Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 16 February 2024
Sec. Brain-Computer Interfaces
This article is part of the Research Topic Analyzing and Computing Humans - the Role of Language, Culture, Brain and Health View all 6 articles

Analyzing and computing humans by means of the brain using Brain-Computer Interfaces - understanding the user – previous evidence, self-relevance and the user’s self-concept as potential superordinate human factors of relevance

  • Department of Applied Emotion and Motivation Psychology, Institute of Psychology and Education, Ulm University, Ulm, Germany

Brain–computer interfaces (BCIs) are well-known instances of how technology can convert a user’s brain activity taken from non-invasive electroencephalography (EEG) into computer commands for the purpose of computer-assisted communication and interaction. However, not all users are attaining the accuracy required to use a BCI consistently, despite advancements in technology. Accordingly, previous research suggests that human factors could be responsible for the variance in BCI performance among users. Therefore, the user’s internal mental states and traits including motivation, affect or cognition, personality traits, or the user’s satisfaction, beliefs or trust in the technology have been investigated. Going a step further, this manuscript aims to discuss which human factors could be potential superordinate factors that influence BCI performance, implicitly, explicitly as well as inter- and intraindividually. Based on the results of previous studies that used comparable protocols to examine the motivational, affective, cognitive state or personality traits of healthy and vulnerable EEG-BCI users within and across well-investigated BCIs (P300-BCIs or SMR-BCIs, respectively), it is proposed that the self-relevance of tasks and stimuli and the user’s self-concept provide a huge potential for BCI applications. As potential key human factors self-relevance and the user’s self-concept (self-referential knowledge and beliefs about one’s self) guide information processing and modulate the user’s motivation, attention, or feelings of ownership, agency, and autonomy. Changes in the self-relevance of tasks and stimuli as well as self-referential processing related to one’s self (self-concept) trigger changes in neurophysiological activity in specific brain networks relevant to BCI. Accordingly, concrete examples will be provided to discuss how past and future research could incorporate self-relevance and the user’s self-concept in the BCI setting – including paradigms, user instructions, and training sessions.

1 Introduction

Brain–computer interfaces (BCIs) convert brain activity into adaptive and assistive computer commands. This makes it possible for BCI users to interact and communicate with their environment non-invasively solely by changes in neural activity detected for example by electroencephalographic recordings (EEG). Conventional use cases of BCI applications are EEG-based control of external devices such as a robotic hand, limb or foot. The ultimate goal of using BCIs in this context is to give patients, who are unable to communicate directly through other means, such as patients with motor impairments or neurological impairments, a clinically supportive aid for communication or interaction (for an overview see e.g., Kleih et al., 2011a; Luauté et al., 2015). Progress in BCI technology has made it possible to utilize BCIs for a wide range of clinical user groups. These user groups may include elderly patients or individuals with mental health conditions suffering from stress, depression, cognitive decline, or cognitive symptoms such attention deficit hyperactivity, to name a few examples (e.g., Balcombe and de Leo, 2022). Moreover, a considerable body of BCI research has spotted the healthy user. Use cases of BCIs among healthy users can include the attempt to control the driver’s fatigue during automated driving (e.g., Zhang et al., 2020) or the monitoring and support of the user’s mental state in challenging occupational settings (e.g., a pilot’s attention during a challenging flight maneuver; Dehais et al., 2022). The use of BCIs in occupational settings and even more so simply for fun, entertainment, or leisure activities is growing (Nijholt et al., 2022). Use cases include brain-controlled gaming and sports (e.g., Randolph, 2012; Diya et al., 2019) for example in virtual reality augmented digital settings. According to recent research, expanding the applications of BCIs to recreational activities or as a tool for the workplace could enhance the scientific knowledge about training-induced or learning-induced neuroplasticity in a variety of users (for a comprehensive overview or recent research on occupational neuroplasticity, refer to e.g., Wang et al., 2017; Wu et al., 2020). Therefore, understanding the BCI user has become an important topic in BCI research.

2 The human user in BCI research

A body of studies has shown that despite technical advances in the field, variance in BCI performance is owing to the user. According to previous estimations, about 20% up to 40% of BCI users may not achieve a BCI performance with the necessary accuracy to benefit from an BCI application (for an overview see e.g., Edlinger et al., 2015; for recent estimations and discussions concerning motor imagery based BCI, e.g., see Zhang et al., 2020).

“BCI inefficiency” or “BCI illiteracy” (Allison and Neuper, 2010) has been found among healthy users, as a mentally and behaviorally fully responsive user group of BCIs or among patients, who are mentally handicapped or behaviorally non-responsive (e.g., Guger et al., 2003; Allison and Neuper, 2010; Graimann et al., 2010). This has encouraged discussions about the relevance of research on human factors not only in the domain of human–computer interaction (HCI) in general, but in the domain of BCI research, its deployment and application as well (for an overview see for example, Botte-Lecocq et al., 2014; Kleih and Kübler, 2015). Furthermore, the discussion on the role of the BCI user has promoted theoretical suggestions of how to integrate human factors into BCI technology by means of a user-centered approach (for an overview see e.g., Kübler et al., 2015, 2020; Alonso-Valerdi and Mercado-Garcıa, 2019; or for motor imagery based BCIs, e.g., Lyu et al., 2023). Additionally, guidelines of BCI training protocols have been suggested to incorporate human factors into the BCI settings (Mason and Birch, 2003; Botte-Lecocq et al., 2014; Jeunet et al., 2018).

2.1 Human factors related to BCI engineering and ergonomics

In conjunction with these theoretical efforts, several studies have specifically looked into human factors that, when considering the BCI’s design features, may have an impact on the device’s usability and practicability. To this end, the user’s satisfaction with the BCI system (e.g., Zickler et al., 2009; for a discussion see Kübler, 2020), or the user’s effectiveness of BCI use or the user’s acceptability have been investigated among healthy users or patients. In addition, the user’s previous experience with technology (Al-Taleb et al., 2019; Voinea et al., 2019; Leeuwis et al., 2021; for a discussion, see Kübler et al., 2014) or the mental workload and fatigue imposed by the BCI task and training routines have been examined to this end (e.g., Käthner et al., 2014). Furthermore, some studies investigated if sociodemographic variables like the experimenter’s or user’s age and gender affect the user’s BCI experience. A number of studies found that both factors (gender and age) can modulate the experience of BCI users (Randolph, 2012; Zich et al., 2017; Wood and Kober, 2018; Pillette et al., 2021). In conclusion, the results of these studies suggest that specific human factors are either strongly restricting or enhancing human factors for BCI engineering and ergonomics. For recent reviews on the role of human factors for BCI design engineering, see e.g., Saha et al. (2021); for P300-spellers e.g., Powers et al. (2015); for SMR-BCIs, see e.g., Lyu et al. (2023).

2.2 Psychological human factors: user traits, states, and BCI performance

Furthermore, a significant body of research has investigated how human factors, specifically psychological factors related to the user’s mental traits or state influence BCI performance. Psychological human factors that relate to the BCI user’s mental states or traits are factors such as the user’s personality (trait) or current motivation, mood, emotion, or affect, or the user’s cognitive skills and aptitudes such as the user’s ability to pay attention, the user’s learning capacities, intelligence, self-regulation or cognitive control strategies. Remarkably, these user traits and states might affect BCI performance explicitly or implicitly (e.g., hidden from direct observation) by modulating the different BCI’s outcome measures of interest on a behavioral, subjective or neural level (e.g., accuracy measures or brain signals used for BCI classification). Moreover, not all psychological user traits and states of interest are directly accessible for the users themselves via introspection or self-report. Thus, the assessment of such factors benefits from sophisticated examination by measures of experimental manipulation or psychological testing.

3 Challenges of previous BCI human factor research and aims of the present study

Essentially, the key to increasing BCI literacy and decreasing BCI inefficiency across and beyond the individual BCI user, is knowing which of the psychological user’s traits and states are influencing BCI performance, when, how, and in whom across the BCI types and tasks, or technical solutions.

Up to now and as summarized above, several studies already examined human factors in the context of BCIs. For human psychological factors that influence neurofeedback learning outcomes, see for example, Kadosh and Staunton (2019). For recent overviews on SMR-BCIs and internal factors, see for example, Horowitz et al. (2021), or the previous reviews by Ahn and Jun (2015), Roc et al. (2021), or Wierzgawa et al. (2018). Despite these previous attempts, so far, it is difficult to draw definitive conclusions about the overall influence of user traits and states specifically on BCI performance across BCIs and user groups. Such overviews are currently still rare in the literature, probably because of variations among the different studies in study protocols and methodology. From a methodological point of view, however, a number of the existing previous studies are available whose main goal was to investigate psychological human factors, specifically the impact of user characteristics or states on BCI performance (see Figure 1) and that used similar study protocols. For example, these studies either experimentally manipulated the user’s traits and states, or measured the user’s traits and states via psychological test batteries or via standardized self-report. As a result, the evaluation techniques were carefully controlled and comparable among studies. In addition, among these studies, a number of studies explored the user’s traits and states on BCI performance among healthy users or disabled patient groups with similar protocols. The BCI systems comprised well-established non-invasive EEG-BCIs that were either based on motor imagery as a task to elicit sensorimotor rhythms (in short: SMR-BCIs or MI-BCIs) or on P300 modulation elicited by the voluntary attention of the user (e.g., P300-BCI spellers), for further details, see section “4 Psychological human factors and their impact on BCI performance: evidence from previous studies using P300-BCI or SMR-BCI among healthy and disabled user groups.”

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Overview of the selection procedure of the previous studies summarized in the section “4 Psychological human factors and their impact on BCI performance: evidence from previous studies using P300-BCI or SMR-BCI among healthy and disabled user groups” that investigated user traits and states. Asterisk indicates the key words for the literature search.

As a result, these studies could allow a valid comparison of the effects of user traits and states on BCI performance across two different but very frequently used BCIs, SMR-BCI and P300-BCI, respectively. Moreover, their findings might allow a first cross-sectional evaluation of how user traits and states influence BCI performance across user groups and across the two different but well-established BCI systems of SMR-BCI and P300-BCI. Most importantly for future aims, recommendations concerning superordinate or key human factors that could influence BCI performance beyond tasks and BCI applications can be made based on a joint analysis of these studies’ findings.

In line with this, the following sections of this manuscript aim to provide a brief overview, review and discussion of the findings of these studies that investigated psychological human factors. Selection of the studies of interest was based on a literature search according to the criteria mentioned above and as illustrated in detail in Figure 1. Based on the summary of the research findings from these studies (see also Table 1 and Figure 2), the following sections will propose that, in accordance with the above recommendations, self-relevance, along with the user’s self-concept, may be potential superordinate human factors that affect BCI performance both within and across the well-investigated BCIs (P300-BCIs or SMR-BCIs, respectively). Consequently, concrete examples and a hypothetical model discussed in the following sections (for a graphical summary see Figures 3, 4) will demonstrate that self-relevance and the user’s self-concept should be thoroughly examined in subsequent BCI studies. These studies might also include BCIs other than P300- or SMR-BCIs.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. This table provides an overview of the studies discussed in the subsections of section “4 Psychological human factors and their impact on BCI performance: evidence from previous studies using P300-BCIs or SMR-BCIs among healthy and disabled user groups.”

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. (Left column) Overview of the user traits and states examined in previous studies using SMR-BCI or P300-BCI in samples of healthy users and patients (for a detailed summary see Table 1 and section “4 Psychological human factors and their impact on BCI performance: evidence from previous studies using P300-BCI or SMR-BCI among healthy and disabled user groups”). (Right column) Self-relevance as a key human factor, its potential influence on BCI performance and its implementation in the BCI design. For a detailed discussion, see section “Self-relevance and the user’s self-concept as potential superordinate human factors in the BCI setting” and subsections in the text and Figures 3, 4.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Self-relevance and BCI performance as illustrated by different examples discussed in the text in section “Self-relevance and the user’s self-concept as potential superordinate human factors in the BCI setting” and subsections. The number in brackets refer to the studies discussed in section “Self-relevance and the user’s self-concept as potential superordinate human factors in the BCI setting.” [1] Resting state (e.g., Blankertz et al., 2010), [2] first person perspective or own body parts (e.g., Neuper et al., 2005; Škola and Liarokapis, 2018; Ziadeh et al., 2021) and [3] autobiographical events (e.g., Dong et al., 2015; Placidi et al., 2018).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Hypothetical model that illustrates the possible relationship between the self-concept of the user, the self-relevance of tasks and stimuli, BCI performance, and the previously examined user traits and states. For a detailed discussion, see section “Self-relevance and the user’s self-concept as potential superordinate human factors in the BCI setting.”

4 Psychological human factors and their impact on BCI performance: evidence from previous studies using P300-BCI or SMR-BCI among healthy and disabled user groups

4.1 Current motivation and mood states

The use of BCIs such as the P300-BCI or SMR-BCI require deliberate engagement of the user with a task for the purpose of active interaction and communication with the system. SMR-BCIs are using motor imagery (MI) paradigms to elicit sensorimotor rhythms (SMR) (SMR-BCIs or MI-BCIs). Similarly frequent are BCI systems for spelling that elicit discrete event-related potentials (ERPs) in the EEG based on the voluntary attention of the user and his/her choice of numbers, letters or symbols rapidly presented in rows and columns. The most traditionally used BCI speller is the well know P300-BCI (for an overview of BCIs, e.g., Edlinger et al., 2015). Consequently, a number of the existing previous studies investigated whether influencing or controlling for the user’s motivation and affective state could intentionally enhance BCI performance in either the P300-BCI or the SMR-BCI. Kleih-Dahms et al. (2021) for example manipulated the user’s mood by experimental mood induction via video clips or music. Examination of the performance of healthy users included SMR-BCI-based motor imagery-induced movement and action control. Moreover, akin to the study by Kleih et al. (2010) that used a P300-BCI, Kleih-Dahms et al. (2021) modulated the user’s motivation experimentally. They set a monetary incentive (extrinsic motivation) and divided the users according to their intrinsic motivation [ratings on visual analog scale (VAS) about their current motivation] into high vs. low motivated users. In a third study by the authors (see Kleih et al., 2011b), data from healthy users and patients diagnosed with amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS) were pooled to further exploit the role of the user’s current motivation for P300-BCI performance. Again, the user’s current motivation was assessed via ratings on the intuitive and easy to use VAS. In addition, the authors used the Questionnaire for Current Motivation (QCM)-BCI to measure motivational dimensions related to BCI use. The QCM-BCI is a standardized questionnaire used in many BCI studies as well (for an overview see Table 1). It is adapted from the QCM (Rheinberg et al., 2001), a questionnaire to assess current motivation in learning situations. The QCM questionnaire is a state measure and allows determining the degree of the achievement motivation of a person (trait) in a situational context (state). Moreover, in a fourth study by Kleih and Kübler (2013), the authors investigated healthy individuals as novices in P300-BCI and divided the users according to their intrinsic motivation (ratings on VAS about their current motivation) or their habitual motivation to help. In addition, a number of further psychological human factors were investigated in the study such as the user’s habitual affective ability to empathize with others (perspective taking), which was measured by self-report using standardized questionnaires (for an overview see Table 1). Furthermore, the user’s memory and ability to pay attention was tested via a cognitive test battery (see Table 1). In some of the previously stated studies that used P300-BCIs, a significant effect of current motivation as measured by self-report scales (VAS) was found (see Table 1). Additionally, the user’s self-reported degree of incompetence fear as assessed with the QCM-BCI was found to correlate with the user’s performance accuracy measures (e.g., spelling accuracy). In the SMR-BCI, a significant effect of reward (experimental manipulation of the user’s current state of extrinsic motivation) was found to influence SMR-BCI performance. The study by Kleih et al. (2010) could not find a significant direct impact of reward on the user’s motivation or BCI performance (spelling) but found differences in P300 modulation during calibration and copy-spelling in the rewarded and unrewarded groups.

The study by Nijboer et al. (2008) investigated the influence of mood and motivation on BCI performance in young adults in the SMR-BCI (see Table 1). In this study, the authors explored the impact of mood and motivation across sensory modalities using auditory or visual feedback during training. The authors used the QCM-BCI to determine motivational facets related to BCI use and assessed changes in the current mood or wellbeing by standardized scales for quality of life (QoL) (for details, see Table 1). The participants performed a motor imagery task to elicit sensorimotor rhythms in the EEG for BCI based control of the participants’ choices: the participants had to select by instruction to play sounds of different music instruments and the choices were rewarded either auditorily (sound related to a selected instrument) or visually by verbal prompts (correct vs. incorrect). During visual feedback, BCI performance varied with the self-reported differences in mood of the healthy users and with their self-reported motivation to use a BCI, in particular with the degree of mastery confidence and incompetence fear reported by the participants in the QCM-BCI. Higher scores of mood and mastery confidence were related to better performance, whereas incompetence fear showed a negative correlation with performance. During auditory feedback, incompetence fear however showed a positive relationship with BCI performance and was the only factor with a significant effect on BCI performance. To elucidate these observations further, in a second study (see Table 1), Nijboer et al. (2010) compared the influence of individual differences in mood and wellbeing and differences in the current motivation of the users on BCI performance among healthy users and patients diagnosed with ALS. The comparison included the performance in two different BCI set-ups comprising SMR- and P300-BCIs. Again, mood and wellbeing were assessed via self-report measures, included scales for QoL as in the previous study, and additionally scales for assessment of depressive symptoms as potential further interindividual modifier of BCI performance (Nijboer et al., 2010). In this study, the authors found a relationship between mood, motivation, and the number of BCI sessions. Moreover, the authors could validate a relationship between BCI performance and motivational factors in half of their participant sample. This included the user’s self-reported challenge, mastery confidence and incompetence fear as measured with the Questionnaire for Current Motivation (QCM-BCI). When comparing the performance of healthy and disabled users, Nijboer et al. (2010) found that motivation for BCI use such as mastery confidence or incompetence fear correlated with BCI performance positively or negatively among healthy or individual ALS users in both, the P300-BCI or the SMR-BCI (Nijboer et al., 2010). However, they could not find a relationship between mood and BCI performance in any of the participants: the scores obtained from the measures of current mood related to QoL and wellbeing or the self-reported current symptoms of depression as further measures of a user’s mood state or trait showed no significant effects.

The findings of the above reported studies suggest that a proportion of the variation in BCI performance is accounted by the user’s motivation among both, healthy users or patients. However with different results across the studies and the measures used, i.e., intrinsic vs. extrinsic motivation, measured experimentally vs. by self-report, and whether related to the performance in the P300-BCI or SMR-BCI, respectively. So far, extrinsic motivation (reward and incentives), the user’s self-reported motivation, as assessed with an intuitive self-report scale (VAS) or as measured in relation to BCI use were often but not consistently correlated with BCI performance across the studies. Moreover, as stated above and as summarized in Table 1, some studies were not able to find any significant effects.

However, as shown in Table 1, some further studies support the suggestion that the motivation of the user plays a role and can affect BCI performance, both in the P300-BCI or the SMR-BCI at least among healthy users. Baykara et al. (2016) used a P300 multi-class speller paradigm. As in the studies discussed above, the authors assessed current motivation in healthy participants with the QCM-BCI and a VAS, akin to the studies by Kleih et al. or Nijboer et al. that were using both or one of these measures. In Baykara et al. (2016), both measures of current motivation showed correlations with BCI performance efficiency assessed via information transfer rate (ITR) and with the amplitude of the P300. Moreover, the participants with high motivation as assessed with the intuitive self-report measure of the VAS had higher P300 amplitudes and higher ITR than the group of participants reporting low current motivation on the VAS (Baykara et al., 2016). The results of this study complement observations from another study conducted by Ahn et al. (2018). This study used the user’s predictions of their BCI performance as a self-report measure of a user’s motivation in the SMR-BCI. In Ahn et al. (2018), healthy participants performed a motor imagery task for SMR-BCI. Between the training sessions, the participants self-reported their current mood, calmness, fatigue, and their mental and physical state were assessed (see Table 1 for details). In addition, the participants should make accuracy predictions about their BCI performance and difficulty of motor imagery. The authors found that the predicted motor imagery performance of the individual user showed a relationship with offline analysis of the participants’ BCI accuracy measures and with the factually achieved classification accuracy.

To conclude, the studies on mood and motivation outlined above and in Table 1 point to the possibility that motivational aspects associated with the use of BCIs impact BCI performance. The motivational dimensions of mastery competence and incompetence fear, the users’ own intuitive evaluation of their task performance (measured by self-prediction or with simple intuitive scales such as the VAS) turn out to be correlates of BCI performance across BCI systems and user groups. Moreover, some of the studies found that in some users, mood may change across sessions either enhancing or decreasing motivation across sessions. However, as discussed in detail in the following sections, as far as mood and affect are concerned, trait variables such as the user’s degree of empathy (e.g., Kleih and Kübler, 2013) or emotional stability as a personality trait (e.g., Hammer et al., 2012) might play a more important role among healthy users than state measures of mood and affect.

4.2 Mental states (perception, attention, and cognition)

In an attempt to find best predictors of the user’s traits and states for BCI performance in the P300-BCI or SMR-BCI, a series of previous studies further elaborated and extended the investigation of user variables from motivation and affect or mood to mental states including perceptual and cognitive states of the user. The assessment methods of these studies cover a broad range of perceptual, cognitive, affective or motivational traits and states of the users. Hammer et al. (2012, 2014, 2018) for example used a mixed-method approach that comprised structured psychological test batteries as well as self-report measures to capture a broad range of traits and states (for a detailed overview, see Table 1). The test battery relevant for SMR-BCI performance (Hammer et al., 2012, 2014) included for example perceptual and cognitive indices, sensorimotor coordination, attention, concentration (ability to concentrate on the task and sustain attention to it), or measures of the user’s verbal and non-verbal learning ability. These factors according to the authors critically influenced cognitive performance in a number of tasks in previous research including motor imagery. In addition, to replicate results from earlier studies outlined in section “4.1 Current motivation and mood states,” the authors assessed affective, motivational, and personality variables. These comprised the user’s current mood and motivation, locus of control, Big Five personality traits and clinically relevant current mood symptoms related to anxiety and depressive disorders assessed via standardized self-report measures (for details and comparison with the studies discussed in section “4.1 Current motivation and mood states,” see Table 1). Effects were examined among naive healthy BCI users in the Berlin Brain–Computer Interface (BBCI; Blankertz et al., 2006). The BBCI is based on neurofeedback training and uses direct reinforcement of task-related and task-contingent brain signals of the user. Feedback learning is well known to be an integral part of neurofeedback studies, though not the only constituent to perform well in BCI settings.

From all psychological variables measured, the ability to concentrate on the task and sustain attention to it as well as the user’s sensorimotor skills were significant predictors of the user’s SMR-BCI performance in Hammer et al. (2012). In Hammer et al. (2014), the authors aimed to replicate their results (see Table 1). This revealed two user characteristics, i.e., the user’s visuo-motor coordination ability and the ability to concentrate on the task as significant predictors of SMR-BCI performance. Sprague et al. (2016) also used psychological measures (e.g., current motivation, mood, or fatigue) and psychological tests of cognition that additionally included working memory tasks and on a trait level, the user’s general intelligence. The authors investigated if these factors could be predictors not only of SMR-BCI performance but as well of BCI performance in the P300-BCI among healthy users. The authors found that in P300-BCI, the BCI user’s performance correlated significantly with their performance obtained from the memory tasks. Moreover, general intelligence significantly predicted BCI performance. In addition, the user’s working memory could predict BCI performance but failed as a predictor after adding the additional user traits and states (e.g., the user’s level of fatigue, mood, or motivation). This shows that psychological characteristics of the user, could additionally play a significant role as mediators of BCI performance. Hammer et al. (2018) further exploited the impact of psychological user traits and states on P300-BCI performance. Like in their earlier studies and in the study by Sprague et al. (2016), Hammer et al. (2018) were using an extended psychological test battery that additionally included tests of selective attention that are relevant for attention selection when target stimuli are presented rapidly such as in the P300-BCI spellers (for an overview see Table 1).

The study by Hammer et al. (2018) could not replicate the findings reported in the earlier studies of the authors nor the results reported by Sprague et al. (2016) outlined above. None of the investigated user state factors showed a significant correlation with P300-BCI performance indices in the sample of healthy BCI novices, neither in an auditory nor in a visual version of the P300 speller.

Interestingly, Jeunet et al. (2015) using SMR-BCI investigated the same broad range of psychological self-report variables like for example in the studies conducted by Hammer et al. Additionally, the authors used a broad range of neurophysiological indicators such as changes in EEG frequency bands as predictors of SMR-BCI performance. In line with Hammer et al. (2012, 2014), but in contrast to the results observed in Hammer et al. (2018), Jeunet et al. (2015) found a relationship between a number of cognitive factors and BCI performance in the motor imagery SMR-BCI. Among the sample of healthy users, the ability for spatial mental rotation as well as the user’s habitual level of self-reported tension (impatience and frustration) and the user’s self-reported self-reliance (i.e., the user’s ability to learn autonomously) correlated significantly with the user’s BCI performance across six different training sessions. Besides this, further predictors such as the user’s visuo-spatial memory span or visual or verbal learning abilities contributed to the user’s motor imagery based SMR-BCI performance. However, Jeunet et al. (2015) additionally report that these user characteristics did not predict the user’s BCI performance across all the different training sessions. The training sessions comprised different motor imagery tasks such as left-hand motor imagery, mental rotation or mental subtraction.

Recent studies aimed to scrutinize the potential impact of the user’s cognitive state and cognitive skills on BCI performance, such as the study by Leeuwis et al. (2021). The authors used similar self-report methods as the previous studies outlined in this section but not identical psychological tests and focused on SMR-BCI like most of the afore mentioned previous studies (see Table 1). Like Hammer et al. (2018) for P300-BCI, the study by Leeuwis et al. (2021) could only partly replicate the observations from previous SMR-BCI studies outlined above although investigating a much larger sample size of healthy participants than the sample size of some of the previous studies (see Table 1).

Taken together, the results of the studies summarized in this section underscore the relevance of monitoring the mental cognitive states of the user to ensure accurate BCI performance. However, they also show that cognitive state assessment requires to include a broad number of cognitive states to identify which of the many cognitive variables and cognitive abilities of the user might specifically modulate BCI performance in a given setting within and across the different BCI systems and training sessions.

4.3 Role of assessment methods

Therefore, a considerable number of studies have investigated which measures (e.g., neurophysiological, experimental, or self-report) might best predict BCI performance due to the various ways in which cognitive, affective, or motivational states of the user can be measured and how they might influence performance measures in the BCI setting. Of these studies (for an overview see Table 1), a number of studies used external validation techniques to further define the relationship between various measures of a user’s characteristics and states and BCI performance (see Ahn and Jun, 2015 for a brief overview).

For example, Halder et al. (2011) compared the brain activity patterns acquired outside of the BCI context to the performance outcome measures of healthy BCI users obtained from motor imagery driven SMR-BCI (see Table 1). In the study, the same subjects who performed the BCI setting were invited to a second study in which they were asked to perform different motor tasks (e.g., motor observation vs. motor imagery) while their brain activity was monitored by functional magnetic resonance imaging methodology (fMRI) (Halder et al., 2011). The authors grouped the BCI users into those with poor and good BCI performance measures. The authors found that what differentiated best between “poor” and “good” users was the users’ brain patterns in the fMRI experiment and thus there “implicit” ability “to recruit the relevant areas for symbolic motor tasks required for motor imagery and motor observation” (cited from Halder et al., 2011).

In line with this, a series of studies explored the degree to which neurophysiological measures as correlates of a user’s mental state can serve as predictors of BCI performance. Some studies explored the mental state and its neurophysiological correlates during a resting state (e.g., Blankertz et al., 2010). Other studies explored if task-induced changes in neurophysiological activity as biological markers of the user’s cognitive or motor performance can serve as predictors of BCI performance (e.g., Halder et al., 2013; for SMR-BCI: Ahn et al., 2013; Robinson et al., 2018; Tzdaka et al., 2020). Further studies assessed the impact of particular training interventions such as for example mindfulness training or relaxation training on BCI performance (Botrel et al., 2017; Botrel and Kübler, 2019) and for example examined if mindfulness or relaxation training before the start of a BCI session improves motor imagery performance in the SMR-BCI (see Table 1)).

Similar to the studies using psychological measures such as questionnaires or test scores, the results of these studies suggest that many neurophysiological measures are task dependent, BCI type, or performance specific (for an overview see Table 1). In addition, interventions such as short-term relaxation training or mindfulness training, visuomotor coordination training or progressive muscle relaxation seem to have no effects on SMR-BCI performance of healthy BCI users (Botrel et al., 2017; Botrel and Kübler, 2019). A study by Jiang et al. (2021) however found that participants with long-term training in meditation had more stable resting EEG mu rhythms, better resting state SMR predictors than non-mediators, achieved larger control signal contrasts in the motor imagery tasks and fewer subjects in the mediating group were BCI illiterates. For short-term and long-term effects of meditation training interventions on BCI performance testing of healthy users see Tan et al. (2014) or Tan et al. (2015). These latter results corroborate the finding that neurophysiological correlates obtained from the analysis of a user’s mental state during rest (e.g., Blankertz et al., 2010) are reliable predictors of BCI performance. Out of all the neurophysiological predictors investigated thus far, the assessment of activity changes during a resting state appears to predict the user’s unique BCI performance the best in addition to psychological predictors (e.g., Lee et al., 2020). The fact that a resting state increases self-referential processing could be one explanation for these findings (see section “6 Self-relevance as potential superordinate human factor in the BCI setting”).

4.4 User traits and BCI performance: the role of personality traits

While several studies examined the impact of user states (e.g., current motivation or mood states or mental states) on BCI performance (see sections above), a considerable number of studies additionally aimed to determine the role of user traits as intraindividually stable differences between subjects as predictors of BCI performance. Most of these studies focused on personality traits. Theoretically, individuals can be distinguished according to a set of fundamental personality traits that predict interindividual variations in human experience and behavior across situations. Thus, personality traits imply stability and may therefore predict a user’s BCI performance across situations. Usually, personality traits are assessed by standardized questionnaires. Most of these questionnaires are based on personality taxonomies and models of personality such as the Big Five-Factor Model (e.g., De Raad and Mlacic, 2015) that distinguishes between Openness, Conscientiousness, Extraversion, Agreeableness, and Neuroticism, respectively. Each of these traits is assumed to be additionally associated with facets of these traits (e.g., extraversion being linked to warmness, assertiveness, positive emotionality, etc. or neuroticism to anxiety, depression, anger, vulnerability, self-consciousness, or impulsivity). As summarized in Table 1, personality traits or facets thereof correlate with BCI performance either positively or negatively in several studies. For example, the studies by Hammer et al. found that “emotional stability” predicted P300-BCI performance among healthy users across the visual and auditory spelling modalities. “Emotional stability” is one of six personality factors from the Big Five Plus One Personality-Inventory used by Hammer et al. (2014) and is associated with the Big Five trait of neuroticism. In the study by Kleih and Kübler (2013), P300-BCI performance correlated negatively with empathy in healthy BCI users (all novices of BCI). Based on these findings, one may speculate that users who are scoring high in empathy and perspective taking might have lower BCI performance, possibly despite or due to their higher motivation to help or their greater incompetence fear compared to user’s scoring lower on empathy and perspective taking.

A study by Bobrova et al. (2018) also discovered this. The authors measured personality traits via standardized questionnaires, and investigated if interindividual differences in neuroticisms - as a personality trait that is related to empathy and emotional stability - modulates SMR-BCI performance. Neuroticism correlated negatively with motor imagery skills and SMR-BCI performance much like Hammer et al. found for emotional stability or Kleih and Kübler (2013) for empathy (see Table 1). The study by Jeunet et al. (2015) that investigated a range of psychological human factors during SMR-BCI (psychological, neuropsychological, or cognitive) and that additionally included the assessment of the user’s personality traits point into a similar direction. In their study, the authors used a version of the Sixteen Personality Factor Questionnaire (16PF, see Table 1). They discovered that during the SMR training sessions, 3 of the 16 personality factors and a fourth factor associated with the users’ reflective and active learning style (tension, abstractness, self-reliance, and self-reliability) accounted for over 80% of the variance in SMR-BCI performance.

Furthermore, the study by Leeuwis et al. (2021) found a significant influence of personality traits on SMR-BCI performance among healthy users. As described in section “4.2 Mental states (perception, attention, and cognition).” Leeuwis et al. (2021) investigated a number of different motivational and cognitive states as well. The authors used the Five Factor Personality Inventory (FFPI) to measure basic personality traits. The five personality factors comprise the personality traits mildness, emotional stability, orderliness, extraversion, and autonomy. BCI performance was accurately predicted by the personality traits “autonomy” and “orderliness” as well as by the vividness of mental imagery across BCI runs. Moreover, the sociodemographic variable “gender” (women performing better than men) affected BCI performance. However, emotional stability did not predict BCI performance across runs as found and reported in other studies (see Table 1). The study conducted by Benaroch et al. (2019) also supports this conclusion. Although personality traits are considered stable and situation-independent, Benaroch et al. (2019) found no effects. The authors used a machine learning approach and data from three different SMR-BCI experiments all using the same study protocol. Personality traits did not predict SMR-BCI performance between different motor imagery tasks in the SMR-BCI across BCI sessions (see Table 1).

Hence, taken together there are differing effects of personality traits on BCI performance. This stands in opposition to studies that found that user’s traits can modulate the user’s BCI experience (see section “2.1 Human factors related to BCI engineering and ergonomics”). In this case, both past and current research suggest a strong correlation between the user’s self-reported operation performance and their self-reported performance satisfaction with their BCI performance (see for a discussion Kübler et al., 2015). In addition, the user’s self-reported and perceived effectiveness (Kübler et al., 2014) and their acceptability of the BCIs as well as their affinity with technology (e.g., Hagedorn et al., 2021; Leeuwis et al., 2021) seem to modulate BCI performance among different vulnerable end-user groups. This may include patients with stroke or with mental health conditions (e.g., Al-Taleb et al., 2019; Voinea et al., 2019). Several of these studies reported effects of these user characteristics on BCI performance measures such as P300 spelling frequency or motor imagery and in an off-line analysis with the brain signals under investigation (e.g., P300 amplitude elicited in a BCI-P300 speller).

5 Challenges for future BCI research: finding superordinate key human factors

In summary, the studies described above show that several user traits and states significantly modulate BCI performance in the most well established BCI systems (P300-BCI and SMR-BCIs). The effort of assessing all of these user traits and states with standardized test assessment batteries and different measures however is huge, as pointed out by for example Hammer et al. (2018). Moreover, extended assessment and testing may increase the mental workload or fatigue of the users, and both, workload and fatigue can significantly affect BCI performance negatively as for example reported by Käthner et al. (2014).

Thus, a significant future challenge for user-centered or personalized BCI approaches is the timely or time-efficient assessment of the user’s traits and states. Finding human key factors that influence information processing related to BCI performance across various user groups or BCI systems is one way to address this challenge. Finding superordinate human factors allows discussing how these factors are associated with the user traits and states previously investigated (see Figure 2) and as illustrated in Figure 2 how to implement them as core dimensions in BCI applications for healthy and vulnerable BCI users. Additionally, as illustrated in Figure 4, this may enable the development and proposal of models and study protocols for their standardized assessments across studies (e.g., for designing human factor models; e.g., Jeunet et al., 2017).

Taking into account the results of the studies mentioned above, the following sections will show that the user’s self-concept and the tasks’ and stimuli’s self-relevance may be fundamental superordinate factors of theoretical and empirical relevance for BCI research (for illustrations, see Figures 24).

6 Self-relevance and the user’s self-concept as potential superordinate human factors in the BCI setting

As a theoretical construct, self-relevance encompasses a number of the affective, cognitive, and motivational human factors examined in earlier BCI studies. Moreover, theoretically and neuroscientifically, self-relevance is a fundamental dimension of information processing (Schmitz and Johnson, 2007). All incoming sensory information is spontaneously appraised according to its relevance for the perceiver. This self-relevant processing is performed during a cascade of sequential appraisal checks including self-referential processing (Scherer et al., 2001; Schmitz and Johnson, 2007). Thus, by inducing self-referential processing, self-relevance determines which stimulus input and information is capturing the user’s attention and which input will be further processed, elaborated, consolidated, and more easily recalled from memory in subsequent sessions and stimulus exposures (e.g., Conway, 2005 or Scherer, 2013). Furthermore, the degree to which an individual’s intentions, behaviors, and self-relevant goals are pursued with effort, ambition, or pleasure is determined by the level of self-relevance induced by tasks or stimuli (Conway, 2005). Therefore, self-relevance can have positive effects on cognition, on emotion and mood and on performance (see Cunningham and Turk, 2017 for an overview).

Thus, enhancing the self-relevance of the stimuli, trials, instructions, and tasks in BCI may improve the users’ performance accuracy across different BCI systems. This can include passive as well as active or reactive BCI systems such as the P300-BCI or the SMR-BCI, respectively.

6.1 Enhancing self-relevance via stimulus-driven or top-down triggered processing

Several strategies might increase the self-relevance of a BCI setting (see Figure 2 for an example). One way of increasing self-relevance is through stimulus-driven processing that is bottom-up and initiates implicit, self-relevant and self-referential appraisal processes (Schmitz and Johnson, 2007). For example, the presentation of self-referential stimuli, like the subject’s own name, can initiate self-referential processing and modulate the P300 amplitude (e.g., Tacikowski et al., 2011; for reviews see e.g., Knyazev, 2013) relevant in many BCI studies for classification. Self-referential stimuli such as the subject’s own name spontaneously guide the user’s attention, motivation or action without requiring the user’s explicit attention or intention. On the other hand, explicit task-instructed processing that is top-down driven can enhance self-relevance (e.g., Schmitz and Johnson, 2007). Top-down driven explicit self-relevant processing can be induced via task instructions that promote self-referential processing, e.g., by asking participants to process the task and stimuli from a first person perspective. First-person processing evokes a sense of agency and ownership (for an overview see e.g., Northoff et al., 2006; Schmitz and Johnson, 2007) thereby increasing self-relevance. By using self-referential stimuli or self-referential task instructions, self-relevance increases because BCI users can relate stimuli, objects, feelings, thoughts, and actions to themselves and experience themselves as agents and owners of the task and actions. In contrast to stimulus-driven processing, task-instructed self-referential processing requires introspection (e.g., see Northoff et al., 2006; Northoff, 2016). Therefore, inducing self-relevance by stimulus-driven self-referential processing could be a strategy to improve BCI performance even among severely impaired end-users of a BCI (see e.g., Magliacano et al., 2019 and section “6.2 Implementing self-relevance in the BCI setting: first evidence from P300-BCI and SMR-BCI and future examples”). On the contrary, inducing self-relevance by task instructed self-referential processing requires a BCI user who can reflect upon the self as an owner of experience (e.g., see Northoff et al., 2006; Northoff, 2016).

6.2 Implementing self-relevance in the BCI setting: first evidence from P300-BCI and SMR-BCI and future examples

Some previous BCI studies have already made use of the benefits of self-relevance (for an overview see Figure 3). For example, Neuper et al. (2009) used a SMR-BCI in which some sessions comprised self-relevant (feedback with a grasping hand) vs. abstract feedback (moving bar). BCI performance did not differ between sessions in these studies. Therefore, to increase the self-relevance of the BCI setting for the user, recent BCI studies have replaced the flanker stimuli (indicating left or right hand movements) in the mental imagery training conditions in SMR-BCIs by self-referential stimuli such as the user’s own virtual arm (e.g., Škola and Liarokapis, 2018; Ziadeh et al., 2021). The results of these studies found that healthy young adults performed the SMR-BCI equally well in both conditions (own arm vs. flankers). Furthermore, replacing the flankers by the virtual arm in the study by Ziadeh et al. (2021) elicited sense of agency and sense of ownership among the users. This, according to the authors, prevented loss of motivation across 30 trials of repeated motor activation by mental imagery. Likewise, the study by Škola and Liarokapis (2018) found no decline in accuracy measures during the different sessions. Moreover, the participants’ sense of ownership induced during the self-relevant conditions correlated with the modulation of the sensorimotor rhythm used for BCI classification. Stimulus-driven self-relevant processing has been included in P300-BCIs as well. A recent study by Lu et al. (2020) presented self-referential cues and primes such as the user’s own vs. others faces or names during spelling to increase the self-relevance of the spelling sessions and spelling accuracy. Yet another study by Dong et al. (2015) asked healthy BCI users to read self-relevant sentences to increase self-relevance. The classification accuracy of the BCI algorithms (using time-frequency measures of the EEG) improved significantly. A BCI classification accuracy of 75.5% was obtained. This allowed successful discrimination between two different implicit intentions of the user related to agreement or disagreement (Dong et al., 2015).

Taken together and as summarized in Figure 3, as far as stimulus-driven self-relevant processing is concerned, most previous BCI research have focused on increasing self-relevance by triggering self-referential processing implicitly by means of self-referential and highly salient and familiar stimuli such as the subjects own name, own face, voice, or own body parts. This included studies with severely impaired patient groups such as patients with disorders of consciousness as potential user groups of BCIs (e.g., Laureys et al., 2007; Perrin et al., 2015; Hammer et al., 2018; Kempny et al., 2018; for an overview, see Magliacano et al., 2019).

Stimulus-induced self-relevant tasks, that have so far not been used in a BCI setting but seem promising, are tasks that use visual or auditory self-referential vs. other-referential first person vs. third person stimuli, such as personal or possessive pronouns (e.g., Blume and Herbert, 2014; Herbert et al., 2016; Zhou et al., 2019). Due to their simplicity, these paradigms could be a means of facilitating implicit self-referential processing during the BCI training sessions to improve or avoid loss of agency and ownership during spelling or motor imagery. Moreover, promoting self-referential processing of the BCI user, for example, with self-referential instructions or self-referential primes (e.g., personal pronouns or the user’s own name) could be of particular relevance when sense of agency is severely compromised. This can be the case, when controlling a robotic or virtual arm, hand or limb via BCIs (e.g., Caspar et al., 2021; for a discussion of agency in SMR-BCI, see Jeunet et al., 2016).

A number of previous BCI studies explicitly instructed the users to initiate top-down driven self-relevant processing. For example, self-referential instructions that ask participants to imagine actions during motor imagery from a self-referential first person perspective have proven superior for BCI performance in the SMR-BCI over non-self-referential instruction or imagination from a self-detached third person perspective (e.g., Neuper et al., 2005). In line with these results, Placidi et al. (2018) proposed a BCI user interface in which the task is to evoke emotions by instructing the users to imagine self-induced autobiographical events with affective impact. Once the different emotions are detected, the self-referential processing task could be used to train the classifiers to improve communication in severely disabled users with language and cognitive deficits (Placidi et al., 2018).

Strong support that self-relevance – implicit, stimulus-driven or explicit and top-down controlled – plays a superordinate role in BCI designs comes from studies that included a resting state recording. As discussed in section “4.3 Role of assessment methods,” indicators derived from the so-called resting state paradigm have proven one of the strongest predictors for high performance accuracy in SMR-BCIs. Previous BCI studies revealed that there exists a relationship between the resting-state brain activity and SMR-BCI performance (Blankertz et al., 2010; Zhang et al., 2015; Kwon et al., 2020). In line with this, individual differences in the measures of SMR in the resting state (rs-SMR) have been found to predict control over SMR power during the SMR training sessions (e.g., Reichert et al., 2015). In the so-called resting state paradigm, the participants are instructed to relax with eyes open or eyes closed to let their mind wander. Research using these task settings (resting state with eyes open/closed) have shown that the resting state evokes significant alterations in the activity of the brain’s attention-, default-, and salience networks. This includes cortical midline structures (CMS); i.e., brain structures and networks that change activity during self-referential processing (Northoff et al., 2006; Sheline et al., 2009; Costumero et al., 2020). Moreover, the resting state modulates activity in the sensorimotor network (SMN) (Biswal et al., 1995) that comprises the supplementary motor area and the primary motor cortex engaged in the execution of voluntary movements. In addition, resting state conditions facilitate salient stimulus processing, which improves cognitive performance, attention, and memory processes (Kober et al., 2015). Moreover, EEG-neurofeedback training based on oscillations stemming from CMS structures that modulate self-referential processing already proved to be a promising tool to enhance self-regulation abilities in patients with ALS in the late stage of the disease (e.g., Fomina et al., 2016).

These results could explain why a resting state predicts the outcome measures in SMR-BCIs in previous studies. The results suggest that a resting state is a state of increased self-referential processing that modifies neurophysiological activity in specific brain networks of particular relevance for BCI applications such as SMR-BCI. Many recent studies meanwhile incorporate a resting state measure in the BCI training in their study guidelines. These studies include designs with motor imagery and SMR-BCI (for recent suggestions see Zhang et al., 2020) or with P300 spelling (e.g., Won et al., 2022).

6.3 Assessing the self of the user: self-concept, its relationship to user traits examined in previous BCI studies and its relevance for future BCI studies

Self-relevance is a theoretical concept closely related to the self of a person and the person’s self-concept. However, self-relevance characterizes a processing state of information (see section “6.1 Enhancing self-relevance via stimulus-driven or top-down triggered processing” for details) that changes in accordance with the stimuli, task, instruction, or the circumstances at hand (e.g., the user’s mood, motivation, or behaviorally self-relevant goals pertinent to the self). In contrast to self-relevance, the self-concept is related to the concept of who we are including a person’s self and identity. Theoretically, the self-concept is multidimensional, comprises multiple facets and encompasses all processes, ideas, attitudes, and beliefs a person may form about the own identity, abilities, experiences, and behaviors. This includes beliefs about personality traits, physical characteristics and affective and cognitive self-judgments (Baumeister, 1999; McLeod, 2008). Therefore, a person’s self-concept can be theoretically considered as a superordinate human factor that predicts a person’s behavior across situations similar to personality traits. There exist several questionnaires that measure a person’s self-concept and that assess self-relevance and self-referentiality on the individual level as a habit or trait. The Personal Self-Concept Questionnaire (PSQ; Goñi et al., 2011) for example assesses a person’s self-referentiality on the dimensions of self-fulfillment, autonomy, honesty, and emotionality (emotional self-concept). Other questionnaires include self-esteem questionnaires such as the Rosenberg self-esteem scale (Rosenberg, 1965). This scale assesses the degree of the person’s self-confidence vs. self-depreciation. In addition, implicit self-report measures for the assessment of a person’s self-concept exist. The Twenty-Statement Test (TST; Watkins et al., 1997) for example allows cross-cultural assessment of self-attitudes. In the TST participants are asked to answer the question “Who am I.” A modified short version of the TST has recently been successfully used in a survey study in combination with machine learning methods to investigate changes in the self-concept and the mental state and academic performance among healthy adults (students) during the COVID-19 pandemic (Herbert et al., 2021).

Recent research, following previous studies asking BCI users about their experiences using a BCI, showed that many BCI users value achieving independence, autonomy, and social participation by using BCIs. Of additional relevance for the BCI users are the experience of happiness or joy and creativity as well as the consideration of BCIs as a means of self-expression (for a summary and overview see e.g., Kögel et al., 2020). Moreover, several previous BCI studies discussed in the previous sections of this manuscript have investigated facets of the BCI user’s personality. This has shown that scoring high or low on measures of current motivation or on personality traits related to empathy or emotional stability and neuroticism can affect BCI performance across BCIs positively or negatively. Differences in a person’s self-concept and self-esteem have been discussed in the literature as predictors of competence, cognitive performance, happiness, interpersonal success, and even of healthier lifestyles (for a discussion, see e.g., Baumeister et al., 2003). Therefore, it is likely that the user’s self-concept modulates the autonomy and happiness achievement goals of a BCI user that have influenced BCI performance in previous BCI studies (see section “4 Psychological human factors and their impact on BCI performance: evidence from previous studies using P300-BCI or SMR-BCI among healthy and disabled user groups” and Table 1 for a detailed summary).

For the reasons stated above, it would be interesting to examine the relationships between the self-concept of BCI users and personality traits used in previous BCI studies, including the users’ beliefs about their performance competencies. Furthermore, self-concept scales and self-esteem scales can be a reliable way to measure the BCI users’ beliefs about and confidence in their own abilities. This can be especially useful in adjusting for overestimation and underestimation biases in self-enhancement motivation and cognitive performance (Alicke and Sedikides, 2009). Identifying whether BCI users overestimate or underestimate their BCI performance could support giving accurate performance feedback during BCI training sessions (for a discussion of positive feedback, see e.g., Lotte et al., 2018). This could help prevent user over- or underestimation biases as well as loss of motivation such as mastery confidence and loss of self-regulation, all of which can eventually decrease BCI efficiency.

Furthermore, previous studies indicate that healthy individuals typically exhibit optimistic biases as well as positively biased affective evaluation of themselves (e.g., Sharot, 2011). These self-positivity biases can be measured experimentally. For example healthy people often display a tendency to prioritize positive over negative trait adjectives when asked to ascribe these to the self (Matlin, 2016). Studies using EEG have also discovered this bias. For instance, when healthy participants are processing rapidly presented emotional adjectives in rapid serial visual presentation designs (Herbert et al., 2008), or read emotional and neutral first vs. third person pronoun-noun pairs (Herbert et al., 2011) or social vignettes that are either self- or other-relevant (Fields and Kuperberg, 2016). In the EEG, self-referential processing of positive linguistic input modulates a number of event-related brain potentials including early and late ERPs. Later ERP modulation includes modulation of the N400 potential during reading of positive vignettes or of late positive potentials (LPP) during processing of self-related positive word and pronoun-noun pairs. The modulation patterns suggest that positive information is better semantically integrated and more deeply elaborated compared to negative or neutral input that is unrelated to the self.

Theoretically, a positive emotional self-concept in terms of positively biased self-views, high self-esteem, high self-positivity, and positive mood have a positive psychological benefit for a person. Self-positivity and positive mood promote cognitive processes by broadening attention, by fostering motivation, self-esteem, and performance (for a discussion, see Fredrickson, 2004). Patients with mental disorders and depressed mood can possess compromised self-views, low self-positivity or negativity biases, and self-esteem. Moreover, self-evaluations can extend from human–human to human–computer interaction (Moon, 2003; Weis and Herbert, 2022).

Therefore, promoting positive self-views in BCI users during the training sessions should help to promote accurate BCI performance, given the negative relationships that have been found in the past between empathy or emotional stability and BCI performance. Integrating self-concept measures in future BCI studies and examining their relationship with the user traits and states investigated in previous BCI studies seems mandatory to monitor the self-serving attributions of the BCI user that could modulate BCI performance positively or negatively. As outlined above and as illustrated in Figures 3, 4, short assessment tools using free-format responses or self-other referential words and vignettes could be a promising and time-efficient tool to determine the BCI user’s self-concept including the users’ self-views and self-attributions in addition to standardized self-evaluation questionnaires.

7 Conclusion and recommendations for the future

Previous research targeting the user’s traits and states and investigating their impact on the BCI user’s performance during P300-BCI or SMR-BCIs have provided evidence that a considerable number of user traits and states can influence BCI performance of healthy users and patient users. Based on that evidence, the proposal to search for key human factors seems promising because as explained in detail in the above sections, it could integrate many different user traits and states. Self-relevance could be such a potential key human factor for BCIs in future because as argued in this manuscript, the self-relevance of stimuli and task might influence information processing of the BCI user during multiple stages by increasing attention, agency, and ownership. Moreover, incorporating measures of the user’s self-concept might help promoting positive self-views and avoiding negative self-views of the user about their BCI performance. As outlined in this manuscript, the research on the impact of self-relevance and the self-concept on BCI performance is still in its early stages. This manuscript and the hypothetical model shown in Figure 4 may serve as a catalyst for further research highlighting the potential of these two constructs as important human factors to be thoroughly investigated in future BCI studies with regard to BCI performance in P300-BCIs or SMR-BCIs as well as beyond. From the perspective of BCI applications and BCI engineering, this might support the development of self-referential BCIs for a broad range of users. Due to their self-referentiality, self-referential BCIs could closely fit to the individual user with respect to the user’s needs, traits, and states and improve BCI literacy and BCI accuracy of BCI users beyond the current level. Self-referential BCIs may include the application of self-paced BCIs (e.g., Mason et al., 2006; Scherer et al., 2007) or passive BCIs because these BCIs need to autonomously decide whether the ongoing brain activity is intended by the user (healthy or disabled) or not (self-paced BCIs) and reflecting the user’s traits or states (passive BCI).

Author contributions

CH: Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing, Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Validation, Visualization, Formal analysis, Project administration, Resources.

Funding

The author declares financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. The study is funded by the budgetary resources of the Department of Applied Emotion and Motivation Psychology for Publication, Ulm University, Germany for Open Access Fund.

Conflict of interest

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The author declared that they were an editorial board member of Frontiers, at the time of submission. This had no impact on the peer review process and the final decision.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Ahn, M., and Jun, S. C. (2015). Performance variation in motor imagery brain–computer interface: a brief review. J. Neurosci. Methods 243, 103–110.

Google Scholar

Ahn, M., Cho, H., Ahn, S., and Jun, S. C. (2013). High theta and low alpha powers may be indicative of BCI-illiteracy in motor imagery. PLoS One 8:e80886.

Google Scholar

Ahn, M., Cho, H., Ahn, S., and Jun, S. C. (2018). User’s self-prediction of performance in motor imagery brain–computer interface. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 12:59. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00059

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Alicke, M. D., and Sedikides, C. (2009). Self-enhancement and self-protection: what they are and what they do. Eur. Rev. Soc. Psychol. 20, 1–48.

Google Scholar

Allison, B. Z., and Neuper, C. (2010). “Could anyone use a BCI?,” in Brain-computer interfaces: applying our minds to human-computer interaction, eds D. S. Tan and A. Nijholt (London: Scholars Portal), 35–54.

Google Scholar

Alonso-Valerdi, L. M., and Mercado-García, V. R. (2019). Towards designing brain-computer interfaces in terms of user-profiles, neurophysiological factors and user experience. Rev. Mex. Ing. Bioméd. 40, 1–12. doi: 10.17488/RMIB.40.2.3

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Al-Taleb, M. K. H., Purcell, M., Fraser, M., Petric-Gray, N., and Vuckovic, A. (2019). Home used, patient self-managed, brain-computer interface for the management of central neuropathic pain post spinal cord injury: usability study. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 16, 1–24. doi: 10.1186/s12984-019-0588-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Balcombe, L., and de Leo, D. (2022). Human-computer interaction in digital mental health. Informatics 9:14. doi: 10.3390/informatics9010014

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Baumeister, R. F. (1999). “Self-concept, self-esteem, and identity,” in Personality: contemporary theory and research, eds V. J. Derlega, B. A. Winstead, and W. H. Jones (Chicago, IL: Nelson-Hall Publishers), 339–375.

Google Scholar

Baumeister, R. F., Campbell, J. D., Krueger, J. I., and Vohs, K. D. (2003). Does high self-esteem cause better performance, interpersonal success, happiness, or healthier lifestyles? Psychol. Sci. Public Interest 4, 1–44.

Google Scholar

Baykara, E., Ruf, C. A., Fioravanti, C., Käthner, I., Simon, N., Kleih, S. C., et al. (2016). Effects of training and motivation on auditory P300 brain-computer interface performance. Clin. Neurophysiol. 127, 379–387. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2015.04.054

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Benaroch, C., Jeunet, C., and Lotte, F. (2019). “Are users’ traits informative enough to predict/explain their mental-imagery based BCI performances?,” in Proceedings of the 8th Graz brain-computer interface conference 2019, Sep 2019, Graz.

Google Scholar

Biswal, B., Yetkin, F. Z., Haughton, V. M., and Hyde, J. S. (1995). Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo-planar MRI. Magn. Reson. Med. 34, 537–541. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910340409

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Blankertz, B., Dornhege, G., Krauledat, M., Muller, K. R., Kunzmann, V., Losch, F., et al. (2006). The Berlin Brain-Computer Interface: EEG-based communication without subject training. IEEE Trans. Neural Syst. Rehabil. Eng. 14, 147–152.

Google Scholar

Blankertz, B., Sannelli, C., Halder, S., Hammer, E. M., Kübler, A., Müller, K.-R., et al. (2010). Neurophysiological predictor of SMR-based BCI performance. Neuroimage 51, 1303–1309. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.03.022

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Blume, C., and Herbert, C. (2014). The HisMine-Paradigm: a new paradigm to investigate self-awareness employing pronouns. Soc. Neurosci. 9, 289–299. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2014.886616

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bobrova, E. V., Reshetnikova, V. V., Volkova, K. V., and Frolov, A. A. (2018). Effects of emotional stability on success in learning to control a brain–computer interface. Neurosci. Behav. Physiol. 48, 1114–1119.

Google Scholar

Botrel, L., Acqualagna, L., Blankertz, B., and Kübler, A. (2017). Short progressive muscle relaxation or motor coordination training does not increase performance in a brain-computer interface based on sensorimotor rhythms (SMR). Int. J. Psychophysiol. 121, 29–37.

Google Scholar

Botrel, L., and Kübler, A. (2019). Week-long visuomotor coordination and relaxation trainings do not increase sensorimotor rhythms (SMR) based brain–computer interface performance. Behav. Brain Res. 372:111993. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2019.111993

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Botte-Lecocq, C., Bekaert, M.-H., Vannobel, J.-M., Leclerc, S., and Cabestaing, F. (2014). Considering human factors in BCI experiments: a global approach. J. Eur. Syst. Autom. 48, 283–301. doi: 10.3166/jesa.48.283-301

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Caspar, E. A., De Beir, A., Lauwers, G., Cleeremans, A., and Vanderborght, B. (2021). How using brain-machine interfaces influences the human sense of agency. PLoS One 16:e0245191. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0245191

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Conway, M. A. (2005). Memory and the self? J. Memory Lang. 53, 594–628. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2005.08.005

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Costumero, V., Bueichekú, E., Adrián-Ventura, J., and Ávila, C. (2020). Opening or closing eyes at rest modulates the functional connectivity of V1 with default and salience networks. Sci. Rep. 10:9137. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-66100-y

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cunningham, S. J., and Turk, D. J. (2017). A review of self-processing biases in cognition. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 70, 987–995.

Google Scholar

Dehais, F., Ladouce, S., Darmet, L., Nong, T. V., Ferraro, G., et al. (2022). Dual passive reactive brain-computer interface: A novel approach to human-machine symbiosis. Front. Neuroergon. 3:824780. doi: 10.3389/fnrgo.2022.824780

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

De Raad, B., and Mlacic, B. (2015). Big five factor model, theory and structure. Int. Encycl. Soc. Behav. Sci. 2, 559–566.

Google Scholar

Diya, S. Z., Prorna, R. A., Rahman, I. I., Islam, A. B., and Islam, M. N. (2019). “Applying brain-computer interface technology for evaluation of user experience in playing games,” in Proceedings of the international conference on electrical, computer and communication engineering (ECCE), (Piscataway, NJ: IEEE), 1–6.

Google Scholar

Dong, S. Y., Kim, B. K., and Lee, S. Y. (2015). EEG-based classification of implicit intention during self-relevant sentence reading. IEEE Trans. Cybern. 46, 2535–2542. doi: 10.1109/TCYB.2015.2479240

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Edlinger, G., Allison, B. Z., and Guger, C. (2015). “How many people can use a BCI system?,” in Clinical systems neuroscience, eds K. Kansaku, L. Cohen, and N. Birbaumer (Tokyo: Springer), 33–66.

Google Scholar

Fields, E. C., and Kuperberg, G. R. (2016). Dynamic effects of self-relevance and task on the neural processing of emotional words in context. Front. Psychol. 6:2003. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.02003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fomina, T., Lohmann, G., Erb, M., Ethofer, T., Schölkopf, B., and Grosse-Wentrup, M. (2016). Self-regulation of brain rhythms in the precuneus: a novel BCI paradigm for patients with ALS. J. Neural Eng. 13:066021.

Google Scholar

Fredrickson, B. L. (2004). The broaden–and–build theory of positive emotions. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. Series B Biol. Sci. 359, 1367–1377.

Google Scholar

Goñi, E., Madariaga, J. M., Axpe, I., and Goñi, A. (2011). Structure of the personal self-concept (PSC) questionnaire. Int. J. Psychol. Psychol. Ther. 11, 509–522.

Google Scholar

Graimann, B., Allison, B. Z., and Pfurtscheller, G. (Eds.). (2010). “Brain-computer interfaces: Revolutionizing human-computer interaction,” in Springer science & business media.

Google Scholar

Guger, C., Edlinger, G., Harkam, W., Niedermayer, I., and Pfurtscheller, G. (2003). How many people are able to operate an EEG-based brain-computer interface (BCI)? IEEE Trans. Neural Syst. Rehabil. Eng. 11, 145–147. doi: 10.1109/TNSRE.2003.814481

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hagedorn, L. J., Leeuwis, N., and Alimardani, M. (2021). “Prediction of inefficient BCI users based on cognitive skills and personality traits,” in Proceedings of the international conference on neural information processing, eds T. Mantoro, M. Lee, M. A. Ayu, K. W. Wong, and A. N. Hidayanto (Cham: Springer), 81–89.

Google Scholar

Halder, S., Agorastos, D., Veit, R., Hammer, E. M., Lee, S., Varkuti, B., et al. (2011). Neural mechanisms of brain–computer interface control. Neuroimage 55, 1779–1790. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.01.021

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Halder, S., Ruf, C. A., Furdea, A., Pasqualotto, E., De Massari, D., van der Heiden, L., et al. (2013). Prediction of P300 BCI aptitude in severe motor impairment. PLoS One 8:e76148. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0076148

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hammer, E. M., Halder, S., Blankertz, B., Sannelli, C., Dickhaus, T., Kleih, S., et al. (2012). Psychological predictors of SMR-BCI performance. Biol. Psychol. 89, 80–86.

Google Scholar

Hammer, E. M., Halder, S., Kleih, S. C., and Kübler, A. (2018). Psychological predictors of visual and auditory P300 brain-computer interface performance. Front. Neurosci. 12:307. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2018.00307

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hammer, E. M., Kaufmann, T., Kleih, S. C., Blankertz, B., and Kübler, A. (2014). Visuo-motor coordination ability predicts performance with brain-computer interfaces controlled by modulation of sensorimotor rhythms (SMR). Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:574. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00574

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Herbert, C., Blume, C., and Northoff, G. (2016). Can we distinguish an “I” and “ME” during listening?—an event-related EEG study on the processing of first and second person personal and possessive pronouns. Self Identity 15, 120–138.

Google Scholar

Herbert, C., El Bolock, A., and Abdennadher, S. (2021). How do you feel during the COVID-19 pandemic? A survey using psychological and linguistic self-report measures, and machine learning to investigate mental health, subjective experience, personality, and behaviour during the COVID-19 pandemic among university students. BMC Psychol. 9:90. doi: 10.1186/s40359-021-00574-x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Herbert, C., Herbert, B. M., Ethofer, T., and Pauli, P. (2011). His or mine? The time course of self–other discrimination in emotion processing. Soc. Neurosci. 6, 277–288.

Google Scholar

Herbert, C., Junghofer, M., and Kissler, J. (2008). Event related potentials to emotional adjectives during reading. Psychophysiology 45, 487–498.

Google Scholar

Horowitz, A. J., Guger, C., and Korostenskaja, M. (2021). What external variables affect sensorimotor rhythm brain-computer interface (SMR-BCI) performance? HCA Healthc. J. Med. 2, 143–162. doi: 10.36518/2689-0216.1188

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jeunet, C., Lotte, F., Batail, J.-M., Philip, P., and Micoulaud Franchi, J.-A. (2018). Using recent BCI literature to deepen our understanding of clinical neurofeedback: a short review. Neuroscience 378, 225–233. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2018.03.013

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jeunet, C., N’Kaoua, B., and Lotte, F. (2016). Advances in user-training for mental-imagery-based BCI control: psychological and cognitive factors and their neural correlates. Prog. Brain Res. 228, 3–35. doi: 10.1016/bs.pbr.2016.04.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jeunet, C., N’Kaoua, B., Subramanian, S., Hachet, M., and Lotte, F. (2015). Predicting mental imagery-based BCI performance from personality, cognitive profile and neurophysiological patterns. PLoS One 10:e0143962. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0143962

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jeunet, C., N’Kaoua, B., and Lotte, F. (2017). “Towards a cognitive model of MI-BCI user training,” in Proceedings of the international Graz BCI conference, Graz.

Google Scholar

Jiang, X., Lopez, E., Stieger, J. R., Greco, C. M., and He, B. (2021). Effects of long-term meditation practices on sensorimotor rhythm-based brain-computer interface learning. Front. Neurosci. 14:584971. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2020.584971

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kadosh, K. C., and Staunton, G. (2019). A systematic review of the psychological factors that influence neurofeedback learning outcomes. Neuroimage 185, 545–555. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.10.021

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Käthner, I., Wriessnegger, S. C., Müller-Putz, G. R., Kübler, A., and Halder, S. (2014). Effects of mental workload and fatigue on the P300, alpha and theta band power during operation of an ERP (P300) brain–computer interface. Biol. Psychol. 102, 118–129. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.07.014

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kempny, A. M., James, L., Yelden, K., Duport, S., Farmer, S. F., Playford, E., et al. (2018). Patients with a severe prolonged Disorder of Consciousness can show classical EEG responses to their own name compared with others’ names. Neuroimage Clin. 19, 311–319. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2018.04.027

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kleih, S. C., and Kübler, A. (2013). Empathy, motivation, and P300 BCI performance. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:642. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00642

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kleih, S. C., and Kübler, A. (2015). “Psychological factors influencing brain-computer interface (BCI) performance,” in Proceedings of the big data analytics for human-centric systems: the international conference on systems, man, and cybernetics (SMC): 09. Dez October 2015 City University of Hong Kong, (Piscataway, NJ: IEEE), 3192–3196.

Google Scholar

Kleih, S. C., Kaufmann, T., Zickler, C., Halder, S., Leotta, F., Cincotti, F., et al. (2011a). Out of the frying pan into the fire—the P300-based BCI faces real-world challenges. Prog. Brain Res. 194, 27–46. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-53815-4.00019-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kleih, S. C., Nijboer, F., Halder, S., and Kübler, A. (2010). Motivation modulates the P300 amplitude during brain–computer interface use. Clin. Neurophysiol. 121, 1023–1031. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2010.01.034

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kleih, S. C., Riccio, A., Mattia, D., Schreuder, M., Tangermann, M., Zickler, C., et al. (2011b). Motivation affects performance in a P300 Brain Computer Interface. Int. J. Bioelectromagn. 13, 46–47.

Google Scholar

Kleih-Dahms, S. C., Botrel, L., and Kübler, A. (2021). The influence of motivation and emotion on sensorimotor rhythm-based brain-computer interface performance. Psychophysiology 58:e13832. doi: 10.1111/psyp.13832

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Knyazev, G. G. (2013). EEG correlates of self-referential processing. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:264. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00264

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kober, S. E., Witte, M., Stangl, M., Väljamäe, A., Neuper, C., and Wood, G. (2015). Shutting down sensorimotor interference unblocks the networks for stimulus processing: an SMR neurofeedback training study. Clin. Neurophysiol. 126, 82–95. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2014.03.031

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kögel, J., Jox, R. J., and Friedrich, O. (2020). What is it like to use a BCI?–insights from an interview study with brain-computer interface users. BMC Med. Ethics 21:2. doi: 10.1186/s12910-019-0442-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kübler, A. (2020). The history of BCI: from a vision for the future to real support for personhood in people with locked-in syndrome. Neuroethics 13, 163–180. doi: 10.1007/s12152-019-09409-4

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kübler, A., Holz, E. M., Riccio, A., Zickler, C., Kaufmann, T., Kleih, S. C., et al. (2014). The user-centered design as novel perspective for evaluating the usability of BCI-controlled applications. PLoS One 9:e112392. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0112392

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kübler, A., Müller-Putz, G., and Mattia, D. (2015). User-centred design in brain-computer interface research and development. Ann. Phys. Rehabil. Med. 58, 312–314. doi: 10.1016/j.rehab.2015.06.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kübler, A., Nijboer, F., and Kleih, S. (2020). Hearing the needs of clinical users. Handb. Clin. Neurol. 168, 353–368. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-63934-9.00026-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kwon, M., Cho, H., Won, K., Ahn, M., and Jun, S. C. (2020). Use of both eyes-open and eyes-closed resting states may yield a more robust predictor of motor imagery BCI performance. Electronics 9:690. doi: 10.3390/electronics9040690

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Laureys, S., Perrin, F., and Brédart, S. (2007). Self-consciousness in non-communicative patients. Conscious. Cogn. 16, 722–741. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2007.04.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, M., Yoon, J. G., and Lee, S. W. (2020). Predicting motor imagery performance from resting-state EEG using dynamic causal modeling. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 14:321. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.00321

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Leeuwis, N., Paas, A., and Alimardani, M. (2021). Vividness of visual imagery and personality impact motor-imagery brain computer interfaces. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 15:634748. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2021.634748

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lotte, F., Bougrain, L., Cichocki, A., Clerc, M., Congedo, M., Rakotomamonjy, A., et al. (2018). A review of classification algorithms for EEG-based brain–computer interfaces: a 10 year update. J. Neural Eng. 15:031005. doi: 10.1088/1741-2552/aab2f2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lu, Z., Li, Q., Gao, N., and Yang, J. (2020). The self-face paradigm improves the performance of the P300-speller system. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 13:93. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2019.00093

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Luauté, J., Morlet, D., and Mattout, J. (2015). BCI in patients with disorders of consciousness: clinical perspectives. Ann. Phys. Rehabil. Med. 58, 29–34. doi: 10.1016/j.rehab.2014.09.015

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lyu, X., Ding, P., Li, S., Dong, Y., Su, L., Zhao, L., et al. (2023). Human factors engineering of BCI: an evaluation for satisfaction of BCI based on motor imagery. Cogn. Neurodyn. 17, 105–118.

Google Scholar

Magliacano, A., De Bellis, F., Galvao-Carmona, A., Estraneo, A., and Trojano, L. (2019). Can salient stimuli enhance responses in disorders of consciousness? A systematic review. Curr. Neurol. Neurosci. Rep. 19, 1–10. doi: 10.1007/s11910-019-1018-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mason, S. G., and Birch, G. E. (2003). A general framework for brain-computer interface design. IEEE Trans. Neural Syst. Rehabil. Eng. 11, 70–85. doi: 10.1109/TNSRE.2003.810426

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mason, S., Kronegg, J., Huggins, J., Fatourechi, M., and Schlögl, A. (2006). Evaluating the performance of self-paced brain-computer interface technology. Technical report. Vancouver, BC: Neil Squire Society.

Google Scholar

Matlin, M. W. (2016). “Pollyanna principle,” in Cognitive illusions: intriguing phenomena in thinking, judgment and memory, ed. R. F. Pohl (London: Routledge), 315–335.

Google Scholar

McLeod, S. A. (2008). Self concept. Simply psychology. Available online at: http://www.simplypsychology.org/self-concept.html (accessed June 3, 2013).

Google Scholar

Moon, Y. (2003). Don’t blame the computer: when self-disclosure moderates the self-serving bias. J. Consum. Psychol. 13, 125–137.

Google Scholar

Neuper, C., Scherer, R., Reiner, M., and Pfurtscheller, G. (2005). Imagery of motor actions: differential effects of kinesthetic and visual–motor mode of imagery in single-trial EEG. Cogn. Brain Res. 25, 668–677. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.08.014

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Neuper, C., Scherer, R., Wriessnegger, S., and Pfurtscheller, G. (2009). Motor imagery and action observation: modulation of sensorimotor brain rhythms during mental control of a brain–computer interface. Clin. Neurophysiol. 120, 239–247. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2008.11.015

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nijboer, F., Birbaumer, N., and Kübler, A. (2010). The influence of psychological state and motivation on brain-computer interface performance in patients with amyotrophic lateral sclerosis - a longitudinal study. Front. Neurosci. 4:55. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2010.00055

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nijboer, F., Furdea, A., Gunst, I., Mellinger, J., McFarland, D. J., Birbaumer, N., et al. (2008). An auditory brain-computer interface (BCI). J. Neurosci. Methods 167, 43–50. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2007.02.009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nijholt, A., Contreras-Vidal, J. L., Jeunet, C., and Väljamäe, A. (2022). Editorial: brain-computer interfaces for non-clinical (home, sports, art, entertainment, education, well-being) applications. Front. Comput. Sci. 4:860619. doi: 10.3389/fcomp.2022.860619

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Northoff, G. (2016). Is the self a higher-order or fundamental function of the brain? The “basis model of self-specificity” and its encoding by the brain’s spontaneous activity. Cogn. Neurosci. 7, 203–222. doi: 10.1080/17588928.2015.1111868

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Northoff, G., Heinzel, A., De Greck, M., Bermpohl, F., Dobrowolny, H., and Panksepp, J. (2006). Self-referential processing in our brain—a meta-analysis of imaging studies on the self. Neuroimage 31, 440–457.

Google Scholar

Perrin, F., Castro, M., Tillmann, B., and Luauté, J. (2015). Promoting the use of personally relevant stimuli for investigating patients with disorders of consciousness. Front. Psychol. 6:1102. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01102

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pillette, L., Roc, A., N’kaoua, B., and Lotte, F. (2021). Experimenters’ influence on mental-imagery based brain-computer interface user training. Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud. 149:102603.

Google Scholar

Placidi, G., Polsinelli, M., Spezialetti, M., Cinque, L., Di Giamberardino, P., and Iacoviello, D. (2018). Self-induced emotions as alternative paradigm for driving brain–computer interfaces. Comput. Methods Biomech. Biomed. Eng. Imaging Vis. 7, 512–519.

Google Scholar

Powers, J. C., Bieliaieva, K., Wu, S., and Nam, C. S. (2015). The human factors and ergonomics of P300-based brain-computer interfaces. Brain Sci. 5, 318–356. doi: 10.3390/brainsci5030318

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Randolph, A. B. (2012). “Not all created equal: individual technology fit of brain-computer interfaces,” in Proceedings of the 45th Hawaii international conference on system sciences, (Piscataway, NJ: IEEE), 572–578.

Google Scholar

Reichert, J. L., Kober, S. E., Neuper, C., and Wood, G. (2015). Resting-state sensorimotor rhythm (SMR) power predicts the ability to up-regulate SMR in an EEG-instrumental conditioning paradigm. Clin. Neurophysiol. 126, 2068–2077. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2014.09.032

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rheinberg, F., Vollmeyer, R., and Burns, B. D. (2001). Fam: ein fragebogen zur erfassung aktueller motivation in lern-und leistungssituationen (langversion, 2001). Diagnostica 2, 57–66.

Google Scholar

Robinson, N., Thomas, K. P., and Vinod, A. P. (2018). Neurophysiological predictors and spectro-spatial discriminative features for enhancing SMR-BCI. J. Neural Eng. 15:066032. doi: 10.1088/1741-2552/aae597

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Roc, A., Pillette, L., Mladenovic, J., Benaroch, C., N’Kaoua, B., Jeunet, C., et al. (2021). A review of user training methods in brain computer interfaces based on mental tasks. J. Neural Eng. 18:011002. doi: 10.1088/1741-2552/abca17

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosenberg, M. (1965). Rosenberg self-esteem scale. J. Relig. Health 59, 381–398.

Google Scholar

Saha, S., Mamun, K. A., Ahmed, K., Mostafa, R., Naik, G. R., Darvishi, S., et al. (2021). Progress in brain computer interface: challenges and opportunities. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 15:578875.

Google Scholar

Scherer, K. R. (2013). The nature and dynamics of relevance and valence appraisals: theoretical advances and recent evidence. Emot. Rev. 5, 150–162.

Google Scholar

Scherer, K. R., Schorr, A., and Johnstone, T. (Eds.) (2001). Appraisal processes in emotion: theory, methods, research. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Scherer, R., Schloegl, A., Lee, F., Bischof, H., Janša, J., and Pfurtscheller, G. (2007). The self-paced Graz brain-computer interface: methods and applications. Comput. Intell. Neurosci. 2007:79826. doi: 10.1155/2007/79826

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmitz, T. W., and Johnson, S. C. (2007). Relevance to self: a brief review and framework of neural systems underlying appraisal. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 31, 585–596. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.12.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sharot, T. (2011). The optimism bias. Curr. Biol. 21, 941–945.

Google Scholar

Sheline, Y. I., Barch, D. M., Price, J. L., Rundle, M. M., Vaishnavi, S. N., Snyder, A. Z., et al. (2009). The default mode network and self-referential processes in depression. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 1942–1947.

Google Scholar

Škola, F., and Liarokapis, F. (2018). Embodied VR environment facilitates motor imagery brain–computer interface training. Comput. Graph. 75, 59–71. doi: 10.1016/j.cag.2018.05.024

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sprague, S. A., McBee, M. T., and Sellers, E. W. (2016). The effects of working memory on brain–computer interface performance. Clin. Neurophysiol. 127, 1331–1341.

Google Scholar

Tacikowski, P., Jednoróg, K., Marchewka, A., and Nowicka, A. (2011). How multiple repetitions influence the processing of self-, famous and unknown names and faces: an ERP study. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 79, 219–230. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2010.10.010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tan, L. F., Dienes, Z., Jansari, A., and Goh, S. Y. (2014). Effect of mindfulness meditation on brain–computer interface performance. Conscious. Cogn. 23, 12–21.

Google Scholar

Tan, Y. Q., Tan, L. F., Mok, S. Y., and Goh, S. Y. (2015). Effect of short term meditation on brain computer interface performance. J. Med. Bioeng. 4, 135–138. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2022.1019279

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tzdaka, E., Benaroch, C., Jeunet, C., and Lotte, F. (2020). “Assessing the relevance of neurophysiological patterns to predict motor imagery-based BCI users’ performance,” in Proceedings of the IEEE international conference on systems, man, and cybernetics (SMC), (Piscataway, NJ: IEEE), 2490–2495.

Google Scholar

Voinea, G. D., Boboc, R., Gîrbacia, F., and Postelnicu, C. C. (2019). “Technology acceptance of a hybrid brain-computer interface for instruction manual browsing,” in Proceedings of the 14th international conference on virtual learning (ICVL), Bucharest.

Google Scholar

Wang, N., Zeng, W., Shi, Y., and Yan, H. (2017). Brain functional plasticity driven by career experience: a resting-state fMRI study of the seafarer. Front. Psychol. 8:1786. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01786

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Watkins, D., Yau, J., Dahlin, B., and Wondimu, H. (1997). The twenty statements test: some measurement issues. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 28, 626–633.

Google Scholar

Weis, P. P., and Herbert, C. (2022). Do I still like myself? Human-robot collaboration entails emotional consequences. Comput. Hum. Behav. 127:107060.

Google Scholar

Wierzgawa, P., Zapada, D., Wojcik, G. M., and Masiak, J. (2018). Most popular signal processing methods in motor-imagery BCI: a review and meta-analysis. Front. Neuroinform. 12:78. doi: 10.3389/fninf.2018.00078

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Won, K., Kwon, M., Ahn, M., and Jun, S. C. (2022). EEG dataset for RSVP and P300 speller brain-computer interfaces. Sci. Data 9:388. doi: 10.1038/s41597-022-01509-w

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wood, G., and Kober, S. E. (2018). EEG neurofeedback is under strong control of psychosocial factors. Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeedb. 43, 293–300. doi: 10.1007/s10484-018-9407-3

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, H., Yan, H., Yang, Y., Xu, M., Shi, Y., Zeng, W., et al. (2020). Occupational neuroplasticity in the human brain: a critical review and meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 14:215. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.00215

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, R., Li, F., Zhang, T., Yao, D., and Xu, P. (2020). Subject inefficiency phenomenon of motor imagery brain-computer interface: influence factors and potential solutions. Brain Sci. Adv. 6, 224–241.

Google Scholar

Zhang, R., Yao, D., Valdés-Sosa, P. A., Li, F., Li, P., Zhang, T., et al. (2015). Efficient resting-state EEG network facilitates motor imagery performance. J. Neural Eng. 12:66024. doi: 10.1088/1741-2560/12/6/066024

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhou, A., Hu, Y., Lu, X., and Pan, T. (2019). “Acoustic cues for sensory modality and referential vagueness of personal pronouns: me or not? You or not?,” in Proceedings of the international conference on Asian language processing (IALP), (Piscataway, NJ: IEEE), 126–131.

Google Scholar

Ziadeh, H., Gulyas, D., Nielsen, L. D., Lehmann, S., Nielsen, T. B., Kjeldsen, T. K. K., et al. (2021). Mine works better: examining the influence of embodiment in virtual reality on the sense of agency during a binary motor imagery task with a brain-computer interface. Front. Psychol. 12:806424. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.806424

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zich, C., Debener, S., Thoene, A.-K., Chen, L.-C., and Kranczioch, C. (2017). Simultaneous eeg-fnirs reveals how age and feedback affect motor imagery signatures. Neurobiol. Aging 49, 183–197. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2016.10.011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zickler, C., Di Donna, V., Kaiser, V., Al-Khodairy, A., Kleih, S., Kübler, A., et al. (2009). BCI applications for people with disabilities: defining user needs and user requirements. Assist. Technol. Res. Ser. 25, 185–189.

Google Scholar

Keywords: brain–computer interfaces, human factors, self-relevance, self-concept, user traits and states, SMR-BCI, P300-BCI, BCI performance

Citation: Herbert C (2024) Analyzing and computing humans by means of the brain using Brain-Computer Interfaces - understanding the user – previous evidence, self-relevance and the user’s self-concept as potential superordinate human factors of relevance. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 17:1286895. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2023.1286895

Received: 31 August 2023; Accepted: 11 December 2023;
Published: 16 February 2024.

Edited by:

Jiahui Pan, South China Normal University, China

Reviewed by:

Sheng-Hsiou Hsu, University of California, San Diego, United States
Stephen Gordon, DCS Corporation, United States

Copyright © 2024 Herbert. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Cornelia Herbert, Y29ybmVsaWEuaGVyYmVydEB1bmktdWxtLmRl

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.