia neurons during movement. Brain Res. 40, 127–135. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(72)90118-7

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Delong, M. R. (1990). Primate models of movement disorders of basal ganglia origin. Trends Neurosci. 13, 281–285. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(90)90110-V

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Delong, M. R., Crutcher, M. D., and Georgopoulos, A. P. (1985). Primate globus pallidus and subthalamic nucleus: functional organization. J. Neurophysiol. 53, 530–543. doi: 10.1152/jn.1985.53.2.530

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Desmurget, M., Grafton, S. T., Vindras, P., Grea, H., and Turner, R. S. (2004). The basal ganglia network mediates the planning of movement amplitude. Eur. J. Neurosci. 19, 2871–2880. doi: 10.1111/j.0953-816X.2004.03395.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Desmurget, M., and Turner, R. S. (2008). Testing basal ganglia motor functions through reversible inactivations in the posterior internal globus pallidus. J. Neurophysiol. 99, 1057–1076. doi: 10.1152/jn.01010.2007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Di Mauro, R., Di Lazzaro, G., Schirinzi, T., Martino, F., and Mercuri, N. (2017). Assessment of hearing impairment in Parkinson’s disease: implications for differential diagnosis and disease progression. J. Alzheimers Dis. Parkinsonism 7:396. doi: 10.4172/2161-0460.1000396

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Doyon, J., Bellec, P., Amsel, R., Penhune, V., Monchi, O., Carrier, J., et al. (2009). Contributions of the basal ganglia and functionally related brain structures to motor learning. Behav. Brain Res. 199, 61–75. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2008.11.012

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dressler, D., Altenmueller, E., Bhidayasiri, R., Bohlega, S., Chana, P., Chung, T. M., et al. (2016). Strategies for treatment of dystonia. J. Neural Transm. (Vienna) 123, 251–258. doi: 10.1007/s00702-015-1453-x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dromey, C., and Bjarnason, S. (2011). A preliminary report on disordered speech with deep brain stimulation in individuals with Parkinson's disease. Parkinsons Dis. 2011:796205, doi: 10.4061/2F2011/2F796205

Google Scholar

Dromey, C., Kumar, R., Lang, A. E., and Lozano, A. M. (2000). An investigation of the effects of subthalamic nucleus stimulation on acoustic measures of voice. Mov. Disord. 15, 1132–1138. doi: 10.1002/1531-8257(200011)15:6<1132::AID-MDS1011>3.0.CO;2-O

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Duffy, J. (2019). Motor speech disorders: Substrates, differential diagnosis, and management. Maryland Heights: Elsevier.

Google Scholar

Dworkin, J. P., and Aronson, A. E. (1986). Tongue strength and alternate motion rates in normal and dysarthric subjects. J. Commun. Disord. 19, 115–132. doi: 10.1016/0021-9924(86)90015-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fabbri, M., Guimaraes, I., Cardoso, R., Coelho, M., Guedes, L. C., Rosa, M. M., et al. (2017). Speech and voice response to a levodopa challenge in late-stage Parkinson’s disease. Front. Neurol. 8:432. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2017.00432

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Favre, J., Burchiel, K. J., Taha, J. M., and Hammerstad, J. (2000). Outcome of unilateral and bilateral pallidotomy for Parkinson's disease: patient assessment. Neurosurgery 46, 344–355; discussion 353-5. doi: 10.1097/00006123-200002000-00017

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Feigin, V. L., Vos, T., Nichols, E., Owolabi, M. O., Carroll, W. M., Dichgans, M., et al. (2020). The global burden of neurological disorders: translating evidence into policy. Lancet Neurol. 19, 255–265. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(19)30411-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Filion, M., Tremblay, L., and Bedard, P. J. (1988). Abnormal influences of passive limb movement on the activity of globus pallidus neurons in parkinsonian monkeys. Brain Res. 444, 165–176. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(88)90924-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Finger, M. E., Siddiqui, M. S., Morris, A. K., Ruckart, K. W., Wright, S. C., Haq, I. U., et al. (2020). Auditory-perceptual evaluation of deep brain stimulation on voice and speech in patients with dystonia. J. Voice 34, 636–644. doi: 10.1016/j.jvoice.2019.02.010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischer, E., and Goberman, A. M. (2010). Voice onset time in Parkinson disease. J. Commun. Disord. 43, 21–34. doi: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2009.07.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Flasskamp, A., Kotz, S. A., Schlegel, U., and Skodda, S. (2012). Acceleration of syllable repetition in Parkinson's disease is more prominent in the left-side dominant patients. Parkinsonism Relat. Disord. 18, 343–347. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2011.11.021

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Folmer, R. L., Vachhani, J. J., Theodoroff, S. M., Ellinger, R., and Riggins, A. (2017). Auditory processing abilities of Parkinson's disease patients. Biomed. Res. Int. 2017:2618587. doi: 10.1155/2017/2618587

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fukuda, M., Ghilardi, M. F., Carbon, M., Dhawan, V., Ma, Y., Feigin, A., et al. (2002). Pallidal stimulation for Parkinsonism: improved brain activation during sequence learning. Ann. Neurol. 52, 144–152. doi: 10.1002/ana.10261

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gallena, S., Smith, P. J., Zeffiro, T., and Ludlow, C. L. (2001). Effects of levodopa on laryngeal muscle activity for voice onset and offset in Parkinson disease. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 44, 1284–1299. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2001/100)

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Georgopoulos, A. P., Delong, M. R., and Crutcher, M. D. (1983). Relations between parameters of step-tracking movements and single cell discharge in the globus pallidus and subthalamic nucleus of the behaving monkey. J. Neurosci. 3, 1586–1598. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.03-08-01586.1983

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gessani, A., Cavallieri, F., Fioravanti, V., Campanini, I., Merlo, A., Di Rauso, G., et al. (2023). Long-term effects of subthalamic nucleus deep brain stimulation on speech in Parkinson's disease. Sci. Rep. 13:11462. doi: 10.1038/s41598-023-38555-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ghika, J., Ghika-Schmid, F., Fankhauser, H., Assal, G., Vingerhoets, F., Albanese, A., et al. (1999). Bilateral contemporaneous posteroventral pallidotomy for the treatment of Parkinson's disease: neuropsychological and neurological side effects. Report of four cases and review of the literature. J. Neurosurg. 91, 313–321

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Goberman, A. M., Coelho, C. A., and Robb, M. P. (2005). Prosodic characteristics of parkinsonian speech: the effect of levodopa-based medication. J. Med. Speech Lang. Pathol. 13, 51–69. doi: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2004.10.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Graber, S., Hertrich, I., Daum, I., Spieker, S., and Ackermann, H. (2002). Speech perception deficits in Parkinson's disease: underestimation of time intervals compromises identification of durational phonetic contrasts. Brain Lang. 82, 65–74. doi: 10.1016/S0093-934X(02)00002-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Green, J., Mcdonald, W. M., Vitek, J. L., Haber, M., Barnhart, H., Bakay, R. A., et al. (2002). Neuropsychological and psychiatric sequelae of pallidotomy for Pd: clinical trial findings. Neurology 58, 858–865. doi: 10.1212/WNL.58.6.858

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gronholm, E. O., Roll, M. C., Horne, M. A., Sundgren, P. C., and Lindgren, A. G. (2016). Predominance of caudate nucleus lesions in acute ischaemic stroke patients with impairment in language and speech. Eur. J. Neurol. 23, 148–153. doi: 10.1111/ene.12822

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Guenther, F. H. (2016). Neural control of speech. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press, 1–410.

Google Scholar

Guenther, F. H., Ghosh, S. S., and Tourville, J. A. (2006). Neural modeling and imaging of the cortical interactions underlying syllable production. Brain Lang. 96, 280–301. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2005.06.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Guiot, G., and Brion, S. (1953). Treatment of abnormal movement by pallidal coagulation. Rev. Neurol. (Paris) 89, 578–580,

Google Scholar

Guridi, J., and Lozano, A. M. (1997). A brief history of pallidotomy. Neurosurgery 41, 1169–1183; discussion 1180-3. doi: 10.1097/00006123-199711000-00029

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Haesler, S., Rochefort, C., Georgi, B., Licznerski, P., Osten, P., and Scharff, C. (2007). Incomplete and inaccurate vocal imitation after knockdown of FoxP2 in songbird basal ganglia nucleus area X. PLoS Biol. 5:e321. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0050321

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Halperin, O., Israeli-Korn, S., Yakubovich, S., Hassin-Baer, S., and Zaidel, A. (2021). Self-motion perception in Parkinson's disease. Eur. J. Neurosci. 53, 2376–2387. doi: 10.1111/ejn.14716

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Halsband, U., Matsuzaka, Y., and Tanji, J. (1994). Neuronal activity in the primate supplementary, pre-supplementary and premotor cortex during externally and internally instructed sequential movements. Neurosci. Res. 20, 149–155. doi: 10.1016/0168-0102(94)90032-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hammer, M. J. (2009). Design of a New Somatosensory Stimulus Delivery Device for measuring laryngeal Mechanosensory detection thresholds in humans. IEEE Trans. Biomed. Eng. 56, 1154–1159. doi: 10.1109/TBME.2008.2007968

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hammer, M. J., and Barlow, S. M. (2010). Laryngeal somatosensory deficits in Parkinson's disease: implications for speech respiratory and phonatory control. Exp. Brain Res. 201, 401–409. doi: 10.1007/s00221-009-2048-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hammer, M. J., Murphy, C. A., and Abrams, T. M. (2013). Airway somatosensory deficits and dysphagia in Parkinson’s disease. J. Parkinsons Dis. 3, 39–44. doi: 10.3233/JPD-120161

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hanganu, A., Bedetti, C., Jubault, T., Gagnon, J. F., Mejia-Constain, B., Degroot, C., et al. (2013). Mild cognitive impairment in patients with Parkinson's disease is associated with increased cortical degeneration. Mov. Disord. 28, 1360–1369. doi: 10.1002/mds.25541

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hanson, D. G., Gerratt, B. R., and Ward, P. H. (1984). Cinegraphic observations of laryngeal function in Parkinson's disease. Laryngoscope 94, 348–353. doi: 10.1288/00005537-198403000-00011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Harel, B., Cannizzaro, M., and Snyder, P. J. (2004). Variability in fundamental frequency during speech in prodromal and incipient Parkinson's disease: a longitudinal case study. Brain Cogn. 56, 24–29. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2004.05.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hegland, K. W., Troche, M., and Brandimore, A. (2019). Relationship between respiratory sensory perception, speech, and swallow in Parkinson’s disease. Mov. Disord. Clin. Pract. 6, 243–249. doi: 10.1002/mdc3.12732

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Higuchi, Y., and Iacono, R. P. (2003). Surgical complications in patients with Parkinson's disease after posteroventral pallidotomy. Neurosurgery 52, 558–571; discussion 568-571. doi: 10.1227/01.NEU.0000047817.60776.5C

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hikosaka, O., Nakamura, K., and Nakahara, H. (2006). Basal ganglia orient eyes to reward. J. Neurophysiol. 95, 567–584. doi: 10.1152/jn.00458.2005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ho, A. K., Bradshaw, J. L., and Iansek, R. (2008). For better or worse: the effect of levodopa on speech in Parkinson's disease. Mov. Disord. 23, 574–580. doi: 10.1002/mds.21899

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ho, A. K., Iansek, R., Marigliani, C., Bradshaw, J. L., and Gates, S. (1998). Speech impairment in a large sample of patients with Parkinson's disease. Behav. Neurol. 11, 131–137

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Huang, X., Chen, X., Yan, N., Jones, J. A., Wang, E. Q., Chen, L., et al. (2016). The impact of parkinson's disease on the cortical mechanisms that support auditory-motor integration for voice control. Hum. Brain Mapp. 37, 4248–4261. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23306

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, X., Fan, H., Li, J., Jones, J. A., Wang, E. Q., Chen, L., et al. (2019). External cueing facilitates auditory-motor integration for speech control in individuals with Parkinson's disease. Neurobiol. Aging 76, 96–105. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2018.12.020

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hunker, C. J., Abbs, J. H., and Barlow, S. M. (1982). The relationship between parkinsonian rigidity and hypokinesia in the orofacial system: a quantitative analysis. Neurology 32, 749–754. doi: 10.1212/WNL.32.7.749

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hurtig, H. I., Trojanowski, J. Q., Galvin, J., Ewbank, D., Schmidt, M. L., Lee, V. M., et al. (2000). Alpha-synuclein cortical Lewy bodies correlate with dementia in Parkinson's disease. Neurology 54, 1916–1921. doi: 10.1212/WNL.54.10.1916

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Iacono, R. P., Lonser, R. R., and Kuniyoshi, S. (1995). Unilateral versus bilateral simultaneous posteroventral pallidotomy in subgroups of patients with Parkinson's disease. Stereotact. Funct. Neurosurg. 65, 6–10. doi: 10.1159/000098889

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ilinsky, I., Horn, A., Paul-Gilloteaux, P., Gressens, P., Verney, C., and Kultas-Ilinsky, K. (2018). Human motor thalamus reconstructed in 3D from continuous sagittal sections with identified subcortical afferent territories. eNeuro 5:ENEURO.0060-18.2018. doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0060-18.2018

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Im, H., Adams, S., Abeyesekera, A., Pieterman, M., Gilmore, G., and Jog, M. (2019). Effect of levodopa on speech dysfluency in Parkinson’s disease. Mov. Disord. Clin. Pract. 6, 150–154. doi: 10.1002/mdc3.12714

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jin, X., and Costa, R. M. (2015). Shaping action sequences in basal ganglia circuits. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 33, 188–196. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2015.06.011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Johari, K., Kelley, R. M., Tjaden, K., Patterson, C. G., Rohl, A. H., Berger, J. I., et al. (2023). Human subthalamic nucleus neurons differentially encode speech and limb movement. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 17:962909. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2023.962909

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, C. R. G., and JAHANSHAHI, M. (2014). Motor and perceptual timing in Parkinson’s disease. Adv. Exp. Med. Biol. 829, 265–290. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4939-1782-2_14

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jorge, A., Dastolfo-Hromack, C., Lipski, W. J., Kratter, I. H., Smith, L. J., Gartner-Schmidt, J. L., et al. (2020). Anterior sensorimotor subthalamic nucleus stimulation is associated with improved voice function. Neurosurgery 87, 788–795. doi: 10.1093/neuros/nyaa024

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaipa, R., Jones, R. D., and Robb, M. P. (2016). Are individuals with Parkinson's disease capable of speech-motor learning? A preliminary evaluation. Parkinsonism Relat. Disord. 28, 141–145. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2016.05.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Karlsson, F., Olofsson, K., Blomstedt, P., Linder, J., and Van Doorn, J. (2013). Pitch variability in patients with Parkinson's disease: effects of deep brain stimulation of caudal zona incerta and subthalamic nucleus. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 56, 150–158. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2012/11-0333)

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Katzenschlager, R., and Lees, A. J. (2002). Treatment of Parkinson's disease: levodopa as the first choice. J. Neurol. 249:II19-24. doi: 10.1007/s00415-002-1204-4

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Klobusiakova, P., Mekyska, J., Brabenec, L., Galaz, Z., Zvoncak, V., Mucha, J., et al. (2021). Articulatory network reorganization in Parkinson's disease as assessed by multimodal MRI and acoustic measures. Parkinsonism Relat. Disord. 84, 122–128. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2021.02.012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Klostermann, F., Ehlen, F., Vesper, J., Nubel, K., Gross, M., Marzinzik, F., et al. (2008). Effects of subthalamic deep brain stimulation on dysarthrophonia in Parkinson's disease. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 79, 522–529. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2007.123323

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kompoliti, K., Wang, Q. E., Goetz, C. G., Leurgans, S., and Raman, R. (2000). Effects of central dopaminergic stimulation by apomorphine on speech in Parkinson's disease. Neurology 54, 458–462. doi: 10.1212/WNL.54.2.458

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Konczak, J., Corcos, D. M., Horak, F., Poizner, H., Shapiro, M., Tuite, P., et al. (2009). Proprioception and motor control in Parkinson's disease. J. Mot. Behav. 41, 543–552. doi: 10.3200/35-09-002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kopf, L. M., Rohl, A. H. G., Nagao, T., Bryant, K. N. T., Johari, K., Tjaden, K., et al. (2022). Voice handicap index in Parkinson’s patients: subthalamic versus globus pallidus deep brain stimulation. J. Clin. Neurosci. 98, 83–88. doi: 10.1016/j.jocn.2022.01.029

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kosubek-Langer, J., and Scharff, C. (2020). Dynamic FoxP2 levels in male zebra finches are linked to morphology of adult-born area X medium spiny neurons. Sci. Rep. 10:4787. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-61740-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kovari, E., Gold, G., Herrmann, F. R., Canuto, A., Hof, P. R., Bouras, C., et al. (2003). Lewy body densities in the entorhinal and anterior cingulate cortex predict cognitive deficits in Parkinson's disease. Acta Neuropathol. 106, 83–88. doi: 10.1007/s00401-003-0705-2

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Krishna, V., Fishman, P. S., Eisenberg, H. M., Kaplitt, M., Baltuch, G., Chang, J. W., et al. (2023). Trial of Globus pallidus focused ultrasound ablation in Parkinson’s disease. N. Engl. J. Med. 388, 683–693. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa2202721

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Laansma, M. A., Bright, J. K., Al-Bachari, S., Anderson, T. J., Ard, T., Assogna, F., et al. (2021). International multicenter analysis of brain structure across clinical stages of Parkinson’s disease. Mov. Disord. 36, 2583–2594. doi: 10.1002/mds.28706

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lai, C. S., Gerrelli, D., Monaco, A. P., Fisher, S. E., and Copp, A. J. (2003). Foxp2 expression during brain development coincides with adult sites of pathology in a severe speech and language disorder. Brain 126, 2455–2462. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg247

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Laitinen, L. V., Bergenheim, A. T., and Hariz, M. I. (1992). Leksell's posteroventral pallidotomy in the treatment of Parkinson's disease. J. Neurosurg. 76, 53–61. doi: 10.3171/jns.1992.76.1.0053

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lane, H., Wozniak, J., Matthies, M., Svirsky, M., Perkell, J., O'connell, M., et al. (1997). Changes in sound pressure and fundamental frequency contours following changes in hearing status. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 101, 2244–2252,

Google Scholar

Lechien, J. R., Blecic, S., Ghosez, Y., Huet, K., Harmegnies, B., and Saussez, S. (2019). Voice quality and orofacial strength as outcome of levodopa effectiveness in patients with early idiopathic Parkinson Disease: a preliminary report. J. Voice 33, 716–720. doi: 10.1016/j.jvoice.2018.04.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, M. S., Lee, M. J., Conte, A., and Berardelli, A. (2018). Abnormal somatosensory temporal discrimination in Parkinson's disease: pathophysiological correlates and role in motor control deficits. Clin. Neurophysiol. 129, 442–447. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2017.11.022

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Leuk, J. S. P., Low, L. L. N., and Teo, W. P. (2020). An overview of acoustic-based interventions to improve motor symptoms in Parkinson’s disease. Front. Aging Neurosci. 12:243. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2020.00243

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Levy, R., Hutchison, W. D., Lozano, A. M., and Dostrovsky, J. O. (2000). High-frequency synchronization of neuronal activity in the subthalamic nucleus of parkinsonian patients with limb tremor. J. Neurosci. 20, 7766–7775. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-20-07766.2000

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, Y., Tan, M., Fan, H., Wang, E. Q., Chen, L., Li, J., et al. (2021). Neurobehavioral effects of Lsvt((R)) loud on auditory-vocal integration in Parkinson’s disease: a preliminary study. Front. Neurosci. 15:624801. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2021.624801

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lima, C. F., Garrett, C., and Castro, S. L. (2013). Not all sounds sound the same: Parkinson's disease affects differently emotion processing in music and in speech prosody. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 35, 373–392. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2013.776518

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Liotti, M., Ramig, L. O., Vogel, D., New, P., Cook, C. I., Ingham, R. J., et al. (2003). Hypophonia in Parkinson's disease: neural correlates of voice treatment revealed by pet. Neurology 60, 432–440. doi: 10.1212/WNL.60.3.432

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lipski, W. J., Alhourani, A., Pirnia, T., Jones, P. W., Dastolfo-Hromack, C., Helou, L. B., et al. (2018). Subthalamic nucleus neurons differentially encode early and late aspects of speech production. J. Neurosci. 38, 5620–5631. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3480-17.2018

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lipski, W. J., Bush, A., Chrabaszcz, A., Crammond, D. J., Fiez, J. A., Turner, R. S., et al. (2023). Subthalamic nucleus neuronal firing encodes sequence and phonetic information during speech. BioRxiv preprint. doi: 10.1101/2023.12.11.569290

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Little, S., Tripoliti, E., Beudel, M., Pogosyan, A., Cagnan, H., Herz, D., et al. (2016). Adaptive deep brain stimulation for Parkinson's disease demonstrates reduced speech side effects compared to conventional stimulation in the acute setting. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 87, 1388–1389. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2016-313518

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, H., Wang, E. Q., Metman, L. V., and Larson, C. R. (2012). Vocal responses to perturbations in voice auditory feedback in individuals with Parkinson's disease. PLoS One 7:e33629. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0033629

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lloyd, A. J. (1999). Comprehension of prosody in Parkinson's disease. Cortex 35, 389–402. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70807-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Logemann, J. A., Fisher, H. B., Boshes, B., and Blonsky, E. R. (1978). Frequency and cooccurrence of vocal tract dysfunctions in the speech of a large sample of Parkinson patients. J. Speech Hear. Disord. 43, 47–57. doi: 10.1044/jshd.4301.47

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lowit, A., Brendel, B., Dobinson, C., and Howell, P. (2006). An investigation into the influences of age, pathology and cognition on speech production. J. Med. Speech Lang. Pathol. 14, 253–262

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Ludlow, C. L., Connor, N. P., and Bassich, C. J. (1987). Speech timing in Parkinson's and Huntington's disease. Brain Lang. 32, 195–214. doi: 10.1016/0093-934X(87)90124-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lundgren, S., Saeys, T., Karlsson, F., Olofsson, K., Blomstedt, P., Linder, J., et al. (2011). Deep brain stimulation of caudal zona incerta and subthalamic nucleus in patients with Parkinson's disease: effects on voice intensity. Parkinsons Dis. 2011:658956. doi: 10.4061/2011/658956

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Magnusson, J. L., and Leventhal, D. K. (2021). Revisiting the "paradox of stereotaxic surgery": insights into basal ganglia-thalamic interactions. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 15:725876. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2021.725876

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Manes, J. L., Kurani, A. S., Herschel, E., Roberts, A. C., Tjaden, K., Parrish, T., et al. (2023). Premotor cortex is hypoactive during sustained vowel production in individuals with Parkinson's disease and hypophonia. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 17:1250114. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2023.1250114

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Manes, J. L., Parkinson, A. L., Larson, C. R., Greenlee, J. D., Eickhoff, S. B., Corcos, D. M., et al. (2014). Connectivity of the subthalamic nucleus and globus pallidus pars interna to regions within the speech network: a meta-analytic connectivity study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 35, 3499–3516. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22417

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Manes, J. L., Tjaden, K., Parrish, T., Simuni, T., Roberts, A., Greenlee, J. D., et al. (2018). Altered resting-state functional connectivity of the putamen and internal globus pallidus is related to speech impairment in Parkinson's disease. Brain Behav. 8:e01073. doi: 10.1002/brb3.1073

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Marsden, C. D., and Obeso, J. A. (1994). The functions of the basal ganglia and the paradox of stereotaxic surgery in Parkinson's disease. Brain 117, 877–897. doi: 10.1093/brain/117.4.877

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Martel-Sauvageau, V., and Tjaden, K. (2017). Vocalic transitions as markers of speech acoustic changes with STN-DBS in Parkinson’s disease. J. Commun. Disord. 70, 1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2017.10.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mattila, P. M., Rinne, J. O., Helenius, H., Dickson, D. W., and Roytta, M. (2000). Alpha-synuclein-immunoreactive cortical Lewy bodies are associated with cognitive impairment in Parkinson's disease. Acta Neuropathol. 100, 285–290. doi: 10.1007/s004019900168

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mazzoni, P., Hristova, A., and Krakauer, J. W. (2007). Why don't we move faster? Parkinson's disease, movement vigor, and implicit motivation. J. Neurosci. 27, 7105–7116. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0264-07.2007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mclennan, J. E., Nakano, K., Tyler, H. R., and Schwab, R. S. (1972). Micrographia in Parkinson's disease. J. Neurol. Sci. 15, 141–152. doi: 10.1016/0022-510X(72)90002-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mefferd, A., and Bissmeyer, M. (2016). Bite block effects on vowel acoustics in talkers with amyotrophic lateral sclerosis and Parkinson’s disease. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 140:3442. doi: 10.1121/1.4971098

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Meissner, S. N., Sudmeyer, M., Keitel, A., Pollok, B., and Bellebaum, C. (2016). Facilitating effects of deep brain stimulation on feedback learning in Parkinson's disease. Behav. Brain Res. 313, 88–96. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2016.06.062

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, N., Noble, E., Jones, D., and Burn, D. (2006). Life with communication changes in Parkinson's disease. Age Ageing 35, 235–239. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afj053

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mink, J. W. (1996). The basal ganglia: focused selection and inhibition of competing motor programs. Prog. Neurobiol. 50, 381–425. doi: 10.1016/S0301-0082(96)00042-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mink, J. W., and Thach, W. T. (1991). Basal ganglia motor control. I. Nonexclusive relation of pallidal discharge to five movement modes. J. Neurophysiol. 65, 273–300

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Mitchell, S. J., Richardson, R. T., Baker, F. H., and Delong, M. R. (1987). The primate globus pallidus: neuronal activity related to direction of movement. Exp. Brain Res. 68, 491–505,

Google Scholar

Miyachi, S., Hikosaka, O., Miyashita, K., Karadi, Z., and Rand, M. K. (1997). Differential roles of monkey striatum in learning of sequential hand movement. Exp. Brain Res. 115, 1–5. doi: 10.1007/PL00005669

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mollaei, F., Shiller, D. M., Baum, S. R., and Gracco, V. L. (2016). Sensorimotor control of vocal pitch and formant frequencies in Parkinson's disease. Brain Res. 1646, 269–277. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2016.06.013

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mollaei, F., Shiller, D. M., Baum, S. R., and Gracco, V. L. (2019). The relationship between speech perceptual discrimination and speech production in Parkinson’s disease. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 62, 4256–4268. doi: 10.1044/2019_JSLHR-S-18-0425

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mollaei, F., Shiller, D. M., and Gracco, V. L. (2013). Sensorimotor adaptation of speech in Parkinson's disease. Mov. Disord. 28, 1668–1674. doi: 10.1002/mds.25588

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Moreau, C., Ozsancak, C., Blatt, J. L., Derambure, P., Destee, A., and Defebvre, L. (2007). Oral festination in Parkinson's disease: biomechanical analysis and correlation with festination and freezing of gait. Mov. Disord. 22, 1503–1506. doi: 10.1002/mds.21549

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Moreau, C., Pennel-Ployart, O., Pinto, S., Plachez, A., Annic, A., Viallet, F., et al. (2011). Modulation of dysarthropneumophonia by low-frequency Stn Dbs in advanced Parkinson's disease. Mov. Disord. 26, 659–663. doi: 10.1002/mds.23538

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mu, L., Chen, J., Sobotka, S., Nyirenda, T., Benson, B., Gupta, F., et al. (2015). Alpha-Synuclein pathology in sensory nerve terminals of the upper aerodigestive tract of Parkinson’s disease patients. Dysphagia 30, 404–417. doi: 10.1007/s00455-015-9612-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mu, L., Sobotka, S., Chen, J., Su, H., Sanders, I., Adler, C. H., et al. (2013a). Alpha-synuclein pathology and axonal degeneration of the peripheral motor nerves innervating pharyngeal muscles in Parkinson disease. J. Neuropathol. Exp. Neurol. 72, 119–129. doi: 10.1097/NEN.0b013e3182801cde

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mu, L., Sobotka, S., Chen, J., Su, H., Sanders, I., Nyirenda, T., et al. (2013b). Parkinson disease affects peripheral sensory nerves in the pharynx. J. Neuropathol. Exp. Neurol. 72, 614–623. doi: 10.1097/NEN.0b013e3182965886

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Muehlberg, C., Fricke, C., Wegscheider, M., Wawrzyniak, M., Tzvi, E., Winkler, D., et al. (2023). Motor learning is independent of effects of subthalamic deep brain stimulation on motor execution. Brain Commun. 5:fcad070. doi: 10.1093/braincomms/fcad070

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mure, H., Tang, C. C., Argyelan, M., Ghilardi, M. F., Kaplitt, M. G., Dhawan, V., et al. (2012). Improved sequence learning with subthalamic nucleus deep brain stimulation: evidence for treatment-specific network modulation. J. Neurosci. 32, 2804–2813. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4331-11.2012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mushiake, H., Inase, M., and Tanji, J. (1991). Neuronal activity in the primate premotor, supplementary, and precentral motor cortex during visually guided and internally determined sequential movements. J. Neurophysiol. 66, 705–718. doi: 10.1152/jn.1991.66.3.705

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nambu, A. (2004). A new dynamic model of the cortico-basal ganglia loop. Prog. Brain Res. 143, 461–466. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(03)43043-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nambu, A. (2011). Somatotopic organization of the primate basal ganglia. Front. Neuroanat. 5:26. doi: 10.3389/fnana.2011.00026

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nambu, A., Tokuno, H., and Takada, M. (2002). Functional significance of the cortico-subthalamo-pallidal 'hyperdirect' pathway. Neurosci. Res. 43, 111–117. doi: 10.1016/S0168-0102(02)00027-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Narabayashi, H., Okuma, T., and Shikiba, S. (1956). Procaine oil blocking of the globus pallidus. A.M.A. Arch. Neurol. Psychiatry 75, 36–48. doi: 10.1001/archneurpsyc.1956.02330190052003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Narayana, S., Parsons, M. B., Zhang, W., Franklin, C., Schiller, K., Choudhri, A. F., et al. (2020). Mapping typical and hypokinetic dysarthric speech production network using a connected speech paradigm in functional Mri. Neuroimage Clin. 27:102285. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2020.102285

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nelson, A. J., Hoque, T., Gunraj, C., and Chen, R. (2018). Altered somatosensory processing in Parkinson's disease and modulation by dopaminergic medications. Parkinsonism Relat. Disord. 53, 76–81. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2018.05.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Netsell, R., Daniel, B., and Celesia, G. G. (1975). Acceleration and weakness in parkinsonian dysarthria. J. Speech Hear. Disord. 40, 170–178. doi: 10.1044/jshd.4002.170

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Neudorfer, C., Kultas-Ilinsky, K., Ilinsky, I., Paschen, S., Helmers, A.-K., Cosgrove, G. R., et al. (2024). The role of the motor thalamus in deep brain stimulation for essential tremor. Neurotherapeutics 21:e00313. doi: 10.1016/j.neurot.2023.e00313

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Neumann, W.-J., Steiner, L. A., and Milosevic, L. (2023). Neurophysiological mechanisms of deep brain stimulation across spatiotemporal resolutions. Brain 146, 4456–4468. doi: 10.1093/brain/awad239

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Obeso, J. A., Jahanshahi, M., Alvarez, L., Macias, R., Pedroso, I., Wilkinson, L., et al. (2009). What can man do without basal ganglia motor output? The effect of combined unilateral subthalamotomy and pallidotomy in a patient with Parkinson's disease. Exp. Neurol. 220, 283–292. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2009.08.030

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Obrador, S. (1957). A simplified neurosurgical technique for approaching and damaging the region of the globus pallidus in Parkinson's disease. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 20, 47–49. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.20.1.47

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pah, N. D., Motin, M. A., Kempster, P., and Kumar, D. K. (2021). Detecting effect of levodopa in Parkinson's disease patients using sustained phonemes. IEEE J. Transl. Eng. Health Med. 9:4900409,

Google Scholar

Parkin, S. G., Gregory, R. P., Scott, R., Bain, P., Silburn, P., Hall, B., et al. (2002). Unilateral and bilateral pallidotomy for idiopathic Parkinson's disease: a case series of 115 patients. Mov. Disord. 17, 682–692. doi: 10.1002/mds.10186

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Parkinson, A. L., Flagmeier, S. G., Manes, J. L., Larson, C. R., Rogers, B., and Robin, D. A. (2012). Understanding the neural mechanisms involved in sensory control of voice production. NeuroImage 61, 314–322. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.02.068

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pasupathy, A., and Miller, E. K. (2005). Different time courses of learning-related activity in the prefrontal cortex and striatum. Nature 433, 873–876. doi: 10.1038/nature03287

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pell, M. D., Cheang, H. S., and Leonard, C. L. (2006). The impact of Parkinson's disease on vocal-prosodic communication from the perspective of listeners. Brain Lang. 97, 123–134. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2005.08.010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pellat, J., Gentil, M., Chevalier, C., Vila, A., Pollak, P., and Perret, J. (1983). Electromyographic study of 4 lip muscles and acoustic findings in parkinsonian dysarthria. Rev. Neurol. (Paris) 139, 205–213,

Google Scholar

Perkell, J. S., Lane, H., Denny, M., Matthies, M. L., Tiede, M., Zandipour, M., et al. (2007). Time course of speech changes in response to unanticipated short-term changes in hearing state. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 121, 2296–2311. doi: 10.1121/1.2642349

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Perkell, J., Lane, H., Svirsky, M., and Webster, J. (1992). Speech of cochlear implant patients: a longitudinal study of vowel production. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 91, 2961–2978. doi: 10.1121/1.402932

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Petersen, M. V., Mlaker, J., Haber, S. N., Parent, M., Smith, Y., Strick, P. L., et al. (2019). Holographic reconstruction of axonal pathways in the human brain. Neuron 1043, 1056–1064. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2019.09.030

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pickett, E. R., Kuniholm, E., Protopapas, A., Friedman, J., and Lieberman, P. (1998). Selective speech motor, syntax and cognitive deficits associated with bilateral damage to the putamen and the head of the caudate nucleus: a case study. Neuropsychologia 36, 173–188. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(97)00065-1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pillon, B., Ardouin, C., Damier, P., Krack, P., Houeto, J. L., Klinger, H., et al. (2000). Neuropsychological changes between "off" and "on" Stn or Gpi stimulation in Parkinson's disease. Neurology 55, 411–418. doi: 10.1212/WNL.55.3.411

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pina-Fuentes, D., Van Dijk, J. M. C., Van Zijl, J. C., Moes, H. R., Van Laar, T., Oterdoom, D. L. M., et al. (2020). Acute effects of adaptive deep brain stimulation in Parkinson's disease. Brain Stimul. 13, 1507–1516. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2020.07.016

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pinto, S., Mancini, L., Jahanshahi, M., Thornton, J. S., Tripoliti, E., Yousry, T. A., et al. (2011). Functional magnetic resonance imaging exploration of combined hand and speech movements in Parkinson's disease. Mov. Disord. 26, 2212–2219. doi: 10.1002/mds.23799

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pinto, S., Thobois, S., Costes, N., Le Bars, D., Benabid, A. L., Broussolle, E., et al. (2004). Subthalamic nucleus stimulation and dysarthria in Parkinson's disease: a pet study. Brain 127, 602–615. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh074

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Piron, C., Kase, D., Topalidou, M., Goillandeau, M., Orignac, H., N'guyen, T. H., et al. (2016). The globus pallidus pars interna in goal-oriented and routine behaviors: resolving a long-standing paradox. Mov. Disord. 31, 1146–1154. doi: 10.1002/mds.26542

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pisani, V., Sisto, R., Moleti, A., Di Mauro, R., Pisani, A., Brusa, L., et al. (2015). An investigation of hearing impairment in de-novo Parkinson's disease patients: a preliminary study. Parkinsonism Relat. Disord. 21, 987–991. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2015.06.007

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Plowman-Prine, E. K., Okun, M. S., Sapienza, C. M., Shrivastav, R., Fernandez, H. H., Foote, K. D., et al. (2009). Perceptual characteristics of parkinsonian speech: a comparison of the pharmacological effects of levodopa across speech and non-speech motor systems. NeuroRehabilitation 24, 131–144. doi: 10.3233/NRE-2009-0462

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramig, L. O., Fox, C., and Sapir, S. (2008). Speech treatment for Parkinson's disease. Expert. Rev. Neurother. 8, 297–309. doi: 10.1586/14737175.8.2.297

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramig, L., Fox, C., and Sapir, S. (2011). “Speech and voice disorders in Parkinson's disease” in Parkinson's disease: non-motor and non-dopaminergic features. eds. W. C. Olanow, F. Stocchi, and A. E. Lang. 1st ed (Hoboken, NJ: Blackwell Publishing).

Google Scholar

Ravizza, S. M. (2003). Dissociating the performance of cortical and subcortical patients on phonemic tasks. Brain Cogn. 53, 301–310. doi: 10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00131-3

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rektorova, I., Barrett, J., Mikl, M., Rektor, I., and Paus, T. (2007). Functional abnormalities in the primary orofacial sensorimotor cortex during speech in Parkinson's disease. Mov. Disord. 22, 2043–2051. doi: 10.1002/mds.21548

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Robertson, L. T., and Hammerstad, J. P. (1996). Jaw movement dysfunction related to Parkinson's disease and partially modified by levodopa. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 60, 41–50. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.60.1.41

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosenbaum, R., Zimnik, A., Zheng, F., Turner, R. S., Alzheimer, C., Doiron, B., et al. (2014). Axonal and synaptic failure suppress the transfer of firing rate oscillations, synchrony and information during high frequency deep brain stimulation. Neurobiol. Dis. 62, 86–99. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2013.09.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rusz, J., Cmejla, R., Ruzickova, H., and Ruzicka, E. (2011). Quantitative acoustic measurements for characterization of speech and voice disorders in early untreated Parkinson's disease. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 129, 350–367. doi: 10.1121/1.3514381

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rusz, J., Hlavnicka, J., Cmejla, R., and Ruzicka, E. (2015). Automatic evaluation of speech rhythm instability and acceleration in Dysarthrias associated with basal ganglia dysfunction. Front. Bioeng. Biotechnol. 3:104. doi: 10.1007/s00415-015-7671-1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rusz, J., Tykalova, T., Novotny, M., Zogala, D., Sonka, K., Ruzicka, E., et al. (2021). Defining speech subtypes in De novo Parkinson Disease: response to long-term levodopa therapy. Neurology 97, e2124–e2135. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000012878

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sadagopan, N., and Huber, J. E. (2007). Effects of loudness cues on respiration in individuals with Parkinson's disease. Mov. Dis. 22, 651–659. doi: 10.1002/mds.21375

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sage, J. R., Anagnostaras, S. G., Mitchell, S., Bronstein, J. M., De Salles, A., Masterman, D., et al. (2003). Analysis of probabilistic classification learning in patients with Parkinson's disease before and after pallidotomy surgery. Learn. Mem. 10, 226–236. doi: 10.1101/lm.45903

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sailer, A., Cunic, D. I., Paradiso, G. O., Gunraj, C. A., Wagle-Shukla, A., Moro, E., et al. (2007). Subthalamic nucleus stimulation modulates afferent inhibition in Parkinson disease. Neurology 68, 356–363. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000252812.95774.aa

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sailer, A., Molnar, G. F., Paradiso, G., Gunraj, C. A., Lang, A. E., and Chen, R. (2003). Short and long latency afferent inhibition in Parkinson's disease. Brain 126, 1883–1894. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg183

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sanabria, J., Ruiz, P. G., Gutierrez, R., Marquez, F., Escobar, P., Gentil, M., et al. (2001). The effect of levodopa on vocal function in Parkinson's disease. Clin. Neuropharmacol. 24, 99–102. doi: 10.1097/00002826-200103000-00006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sapir, S. (2014). Multiple factors are involved in the dysarthria associated with Parkinson's disease: a review with implications for clinical practice and research. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 57, 1330–1343. doi: 10.1044/2014_JSLHR-S-13-0039

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sapir, S., Ramig, L., and Fox, C. (2008). Speech and swallowing disorders in Parkinson disease. Curr. Opin. Otolaryngol. Head Neck Surg. 16, 205–210. doi: 10.1097/MOO.0b013e3282febd3a

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Scarpa, A., Cassandro, C., Vitale, C., Ralli, M., Policastro, A., Barone, P., et al. (2020). A comparison of auditory and vestibular dysfunction in Parkinson's disease and multiple system atrophy. Parkinsonism Relat. Disord. 71, 51–57. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2020.01.018

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneider, J. S., Diamond, S. G., and Markham, C. H. (1986). Deficits in orofacial sensorimotor function in Parkinson's disease. Ann. Neurol. 19, 275–282. doi: 10.1002/ana.410190309

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schulz, G. M., Sulc, S., Leon, S., and Gilligan, G. (2000). Speech motor learning in Parkinson disease. J. Med. Speech Lang. Pathol. 8, 243–248,

Google Scholar

Schwab, B. C., Kase, D., Zimnik, A., Rosenbaum, R., Codianni, M. G., Rubin, J. E., et al. (2020). Neural activity during a simple reaching task in macaques is counter to gating and rebound in basal ganglia-thalamic communication. PLoS Biol. 18:e3000829. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.3000829

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Scott, R., Gregory, R., Hines, N., Carroll, C., Hyman, N., Papanasstasiou, V., et al. (1998). Neuropsychological, neurological and functional outcome following pallidotomy for Parkinson's disease. A consecutive series of eight simultaneous bilateral and twelve unilateral procedures. Brain J. Neurol. 121, 659–675. doi: 10.1093/brain/121.4.659

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shalash, A. S., Hassan, D. M., Elrassas, H. H., Salama, M. M., Mendez-Hernandez, E., Salas-Pacheco, J. M., et al. (2017). Auditory- and vestibular-evoked potentials correlate with motor and non-motor features of Parkinson's disease. Front. Neurol. 8:55. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2017.00055

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Simonyan, K., Herscovitch, P., and Horwitz, B. (2013). Speech-induced striatal dopamine release is left lateralized and coupled to functional striatal circuits in healthy humans: a combined PET, fMRI and DTI study. NeuroImage 70, 21–32. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.12.042

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Singh, P., Lenka, A., Stezin, A., Jhunjhunwala, K., Pal, P. K., Ghosal, A., et al. (2019). Basal ganglia contributions during the learning of a visuomotor rotation: effect of dopamine, deep brain stimulation and reinforcement. Eur. J. Neurosci. 50, 3349–3364. doi: 10.1111/ejn.14494

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Skodda, S. (2012). Effect of deep brain stimulation on speech performance in Parkinson's disease. Parkinson's Dis. 2012:850596. doi: 10.1155/2012/850596

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Skodda, S., Flasskamp, A., and Schlegel, U. (2011). Instability of syllable repetition in Parkinson's disease--influence of levodopa and deep brain stimulation. Mov. Disord. 26, 728–730. doi: 10.1002/mds.23439

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Skodda, S., Gronheit, W., Schlegel, U., Sudmeyer, M., Schnitzler, A., and Wojtecki, L. (2014). Effect of subthalamic stimulation on voice and speech in Parkinson's disease: for the better or worse? Front. Neurol. 4:218. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2013.00218

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Skodda, S., and Schlegel, U. (2008). Speech rate and rhythm in Parkinson's disease. Mov. Disord. 23, 985–992. doi: 10.1002/mds.21996

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Skodda, S., Visser, W., and Schlegel, U. (2010). Short- and long-term dopaminergic effects on dysarthria in early Parkinson's disease. J. Neural Transm. (Vienna) 117, 197–205. doi: 10.1007/s00702-009-0351-5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, C., Malek, N., Grosset, K., Cullen, B., Gentleman, S., and Grosset, D. G. (2019). Neuropathology of dementia in patients with Parkinson's disease: a systematic review of autopsy studies. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 90, 1234–1243. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2019-321111

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Solomon, N. P., and Hixon, T. J. (1993). Speech breathing in Parkinson’s disease. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 36, 294–310. doi: 10.1044/jshr.3602.294

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Spielman, J., Mahler, L., Halpern, A., Gilley, P., Klepitskaya, O., and Ramig, L. (2011). Intensive voice treatment (Lsvt(R)loud) for Parkinson's disease following deep brain stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus. J. Commun. Disord. 44, 688–700. doi: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2011.05.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Spraker, M. B., Yu, H., Corcos, D. M., and Vaillancourt, D. E. (2007). Role of individual basal ganglia nuclei in force amplitude generation. J. Neurophysiol. 98, 821–834. doi: 10.1152/jn.00239.2007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Starr, P. A., Vitek, J. L., and Bakay, R. A. (1998). Ablative surgery and deep brain stimulation for Parkinson's disease. Neurosurgery 43, 989–1013; discussion 1013-5. doi: 10.1097/00006123-199811000-00001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Svennilson, E., Torvik, A., Lowe, R., and Leksell, L. (1960). Treatment of Parkinsonism by stereotatic thermolesions in the pallidal region. A clinical evaluation of 81 cases. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 35, 358–377. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1960.tb07606.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Svirsky, M. A., Lane, H., Perkell, J. S., and Wozniak, J. (1992). Effects of short-term auditory deprivation on speech production in adult cochlear implant users. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 92, 1284–1300. doi: 10.1121/1.403923

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tamburin, S., Fiaschi, A., Idone, D., Lochner, P., Manganotti, P., and Zanette, G. (2003). Abnormal sensorimotor integration is related to disease severity in Parkinson's disease: a TMS study. Mov. Disord. 18, 1316–1324. doi: 10.1002/mds.10515

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tanaka, Y., Tsuboi, T., Watanabe, H., Nakatsubo, D., Maesawa, S., Kato, S., et al. (2020). Longitudinal speech change after subthalamic nucleus deep brain stimulation in Parkinson’s disease patients: a 2-year prospective study. J. Parkinsons Dis. 10, 131–140. doi: 10.3233/JPD-191798

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tankus, A., and Fried, I. (2019). Degradation of neuronal encoding of speech in the subthalamic nucleus in Parkinson’s disease. Neurosurgery 84, 378–387. doi: 10.1093/neuros/nyy027

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tankus, A., Lustig, Y., Fried, I., and Strauss, I. (2021). Impaired timing of speech-related neurons in the subthalamic nucleus of Parkinson Disease patients suffering speech disorders. Neurosurgery 89, 800–809. doi: 10.1093/neuros/nyab293

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tawadros, P. B., Cordato, D., Cathers, I., and Burne, J. A. (2012). An electromyographic study of parkinsonian swallowing and its response to levodopa. Mov. Disord. 27, 1811–1815. doi: 10.1002/mds.25262

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Thobois, S., Ballanger, B., Baraduc, P., Le Bars, D., Lavenne, F., Broussolle, E., et al. (2007). Functional anatomy of motor urgency. NeuroImage 37, 243–252. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.04.049

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tjaden, K. (2000). A preliminary study of factors influencing perception of articulatory rate in Parkinson disease. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 43, 997–1010. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4304.997

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tornqvist, A. L., Schalen, L., and Rehncrona, S. (2005). Effects of different electrical parameter settings on the intelligibility of speech in patients with Parkinson's disease treated with subthalamic deep brain stimulation. Mov. Disord. 20, 416–423. doi: 10.1002/mds.20348

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tourville, J. A., Reilly, K. J., and Guenther, F. H. (2008). Neural mechanisms underlying auditory feedback control of speech. NeuroImage 39, 1429–1443. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.09.054

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tremblay, C., Rahayel, S., Vo, A., Morys, F., Shafiei, G., Abbasi, N., et al. (2021). Brain atrophy progression in Parkinson's disease is shaped by connectivity and local vulnerability. Brain Commun. 3:fcab269. doi: 10.1093/braincomms/fcab269

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tripoliti, E., Limousin, P., Foltynie, T., Candelario, J., Aviles-Olmos, I., Hariz, M. I., et al. (2014). Predictive factors of speech intelligibility following subthalamic nucleus stimulation in consecutive patients with Parkinson's disease. Mov. Disord. 29, 532–538. doi: 10.1002/mds.25816

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tripoliti, E., Zrinzo, L., Martinez-Torres, I., Frost, E., Pinto, S., Foltynie, T., et al. (2011). Effects of subthalamic stimulation on speech of consecutive patients with Parkinson disease. Neurology 76, 80–86. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e318203e7d0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tripoliti, E., Zrinzo, L., Martinez-Torres, I., Tisch, S., Frost, E., Borrell, E., et al. (2008). Effects of contact location and voltage amplitude on speech and movement in bilateral subthalamic nucleus deep brain stimulation. Mov. Disord. 23, 2377–2383. doi: 10.1002/mds.22296

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Troche, M. S., Brandimore, A. E., Okun, M. S., Davenport, P. W., and Hegland, K. W. (2014). Decreased cough sensitivity and aspiration in Parkinson disease. Chest 146, 1294–1299. doi: 10.1378/chest.14-0066

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Troster, A. I., Woods, S. P., and Fields, J. A. (2003). Verbal fluency declines after pallidotomy: an interaction between task and lesion laterality. Appl. Neuropsychol. 10, 69–75. doi: 10.1207/S15324826AN1002_02

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsuboi, T., Watanabe, H., Tanaka, Y., Ohdake, R., Yoneyama, N., Hara, K., et al. (2015). Distinct phenotypes of speech and voice disorders in Parkinson's disease after subthalamic nucleus deep brain stimulation. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 86, 856–864. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2014-308043

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Turner, R. S., and Anderson, M. E. (1997). Pallidal discharge related to the kinematics of reaching movements in two dimensions. J. Neurophysiol. 77, 1051–1074. doi: 10.1152/jn.1997.77.3.1051

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Turner, R. S., and Anderson, M. E. (2005). Context-dependent modulation of movement-related discharge in the primate Globus pallidus. J. Neurosci. 25, 2965–2976. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4036-04.2005

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Turner, R. S., and Desmurget, M. (2010). Basal ganglia contributions to motor control: a vigorous tutor. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 20, 704–716. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2010.08.022

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Turner, R. S., Desmurget, M., Grethe, J., Crutcher, M. D., and Grafton, S. T. (2003). Motor subcircuits mediating the control of movement extent and speed. J. Neurophysiol. 90, 3958–3966. doi: 10.1152/jn.00323.2003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tykalova, T., Novotny, M., Ruzicka, E., Dusek, P., and Rusz, J. (2022). Short-term effect of dopaminergic medication on speech in early-stage Parkinson's disease. NPJ Parkinsons Dis. 8:22. doi: 10.1038/s41531-022-00286-y

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Brenk, F., Stipancic, K. L., Rohl, A., Corcos, D., Tjaden, K., and Greenlee, D. W. (2024). No differential effects of subthalamic nucleus vs. globus pallidus deep brain stimulation in Parkinson’s disease: speech acoustic and perceptual findings. IBRO Neurosci. Rep. 16, 361–367. doi: 10.1016/j.ibneur.2024.01.015

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Lancker Sidtis, D., Rogers, T., Godier, V., Tagliati, M., and Sidtis, J. J. (2010). Voice and fluency changes as a function of speech task and deep brain stimulation. J. Speech Lang Hear. Res. 53, 1167–1177. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2010/09-0154)

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vargha-Khadem, F., Gadian, D. G., Copp, A., and Mishkin, M. (2005). Foxp2 and the neuroanatomy of speech and language. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 6, 131–138. doi: 10.1038/nrn1605

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vitale, C., Marcelli, V., Abate, T., Pianese, A., Allocca, R., Moccia, M., et al. (2016). Speech discrimination is impaired in parkinsonian patients: expanding the audiologic findings of Parkinson's disease. Parkinsonism Relat. Disord. 22, S138–S143. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2015.09.040

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vitale, C., Marcelli, V., Allocca, R., Santangelo, G., Riccardi, P., Erro, R., et al. (2012). Hearing impairment in Parkinson's disease: expanding the nonmotor phenotype. Mov. Disord. 27, 1530–1535. doi: 10.1002/mds.25149

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wasson, P., Prodoehl, J., Coombes, S. A., Corcos, D. M., and Vaillancourt, D. E. (2010). Predicting grip force amplitude involves circuits in the anterior basal ganglia. NeuroImage 49, 3230–3238. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.11.047

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Watson, P., and Montgomery, E. (2006). The relationship of neuronal activity within the sensori-motor region of the subthalamic nucleus to speech. Brain Lang. 97, 233–240. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2005.11.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wertheimer, J., Gottuso, A. Y., Nuno, M., Walton, C., Duboille, A., Tuchman, M., et al. (2014). The impact of Stn deep brain stimulation on speech in individuals with Parkinson's disease: the patient's perspective. Parkinsonism Relat. Disord. 20, 1065–1070. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2014.06.010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Whitfield, J. A., and Goberman, A. M. (2017). Speech motor sequence learning: acquisition and retention in Parkinson Disease and Normal aging. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 60, 1477–1492. doi: 10.1044/2016_JSLHR-S-16-0104

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Whitfield, J. A., Reif, A., and Goberman, A. M. (2018). Voicing contrast of stop consonant production in the speech of individuals with Parkinson disease on and off dopaminergic medication. Clin. Linguist. Phon. 32, 587–594. doi: 10.1080/02699206.2017.1387816

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wichmann, T., Bergman, H., and Delong, M. R. (1994). The primate subthalamic nucleus. I. Functional properties in intact animals. J. Neurophysiol. 72, 494–506

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Wichmann, T., Bergman, H., and Delong, M. R. (2018). Basal ganglia, movement disorders and deep brain stimulation: advances made through non-human primate research. J. Neural Transm. (Vienna) 125, 419–430. doi: 10.1007/s00702-017-1736-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

World Health Organization . (2023). Parkinson’s disease [online]. Available at: https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheests/detail/parkinson-disease (Accessed January 30, 2024).

Google Scholar

York, M. K., Lai, E. C., Jankovic, J., Macias, A., Atassi, F., Levin, H. S., et al. (2007). Short and long-term motor and cognitive outcome of staged bilateral pallidotomy: a retrospective analysis. Acta Neurochir. 149, 857–866. doi: 10.1007/s00701-007-1242-x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yylmaz, S., Karaly, E., Tokmak, A., Guclu, E., Kocer, A., and Ozturk, O. (2009). Auditory evaluation in parkinsonian patients. Eur. Arch. Otorrinolaringol. 266, 669–671. doi: 10.1007/s00405-009-0933-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zarei, M., Ibarretxe-Bilbao, N., Compta, Y., Hough, M., Junque, C., Bargallo, N., et al. (2013). Cortical thinning is associated with disease stages and dementia in Parkinson's disease. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 84, 875–882. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2012-304126

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ziegler, W., and Ackermann, H. (2017). Subcortical contributions to motor speech: phylogenetic, developmental, clinical. Trends Neurosci. 40, 458–468. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2017.06.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: Parkinson’s disease, speech, modeling, dysarthria, basal ganglia, deep brain stimulation

Citation: Manes JL, Bullock L, Meier AM, Turner RS, Richardson RM and Guenther FH (2024) A neurocomputational view of the effects of Parkinson’s disease on speech production. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 18:1383714. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2024.1383714

Received: 07 February 2024; Accepted: 23 April 2024;
Published: 15 May 2024.

Edited by:

Fatemeh Mollaei, University of Reading, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Arun Singh, University of South Dakota, United States
Dobromir Dotov, University of Nebraska Omaha, United States

Copyright © 2024 Manes, Bullock, Meier, Turner, Richardson and Guenther. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Frank H. Guenther, guenther@bu.edu

ORCID: Robert S. Turner, https://orcid.org/0000-0002-6074-4365

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.