Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 28 November 2023
Sec. Motor Neuroscience

Understanding voluntary human movement variability through data-driven segmentation and clustering

  • 1Department of Rehabilitation and Movement Sciences, Rutgers University, Newark, NJ, United States
  • 2College of Information and Computer Sciences, University of Massachusetts Amherst, Amherst, MA, United States
  • 3Biosystems Lab, Physics Institute of Universidade Federal da Bahia, Salvador, Brazil

Recently, we proposed a novel approach where movements are decomposed into sub-segments, termed movement elements. This approach, to date, provides a robust construct of how the brain may generate simple as well as complex movements. Here, we address the issue of motor variability during voluntary movements by applying an unsupervised clustering algorithm to group movement elements according to their morphological characteristics. We observed that most movement elements closely match the theoretical bell-shaped velocity profile expected from goal-directed movements. However, for those movement elements that deviate from this theoretical shape, a small number of defined patterns in their shape can be identified. Furthermore, we observed that the axis of the body from which the movement elements are extracted (i.e., medio-lateral, antero-posterior, and vertical) affect the proportion of the movement elements matching the theoretical model. These results provide novel insight into how the nervous system controls voluntary movements and may use variability in movement element properties to explore the environment.

1 Introduction

We recently proposed the concept of movement elements (MEs) (Miranda et al., 2018). This concept is based on the principle that any voluntary movement can be decomposed into one-dimensional point-to-point movements within a Cartesian coordinate system originating at the center of mass. These MEs are related to the minimum-jerk principle and account for the cost of time as proposed by Hoff (1994) and are an extension of the work of Meyer et al. (1982, 1988). While significant additional work is necessary to better understand the importance of MEs in the control of movement, we recently showed that features of MEs are associated with a variety of movements and motor impairments (Miranda et al., 2018; de Lemos Fonseca et al., 2020; Oubre et al., 2020, 2021; Lee et al., 2022; Liu et al., 2022).

Taken together, our prior work highlights that the features extracted from the ME decomposition approach yield important information about the motor system in health and disease. While this sub-movement approach to the evaluation of movement kinematics has been abandoned by most researchers, our prior work clearly indicates the relevance of this approach in characterizing voluntary and involuntary movements, and that the sub-movement approach to the assessment of movement kinematics needs to be revisited as it may have been too hastily dismissed. Here, we investigate the distribution of the variability around the cost function that we have observed in our prior work. We hypothesized that during complex movements, a large proportion of MEs would closely fit Hoff’s cost function while a small portion would exhibit larger deviations. We asked healthy individuals to perform random 3D arm movements (see Supplementary Figure 1 for example) while tracking the movement patterns of their wrist. We observed that a majority of MEs fit Hoff’s cost function as expected, but the MEs that did not could be clustered into a small number of movement patterns across all subjects. This suggests that while most movements are optimized to minimize jerk and time, the variability in movement kinematics is not random but also follows certain patterns.

2 Methods

2.1 Subjects

We recruited 15 healthy individuals (24.7 ± 4.2; 11 male). Inclusion criteria were: (i) no known motor or neurological impairments affecting voluntary upper-limb movements, (ii) aged 18 years and older. The protocol was approved by the University of Massachusetts Amherst Institutional Review Board (#2018-4722) and all subjects provided informed consent.

2.2 Procedure

Subjects were asked to perform continuous 3D random upper-limb movements for approximately 2.5 min, twice. These movements had to span as much of the space as they could without moving their torso (trunk movement was monitored visually by the experimenter and, as such, minimal movements could have occurred, but these would not have impacted the current results because of their relative amplitude compared to the upper-limb movements). As such, no two trials were the same (see Supplementary Figure 1 for an illustration of two trials from two different subjects). The instructions to the subjects were simply: “without moving your trunk, move your arm randomly until we tell you to stop, similar to movements you would do if you were conducting an orchestra.” The intended effect of the guidance was to have very large movements that would span the majority of the subjects’ workspace. In unpublished pilot testing, not providing such an example led to movements that were performed in a significantly restrained space. However, the analytical approach to extract MEs considers the size and velocity of the movement so, while providing this instruction may have altered the movement space covered by the participants, it did not impact the outcomes. Movement patterns of the wrist of their dominant upper-limb were recorded at 100 Hz using a motion capture system (Qualisys AB).

2.3 Data analysis

3D marker trajectories were low-pass filtered at 6 Hz and differentiated to obtain velocity time-series. MEs were then extracted using the previously described technique (Miranda et al., 2018; see Supplementary material for details). In short, the velocity time-series, in each Cartesian axis (antero-posterior, medio-lateral, and rostro-caudal) (see Supplementary Figure 2), was decomposed using zero crossings such that the continuous movement was segmented into MEs of zero initial and terminal velocities. The selection of the coordinate system was originally based on the best fit of the MEs to the theoretical model proposed by Hoff (1994). Since ME decomposition can be sensitive to noise (Miranda et al., 2018), we defined thresholds based on the properties of the motion capture system below which MEs would be discarded. Thresholds for ME displacement (1 mm), and time (80 ms) were implemented based on recordings of a stationary marker. This led to the removal of 9.95% of the initially extracted MEs (11.40% from the x-axis, 14.40% from the y-axis, and 3.55% from the z-axis). This represented 0.52% of the duration of all combined time-series (0.46% of the duration of the x-axis time-series, 0.88% of the duration of the y-axis time-series, and 0.21% of the duration of the z-axis time-series). Each of the remaining ME was then spatially and temporally normalized to enable direct comparison.

Our hypothesis that the majority of MEs would exhibit the bell-shaped velocity profile implies that our dataset should contain a single, dense region of MEs with a similar shape. We identified this region by applying the Density-Based Spatial Clustering of Applications with Noise (DBSCAN) algorithm. Euclidean distance was used as the distance metric between normalized ME vectors because it is a constant multiple of the root mean square error (RMSE). The specific methods used to define the fitness measure developed for this and the parameters used are detailed in the Supplementary material. In short, we defined a fitness measure that constructs a homogeneous cluster containing as many MEs as possible while keeping the variance low (Supplementary Figure 3). Once the dense cluster and the outlier set were identified, we explored whether there were any sub-clusters of movement elements using the k-Means algorithm (see Supplementary material; Figure 1). Since the homogeneous cluster is, by definition, one dense cluster, the k-Means algorithm, in combination with the Davis-Bouldin Index, was implemented on the outliers set and those parameters were then applied to the homogeneous cluster.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Illustration of the average ME patterns from the homogeneous cluster (A) and the outlier cluster (B). The dashed line represents the theoretical shape obtained from Equation 2 (see Supplementary material). The black line represents the average shape of the data included in the graph. The shaded area represents the standard deviation (SD).

2.4 Statistical analyses

Descriptive statistics are in the format mean ± standard deviation. When comparing the percentage of MEs in the different axes as well as the K-value between subjects in the homogeneous cluster and outlier set, normality was assessed using the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test and a one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was performed (OriginPro 2018b). Tukey’s post-hoc was used to assess significance. One-sample t-tests were performed to assess whether the α values obtained were different from the expected theoretical 2/3. Independent sample t-tests were performed to compare mean velocity, duration, and displacement of MEs between the homogeneous cluster and outlier set. The threshold for significance was set to 0.05 a priori.

3 Results

A total of 24,992 MEs meeting thresholds aimed at minimizing noise were extracted from the motion capture data. DBSCAN was used to identify a single, dense cluster of MEs with homogeneous shape (the “homogeneous cluster”), which accounted for about 74% of all MEs (Figure 1 and Supplementary Figure 4). All other MEs that fell outside of this dense cluster (about 26%) were pooled in the “outliers set” (Figure 1 and Supplementary Figure 4).

The average shape of MEs in the homogeneous cluster closely fits the theoretical cost function whether the subjects were pooled (Figure 1A), or their data examined individually (Supplementary Figure 5). In contrast, MEs in the outliers set showed high variability and deviation from the theoretical cost function (Figure 1B). Note also that the shape of MEs did not significantly differ between the beginning and end of the trials (Supplementary Figure 6).

Since the homogeneous cluster was a single highly dense region, it was not possible to identify further sub-clusters using cluster validity indices (Supplementary material). Conversely, the large variability in ME shape observed in the outliers set enabled us to identify an optimal number of sub-clusters using the k-Means algorithm and the Davis-Bouldin Index (k = 4) (Supplementary Figure 3). Using the same value of k to segment the homogeneous cluster and the outliers for comparative analysis, we observed that the shape of MEs within the sub-clusters of the homogeneous cluster showed minimal deviations from the expected model (Figure 2A). However, the sub-clusters of the outliers showed significant deviations from the expected model (Figure 2B).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Illustration of the four sub-clusters from the homogenous cluster (A) and outlier cluster (B). Indicated above each graph is the number of MEs and percentage of total MEs that fall within that specific pattern. The dashed line represents the theoretical shape obtained from Equation 2 (see Supplementary material). The black line represents the average shape of the data included in the graph. The shaded area represents the SD.

The percentage of MEs included within the homogeneous cluster differed across subjects (ranging from 57.7 to 87.3%). Furthermore, when we examined the percentage of MEs that were included from each axis on a subject-by-subject basis, there was always a higher proportion of MEs from the z-axis within the homogeneous cluster (Table 1). On average, 63% of MEs from the x-axis, 65% of MEs from the y-axis, and 90% of MEs from the z-axis were in the homogeneous cluster. A one-way ANOVA demonstrated a significant difference between the average number of MEs included in the homogeneous cluster from each axis (F: 26.296; df: 2; p < 0.05). A Tukey’s post-hoc analysis revealed that the average number of MEs included in the homogeneous cluster from the z-axis was statistically greater than in the x-axis (q: 9.32; p < 0.05) and y-axis (q: 8.37; p < 0.05) while no difference was observed between the x- and y-axis (q: 0.95; p > 0.05). Additional data related to the distribution of MEs based on sub-clusters and axes can be found in the Supplementary material.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Percentage of MEs from each subject that were included in the homogenous cluster per movement axis.

Next, we examined whether the velocity of the MEs within the homogeneous cluster and the outliers set scaled with their displacement using the 2/3 power law previously described (Miranda et al., 2018). We examined whether the mean velocity, duration, and displacement of MEs were different between the homogeneous cluster and outliers set. Independent t-tests showed that mean velocity (t = 64.07; df = 24990; p < 0.05), duration (t = −23.53; df = 24990; p < 0.05), and displacement of MEs (t = 27.85; df = 24990; p < 0.05) were different between the two sets. MEs in the homogeneous cluster had on average, greater mean velocity, shorter duration, and longer displacement than MEs in the outliers set. The scaling exponent (i.e., relationship between mean velocity and displacement) observed for all MEs was 0.709 while the K-value from Equation (2) was 6.32 × 10–4 ± 61.27 × 10–4 (Figure 3A and Supplementary Table 1). The scaling exponent observed for MEs in the homogeneous cluster was 0.703 while their K-value was 1.29 × 10–4 ± 4.53 × 10–4 (Figure 3B and Supplementary Table 1). The scaling exponent observed for MEs in the outliers set was 0.621 while their K-value was 4.89 × 10–3 ± 33.24 × 10–3 (Figure 3C and Supplementary Table 1). These results emphasize that the 2/3 power law relationship between mean velocity and displacement can be observed in the homogeneous cluster and the outliers set even though the average value from the homogeneous cluster deviated slightly from the theoretical 2/3 (t: −3.216; df: 14; p < 0.05) and the average value from the outliers set was also statistically different from 2/3 (t: −16.423; df: 14; p < 0.05). The range of values in the current study are in line with those reported by Miranda et al. (2018). The larger deviation from the theoretical 2/3 relationship observed in the outliers set was expected because the shape of the MEs in this set differs from the expected model. Furthermore, the K-value is approximately 40 times greater in the outliers set than in the homogeneous cluster (t: −2.5934; df: 14.009; p < 0.05). We observed significant differences in K-values across subjects in the homogeneous cluster (F: 4.257; df: 14; p < 0.05) and outliers set (F: 203.72; df: 14; p < 0.05). Results show that the K-value differs between most subjects in the homogeneous cluster (Supplementary Table 2), however, the K-value of the MEs in the outliers set only showed a significant difference in one subject (Supplementary Table 3).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Illustration of the power law relationship between the mean velocity and displacement of all MEs (A), those in the homogenous cluster (B), and the remaining outliers (C).

4 Discussion

Here, we examined the variability observed around the cost function of the recently proposed ME decomposition approach (Miranda et al., 2018). The ME decomposition approach is an extension of the work done by Meyer et al. (1982, 1988) where sub-movements are extracted based on a Cartesian coordinate system whose origin is located at the center of mass. We have demonstrated that this approach can be successfully implemented to describe a variety of upper-limb movements in healthy individuals (Miranda et al., 2018) as well as to identify short-term features related to motor learning during a balance task (de Lemos Fonseca et al., 2020). We also recently showed that features of MEs are associated with a variety of movements and motor impairments (Miranda et al., 2018; de Lemos Fonseca et al., 2020; Oubre et al., 2020, 2021; Lee et al., 2022; Liu et al., 2022). Therefore, this study aimed to better understand ME characteristics in healthy individuals and explore their potential link with currently accepted motor control concepts.

Our data show that, when healthy individuals perform voluntary random 3D movements of the upper-limb, approximately 74% of MEs match the theoretical shape expected by Equation (1) (see Supplementary material) with no or very slight deviations. The remaining MEs whose shape significantly deviated from the theoretical shape expected by Equation (1) (see Supplementary material) could be further classified into four defined patterns. This replicates and extends our prior work examining MEs of the upper-limb in healthy individuals (Miranda et al., 2018). Prior work has shown that one person at different times, or several different people, never perform a movement in exactly the same way twice (Mathiassen et al., 2002, 2003; Madeleine et al., 2003; Jackson et al., 2009). This is related to the abundancy of the motor system (Latash, 2012) where variance that has no effect on the overall performance across a variety of natural actions helps an abundant system to deal with secondary tasks and unexpected perturbations (Diedrichsen et al., 2010). Our work here extends this concept to MEs during complex upper-limb movements as the observed variability did not lead to any impaired motor performance. Furthermore, it shows that this variability is not random but follows specific patterns in its features and its distribution. Specifically, 63 and 65% of MEs extracted from the medio-lateral and antero-posterior axes, respectively, fell within the homogeneous cluster, while 91% of MEs from the rostro-caudal axis fell within it, highlighting potential differences in motor control strategies when dealing with known forces (i.e., gravity). This is in line with motor adaptation studies where individuals perform tasks within velocity-dependent force fields. These studies have highlighted that a proper balance between stability and flexibility of motor control is necessary so that ingrained skills (e.g., gait, reaching, etc.) remain sensitive to steady and persistent changes in the environment (e.g., gravity) without being disproportionately influenced by incidental events; and where new skills can be learned and developed. In fact, studies have shown that as infants learn to control their limbs, there is a significant impact from gravity during their first few months of life (Kamm et al., 1990; Jensen et al., 2010). This could explain the distribution of MEs within the homogeneous and outliers clusters such that the life-long learned motor patterns performed against gravity are more stable than those not influenced by it. It is important to note that while the MEs within the outliers cluster exhibited variations from the expected theoretical model, our current study cannot determine whether this variability is “good” (having no negative effect on motor performance) or “bad” (leading to detrimental effects on motor performance). As such, future studies should examine this issue. However, as we did not visually observe any “impaired” motor performance, we would hypothesize that the variability in MEs within the outliers cluster is a manifestation of abundance, therefore this could be “good” variability.

Remarkably, the actual shape of the MEs seemed to have little impact on the power law relationship between their mean velocity and displacement previously described (Miranda et al., 2018). While the 2/3 scaling was most closely observed in the homogeneous cluster, the outliers still exhibited a power law relationship between mean velocity and displacement of MEs that approached the theoretical value of 2/3. This indicates that the relationship between mean velocity and displacement may be a more basic principle of goal-directed voluntary movements than the shape of the MEs. We argue here that this relationship may be related to movement vigor. Movement vigor can vary among healthy individuals as some individuals move consistently faster than others. These between-subject differences in movement vigor have been observed in eye (Choi et al., 2014; Bargary et al., 2017; Reppert et al., 2018) and reaching movements (Reppert et al., 2018); and may be related to differences in how the CNS of each subject defines reward as a function of time (Choi et al., 2014). In fact, in a number of motor control models (Shadmehr et al., 2010, 2016; Rigoux and Guigon, 2012; Berret and Jean, 2016), it is thought that movement vigor is related to how the brain evaluates the usefulness of movements. In our proposed concept of MEs, while the 2/3 relationship observed between mean velocity and displacement seems to be conserved between and within subjects, the K-value that is observed within Equation (2) (see Supplementary material) shows between-subject variability (Supplementary Tables 13), especially for MEs in the homogeneous cluster. We know from Equation (2) (see Supplementary material) that K is related to mean velocity of MEs (i.e., for a given value of D, changing the value of K will necessarily alter the mean velocity). This may indicate the subject-specific representation of how the CNS evaluates the importance of the cost of time. Our results show that most subjects exhibit a different K-value, albeit only for MEs within the homogeneous cluster (Supplementary Table 3). This between-subject variability all but disappears for MEs within the outlier set. So, it is possible that the subject-specific movement vigor can be represented by the value of K within the homogeneous cluster because these movements are deemed already optimal by the CNS as they closely mirror the theoretical trajectory and thus, are likely performed in a feedforward manner. Conversely, the lack of variability in K-value within the outliers set (Supplementary Table 3) may be a steady-state level of movement vigor exhibited by the CNS while exploring the movement sub-space and relying on feedback. The higher K-values and consistency between subjects may be a byproduct or a necessary condition of the sensorimotor integration required for feedback control. This is supported by the fact that MEs in the homogeneous cluster had on average greater mean velocity, and shorter duration than MEs in the outliers set. Prior work has demonstrated that feedforward responses are significantly faster than feedback responses (Leonard et al., 2009; Tsao et al., 2009). Thus, the fact that the MEs in the homogeneous cluster are performed faster and are shorter is in line with the hypothesis that they are part of the feedforward system while the slower and longer MEs in the outliers cluster allow for feedback and sensory motor integration.

There are limitations to the current study. Several studies have suggested that invariances such as the 2/3 power law and planar invariances might arise from musculoskeletal mechanics rather than neural control (Sternad and Schaal, 1999; Gribble and Ostry, 2000). Others proposed alternative hypotheses to explain the control of movement such as the equilibrium-point hypothesis (Fel’dman, 1966) and the uncontrolled manifold hypothesis (Scholz and Schöner, 1999). Unfortunately, as only one anatomical landmark was tracked in this study, it is not possible to exclude the possibility that similar sources might explain the regularities of the MEs. However, while results from Miranda et al. (2018) suggest that MEs are generated within the CNS and are part of the motor planning process, future studies are required. Furthermore, a larger number of males were recruited than females (this was coincidental). While the sample size does not afford the possibility of comparing the results based on sex, prior work is inconclusive regarding differences in upper-limb movement patterns during various tasks (e.g., Gromeier et al., 2017; Nakatake et al., 2017). As such, further studies on the impact of sex on MEs are needed.

5 Conclusion

The current study confirms the results of Miranda et al. (2018) that 3D random movements can be decomposed into sub-units termed MEs within individual body axes. Furthermore, we demonstrate here that the variability in ME shape can be defined by a small number of patterns and that MEs that best display a bell-shaped curve are distributed unevenly across the different body axes.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by the University of Massachusetts Amherst Institutional Review Board. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

J-FD: Conceptualization, Investigation, Supervision, Writing – review and editing. BO: Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing – original draft. JM: Conceptualization, Writing – review and editing. SL: Conceptualization, Investigation, Supervision, Writing – review and editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This material was in part supported by the NSF Smart and Connected Health Program under Grant No. 1755687. JM received financial support from the National Council for Technical and Scientific Development (CNPq), grant number 234059/2014-1.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the subjects who participated in this study.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The author(s) declared that they were an editorial board member of Frontiers, at the time of submission. This had no impact on the peer review process and the final decision.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnhum.2023.1278653/full#supplementary-material

References

Bargary, G., Bosten, J. M., Goodbourn, P. T., Lawrance-Owen, A. J., Hogg, R. E., and Mollon, J. D. (2017). Individual differences in human eye movements: An oculomotor signature? Vision Res. 141, 157–169. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2017.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berret, B., and Jean, F. (2016). Why don’t we move slower? The value of time in the neural control of action. J. Neurosci. 36, 1056–1070. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1921-15.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Choi, J. E., Vaswani, P. A., and Shadmehr, R. (2014). Vigor of movements and the cost of time in decision making. J. Neurosci. 34, 1212–1223. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2798-13.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Lemos Fonseca, M., Daneault, J. F., Vergara-Diaz, G., Quixadá, A. P., Souza de Oliveira E Torres, ÂF., and Pondé de Sena, E. (2020). Motor skill acquisition during a balance task as a process of optimization of motor primitives. Eur. J. Neurosci. 51, 2082–2094. doi: 10.1111/ejn.14649

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diedrichsen, J., Shadmehr, R., and Ivry, R. B. (2010). The coordination of movement: optimal feedback control and beyond. Trends Cogn. Sci. 14, 31–39. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.11.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fel’dman, A. G. (1966). [On the functional tuning of the nervous system in movement control or preservation of stationary pose. II. Adjustable parameters in muscles]. Biofizika 11, 498–508.

Google Scholar

Gribble, P. L., and Ostry, D. J. (2000). Compensation for loads during arm movements using equilibrium-point control. Exp. Brain Res. 135, 474–482. doi: 10.1007/s002210000547

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gromeier, M., Koester, D., and Schack, T. (2017). Gender differences in motor skills of the overarm throw. Front. Psychol. 8:212. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00212

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoff, B. (1994). A model of duration in normal and perturbed reaching movement. Biol. Cybern. 71, 481–488. doi: 10.1007/BF00198466

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jackson, J. A., Mathiassen, S. E., and Dempsey, P. G. (2009). Methodological variance associated with normalization of occupational upper trapezius EMG using sub-maximal reference contractions. J. Electromyogr. Kinesiol. 19, 416–427. doi: 10.1016/j.jelekin.2007.11.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jensen, R. K., Sun, H., Treitz, T., and Parker, H. E. (2010). Gravity constraints in infant motor development. J. Mot. Behav. 29, 64–71. doi: 10.1080/00222899709603470

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kamm, K., Thelen, E., and Jensen, J. L. (1990). A dynamical systems approach to motor development. Phys. Ther. 70, 763–775. doi: 10.1093/ptj/70.12.763

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Latash, M. L. (2012). The bliss (not the problem) of motor abundance (not redundancy). Exp. Brain Res. 217, 1–5. doi: 10.1007/s00221-012-3000-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, J., Oubre, B., Daneault, J. F., Stephen, C. D., Schmahmann, J. D., Gupta, A., et al. (2022). Analysis of gait sub-movements to estimate ataxia severity using ankle inertial data. IEEE Trans. Biomed. Eng. 69, 2314–2323. doi: 10.1109/TBME.2022.3142504

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leonard, J. A., Brown, R. H., and Stapley, P. J. (2009). Reaching to multiple targets when standing: the spatial organization of feedforward postural adjustments. J. Neurophysiol. 101, 2120–2133. doi: 10.1152/jn.91135.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, Y., Oubre, B., Duval, C., Lee, S. I., and Daneault, J. F. A. (2022). Kinematic Data-Driven Approach to Differentiate Involuntary Choreic Movements in Individuals With Neurological Conditions. IEEE Trans. Biomed. Eng. 69, 3784–3791. doi: 10.1109/TBME.2022.3177396

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Madeleine, P., Lundager, B., Voigt, M., and Arendt-Nielsen, L. (2003). Standardized low-load repetitive work: evidence of different motor control strategies between experienced workers and a reference group. Appl. Ergon. 34, 533–542. doi: 10.1016/S0003-6870(03)00083-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mathiassen, S. E., Burdorf, A., and van der Beek, A. J. (2002). Statistical power and measurement allocation in ergonomic intervention studies assessing upper trapezius EMG amplitude. A case study of assembly work. J. Electromyogr. Kinesiol. 12, 45–57. doi: 10.1016/s1050-6411(01)00028-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mathiassen, S. E., Möller, T., and Forsman, M. (2003). Variability in mechanical exposure within and between individuals performing a highly constrained industrial work task. Ergonomics 46, 800–824. doi: 10.1080/0014013031000090125

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meyer, D. E., Abrams, R. A., Kornblum, S., Wright, C. E., and Smith, J. E. (1988). Optimality in human motor performance: ideal control of rapid aimed movements. Psychol. Rev. 95, 340–370. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.95.3.340

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meyer, D. E., Smith, J. E., and Wright, C. E. (1982). Models for the speed and accuracy of aimed movements. Psychol. Rev. 89, 449–482.

Google Scholar

Miranda, J. G., Daneault, J. F., Vergara-Diaz, G., Torres, A. F., Quixadá, A. P., and Fonseca, M. L. (2018). Complex upper-limb movements are generated by combining motor primitives that scale with the movement size. Sci. Rep. 8:12918. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-29470-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nakatake, J., Totoribe, K., Chosa, E., Yamako, G., and Miyazaki, S. (2017). Influence of Gender Differences on Range of Motion and Joint Angles During Eating in Young, Healthy Japanese Adults. Prog. Rehabil. Med. 2:20170011. doi: 10.2490/prm.20170011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oubre, B., Daneault, J. F., Jung, H. T., Whritenour, K., Miranda, J. G., Park, J., et al. (2020). Estimating Upper-Limb Impairment Level in Stroke Survivors Using Wearable Inertial Sensors and a Minimally-Burdensome Motor Task. IEEE Trans. Neural Syst. Rehabil. Eng. 28, 601–611. doi: 10.1109/TNSRE.2020.2966950

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oubre, B., Daneault, J. F., Whritenour, K., Khan, N. C., Stephen, C. D., Schmahmann, J. D., et al. (2021). Decomposition of Reaching Movements Enables Detection and Measurement of Ataxia. Cerebellum 20, 811–822. doi: 10.1007/s12311-021-01247-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reppert, T. R., Rigas, I., Herzfeld, D. J., Sedaghat-Nejad, E., Komogortsev, O., and Shadmehr, R. (2018). Movement vigor as a traitlike attribute of individuality. J. Neurophysiol. 120, 741–757. doi: 10.1152/jn.00033.2018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rigoux, L., and Guigon, E. (2012). A model of reward- and effort-based optimal decision making and motor control. PLoS Comput. Biol. 8:e1002716. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002716

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scholz, J. P., and Schöner, G. (1999). The uncontrolled manifold concept: identifying control variables for a functional task. Exp. Brain Res. 126, 289–306. doi: 10.1007/s002210050738

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shadmehr, R., Huang, H. J., and Ahmed, A. A. (2016). A representation of effort in decision-making and motor control. Curr. Biol. 26, 1929–1934. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2016.05.065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shadmehr, R., Orban de Xivry, J. J., Xu-Wilson, M., and Shih, T. Y. (2010). Temporal discounting of reward and the cost of time in motor control. J. Neurosci. 30, 10507–10516. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1343-10.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sternad, D., and Schaal, S. (1999). Segmentation of endpoint trajectories does not imply segmented control. Exp. Brain Res. 124, 118–136. doi: 10.1007/s002210050606

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsao, H., Galea, M. P., and Hodges, P. W. (2009). How fast are feedforward postural adjustments of the abdominal muscles? Behav. Neurosci. 123, 687–693. doi: 10.1037/a0015593

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: motor control, action control, movement control, movement kinematics, movement elements, upper limb

Citation: Daneault J-F, Oubre B, Miranda JGV and Lee SI (2023) Understanding voluntary human movement variability through data-driven segmentation and clustering. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 17:1278653. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2023.1278653

Received: 16 August 2023; Accepted: 30 October 2023;
Published: 28 November 2023.

Edited by:

Jason Friedman, Tel Aviv University, Israel

Reviewed by:

Nada Yousif, University of Hertfordshire, United Kingdom
Jiahao Chen, Chinese Academy of Sciences (CAS), China

Copyright © 2023 Daneault, Oubre, Miranda and Lee. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jean-Francois Daneault, amYuZGFuZWF1bHRAcnV0Z2Vycy5lZHU=; Sunghoon Ivan Lee, c2lsZWVAY3MudW1hc3MuZWR1

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.