Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 11 January 2024
Sec. Motor Neuroscience

Neural underpinnings of the interplay between actual touch and action imagination in social contexts

\r\nYumna AliYumna AliVeronica MontaniVeronica MontaniPaola Cesari*Paola Cesari*
  • Department of Neuroscience, Biomedicine and Movement Sciences, University of Verona, Verona, Italy

While there is established evidence supporting the involvement of the sense of touch in various actions, the neural underpinnings of touch and action interplay in a social context remain poorly understood. To prospectively investigate this phenomenon and offer further insights, we employed a combination of motor and sensory components by asking participants to imagine exerting force with the index finger while experiencing their own touch, the touch of one another individual, the touch of a surface, and no touch. Based on the assumption that the patterns of activation in the motor system are similar when action is imagined or actually performed, we proceeded to apply a single-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation over the primary motor cortex (M1) while participants engaged in the act of imagination. Touch experience was associated with higher M1 excitability in the presence and in the absence of force production imagination, but only during force production imagination M1 excitability differed among the types of touch: both biological sources, the self-touch and the touch of one other individual, elicited a significant increase in motor system activity when compared to touching a non-living surface or in the absence of touch. A strong correlation between individual touch avoidance questionnaire values and facilitation in the motor system was present while touching another person, indicating a social aspect for touch in action. The present study unveils the motor system correlates when the sensory/motor components of touch are considered in social contexts.

Introduction

The influence of physical touch permeates our entire life by deeply influencing how we act. Through touch, we acquire information about the surface’s physical components like hardness, roughness, softness, and temperature. By engaging in tactile experiences, we regulate the amount of pressure exerted while grasping objects. Self-touch and touching other individuals allow the processing of information about the self and the body (Martin, 1993). The experience of touch encompasses a complex interplay of cognitive and emotional factors, and pre-existing knowledge and expectations shape the individual’s perception leading to the emergence of personal traits (Bensmaia and Hollins, 2003; Haans and IJsselsteijn, 2006; Jones, 2016).

Touch can be passively sensed or actively perceived through action. While passive touch relies on the activation of cutaneous receptors, active touch receives additional input from the kinesthetic and proprioceptive senses, resulting in a different pattern of brain activation (e.g., Simões-Franklin et al., 2011). Since the seminal work of Gibson (Gibson, 1962; West and Gibson, 1969) much effort has been put into distinguishing the effects that those different exploratory procedures exert on object perception (Sonneveld and Schifferstein, 2008; Smith and Collins, 2009; Simões-Franklin et al., 2011; Ackerley et al., 2012; Fernandes and Albuquerque, 2012). The basic ability to discriminate different textures appears substantially equivalent in passive and active touch (Lederman, 1981; Heller, 1989; Verrillo et al., 1999). However, during active touch, specialized exploratory movements, and adjustment of movement parameters such as force, displacement, and related derivatives, allow to optimize perceptual precision (Lederman and Klatzky, 1987; Chapman, 1994; Srinivasan and LaMotte, 1995; Louw et al., 2000; Gamzu and Ahissar, 2001; Giachritsis et al., 2009; Kaim and Drewing, 2011; Drewing, 2012; Lezkan and Drewing, 2014; Metzger et al., 2018). In addition, more and more attention is devoted to the fact that to generate the appropriate percept, the afferent signal provided by the sensory receptors is integrated with the predictions of the sensory consequences of one’s own actions (generated as a consequence of the motor command, i.e., internal forward model or efference copy). In this way, not only the brain may extract the tactile signal more efficiently (Willemet et al., 2021), but the information about the perceptual context that is provided by the internal model can be critical to solving more complex tasks such as ambiguous shapes discrimination (Verrillo et al., 1999; Smith et al., 2009).

The tight functional coupling between touch and action is supported by strong neural interconnections between the somatic and the motor systems. After the initial processing within the primary somatosensory cortex (S1), tactile information travels through the secondary somatosensory cortex (S2), the posterior parietal cortex (PPC), and the primary motor (M1) cortex, (see review Delhaye et al., 2016). The primary motor cortex (M1) and primary somatosensory cortex (S1) are directly interconnected (Haegens et al., 2011; Ifft et al., 2013; Schwarz et al., 2014; Umeda et al., 2019). M1 and S1 work together to integrate tactile and proprioceptive feedback for skilled movements of the hand (Salimi et al., 1999; Gardner et al., 2007). Indeed, when the action of grasping is performed, a significant proportion of S1 neurons discharge in response (Sinclair and Burton, 1991), and some S1 neurons are sensitive to force direction (Fortier-Poisson and Smith, 2015; Fortier-Poisson et al., 2015). On the other hand, surface texture can be encoded by neurons in M1 (Jiang et al., 2018).

An experimental approach aimed to differentiate the neural mechanisms underlying perception from those involved in the action is applying motor imagery. Participants imagine performing an action while receiving sensory input, without physically executing any movement. The prevailing theoretical framework assumes that imagined actions are an internal simulation of actual movements (Jeannerod, 1994; Jeannerod and Decety, 1995) and that the cognitive mechanisms involved in action generation, perception, and imagination share a common network (Prinz, 1997; Hommel, 2009; Cesari et al., 2011). Even though the mechanisms behind imagined movements are still not fully comprehended, empirical findings indicate that these mental representations exhibit physiological and temporal patterns that resemble those of actual movements (Bufalari et al., 2010). Moreover, pathophysiological constraints seem to similarly affect both imagined and executed movements (Sirigu et al., 1996; Ridderinkhof and Brass, 2015).

Based on this method, few studies have examined the role of tactile information in controlling action (Mizuguchi et al., 2009, 2011; Ali et al., 2023), The corticospinal excitability of M1 during action imagination is enhanced by the real touch of the object involved in the action compared to the condition in which no touch is present (Mizuguchi et al., 2009, 2011), suggesting a facilitatory effect of touch on the motor system. Recently, we demonstrated that this excitability is muscle-specific (Ali et al., 2023). In our previous study, participants imagined producing different amounts of force with the index finger while touching or not a rigid surface. The facilitatory effect of touch scaled properly with the amount of force “exerted,” but crucially, the effect was restricted to the body part that would be involved in the execution of the action (Ali et al., 2023).

The ability to discriminate physical properties through touch is accompanied by the integration and processing of affective and socially relevant information. Touch received from other individuals, i.e., social touch, is crucial for the development of the individual’s social cognition, and then has a profound impact throughout the entire lifespan (Jönsson, 2017; Cascio et al., 2019). Affective touch is mediated by a specialized sub-modality of touch, the c-touch system (McGlone et al., 2014), and tactile stimulation from another person is associated with a distinct pattern of cortical activation compared to self-touch (Boehme et al., 2019, Boehme and Olausson, 2022). For example, (Boehme et al., 2019) used fMRI, behavioral testing, and somatosensory-evoked potentials (SEPs) measured at the spinal and cortical levels, to examine the difference between the sensation of touching oneself and the sensation of being touched by another person. Stimulation from another person activated several areas including the somatosensory cortex, insula, superior temporal gyrus, supramarginal gyrus, striatum, amygdala, cerebellum, and prefrontal cortex. In contrast, self-touch was associated with deactivation in the insula, anterior cingulate cortex, superior temporal gyrus, amygdala, parahippocampal gyrus, prefrontal areas, and brain areas encoding low-level sensory representations. Somatosensory Evoked Potentials (SEP) analysis showed reduced cortical amplitudes during self-touch, and in contrast, shorter latencies for other touch. Crucially, during self-touch, the functional connectivity between the sensorimotor cortex and the insula was accompanied by an elevated threshold for detecting additional tactile stimuli. The discrimination of self- or other-touch would be based on a mechanism of attenuation in which the internal forward model or efference copy allows the brain to predict the sensory consequences of one’s own action (see e.g., Boehme et al., 2019; Boehme and Olausson, 2022). Subsequently, the brain can reduce or not these sensations depending on the task at hand (Juravle et al., 2017). However, the neural processes that mediate the attenuation or non-attenuation remain to be understood.

How exactly affective touch can influence action control is relatively unexplored. The amplitude of the readiness potential was modulated by the pleasantness of the stimuli to be grasped (compatible vs. incompatible, de Oliveira et al., 2012), or by the emotional context induced before performing a pleasant action (gently caressing a soft cloth, Campagnoli et al., 2015). The facilitatory effect on the motor cortex induced by the pleasant emotional context before the action, suggests that anticipating the outcomes of an action involves assessing the emotional value of a stimulus one is going to engage with and this is in line with the hypothesis of a general facilitatory effect of touch (Mizuguchi et al., 2009, 2011; Ali et al., 2023). However, because of the relevance of social interaction, and the role of affective touch in social contexts (Dunbar, 2010), it is plausible that the influence of affective touch on action control might be based on a different mechanism.

Based on this framework, the aim of the present study was to clarify the neural mechanisms of the difference between the self and other people’s touch while the motor system is activated. Above we have briefly reviewed the thigh coupling between touch and action, and in particular how sensorimotor control and haptic exploration depend on the ability of the touch system to discriminate physical attributes. Much less is known about how affective touch can influence action control. We investigated whether the motor system resonates selectively for a task parameter such as force production imagination when different conditions of tactile stimuli are given (Cesari et al., 2011; Kilteni et al., 2018; Fukumoto et al., 2021). Specifically, we examined whether the activity of the motor system is differently modulated when an individual imagines producing and not producing force with her/his index finger while having the hand touching different surfaces: Self-Touch (put in contact his/her right with the left hand’s index fingertips), We-Touch (when subject receive the touch on his index finger from the index finger’s tip of one other person), Surface-Touch (touch a hard surface with his/her index fingertip), and No-Touch (do not have the index fingertip or other parts of the hand in touch with anything). To measure motor cortex activity, a single pulse Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation (TMS) was delivered while participants imagined producing ∼3N and not producing force (<∼1N) with the index finger of their right hand. As the target muscle, we selected the First Dorsal Interosseous (FDI) which is involved in the flexion-abduction of the index finger, while as the control muscle, the Abductor Digiti Minimi (ADM) since it is involved in the flexion-abduction of the little finger.

Considering the touch condition, we hypothesized that the motor system will show higher cortico-spinal activation when biological sources are touched, i.e., when individuals are either touching themselves and when touching someone else, as compared to when they are touching non-living sources, or not touching at all. However, when social interaction is included, we might expect to find higher cortico-spinal activation when touching someone else compared to the self-touch condition since it has been shown that the processing of social information undergoes distinctive and privileged processing mechanisms (de Oliveira et al., 2012; Souza et al., 2012; Campagnoli et al., 2015; Boehme and Olausson, 2022; Grichtchouk et al., 2022). Alternatively, participants may perceive self-generated tactile stimulation as relatively weaker, when compared to external tactile stimulation (Kilteni et al., 2018). In this case, participants could enhance the level of applied force, resulting in higher Motor Evoked Potentials (MEPs) in the Self-Touch condition. When considering the MEPs modulation based on the level of force imagined, we expected to replicate the results obtained in previous works (Helm et al., 2015; Ali et al., 2023), i.e., higher activation when individuals imagine producing higher force as compared to lower force. Here we asked individuals to imagine producing on the provided surface ∼3N of force (from now on referred as ∼3N condition), and to imagine the digit lying on the provided surface without imagining force production (from now on referred as <∼1N condition). For the interaction between the touch and the force conditions, we were expecting to find a generalized amplified effect of touch during the imagination of force production.

Materials and methods

Participants

The study included twenty healthy volunteers, 10 men, and 10 women, ranging in age from 18 to 26 years (mean age = 21.5 years, SD = 1.71 years). None of the participants were aware of the study’s purpose, and none of them had any neurological, psychiatric, or other health conditions. They also didn’t experience any negative effects from TMS (Rossi et al., 2021). There were no complaints of pain or adverse effects during TMS, and none were observed. Before entering the lab, each participant signed an informed consent form. The local ethics commission authorized the procedures, which adhered to the ethical standards set forth in the Declaration of Helsinki from 1964.

Equipment

A biphasic single TMS pulse was applied using a figure-of-eight coil; the outside diameter of each wing was 110 mm (STM 9000 magnetic stimulator, Ates-EBNeuro, Italy). The coil was positioned on an extended arm at a 45° angle to the sagittal axis on the left side of the head, tangential to the skull (Brasil-Neto et al., 1992). The coil was moved laterally in small steps to the vertex in the left hemisphere and TMS pulses were given once stable motor evoked potential (MEP) amplitudes were evoked in the relaxed FDI (Jong et al., 2009). The resting motor threshold is the lowest stimulus intensity that can elicit MEPs in the muscles with an amplitude of at least 50 V in at least five out of ten trials (rMT). This measurement was used to identify the best area of the scalp for FDI. Throughout the experiment, the amount of stimulation was set at 120% rMT (Hallett, 2000).

Procedure

The task required to imagine producing or not forces while lying the right hand’s index finger on the various surfaces defining the different touch conditions. The subjects were asked to maintain their right arm relaxed on their lap, which was supported by a hard surface while sitting in a chair. Because the compatibility of the hand position could impact the MEP amplitude (Vargas et al., 2004; Casetta et al., 2020), the hand remained in the same position throughout the entire experiment, to ensure a posture compatible with the imaged action. The experiment was recorded in a block based on conditions where each block of four touch conditions contains two levels of force counterbalanced among subjects. The task was to imagine with closed eyes on two levels i.e., <∼1 and ∼3N of forces. Fifteen trials of TMS stimulation were delivered for each combination of force condition and the types of touch condition i.e., Self-Touch when the subject receives the touch of one other person (We-Touch), Surface Touch, and without any touch (No-Touch). A total of 120 trials were recorded from each participant.

A training session was conducted prior to the start of the imagination task. Participants were instructed to practice, utilizing the tip of their index fingers, to produce a specific amount of force on the balance scale. During the training session, the experimenter provided guidance to the subjects regarding the timing of the task, including when to initiate the action, the duration of force application, and when to release the force. Once the participants became accustomed to the timing of the task, a recorded voice signaled the start and end of each trial. The participants were instructed to actively remember their experiences of producing force during the training session, with the goal of being able to mentally simulate the same experiences during the experimental trials. Following the training session, participants were instructed to maintain a seated position with their arms at rest and their heads facing forward for the entire duration of the data collection (see Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Schematic illustration of test situations. (A) Overview; (B) 1. Self-touch - Individual touching own index finger, 2. We touch - Another person touching the participant’s index finger, 3. Surface touch - Surface provided beneath participant’s index finger, 4. No touch - No touch provided.

As previously mentioned, the experimental task required participants to engage in mental imagery of force production (3N) and no force production (>∼1N) using their index fingers. The magnitude of the imagined force was matched to the force levels practiced during the training session. Throughout the entire experiment, participants were not provided with any feedback regarding their performance. Fatigue was carefully monitored, and rest periods were incorporated every 30 trials to mitigate any potential fatigue effects. Each trial involved the delivery of a single transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) pulse. To avoid any potential effect associated with expectation, TMS was applied at random delays (selected among intervals of 300, 450, 600, 750, or 1000 ms, balanced across trials) after the vocal instruction. The inter-trial interval, approximately 5−10 s, was designed to prevent the accumulation of brain activity from one TMS pulse to the next, following the guidelines proposed by Wassermann et al. (2008). The entire experimental session for each participant lasted approximately 80 min, encompassing multiple trials and TMS pulses. At the end of the experiment, participants were asked to fill out some questionnaires, the Vividness of Visual Imagery Questionnaire (VVIQ) to test the effectiveness of their imagery (Marks, 1973), and the Touch Avoidance measure, and touch avoidance questionnaire (Casetta et al., 2020) to assess the relevant personality dimensions.

Statistical analysis

We employed mixed-effect multiple regression modeling (Gelman and Hill, 2006; Yu et al., 2022) to examine the impact of touch, force, and muscle on motor-evoked potential (MEP) amplitude. The model incorporated three fixed effects (touch, force, and muscle) and their interaction. Touch had four levels (you-touch, me-touch, surface-touch, and no-touch), the force had two levels (<∼1 Newton, ∼3 Newton), and muscle had two levels (FDI, ADM). The random structure of the model included by-subject random intercepts and by-subject random slopes for the force factor. To account for multiple comparisons, we applied the Bonferroni procedure for p-value correction (Simes, 1986). Observations with standardized residuals exceeding 2.5 standard deviations from zero were excluded (3.90% of the data), following guidelines by Krajewski and Matthews (2010).

In addition, we assessed whether there was an association between the individual differences between the Self-Touch and We-Touch conditions (that we refer to as the “Self-We Touch effect”), and the touch avoidance measure obtained from the two questionnaires. As an operational measure of the “Self-We Touch effect,” we computed the ratio between Self-Touch and We-Touch, in the ∼3N force condition for FDI and ADM separately. Statistical analysis was conducted using the lme4 package (Bates et al., 2015), the afex package (Singmann et al., 2022), and the emmeans package (Lenth, 2022) in the R environment (R Core Team, 2022)

Results

Based on the participants’ overall VVIQ ratings, which were a 2.16 (SD = 0.7) average score for participants with open eyes and 2.26 (SD = 0.67) for closed eyes condition, all participants were referred to the group of “good visualizers” (Marks, 1973). All participants successfully performed force production imagination (3N) and no force production imagination (<∼1N) for the 4 levels of touch (Self-Touch, We-Touch, Surface-Touch, and No-Touch) by using two muscles i.e., FDI and ADM. None of the participants reported discomfort during the stimulations.

Considering the motor-evoked potentials, the main effect of Force was significant (F (1,18) = 18.97, p < 0.001) indicating that, overall, MEPs were smaller for <∼1N (M = 0.78, SD = 0.82) compared to ∼3N (M = 1.02, SD = 1.01). The main effect of Condition was significant (F (3,4485) = 26.58, p < 0.001), Post-hoc comparisons using Bonferroni correction indicated that, overall, MEPs were larger for both Self-Touch (M = 1.00, SD = 0.83, p < 0.001), and We-Touch (M = 1.00, SD = 0.68, p < 0.001) compared to Surface-Touch (M = 0.83, SD = 0.64), and, for both Self-Touch (p < 0.001) and We-Touch (p < 0.001) compared to No-Touch (M = 0.77, SD = 0.61). MEPs for Self-Touch did not significantly differ from MEPs for We-Touch (p = 1.00), and MEPs for Surface-Touch did not significantly differ from MEPs for No-Touch (p = 0.72). The main effect of Muscle (F (1,4488) = 840.69, p < 0.001) was significant, indicating that, overall, MEPs were larger for FDI (M = 1.22, SD = 0.80) compared to ADM (M = 0.57, SD = 0.60) (see Figure 2).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Bar plot showing the main effect of Touch ***P < 0.001, with n.s. indicating non-significance. Error bars represent the standard error of the means adjusted to correctly reflect the variance in the within-subject design (Morey, 2008).

The two-way interaction force by muscles was significant (F(1,4490) = 19.18, p < 0.001), indicating that, MEPs were smaller for <∼1N compared to ∼3N for FDI (M = 1.03, SD = 0.38, M = 1.36, SD = 0.56, p < 0.001) and ADM (M = 0.5, SD = 0.49, M = 0.64, SD = 0.39, p = 0.03), but the difference was larger for FDI (see Figure 3).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. (Left) Bar plot showing the group effects of force by muscle interaction ***P < 0.001 and *P < 0.05. Error bars represent the standard error of the means adjusted to correctly reflect the variance in the within-subject design (Morey, 2008). (Right) Paired observation of two muscles ADM and FDI in forces.

The two-way interaction of touch by force conditions was significant (F(3,4486) = 5.48, p < 0.001), indicating that, independently from the effect of muscle, in the <∼1N force condition, MEPs were larger for Self-Touch (M = 0.79, SD = 0.31, p = 0.01), and We-Touch (M = 0.78, SD = 0.12, p = 0.02) compared to No-Touch (M = 0.66, SD = 0.22). In the ∼3N force condition, MEPs were larger for Self-Touch compared to both Surface Touch (M = 1.12, SD = 0.48, and M = 0.87, SD = 0.35, p < 0.001) and No-Touch (M = 1.12, SD = 0.48, and M = 0.84, SD = 0.25, p < 0.001), and in the same vein, for We-Touch compared to both Surface Touch (M = 1.12, SD = 0.32, and M = 0.87, SD = 0.35, p < 0.001) and No-Touch (M = 1.12, SD = 0.32, and M = 0.84, SD = 0.254, p < 0.001) (see Figure 4).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. (Left) Bar plot showing the group effects of touch-by-force interaction ***P < 0.001 and *P < 0.05. Error bars represent the standard error of the means adjusted to correctly reflect the variance in the within-subject design (Morey, 2008). (Right) Paired observation of two forces.

The interaction touch by muscle was significant (F(3,4485) = 21.11, p < 0.001), for ADM muscle, MEPs were larger for the We-Touch condition compared to the Self-Touch condition (M = 0.70, SD = 0.46, and M = 0.52, SD = 0.47 p < 0.001), compared to the Surface-Touch condition (M = 0.70, SD = 0.46, and M = 0.56, SD = 0.45, p = 0.003), and compared to the No-Touch condition (M = 0.70, SD = 0.46, and M = 0.48, SD = 0.48, p < 0.001). For FDI muscle, MEPs were larger for the Self-Touch condition compared to the We-Touch condition (M = 1.43, SD = 0.28, M = 1.29, SD = 0.65, p < 0.001), compared to the Surface Touch condition (M = 1.43, SD = 0.28, M = 1.08, SD = 0.63, p < 0.001), and compared to the No-Touch condition (M = 1.43, SD = 0.28, M = 1.04, SD = 0.35, p < 0.001), MEPs were also larger for the WT condition compared to the Surface Touch condition (M = 1.29, SD = 0.65, M = 1.08, SD = 0.63, p = 0.002), and the No-Touch condition (M = 1.29, SD = 0.65, M = 1.04, SD = 0.35, p < 0.001) (see Figure 5).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. (Left) Bar plot showing the group effects of touch by muscle interaction ***P < 0.001 and **P < 0.01. Error bars represent the standard error of the means adjusted to correctly reflect the variance in the within-subject design (Morey, 2008). (Right) Paired observation of two muscles ADM and FDI in all touch conditions.

The three-way interaction, touch by force by muscle, just approached significance (F (3,4485) = 2.48, p = 0.06).

Correlation

Results of the Pearson correlation indicated that there was a significant negative correlation between the “Self-We Touch effect” -corresponding to the ratio between Self-Touch and We-Touch in the ∼3N force condition, for the ADM muscle activity - and the touch avoidance measure score, r (18) = −0.56, p = 0.01. The finding indicates that individuals with a low index (therefore, individuals in which We-Touch was higher compared to Self-Touch) were associated with a higher level of touch avoidance (i.e., individuals with a high score in the touch avoidance measure) (see Figure 6).

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Correlation between the touch avoidance questionnaire in opposite sex TAM-OS score and the model predictions for the ∼3N force effect of ADM muscle.

Discussion

Our study aimed at investigating the neural cortico-spinal activity changes during action imagination when touch is involved. Specifically, we focused on examining the motor system activity during the mental simulation of force generation while individuals experienced various surfaces’ touch The experimental conditions examined included Self-Touch (touching each other with the two index fingers of the two hands), We-Touch (being touched by another person), Surface Touch (touching a surface with the index finger), and a condition without any tactile involvement i.e., No-Touch. By employing single-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) on the primary motor cortex, we specifically targeted the First Dorsal Interosseous (FDI) as the muscle responsible for the force production through the index finger pressure imagination, and the Abductor Digiti Minimi (ADM) serving as control muscle. Our study sought to provide valuable insights into the modulation of the motor system associated with different contexts in which social and physiological components are intermingled. We utilized imagination instead of actual action to avoid potential confounding factors, and to ensure a more controlled and focused investigation into the distinct contributions of the sensory component (encompassing biological and non-biological touch) and the motor component (involving the application of physical force). This well-established approach is based on the substantial similarities between action and action imagination on psychophysical properties, and patterns of neural activations (Decety et al., 2001; Papaxanthis et al., 2002; Ehrsson et al., 2003; Lotze and Halsband, 2006; McAvinue and Robertson, 2008; Munzert et al., 2009; Hétu et al., 2013; Zabicki et al., 2017).

The results confirmed our main hypothesis that the motor system excitability is enhanced when biological sources are touched, i.e., when individuals are either touching themselves and when touching someone else, as compared to when they are touching non-living sources, or not touching at all. We also replicated previous findings showing higher activation when individuals produce higher force as compared to lower force (Ali et al., 2023), and, as expected, the effect was stronger for the target muscle. The implications of these findings are considered in detail in the following discussion.

Overall, the amplitude of motor-evoked potentials (MEPs) of the relevant muscle increased when imagined actions were performed concurrently with tactile stimulation, replicating the facilitatory effect of touch on the motor system (Mizuguchi et al., 2013; Ali et al., 2023). During the action, there is a complex interplay between the somatosensory system and the motor system to jointly control the movement and process tactile information (see review Delhaye et al., 2016). On one hand, action allows to optimize of tactile precision through various mechanisms, such as the adjustment of movement parameters and predictive processes (Lederman and Klatzky, 1987; Chapman, 1994; Srinivasan and LaMotte, 1995; Louw et al., 2000; Gamzu and Ahissar, 2001; Giachritsis et al., 2009; Kaim and Drewing, 2011; Drewing, 2012; Lezkan and Drewing, 2014; Metzger et al., 2018). On the other hand, somatosensory information is integrated with other processes to control action. However, how exactly this interplay is implemented in the brain needs to be addressed. Here we showed that the processing of tactile information directly impacts M1 excitability during motor imagery.

We found increased activation of the motor system when participants imagined producing force (∼3N) as compared to no-force production (<∼1). Importantly, the scaling effect of force was larger for the target muscle, i.e., FDI, compared to the control muscle, i.e., ADM. That is the increased motor system activation when imaging to produce ∼3N compared to <∼1N was significantly more pronounced for FDI, the muscle that would be involved in the execution of the action (Bufalari et al., 2010; Pizzolato et al., 2012; Helm et al., 2015; Ali et al., 2023; Figure 3). It is well established that M1 neurons encode grasp force (Cheney and Fetz, 1980; Wannier et al., 1991; Maier et al., 1993; Sergio and Kalaska, 1997, 1998; Shalit et al., 2012) and carry relevant information pertaining to grasp force (Intveld et al., 2018). In line with Ali et al. (2023), the present finding suggests that forces applied during imagined actions are scaled similarly to physical actions and that the scaling is selective for the body part involved (Bufalari et al., 2010; Pizzolato et al., 2012; Helm et al., 2015; Grosprêtre et al., 2016), even when a modest magnitude of force is employed [∼3N in the present investigation versus ∼15N in Ali et al. (2023)]. This scaling effect is consistent with earlier research that explored various imagery tasks, such as grasping objects of different sizes and shapes, performing movements in different directions, and varying the extent of movement (Bufalari et al., 2010; Cesari et al., 2011; Pizzolato et al., 2012).

The ability to discriminate physical properties through touch is accompanied by the integration of affective and socially relevant information. While studies over the past decades have provided important information about the reciprocal interplay between touch and action in general (Camerota and Celletti, 2014; Moscatelli et al., 2019), how social touch impacts movement control remains to be determined. Another important finding of the present study was the identification of how Self-Touch and We-Touch modulate the MEPs’ amplitude during force imagination. In the condition (<∼1), both Self-Touch and We-Touch showed higher activation compared to No-Touch, while there was no significant difference among the three touching conditions. In other words, the M1 excitabilities in the Self-Touch, the We-Touch, and the Surface Touch conditions were similar. On the other hand, when subjects were imaged to produce force (∼3N force condition), the excitability of M1 in Self-Touch and We-Touch conditions was similar, both showing higher excitability than Surface Touch and No-Touch. Therefore, it appears that while the main facilitatory effect of touch was present even in the absence of action, the presence of action appeared critical to differentiate between touches when in contact with “living” and “not-living” materials. This finding strongly corroborates previous studies that have associated improved tactile perception with action, possibly attributed to the influence of predictive processes (Smith et al., 2009; Willemet et al., 2021).

Critically, the differences among the types of touch were manifest when comparing the patterns of activation of the two muscles. For the target muscle, i.e., FDI, both Self-Touch and We-Touch showed larger amplitude compared to Surface Touch and No-Touch. However, in line with previous evidence indicating that self-touch and social touch are associated with different patterns of neural activation (Boehme et al., 2019), we found that MEPs’ amplitude in the Self-Touch condition was significantly larger than the amplitude in the We-Touch condition. That is, the motor system activation was the highest when the right index finger of the subject was in contact with her own left index finger. This result sustains the hypothesis of the presence of sensory attenuation (SA) explaining why stimuli that are self-generated are associated with a reduction in the perceived intensity of the stimulus, explaining for instance why one cannot tickle oneself (Hughes et al., 2013). As a result, the discrimination between tactile signals produced by the action of the same person and signals that arise from non-self-causes is hypothesized to be based on this mechanism of SA (Blakemore et al., 1998; Master and Tremblay, 2010). When a motor command is generated, an internal copy of this command, referred to as an “efference copy,” is utilized to predict, and subsequently attenuate, the sensory outcome of the action (e.g., Kilteni et al., 2020). Consequently, self-generated tactile sensations are perceived as weaker compared to externally imposed stimuli (Bays and Wolpert, 2012). Therefore, it is tempting to explain the highest activation found in the Self-Touch condition for the target muscle as a neural signature of the consequences of sensory attenuation. Specifically, since the brain might have predicted the specific action (right index finger touching the left index finger) through an efferent copy, the sensory signal associated with the right hand could have been attenuated. Consequently, the amount of force “exerted” by the right index finger to match the required force (∼3N) might have been exaggerated. However, further research should be carried out to establish the validity of this hypothesis.

In stark contrast with FDI, the motor system excitability for the control muscle, i.e., ADM, was highest in the We-Touch condition, that is when the subject’s right index finger was in contact with the index finger of another individual. The We-Touch condition showed a significantly larger amplitude compared to all the other conditions (Self-Touch, Surface Touch, and No-Touch), while among those, no significant difference was detected. Considering that ADM was not directly involved in the action (Ali et al., 2023), this finding confirms that affective touch may have a more pervasive influence compared to other types of touch, aligning with the idea that the brain is hardwired around social dimensions (Dunbar, 2010), and that, social cues and contexts are processed as exceptionally influential information (Souza et al., 2012; Grichtchouk et al., 2022). Affective touch is mediated by a specialized sub-modality of touch, the c-touch system (McGlone et al., 2014), and fMRI evidence suggests that social touch is associated with widespread neural activity (Boehme et al., 2019). In line with that, the We-Touch condition may have engaged a specific and broader neural pathway encompassing ADM control.

Alternatively, some insight into the underlying mechanism of this effect may come from the correlation analysis that we carried out between the MEPs amplitude and the outcomes of the Touch Avoidance Measure (Casetta et al., 2020). We computed an index for each of the two muscles separately (ratio between Self-Touch and We-Touch in the ∼3N force condition) to better identify the difference between the Self-Touch and the We-Touch condition, which we refer to as the “Self-We touch effect.” Remarkably, we found a significant negative association between the ADM index and the scores on the Touch Avoidance Measure specifically concerning participants’ comfort levels when touching someone of the opposite sex. Individuals who exhibited higher ADM activation in the other touch condition (lower Self-Touch and We-Touch ratio) were associated with a higher score in the Touch Avoidance Measure. In other words, participants who showed a tendency toward touch avoidance, as measured by the Touch Avoidance Measure, also showed higher MEPs in the We-Touch condition compared to the Self-Touch condition. The touch avoidance construct serves as an indicator of an individual’s inclination toward initiating and receiving physical touch (Andersen and Leibowitz, 1978). Touch avoidance has been shown to diminish the perceived pleasantness of various forms of tactile stimulation (Hielscher and Mahar, 2017). The primary function of ADM is to move the fifth finger away from the fourth finger, and therefore, it is involved in each movement that requires the spreading or abduction of the little finger from the ring finger. Therefore, another possible explanation for the “Self-We Touch effect” for ADM is that it reflects the intention to move the hand far away to avoid the somewhat unpleasant sensation associated with touching a stranger. Future work might focus on the Self-We Touch differences, systematically varying parameter such as force magnitude and/or movement required and muscle involvement, to determine whether there is a causal link between the personality trait related to touch avoidance and specific muscle activation.

In conclusion, the purpose of the current investigation was to clarify the role of different types of touch during action imagination. In line with our previous study, touch was associated with higher M1 excitability, and as expected, the effect scaled with the force “exerted” and was selective for the target muscle. Crucially, while in the absence of force production imagination, there was just a general effect of touch, when subjects imagined producing force, the effects of different types of touch become manifest. Touch conditions were arranged in a somewhat hierarchical order, with Self-Touch having the greatest impact, followed by We-Touch, Surface Touch, and finally, No-Touch, which exhibited the lowest amplitude of MEPs. Finally, the control muscle (ADM), showed increased activation during the We-Touch condition, which we attributed to the strong effect of the social context or as a possible inclination to avoid touching an unfamiliar person.

The present study contributes to our understanding of the integration between sensory and motor processes, and the reciprocal interplay between touch and action. Overall, the study strengthens the idea that action facilitates sensory discrimination, allowing better differentiation among different types of touch. We provide evidence in favor of the hypothesis that self-other discrimination is based on a mechanism of sensory attenuation. In addition, the study has been one of the first attempts to thoroughly examine how social touch impacts motor control at the level of motor system activation.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by the Ethical committee (code number n.28.R1/2021) of the University of Verona. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

YA: Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Software, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing, Investigation. VM: Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Software, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. PC: Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Software, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing, Conceptualization, Project administration, Supervision, Validation, Visualization.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This financial support was received from the University of Verona.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Ackerley, R., Hassan, E., Curran, A., Wessberg, J., Olausson, H., and McGlone, F. (2012). An fMRI study on cortical responses during active self-touch and passive touch from others. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 6:51. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2012.00051

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ali, Y., Montani, V., and Cesari, P. (2023). The touch in action: Exploring sensorimotor interactions with motor imagery. Cereb. Cortex 33, 8382–8390. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhad123

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Andersen, P. A., and Leibowitz, K. (1978). The development and nature of the construct touch avoidance. Environ. Psychol. Nonverbal Behav. 3, 89–106. doi: 10.1007/BF01135607

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B. M., and Walker, S. C. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J. Stat. Softw. 67, 1–48. doi: 10.18637/jss.v067.i01

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bays, P. M., and Wolpert, D. M. (2012). “Predictive attenuation in the perception of touch,” in Sensorimotor foundations of higher cognition, eds P. Haggard, Y. Rosetti, and M. Kawato (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 339–358. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199231447.003.0016

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bensmaia, S. J., and Hollins, M. (2003). The vibrations of texture. Somatosens. Mot. Res. 20, 33–43. doi: 10.1080/0899022031000083825

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Blakemore, S. J., Wolpert, D. M., and Frith, C. D. (1998). Central cancellation of self-produced tickle sensation. Nat. Neurosci. 1, 635–640.

Google Scholar

Boehme, R., and Olausson, H. (2022). Differentiating self-touch from social touch. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 43, 27–33. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2021.06.012

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Boehme, R., Hauser, S., Gerling, G. J., Heilig, M., and Olausson, H. (2019). Distinction of self-produced touch and social touch at cortical and spinal cord levels. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 116, 2290–2299. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1816278116

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brasil-Neto, J. P., Cohen, L. G., Panizza, M., Nilsson, J., Roth, B. J., and Hallett, M. (1992). Optimal focal transcranial magnetic activation of the human motor cortex: Effects of coil orientation, shape of the induced current pulse, and stimulus intensity. J. Clin. Neurophysiol. 9, 132–136.

Google Scholar

Bufalari, I., Sforza, A., Cesari, P., Aglioti, S. M., and Fourkas, A. D. (2010). Motor imagery beyond the joint limits: A transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Biol. Psychol. 85, 283–290. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.07.015

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Camerota, F., and Celletti, C. (2014). The touch, the sense of body and the sense of action. Senses Sci. 1:3–4. doi: 10.14616/sands-2014-1-0304

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Campagnoli, R. R., Krutman, L., Vargas, C. D., Lobo, I., Oliveira, J. M., Oliveira, L., et al. (2015). Preparing to caress: A neural signature of social bonding. Front. Psychol. 6:16. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00016

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cascio, C. J., Moore, D., and McGlone, F. (2019). Social touch and human development. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 35, 5–11. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2018.04.009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Casetta, L., Rizzi, L., Passarelli, M., Arcara, G., and Perrella, R. (2020). Italian validation of the touch avoidance measure and the touch avoidance questionnaire. Front. Psychol. 11:1673. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01673

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cesari, P., Pizzolato, F., and Fiorio, M. (2011). Grip-dependent cortico-spinal excitability during grasping imagination and execution. Neuropsychologia 49, 2121–2130. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.04.019

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chapman, C. E. (1994). Active versus passive touch: Factors influencing the transmission of somatosensory signals to primary sornatosensory cortex. Can. J. Physiol. Pharmacol. 72, 558–570. doi: 10.1139/y94-080

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheney, P. D., and Fetz, E. E. (1980). Functional classes of primate corticomotoneuronal cells and their relation to active force. J. Neurophysiol. 44, 773–791. doi: 10.1152/jn.1980.44.4.773

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

de Oliveira, L. A. S., Imbiriba, L. A., Russo, M. M., Nogueira-Campos, A. A., Rodrigues, E., de, C., et al. (2012). Preparing to grasp emotionally laden stimuli. PLoS One 7:e45235. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0045235

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Decety, J., Unit, I., and Albert, C. (2001). Functional anatomy of execution, mental simulation, observation, and verb generation of actions: A meta-analysis. Hum. Brain Mapp. 19, 1–19.

Google Scholar

Delhaye, B. P., Long, K. H., and Bensmaia, S. J. (2016). Neural basis of touch and proprioception in primate cortex. Physiol. Behav. 176, 139–148. doi: 10.1002/cphy.c170033.Neural

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Drewing, K. (2012). After experience with the task humans actively optimize shape discrimination in touch by utilizing effects of exploratory movement direction. Acta Psychol. 141, 295–303.

Google Scholar

Dunbar, R. I. M. (2010). The social role of touch in humans and primates: Behavioural function and neurobiological mechanisms. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 34, 260–268. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.07.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ehrsson, H. H., Geyer, S., and Naito, E. (2003). Imagery of voluntary movement of fingers, toes, and tongue activates corresponding body-part-specific motor representations. J. Neurophysiol. 90, 3304–3316. doi: 10.1152/jn.01113.2002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernandes, A. M., and Albuquerque, P. B. (2012). Tactual perception: A review of experimental variables and procedures. Cogn. Process. 13, 285–301.

Google Scholar

Fortier-Poisson, P., and Smith, A. M. (2015). Neuronal activity in somatosensory cortex related to tactile exploration. J. Neurophysiol. 115, 112–126. doi: 10.1152/jn.00747.2014

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fortier-Poisson, P., Langlais, J. S., and Smith, A. M. (2015). Correlation of fingertip shear force direction with somatosensory cortical activity in monkey. J. Neurophysiol. 115, 100–111. doi: 10.1152/jn.00749.2014

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fukumoto, Y., Todo, M., Bizen, H., Kimura, D., and Suzuki, T. (2021). Precision pinch force control via brain and spinal motor neuron excitability during motor imagery. Neurosci. Lett. 754:135843. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2021.135843

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gamzu, E., and Ahissar, E. (2001). Importance of temporal cues for tactile spatial-frequency discrimination. J. Neurosci. 21, 7416–7427.

Google Scholar

Gardner, E. P., Ro, J. Y., Srinivasa Babu, K., and Ghosh, S. (2007). Neurophysiology of prehension. II. Response diversity in primary somatosensory (S-I) and motor (M-I) cortices. J. Neurophysiol. 97, 1656–1670. doi: 10.1152/jn.01031.2006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gelman, A., and Hill, J. (2006). Data analysis using regression and multilevel/hierarchical models. New York, NY: Cambridge university press.

Google Scholar

Giachritsis, C. D., Wing, A. M., and Lovell, P. G. (2009). The role of spatial integration in the perception of surface orientation with active touch. Attent. Percept. Psychophys. 71, 1628–1640.

Google Scholar

Gibson, J. J. (1962). Observations on active touch. Psychol. Rev. 69, 477–491. doi: 10.1037/h0046962

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Grichtchouk, O., Oliveira, J. M., Campagnoli, R. R., Franklin, C., Correa, M. F., Pereira, M. G., et al. (2022). Visuo-motor affective interplay: Bonding scenes promote implicit motor pre-dispositions associated with social grooming–A pilot study. Front. Psychol. 13:817699. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.817699

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Grosprêtre, S., Ruffino, C., and Lebon, F. (2016). Motor imagery and cortico-spinal excitability: A review. Eur. J. Sport Sci. 16, 317–324. doi: 10.1080/17461391.2015.1024756

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Haans, A., and IJsselsteijn, W. (2006). Mediated social touch: A review of current research and future directions. Virtual Real. 9, 149–159. doi: 10.1007/s10055-005-0014-2

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Haegens, S., Nácher, V., Luna, R., Romo, R., and Jensen, O. (2011). α-Oscillations in the monkey sensorimotor network influence discrimination performance by rhythmical inhibition of neuronal spiking. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 19377–19382. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1117190108

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hallett, M. (2000). Transcranial magnetic stimulation and the human brain. Nature 406, 147–150. doi: 10.1038/35018000

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Heller, M. A. (1989). Texture perception in sighted and blind observers. Percept. Psychophys. 45, 49–54. doi: 10.3758/BF03208032

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Helm, F., Marinovic, W., Krüger, B., Munzert, J., and Riek, S. (2015). Corticospinal excitability during imagined and observed dynamic force production tasks: Effortfulness matters. Neuroscience 290, 398–405. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.01.050

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hétu, S., Grégoire, M., Saimpont, A., Coll, M. P., Eugène, F., Michon, P. E., et al. (2013). The neural network of motor imagery: An ALE meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 37, 930–949. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.03.017

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hielscher, E., and Mahar, D. (2017). An exploration of the interaction between touch avoidance and the pleasant touch (C-Tactile Afferent) system. Perception 46:18–30. doi: 10.1177/0301006616661938

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hommel, B. (2009). Action control according to TEC (theory of event coding). Psychol. Res. 73, 512–526. doi: 10.1007/s00426-009-0234-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hughes, G., Desantis, A., and Waszak, F. (2013). Mechanisms of intentional binding and sensory attenuation: The role of temporal prediction, temporal control, identity prediction, and motor prediction. Psychol. Bull. 139:133.

Google Scholar

Ifft, P. J., Shokur, S., Li, Z., Lebedev, M. A., and Nicolelis, M. A. L. (2013). A brain-machine interface enables bimanual arm movements in monkeys. Sci. Transl. Med. 5:210ra154. doi: 10.1126/scitranslmed.3006159

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Intveld, R. W., Dann, B., Michaels, J. A., and Scherberger, H. (2018). Neural coding of intended and executed grasp force in macaque areas AIP, F5, and M1. Sci. Rep. 8:17985. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-35488-z

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jeannerod, M. (1994). The representing brain: Neural correlates of motor intention and imagery. Behav. Brain Sci. 17, 187–245.

Google Scholar

Jeannerod, M., and Decety, J. (1995). Mental motor imagery: A window into the representational stages of action. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 5, 727–732. doi: 10.1016/0959-4388(95)80099-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jiang, W., Tremblay, F., and Elaine Chapman, C. (2018). Context-dependent tactile texture-sensitivity in monkey M1 and S1 cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 120, 2334–2350. doi: 10.1152/jn.00081.2018

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, L. (2016). “Thermal touch,” in Scholarpedia of touch, eds T. Prescott, E. Ahissar, and E. Izhikevich (Paris: Atlantis Press). doi: 10.2991/978-94-6239-133-8_21

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jong, S. B., Sawai, S., Misawa, S., Kanai, K., Isose, S., and Kuwabara, S. (2009). Differences in excitability properties of FDI and ADM motor axons. Muscle Nerve 39, 350–354. doi: 10.1002/mus.21107

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jönsson, E. (2017). Affective touch throughout life: From cortical processing in infancy to touch perception in adulthood. Ph. D Thesis. Göteborg: University of Gothenburg.

Google Scholar

Juravle, G., Binsted, G., and Spence, C. (2017). Tactile suppression in goal-directed movement. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 24, 1060–1076. doi: 10.3758/s13423-016-1203-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaim, L., and Drewing, K. (2011). Exploratory strategies in haptic softness discrimination are tuned to achieve high levels of task performance. IEEE Trans. Hapt. 4, 242–252.

Google Scholar

Kilteni, K., Andersson, B. J., Houborg, C., and Ehrsson, H. H. (2018). Motor imagery involves predicting the sensory consequences of the imagined movement. Nat. Commun. 9:1617. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-03989-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kilteni, K., Engeler, P., and Ehrsson, H. H. (2020). Efference copy is necessary for the attenuation of self-generated touch. iScience 23:100843. doi: 10.1016/j.isci.2020.100843

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Krajewski, G., and Matthews, D. (2010). RH Baayen, Analyzing linguistic data: A practical introduction to statistics using R. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 368.

Google Scholar

Lederman, S. J. (1981). The perception of surface roughness by active and passive touch. Bull. Psychon. Soc. 18, 253–255.

Google Scholar

Lederman, S. J., and Klatzky, R. L. (1987). Hand movements: A window into haptic object recognition. Cogn. Psychol. 19, 342–368.

Google Scholar

Lenth, R. V. (2022). Emmeans: Estimated marginal means, aka least-squares means. R Package Version 1, 3.

Google Scholar

Lezkan, A., and Drewing, K. (2014). “Unequal but fair? Weights in the serial integration of haptic texture information,” in Haptics: Neuroscience, Devices, Modeling, and Applications: 9th International Conference, EuroHaptics 2014, Versailles, France, June 24-26, 2014, Proceedings, Part I 9, M. Auvray, and C. Duriez, (eds) (Berlin: Springer), 386–392.

Google Scholar

Lotze, M., and Halsband, U. (2006). Motor imagery. J. Physiol. Paris 99, 386–395. doi: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2006.03.012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Louw, S., Kappers, A. M. L., and Koenderink, J. J. (2000). Haptic detection thresholds of Gaussian profiles over the whole range of spatial scales. Exp. brain Res. 132, 369–374.

Google Scholar

Maier, M. A., Bennett, K. M. B., Hepp-Reymond, M. C., and Lemon, R. N. (1993). Contribution of the monkey corticomotoneuronal system to the control of force in precision grip. J. Neurophysiol. 69, 772–785. doi: 10.1152/jn.1993.69.3.772

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Marks, D. F. (1973). Visual imagery differences in the recall of pictures. Br. J. Psychol. 64, 17–24. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1973.tb01322.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin, M. (1993). “Sense modalities and spatial properties,” in Spatial representation: Problems in philosophy and psychology, eds N. Eilan, R. McCarthy, and B. Brewer (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 206–218.

Google Scholar

Master, S., and Tremblay, F. (2010). Selective increase in motor excitability with intraactive (self) versus interactive touch. Neuroreport 21, 206–209. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e328335b530

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McAvinue, L. P., and Robertson, I. H. (2008). Measuring motor imagery ability: A review. Eur. J. Cogn. Psychol. 20, 232–251. doi: 10.1080/09541440701394624

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McGlone, F., Wessberg, J., and Olausson, H. (2014). Discriminative and Affective Touch: Sensing and Feeling. Neuron 82, 737–755. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.05.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Metzger, A., Lezkan, A., and Drewing, K. (2018). Integration of serial sensory information in haptic perception of softness. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 44:551.

Google Scholar

Mizuguchi, N., Sakamoto, M., Muraoka, T., and Kanosue, K. (2009). Influence of touching an object on corticospinal excitability during motor imagery. Exp. Brain Res. 196, 529–535. doi: 10.1007/s00221-009-1875-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mizuguchi, N., Sakamoto, M., Muraoka, T., Nakagawa, K., Kanazawa, S., Nakata, H., et al. (2011). The modulation of corticospinal excitability during motor imagery of actions with objects. PLoS One 6:e26006. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0026006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mizuguchi, N., Umehara, I., Nakata, H., and Kanosue, K. (2013). Modulation of corticospinal excitability dependent upon imagined force level. Exp. Brain Res. 230, 243–249. doi: 10.1007/s00221-013-3649-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Morey, R. D. (2008). Confidence intervals from normalized data: A correction to Cousineau (2005). Tutor. Quant. Methods Psychol. 4, 61–64. doi: 10.20982/tqmp.04.2.p061

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Moscatelli, A., Bianchi, M., Ciotti, S., Bettelani, G. C., Parise, C. V., Lacquaniti, F., et al. (2019). Touch as an auxiliary proprioceptive cue for movement control. Sci. Adv. 5:eaaw3121. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.aaw3121

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Munzert, J., Lorey, B., and Zentgraf, K. (2009). Cognitive motor processes: The role of motor imagery in the study of motor representations. Brain Res. Rev. 60, 306–326. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2008.12.024

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Papaxanthis, C., Schieppati, M., Gentili, R., and Pozzo, T. (2002). Imagined and actual arm movements have similar durations when performed under different conditions of direction and mass. Exp. Brain Res. 143, 447–452. doi: 10.1007/s00221-002-1012-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pizzolato, F., Fiorio, M., and Cesari, P. (2012). Motor system modulation for movement direction and rotation angle during motor imagery. Neuroscience 218, 154–160. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.05.031

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Prinz, W. (1997). Perception and action Planning. Eur. J. Cogn. Psychol. 9, 129–154. doi: 10.1080/713752551

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

R Core Team (2022). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.

Google Scholar

Ridderinkhof, K. R., and Brass, M. (2015). How kinesthetic motor imagery works: A predictive-processing theory of visualization in sports and motor expertise. J. Physiol. Paris 109, 53–63. doi: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2015.02.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rossi, S., Antal, A., Bestmann, S., Bikson, M., Brewer, C., Brockmöller, J., et al. (2021). Safety and recommendations for TMS use in healthy subjects and patient populations, with updates on training, ethical and regulatory issues: Expert Guidelines. Clin. Neurophysiol. 132, 269–306. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2020.10.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Salimi, I., Brochier, T., and Smith, A. M. (1999). Neuronal activity in somatosensory cortex of monkeys using a precision grip. II. Responses to object texture and weights. J. Neurophysiol. 81, 835–844. doi: 10.1152/jn.1999.81.2.835

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwarz, D. A., Lebedev, M. A., Hanson, T. L., Dimitrov, D. F., Lehew, G., Meloy, J., et al. (2014). Chronic, wireless recordings of large-scale brain activity in freely moving rhesus monkeys. Nat. Methods 11, 670–676. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.2936

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sergio, L. E., and Kalaska, J. F. (1997). Systematic changes in directional tuning of motor cortex cell activity with hand location in the workspace during generation of static isometric forces in constant spatial directions. J. Neurophysiol. 78, 1170–1174. doi: 10.1152/jn.1997.78.2.1170

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sergio, L. E., and Kalaska, J. F. (1998). Changes in the temporal pattern of primary motor cortex activity in a directional isometric force versus limb movement task. J. Neurophysiol. 80, 1577–1583. doi: 10.1152/jn.1998.80.3.1577

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shalit, U., Zinger, N., Joshua, M., and Prut, Y. (2012). Descending systems translate transient cortical commands into a sustained muscle activation signal. Cereb. Cortex 22, 1904–1914. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr267

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Simes, R. J. (1986). An improved Bonferroni procedure for multiple tests of significance. Biometrika 73, 751–754.

Google Scholar

Simões-Franklin, C., Whitaker, T. A., and Newell, F. N. (2011). Active and passive touch differentially activate somatosensory cortex in texture perception. Hum. Brain Mapp. 32, 1067–1080. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21091

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sinclair, R. J., and Burton, H. (1991). Neuronal activity in the primary somatosensory cortex in monkeys (Macaca mulatta) during active touch of textured surface gratings: Responses to groove width, applied force, and velocity of motion. J. Neurophysiol. 66, 153–169. doi: 10.1152/jn.1991.66.1.153

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Singmann, H., Bolker, B., Westfall, J., Aust, F., Ben-Shachar, M. S., and Højsgaard, S. (2022). Afex: Analysis of factorial experiments. R Package Version 1.2-1.

Google Scholar

Sirigu, A., Duhamel, J. R., Cohen, L., Pillon, B., Dubois, B., and Agid, Y. (1996). The mental representation of hand movements after parietal cortex damage. Science 273, 1564–1566. doi: 10.1126/science.273.5281.1564

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, A. M., Chapman, C. E., Donati, F., Fortier-Poisson, P., and Hayward, V. (2009). Perception of simulated local shapes using active and passive touch. J. Neurophysiol. 102, 3519–3529.

Google Scholar

Smith, E. R., and Collins, E. C. (2009). Contextualizing person perception: Distributed social cognition. Psychol. Rev. 116:343.

Google Scholar

Sonneveld, M. H., and Schifferstein, H. N. J. (2008). “The tactual experience of objects,” in Product experience, eds H. N. J. Schifferstein and P. P. M. Hekkert (Amsterdam: Elsevier), 41–67.

Google Scholar

Souza, G. G. L., Pereira, M. G., Vila, J., Oliveira, L., and Volchan, E. (2012). Affiliative stimuli as primers to prosocial predispositions. Span. J. Psychol. 15, 237–243. doi: 10.5209/rev_sjop.2012.v15.n1.37315

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Srinivasan, M. A., and LaMotte, R. H. (1995). Tactual discrimination of softness. J. Neurophysiol. 73, 88–101.

Google Scholar

Umeda, T., Koizumi, M., Katakai, Y., Saito, R., and Seki, K. (2019). Decoding of muscle activity from the sensorimotor cortex in freely behaving monkeys. Neuroimage 197, 512–526. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.04.045

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vargas, C. D., Olivier, E., Craighero, L., Fadiga, L., Duhamel, J. R., and Sirigu, A. (2004). The influence of hand posture on corticospinal excitability during motor imagery: A transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Cereb. Cortex 14, 1200–1206. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhh080

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Verrillo, R. T., Bolanowski, S. J., and McGlone, F. P. (1999). Subjective magnitude of tactile roughness. Somatosens. Mot. Res. 16, 352–360. doi: 10.1080/08990229970401

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wannier, T. M. J., Maier, M. A., and Hepp-Reymond, M. C. (1991). Contrasting properties of monkey somatosensory and motor cortex neurons activated during the control of force in precision grip. J. Neurophysiol. 65, 572–589. doi: 10.1152/jn.1991.65.3.572

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wassermann, E., Epstein, C., Ziemann, U., Walsh, V., Paus, T., and Lisanby, S. (eds) (2008). Oxford handbook of transcranial stimulation. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

West, C. K., and Gibson, J. J. (1969). The senses considered as perceptual systems. J. Aesthetic Educ. 3, 142. doi: 10.2307/3331482

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Willemet, L., Kanzari, K., Monnoyer, J., Birznieks, I., and Wiertlewski, M. (2021). Initial contact shapes the perception of friction. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A 118:e2109109118.

Google Scholar

Yu, Z., Guindani, M., Grieco, S. F., Chen, L., Holmes, T. C., and Xu, X. (2022). Beyond t test and ANOVA: Applications of mixed-effects models for more rigorous statistical analysis in neuroscience research. Neuron 110, 21–35. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2021.10.030

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zabicki, A., De Haas, B., Zentgraf, K., Stark, R., Munzert, J., and Krüger, B. (2017). Imagined and executed actions in the human motor system: Testing neural similarity between execution and imagery of actions with a multivariate approach. Cereb. Cortex 27, 4523–4536. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw257

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: action imagination, force production, social touch, motor cortex, transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS)

Citation: Ali Y, Montani V and Cesari P (2024) Neural underpinnings of the interplay between actual touch and action imagination in social contexts. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 17:1274299. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2023.1274299

Received: 08 August 2023; Accepted: 21 December 2023;
Published: 11 January 2024.

Edited by:

Nadia Dominici, VU Amsterdam, Netherlands

Reviewed by:

Rafaela Ramos Campagnoli, Fluminense Federal University, Brazil
Magdalena Ietswaart, University of Stirling, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2024 Ali, Montani and Cesari. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Paola Cesari, paola.cesari@univr.it

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.