Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 30 August 2023
Sec. Brain Health and Clinical Neuroscience
This article is part of the Research Topic Women in Brain Health and Clinical Neuroscience Volume II: 2023 View all 6 articles

Gray space and default mode network-amygdala connectivity

  • 1Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee, Milwaukee, WI, United States
  • 2Department of Population and Public Health Sciences, Keck School of Medicine, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, United States

Introduction: Aspects of the built environment relate to health factors and equity in living conditions, and may contribute to racial, ethnic, or economic health disparities. For example, urbanicity is linked with negative factors including exposure to gray space (e.g., impervious surfaces such as concrete, streets, or rooftops). While there is existing research on access to green space and urbanicity on some mental health and cognitive outcomes, there is limited research on the presence of gray space linked with cognitive functioning in youth. The goal of this study was to investigate the link between gray space and amygdala-default mode network (DMN) connectivity.

Methods: This study used data from the ABCD Study. Participants (n = 10,144; age M = 119.11 months, female = 47.62%) underwent resting-state fMRI acquisition at baseline. Impervious surfaces (gray space) were measured via the Child Opportunity Index (COI). To examine the relationship between presence of gray space and -amygdala-DMN (left/right) connectivity, we employed linear mixed effects models. Correlations were run between amygdala-DMN connectivity and internalizing and externalizing symptoms. Finally, post hoc sensitivity analyses were run to assess the impact of race.

Results: More gray space, adjusting for age, sex, and neighborhood-level variables, was significantly associated with increased left amygdala-DMN connectivity (p = 0.0001). This association remained significant after sensitivity analyses for race were completed (p = 0.01). No significant correlations were observed between amygdala-DMN and internalizing or externalizing symptoms.

Discussion: Findings suggest gray space was linked with increased left amygdala-DMN connectivity, circuits that have been implicated in affective processing, emotion regulation, and psychopathology. Thus gray space may be related to alterations in connectivity that may enhance risk for emotion dysregulation. Future investigation of these relationships is needed, as neuroimaging findings may represent early dysregulation not yet observed in the behavioral analyses at this age (i.e., the present study did not find significant relationships with parent-reported behavioral outcomes). These findings can help to inform future public policy on improving lived and built environments.

Introduction

The built environment plays a crucial role in cognitive, emotional, and brain development. The built environment can be made up of many types of land including blue (e.g., sea and coastlines), brown (e.g., pervious surfaces without vegetation), gray (e.g., impervious surfaces), or green spaces (e.g., vegetation). Importantly, the rise of urbanization in the United States has led to reduced access to nature and an increased concentration of gray spaces. Gray space refers to the presence of impervious land including buildings, concrete, parking lots, roads, and rooftops (see Figure 1; Acevedo-Garcia et al., 2020). While urbanization has many benefits to society, it is associated with increased levels of chronic mental health outcomes (e.g., anxiety and depression; Wang, 2004; Peen et al., 2010; Lederbogen et al., 2011) and biological stress responses (e.g., hypertension) that lead to oxidative stress, inflammation, and neuronal injury (Bernabe-Ortiz et al., 2017; Xu et al., 2018), which in turn may impact brain development among youth. Studies have provided evidence that built environmental factors during childhood and adolescence impact brain function and structure. For example, Rakesh et al. (2022) found associations between different socioeconomic status variables (e.g., parent education, income, and the Area Deprivation Index, a composite encompassing housing quality, rankings of neighborhoods, etc.) and child brain structure (e.g., reduced cortical thickness of frontal, parietal, and occipital brain regions). While studies have investigated factors of the physical built environment such as air pollution, heat exposure, and green space on brain health and neurodevelopmental outcomes, research has not investigated gray space exposure.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Example photo of gray space (e.g., roads, parking lots, rooftops) and green space (e.g., trees, grass). Photo by Tim Trad on Unsplash.

While causal relationships between gray space, brain development, and mental health among youth are not well understood, it is posited that access to nature may be a buffer against the development of mental illness via a decrease in rumination by reducing the biological stress response (Bratman et al., 2012; Dadvand et al., 2012, Sudimac et al., 2022). Furthermore, increased access to green space can provide more social opportunities and lead to a reduction in cortisol and a decrease in blood pressure, which can in turn improve emotion regulation and other mental health concerns including anxiety, depression, and stress coping (Fan et al., 2011; Cohen-Cline et al., 2015). Inversely, a lack of access to nature and increased exposure to gray space may reduce opportunities for the restorative effects of the environment (e.g., stress reduction, social opportunities). However, limited research has been conducted on the effects of gray space as a land type on brain connectivity in regions implicated in psychopathology (e.g., affective processing). Understanding the role of gray space in development of brain circuits linked to psychopathology is necessary, as gray space is associated with less access to nature, and, in some cases, with other risk factors, such as extreme heat and increased air and noise pollution (Harlan et al., 2006; Hajat and Kosatky, 2010). Thus, future research investigating prospective and large-scale neuroimaging are needed to identify potential neurobiological mechanisms that may underlie the link between the built environmental, including gray space, and brain development while considering complex social drivers of health (e.g., Fan et al., 2021).

The amygdala is implicated in many aspects of functioning, including emotion regulation, reward and threat processing, and memory (e.g., Phelps, 2004). Throughout development, the amygdala has also been shown to play a role in internalizing disorders such as depression and anxiety, as well as bipolar disorders, irritability, and other related mood disorders (e.g., Yang et al., 2010; Wiggins et al., 2016). One study found that lower SES (i.e., education) was associated with decreased amygdala volume and that, in turn, decreased amygdala volume was associated with internalizing symptoms (e.g., depression; Merz et al., 2017). However, the association with amygdala volume was only observed in adolescents and young adults (ages 13–21), and not younger children (3–12). Another paper (Sudimac et al., 2022), which investigated the effects of a 1-h walk in an urban environment vs. nature, found that amygdala activity decreased after walking in nature but not in an urban environment. These findings underscore the importance of understanding the effects of built environment on amygdala function and internalizing/externalizing behaviors.

The regions of the brain that are consistently active and interconnected at rest (e.g., medial prefrontal cortex (mfPFC), posterior cingulate cortex, angular gyrus) are referred to as the default mode network (DMN). The DMN has been shown to be related to numerous cognitive functions, such as social cognition, theory of mind, perspective taking, creating personal narratives, and daydreaming (Fox et al., 2005; Buckner et al., 2008). Moreover, the DMN, in relation to certain executive functions, is known to go through maturation (i.e., specialization) throughout adolescence (e.g., Sherman et al., 2014). Connections between the amygdala and regions in the DMN have also been implicated in the development and specificity of psychopathology. For example, increased connectivity between the amygdala and posterior cingulate gyrus, a central node of the DMN, has been shown to be related to higher rates of anxiety (Toazza et al., 2016). As such, amygdala-DMN connectivity makes for an intriguing target for investigation of the role of gray space exposure in the development of internalizing symptoms.

To our knowledge, limited studies have directly examined the effects of land characteristics on amygdala-DMN connectivity in adolescence (Kühn et al., 2017; Hackman et al., 2021; Rakesh et al., 2021). Studies have found adverse environmental experiences (e.g., low family income) associated with reduced volume within regions involved in the DMN (e.g., hippocampus, bilateral IPL, insula cortex, inferior frontal gyrus, right occipital, and mPFC; Noble et al., 2015). Rakesh et al. (2021) found that neighborhood disadvantage is associated with alterations in the DMN, and specific links have been reported between amygdala structure and forest coverage and green space in older adults (Kühn et al., 2017). Moreover, associations have been found between the amygdala and other aspects of lived environments (e.g., forest coverage, urban green space; Kühn et al., 2017). Forest coverage has been shown to be significantly associated with increased structural amygdala integrity in older adults (mean age = 70.1; Kühn et al., 2017).

Research has found that the presence of impervious surfaces (i.e., buildings, concrete, parking lots, etc.) is greater in neighborhoods with lower socioeconomic status and higher proportion of residents from minoritized backgrounds (Lu and Weng, 2007; Ogneva-Himmelberger et al., 2009). Urban settings with high levels of impervious surfaces (i.e., gray space), a lack of green space and vegetation, and greater density of buildings experience higher temperatures, greater heat stress, and greater thermal discomfort, especially in disadvantaged neighborhoods (i.e., areas with lower income and less access to resources for extreme weather; Harlan et al., 2006; Hajat and Kosatky, 2010).

This study aimed to expand previous literature on the link between socioeconomic status on amygdala-DMN connectivity by including built environment variables that extend beyond traditional operationalized variables of socioeconomic status. The Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study offers a unique opportunity to explore facets of the built environment among a large, diverse sample of the United States, with varying locations, socioeconomic statuses, and access to neighborhood resources. The goal of the present study was to broadly investigate the link between gray space on bilateral amygdala-DMN resting state functional connectivity (rs-fMRI) and associations with downstream behavioral outcomes including internalizing and externalizing symptoms. Particularly in a preliminary investigation, analyzing the DMN allows for a look into brain connectivity at a “baseline.” This type of imaging is particularly useful among youth, as performance on task-based imaging is less reliable and more variable in younger ages and across childhood. We did not have a directional hypothesis due to the lack of evidence from the literature supporting specific changes in connectivity in relation to gray space. As such, the current analyses present a preliminary investigation into the relationship between the presence of gray space, amygdala-DMN connectivity, and internalizing and externalizing symptoms.

Materials and methods

All protocols were approved by the University of California, San Diego, and local site institutional review boards (IRBs) and informed consent and assent were obtained from caregivers and youth.

Participants

The study used baseline data collected from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) study, a diverse, national, prospective, longitudinal study that recruited 11,878 youth and their caregivers (youth = 9–10 years old). Participants were excluded if youth are not fluent in English, had an MRI contraindication, major neurological disorder, gestational age less than 28 weeks or birth weight less than 1,200 grams, birth complications that resulted in hospitalization for more than 1-month, uncorrected vision, or current diagnosis of schizophrenia, autism spectrum disorder (moderate, severe), intellectual disability, or alcohol/substance use disorder.

At baseline, youth and one caregiver completed one to two in-person sessions, in which they completed a battery of assessments including mental and physical health (Barch et al., 2018), substance use (Lisdahl et al., 2018), peer, family, culture, and environment (Zucker et al., 2018), biological functioning (Uban et al., 2018), genetics (Iacono et al., 2018), and neuropsychological functioning, and MRI scans (Casey et al., 2018; Luciana et al., 2018). The current study utilized complete data from the demographic, residential history, and neuroimaging data (n = 10,144) (Fan et al., 2021). This project used ABCD 4.0 data release (2021) and was limited to a cross-sectional design using geo-coded primary address at baseline, as that is the most recently released data available.

Measures

Geo-coded variables

Primary residential address. Primary residential addresses were collected from each participant’s caregiver at the baseline visit from 2016 to 2018 including questions such as “At what address does your child live?” Primary residential address was defined as the child spending at least 80% of their time at this current address (e.g., a child may have up to three current addresses that they live at one time). Each primary residential address was geocoded by the Data Analysis, Informatics, and Resource Center (DAIRC) of the ABCD Study in which they used the google map application programming interface (API) to generate latitude and longitude (Fan et al., 2021).1

Defining gray space. The Childhood Opportunity Index 2.0 (COI 2.0) was used to derive the gray space variable. The COI 2.0 is a measure that is comprised for all neighborhoods using census tracts in the United States and lends an overall index health and environment including a measure of access to green space (Acevedo-Garcia et al., 2020). More information on this particular measure can be found in Fan et al. (2021) and the COI 2.0 documentation (Acevedo-Garcia et al., 2014; Noelke et al., 2020). Based on the COI 2.0 measure, impervious surfaces are defined as the percentage of any area or land that is covered by impenetrable materials (e.g., brick, pavement, roads, rooftops; see Figure 1; Noelke et al., 2020) surrounding the place of residence for 30-meter pixels based on satellite imagery that was then aggregated to the census tract level (Dewitz and U.S. Geological Survey, 2021). Noelke et al. (2020) computed access to green spaces by standardizing imperviousness values to a z-score and multiplying by −1. For the present study, we utilized this variable before values are multiplied by −1 (i.e., the percentage of impervious surfaces).

Neighborhood disadvantage. Using geo-coded primary residential address at baseline, variables from the American Community Survey, a 5-year estimate between 2011 and 2015 were linked to individuals using the U.S. Census Tract which was used to calculate the area deprivation index (ADI) (Kind et al., 2014). The ADI is compiled of 17 sub-scores to capture neighborhood level deprivation; however, we selected five distinct variables often used in the measurement of disadvantage as previously cited in other work (Sampson et al., 1997; Morenoff et al., 2007; Diemer et al., 2013; Hackman et al., 2021) including median family income, percentage of residents with at least a high school diploma, unemployment rate, percentage of families living below the federal poverty level, and percentage of single-parent households.

Parental education was measured by self-reported highest level of educational attainment of both parents. This variable was a categorical variable with the following categories. (1) <HS Diploma, (2) HS Diploma/GED, (3) Some College, (4) Bachelor, and (5) Post Graduate Degree. Household income was measured by asking the parent their overall household income. The item asks “What is your total combined family income for the past 12 months? This should include income (before taxes and deductions) from all sources, wages, rent from properties, social security, disability and veteran’s benefits, unemployment benefits, workman.” Responses included 1 = Less than USD 50,000; 2 = USD 50,000–100,000; 3 = USD 100,000 +.

Internalizing and externalizing symptoms. The ABCD Study used a computerized Child Behavior Checklist, a self-report measure administered to the parents of youth investigating common internalizing, externalizing, and social behaviors among children and adolescents (Achenbach, 2009).

Parental Monitoring and Warmth. At baseline, items from the Child Report of Behavior Inventory (CRPBI, Schaefer, 1965; see also Barber, 1997) were used to assess youth’s perceptions of caregiver acceptance and were used as covariates in both models. Items were reported by youth to record on their caregiver participating in the study, for example biological mother, and a secondary caregiver chosen by the youth (e.g., father, grandparent). Responses were on a three-point scale reflecting parental warmth and acceptance (e.g., “makes me feel better after talking over my worries;” “smiles at me very often”).

The Parental Monitoring Scale (Karoly et al., 2015) assesses the extent to which the caregiver participating in the study has knowledge of their child’s whereabouts and the degree to which that intersects with shielding their youth from health-risk related behaviors. Details on the social-buffering effects and sociodevelopmental theory behind this measure are described in Zucker et al. (2018). The Parental Monitoring Scale has five questions on a scale ranging from never (1) to almost always (5).

Neuroimaging procedures

Imaging protocols for the ABCD study have been outlined in Casey et al. (2018). See Supplementary material for more information.

The amygdala was chosen a priori as a region of interest due to its association with the default mode network, disadvantaged neighborhoods and the environment (e.g., forest coverage, urban green space; Kühn et al., 2017; Rakesh et al., 2021), and relationship to internalizing symptoms and mood disorders (e.g., Yang et al., 2010; Wiggins et al., 2016). Pair-wise correlations were used for regions of interest (ROI) within defined parcellations, including the Default Mode Network (DMN) Gordon network (Gordon et al., 2016). The correlation values were transformed into Fisher-z-values and examined between the DMN and the subcortical ROIs, left and right amygdala.

Analytic plan

To examine the relationship between the presence of gray space surrounding place of residence and amygdala-DMN connectivity, we specified two linear mixed effects models: One for left amygdala-DMN connectivity and one for right amygdala-DMN connectivity. In each of the linear mixed effects models, we specified one between-subjects factor (gray space) in relation to pre-calculated correlation values between the DMN and each respective amygdala seed. MRI manufacturer and relationship to family [i.e., siblings; in order to account for the nested structure of ABCD data (Saragosa-Harris et al., 2022)] were specified as random effects in the models. Age, sex, and five neighborhood disadvantage variables (median family income, percentage of residents with at least a high school diploma, unemployment rate, percentage of families living below the federal poverty level, percentage of single-parent households, parental monitoring, and parental warmth) derived from the ADI were included as covariates. Models were run with and without race and ethnicity as covariates due to neighborhood level variables potentially explaining more of the variance related to neighborhood-level variables of structural racism and racial segregation over and above individual level characteristics.

To examine the relationships between amygdala-DMN connectivity and internalizing and externalizing symptoms, we specified four correlation models, two for left amygdala-DMN connectivity and two for right-amygdala-DMN connectivity (with internalizing and externalizing symptoms separately for each).

Results

The present sample comprised n = 9,091participants (female = 47.60%; male = 52.40%; white = 67.39%; Black = 13.63%; Asian = 2.25%; Other = 3.85%; Multi-racial = 12.24%; American Indian Alaska Native = 0.64%; Non-Hispanic = 81.09%; Hispanic = 18.91%) at baseline of the ABCD Study between 2016 and 2018. Table 1 summarizes additional participant demographics.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic characteristics.

Left amygdala-DMN. More gray space, adjusting for age, sex, and neighborhood level variables, was significantly associated with increased left amygdala-DMN connectivity (t = 2.582, p = 0.001; Table 2, Model 1; see Supplementary Figure 1). When controlling for race, ethnicity, household income, highest level of parent education, parental monitoring, and parental warmth the model remained significant (t = 2.259, p = 0.02; Table 2, Model 2). For unadjusted analyses, see Supplementary Table 1. No significant relationships were observed from analyses examining the correlation between left amygdala-DMN connectivity and internalizing or externalizing symptoms.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Model summary tables: left amygdala-DMN and gray space.

Right amygdala-DMN. No significant relationship was observed from analyses examining the effect of gray space on right amygdala-DMN connectivity (t = 1.121, p = 0.263; see Supplementary Table 2) or from analyses examining the correlation between right amygdala-DMN connectivity and internalizing or externalizing symptoms.

Discussion

The goal of the present study was to investigate the effect of gray space surrounding place of residence on amygdala-DMN connectivity. There is limited research investigating brain connectivity and gray space relationships as it relates to social drivers of health. Due to the novelty of the present study, and lack of extant literature on gray space and brain connectivity, we did not include directional hypotheses. While framed as a preliminary investigation, the use of the ABCD Study dataset allowed for increased power in our analyses. The present study found evidence of a relationship between gray space and left amygdala-DMN connectivity. We did not observe a significant relationship between gray space and right amygdala-DMN connectivity. This suggests the environment, such as gray space exposure, has a specific effect on left amygdala-DMN connectivity. Future research should further investigate the mechanism behind this specific effect. Additionally, there were no significant relationships observed between right or left amygdala connectivity and internalizing or externalizing symptoms. These findings underscore the importance of understanding how environmental factors (e.g., different land types) can contribute to brain function – in particular, affect and cognition.

Results from the present study suggest that presence of gray space may play a role in the development of emotional processing/regulation (via the amygdala). For example, increased exposure to gray space surrounding primary residential address resulting in increased left amygdala-DMN connectivity may represent dysregulation between these regions. These results relate to amygdala findings that aspects of the lived environment (e.g., forest coverage) can impact the brain (Kühn et al., 2017; Sudimac et al., 2022). Given that Sudimac et al. (2022) observed a decrease in amygdala activity after a 1-h walk in nature, but did not observe changes in amygdala activation as a result of a 1-h walk in an urban setting (i.e., no change in pre and post fMRI scans), we wanted to extend this area of investigation by examining a baseline (i.e., prolonged vs. a change in) measure of gray space exposure on amygdala connectivity. The present study found a detrimental effect of gray space on amygdala-DMN connectivity. These results may identify the potential interplay between the environment (i.e., both gray and green spaces) and neural development which may predispose individuals to future mental health problems. Future research may benefit from an investigation of type of land, including both gray and green spaces, and neural development and subsequent psychopathology.

Evidence on the directionality of amygdala-DMN connectivity on downstream emotion dysregulation is mixed. Some evidence has implicated reduced connectivity between regions of the DMN, including the posterior cingulate, and amygdala with downstream emotional dysregulation in patients with bipolar disorder and major depression (Veer et al., 2010; Townsend and Altshuler, 2012). Studies investigating amygdala resting state functional connectivity and affective and cognitive networks, including regions in the DMN (i.e., precuneus), found abnormal (i.e., both increased and decreased) connectivity in adolescents with major depressive disorder (Tang et al., 2018). Moreover, other research has also found increased amygdala-DMN connectivity in adolescents and young adults with anxiety (Toazza et al., 2016). Research has suggested that throughout development –, from adolescence into early adulthood – the ability to down-regulate (e.g., via the prefrontal cortex to the amygdala) improves and becomes more effective (e.g., Silvers et al., 2014). For example, Silvers et al. (2014) found that, compared to adolescents, young adults were more effectively (i.e., longer lasting) able to down-regulate their amygdalae to aversive stimuli, as mediated by the rostrolateral PFC. Taken together, these findings provide additional support for understanding how aberrant amygdala-DMN resting state-functional connectivity may contribute to downstream emotion dysregulation symptoms.

Evidence has suggested there is a connection between increased amygdala connectivity to regions implicated in emotion and sensory processing and increased internalizing symptoms (e.g., depression and anxiety) among youth. For example, one study found increased internalizing symptoms among young females was associated with increased amygdala RSFC and regions implicated in emotional processing (Padgaonkar et al., 2020). Qin et al. (2014) found in a sample of 8-year-olds increased anxiety symptoms were associated with increased RSFC of the left amygdala and regions related to sensory processing (Qin et al., 2014). However, the present study did not find a link between left amygdala-DMN connectivity and internalizing nor externalizing symptoms. While these analyses did not reveal a link, the neuroimaging findings may be more sensitive than behavioral measures of emotion regulation, thus may be picking up on an aspect of dysregulation that self-report measures are not. Given that these findings were among 9- and 10-year-olds, it’s important for future research to investigate across the developmental span to observe changes throughout adolescence, which is the peak onset of internalizing disorders. Importantly, given that changes in amygdala connectivity have been shown to be related to the development of psychopathology across the lifespan (e.g., Yang et al., 2010; Wiggins et al., 2016), further elucidating these relationships is imperative for both understanding mental health outcomes in children based on lived environment and making policy recommendations related to gray space and urban spaces.

According to Brown et al. (2019), scientists should include health inequities in the context of their discussion of their findings and dissemination of their results. Using the Coll et al. (1996) model of development, we controlled for variables of neighborhood level factors (median family income, percentage of residents with at least a high school diploma, unemployment rate, percentage of families living below the federal poverty level, and percentage of single-parent households) when looking at elements of the built and natural environment on brain development to incorporate an individual’s social position and environment in our model. In particular, the results from the present study, which showed that amygdala connectivity was not significantly linked with race or ethnicity, helps demonstrate how investigating other upstream social and structural drivers of health (e.g., gray space) is necessary for fully understanding brain-environment relationships as they relate to health inequity.

There is limited research in clinical science that addresses the direct impact of systemic and structural biases on health outcomes, particularly among youth. Moreover, there is little research investigating the mechanisms underlying health disparities that go beyond observed differences among individuals, particularly among minoritized backgrounds including ethnic, racial, and socio-economically disadvantaged backgrounds (Causadias et al., 2018). Historically, however, psychological science has investigated white backgrounds, and has seldom strayed beyond investigating observed differences, specifically neglecting the impact of how cultural and economic privilege shapes health outcomes that exist among white and more affluent individuals, suggesting a persistent bias in scientific research (Causadias et al., 2018) and perpetuation of scientific inequities. And, even in instances when race/ethnicity or socioeconomic status are taken into account (e.g., controlled for statistically), they are being used as proxy variables for these types of social drivers of health. Thus, psychological science should investigate the role of culture, systems, and environment among the development of all youth to shape theory, interventions, and public policy.

Limitations

While our preliminary findings suggest that the presence of gray space in the youth’s lived environments is associated with increased left amygdala-DMN connectivity, there are several limitations that need to be discussed. First, it has been cited that the ABCD Study participants that are recommended for analysis (e.g., low noise data, low average motion, etc.) are more socioeconomically advantaged, less diverse, perform higher on neurocognitive assessments, and report better physical and mental health outcomes relative to those within the sample with higher noise and more restricted samples (Cosgrove et al., 2022). Furthermore, while a strength of the ABCD Study is that participants were recruited from schools which were selected based on characteristics such as race, ethnicity, SES, sex, and urbanicity (Garavan et al., 2018), participants were limited to 21 study sites which may contribute to the ecological validity of these findings and confounding factors. However, our current study attempts to consider historical and socio-cultural context and systemic biases as it relates to brain connectivity. Second, the ABCD Study data set does not include estimates for the presence of gray space across the lifetime, and this sample was limited to primary address at baseline. Thus, our sample included individuals that have lived at their current primary address for at least 80% of their time throughout the week, and individuals who may have stayed at this address for as little as a month up to 100% of their lifetime, limiting the generalizability of our findings. Once residential history has been collected in full for each participant, it will be imperative to assess lifetime exposure of gray space. Third, our findings are preliminary as they are cross-sectional and have relatively small effect sizes, thus we are limited in making a causal inference, particularly as this study occurred at the ages of 9–11, and brain development is an ongoing process that continues through young adulthood. Fourth, the dysregulation of the left amygdala-DMN connectivity could possibly be related to other factors (e.g., heat exposure, air pollutants, walkability, population density, etc.). Finally, given how the gray space variable is calculated in the ABCD Study, there are limitations on how much the data can be parsed. For example, the gray space variable in the present study is singular in nature, which, therefore, means it cannot be separated to represent different aspects of the environment (e.g., green and blue space). Studies with additional – and more detailed – measures of the built and natural environment will be able to better investigate these differences and their differential impacts. Future research is needed to determine the relationship between gray space exposure, green space, and other resilience factors, such as markers of distinct neighborhood advantage, intertwined with gray space exposure on brain development from early childhood into adolescence and young adulthood that is available within the ABCD study and other prospective studies.

Conclusion

Our findings, presented as a preliminary investigation into the association between gray space and default mode network-amygdala connectivity, provide evidence that aspects of built environment are linked with brain connectivity in youth aged 9–11. Importantly, these findings add to a small – but growing – body of evidence underscoring the importance of public health relevance in investigating the impact of built environment on affective neural connectivity during development. Moreover, the present study, in line with previous literature, suggests that increased gray space is linked with dysregulation of the amygdala and default mode network. In addition to future work, which can build on and expand these findings, the present study can help to inform future policy on improving lived and built environments.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in this study are included in this article/Supplementary material. The code and corresponding descriptions of the statistical procedures for the current project can be found at: https://github.com/harri585/grayspace/blob/9ef1eebb210350347541f235542f5592a208fb07/grayspace_github.R. Further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by the UC San Diego Institutional Review Board. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardians/next of kin.

Author contributions

JH and ML: conceptualization and writing—original draft preparation. KL, CL, and CC-I: methodology and supervision, project administration, and funding acquisition. JH, ML, and CC-I: software and formal analysis. All authors contributed to review and editing and have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

Data used in the preparation of this article were obtained from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive DevelopmentSM (ABCD) Study (https://abcdstudy.org), held in the NIMH Data Archive (NDA). This is a multisite, longitudinal study designed to recruit more than 10,000 children age 9–10 and follow them over 10 years into early adulthood. The ABCD Study® is supported by the National Institutes of Health and additional federal partners under award numbers U01DA041048, U01DA050989, U01DA051016, U01DA041022, U01DA051018, U01DA051037, U01DA050987, U01DA041174, U01DA041106, U01DA041117, U01DA041028, U01DA041134, U01DA050988, U01DA051039, U01DA041156, U01DA041025, U01DA041120, U01DA051038, U01DA041148, U01DA041093, U01DA041089, U24DA041123, and U24DA041147. A full list of supporters is available at https://abcdstudy.org/federal-partners.html. A listing of participating sites and a complete listing of the study investigators can be found at https://abcdstudy.org/consortium_members/. ABCD consortium investigators designed and implemented the study and/or provided data but did not necessarily participate in the analysis or writing of this report. This manuscript reflects the views of the authors and may not reflect the opinions or views of the NIH or ABCD consortium investigators. The ABCD data repository grows and changes over time. The ABCD data used in this report came from the ABCD 4.0 data release (https://nda.nih.gov/study.html?id=1299). DOIs can be found at 10.15154/1523041. ML was supported by the National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences, National Institutes of Health, through grant number TL1TR001437. Its contents are solely the responsibility of the authors and do not necessarily represent the official views of the NIH. Additional support for this work was made possible from NIEHS R01-ES032295 and R01-ES031074. CC-I was also supported by NIEHS P30-ES007048 and T32-ES013678, NINDS R25-NS089462, R25-NS094094, and R25-NS117356.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnhum.2023.1167786/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

  1. ^ https://developers.google.com/maps/documentation/elevation/overview

References

Acevedo-Garcia, D., McArdle, N., Hardy, E., Crisan, U., Romano, B., Norris, D., et al. (2014). The child opportunity index: improving collaboration between community development and public health. Health Affairs 33, 1948–1957. doi: 10.1377/hlthaff.2014.0679

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Acevedo-Garcia, D., Noelke, C., McArdle, N., Sofer, N., Hardy, E. F., Weiner, M., et al. (2020). Racial and ethnic inequities in children’s neighborhoods: Evidence from the new child opportunity index 2.0. Health Aff. (Millwood) 39, 1693–1701. doi: 10.1377/hlthaff.2020.00735

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Achenbach, T. M. (2009). The Achenbach system of empirically based assessment (ASEBA): Development, findings, theory, and applications. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont.

Google Scholar

Barber, B. K. (1997). Introduction: Adolescent socialization in context—the role of connection, regulation, and autonomy in the family. J. Adolesc. Res. 12, 5–11. doi: 10.1177/0743554897121002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barch, D. M., Albaugh, M. D., Avenevoli, S., Chang, L., Clark, D. B., Glantz, M. D., et al. (2018). Demographic, physical and mental health assessments in the adolescent brain and cognitive development study: Rationale and description. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 32, 55–66. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2017.10.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bernabe-Ortiz, A., Sanchez, J. F., Carrillo-Larco, R. M., Gilman, R. H., Poterico, J. A., Quispe, R., et al. (2017). Rural-to-urban migration and risk of hypertension: Longitudinal results of the PERU MIGRANT study. J. Hum. Hypertens. 31, 22–28. doi: 10.1038/jhh.2015.124

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bratman, G. N., Hamilton, J. P., and Daily, G. C. (2012). The impacts of nature experience on human cognitive function and mental health. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1249, 118–136. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06400.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, A. F., Ma, G. X., Miranda, J., Eng, E., Castille, D., Brockie, T., et al. (2019). Structural interventions to reduce and eliminate health disparities. Am. J. Public Health 109, S72–S78. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2018.304844

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buckner, R. L., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., and Schacter, D. L. (2008). The brain’s default network: Anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1124, 1–38. doi: 10.1196/annals.1440.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Casey, B. J., Cannonier, T., Conley, M. I., Cohen, A. O., Barch, D. M., Heitzeg, M. M., et al. (2018). The adolescent brain cognitive development (ABCD) study: Imaging acquisition across 21 sites. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 32, 43–54. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2018.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Causadias, J. M., Vitriol, J. A., and Atkin, A. L. (2018). Do we overemphasize the role of culture in the behavior of racial/ethnic minorities? Evidence of a cultural (mis)attribution bias in American psychology. Am. Psychol. 73, 243–255. doi: 10.1037/amp0000099

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen-Cline, H., Turkheimer, E., and Duncan, G. E. (2015). Access to green space, physical activity and mental health: A twin study. J. Epidemiol. Commun. Health 69, 523–529. doi: 10.1136/jech-2014-204667

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coll, C. G., Crnic, K., Lamberty, G., Wasik, B. H., Jenkins, R., Garcia, H. V., et al. (1996). An integrative model for the study of developmental competencies in minority children. Child Dev. 67, 1891–1914.

Google Scholar

Cosgrove, K. T., McDermott, T. J., White, E. J., Mosconi, M. W., Thompson, W. K., Paulus, M. P., et al. (2022). Limits to the generalizability of resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging studies of youth: An examination of ABCD study® baseline data. Brain Imaging Behav. 16, 1919–1925. doi: 10.1007/s11682-022-00665-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dadvand, P., de Nazelle, A., Figueras, F., Basagaña, X., Su, J., Amoly, E., et al. (2012). Green space, health inequality and pregnancy. Environ. Int. 40, 110–115. doi: 10.1016/j.envint.2011.07.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dewitz, J. U.S. Geological Survey (2021). National land cover database (NLCD) 2019 products (ver. 2.0, June 2021). Drive Reston, VA: U.S. Geological Survey data release. doi: 10.5066/P9KZCM54

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diemer, M., Mistry, R., Wadsworth, M., López, I., and Reimers, F. (2013). Best practices in conceptualizing and measuring social class in psychological research. Anal. Soc. Issues Public Policy 13, 77–113. doi: 10.1111/asap.12001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fan, C. C., Marshall, A., Smolker, H., Gonzalez, M. R., Tapert, S. F., Barch, D. M., et al. (2021). Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) study linked external data (LED): Protocol and practices for geocoding and assignment of environmental data. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 52:101030. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2021.101030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fan, Y., Das, K. V., and Chen, Q. (2011). Neighborhood green, social support, physical activity, and stress: Assessing the cumulative impact. Health Place 17, 1202–1211.

Google Scholar

Fox, M. D., Snyder, A. Z., Vincent, J. L., Corbetta, M., Van Essen, D. C., and Raichle, M. E. (2005). The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 102, 9673–9678. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0504136102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garavan, H., Bartsch, H., Conway, K., Decastro, A., Goldstein, R. Z., Heeringa, S., et al. (2018). Recruiting the ABCD sample: Design considerations and procedures. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 32, 16–22. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2018.04.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gordon, E. M., Laumann, T. O., Adeyemo, B., Huckins, J. F., Kelley, W. M., and Petersen, S. E. (2016). Generation and evaluation of a cortical area parcellation from resting-state correlations. Cereb. Cortex 26, 288–303. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu239

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hackman, D. A., Cserbik, D., Chen, J. C., Berhane, K., Minaravesh, B., McConnell, R., et al. (2021). Association of local variation in neighborhood disadvantage in metropolitan areas with youth neurocognition and brain structure. JAMA Pediatr. 175:e210426. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2021.0426

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hajat, S., and Kosatky, T. (2010). Heat-related mortality: A review and exploration of heterogeneity. J. Epidemiol. Commun. Health 64, 753–760. doi: 10.1136/jech.2009.087999

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harlan, S. L., Brazel, A. J., Prashad, L., Stefanov, W. L., and Larsen, L. (2006). Neighborhood microclimates and vulnerability to heat stress. Soc. Sci. Med. 63, 2847–2863. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2006.07.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iacono, W. G., Heath, A. C., Hewitt, J. K., Neale, M. C., Banich, M. T., Luciana, M. M., et al. (2018). The utility of twins in developmental cognitive neuroscience research: How twins strengthen the ABCD research design. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 32, 30–42. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2017.09.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karoly, H. C., Callahan, T., Schmiege, S. J., and Feldstein, E. S. W. (2015). Evaluating the hispanic paradox in the context of adolescent risky sexual behavior: The role of parent monitoring. J. Pediatr. Psychol. 41, 429–440. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsv039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kind, A. J., Jencks, S., Brock, J., Yu, M., Bartels, C., Ehlenbach, W., et al. (2014). Neighborhood socioeconomic disadvantage and 30-day rehospitalization: A retrospective cohort study. Ann. Int. Med. 161, 765–774. doi: 10.7326/M13-2946

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kühn, S., Düzel, S., Eibich, P., Krekel, C., Wüstemann, H., Kolbe, J., et al. (2017). In search of features that constitute an “enriched environment” in humans: Associations between geographical properties and brain structure. Sci. Rep. 7:11920. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-12046-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lederbogen, F., Kirsch, P., Haddad, L., Streit, F., Tost, H., Schuch, P., et al. (2011). City living and urban upbringing affect neural social stress processing in humans. Nature 474, 498–501. doi: 10.1038/nature10190

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lisdahl, K. M., Sher, K. J., Conway, K. P., Gonzalez, R., Feldstein Ewing, S. W., Nixon, S. J., et al. (2018). Adolescent brain cognitive development (ABCD) study: Overview of substance use assessment methods. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 32, 80–96. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2018.02.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lu, D., and Weng, Q. (2007). A survey of image classification methods and techniques for improving classification performance. Int. J. Remote Sens. 28, 823–870.

Google Scholar

Luciana, M., Bjork, J. M., Nagel, B. J., Barch, D. M., Gonzalez, R., Nixon, S. J., et al. (2018). Adolescent neurocognitive development and impacts of substance use: Overview of the adolescent brain cognitive development (ABCD) baseline neurocognition battery. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 32, 67–79. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2018.02.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Merz, E. C., Tottenham, N., and Noble, K. G. (2017). Socioeconomic status, amygdala volume, and internalizing symptoms in children and adolescents. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 47, 312–323. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2017.1326122

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morenoff, J. D., House, J. S., Hansen, B. B., Williams, D. R., Kaplan, G. A., and Hunte, H. E. (2007). Understanding social disparities in hypertension prevalence, awareness, treatment, and control: The role of neighborhood context. Soc. Sci. Med. 65, 1853–1866. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2007.05.038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Noble, K. G., Houston, S. M., Brito, N. H., Bartsch, H., Kan, E., Kuperman, J. M., et al. (2015). Family income, parental education and brain structure in children and adolescents. Nat. Neurosci. 18, 773–778. doi: 10.1038/nn.3983

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Noelke, C., McArdle, N., Baek, M., Huntington, N., Huber, R., Hardy, E., et al. (2020). Child opportunity index 2.0 technical documentation. Available online at: diversitydatakids.org/research-library/research-brief/how-we-built-it (accessed June 10, 2022).

Google Scholar

Ogneva-Himmelberger, Y., Pearsall, H., and Rakshit, R. (2009). Concrete evidence & geographically weighted regression: A regional analysis of wealth and the land cover in Massachusetts. Appl. Geogr. 29, 478–487. doi: 10.1016/j.apgeog.2009.03.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Padgaonkar, N. T., Lawrence, K. E., Hernandez, L. M., Green, S. A., Galván, A., and Dapretto, M. (2020). Sex differences in internalizing symptoms and amygdala functional connectivity in neurotypical youth. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 44:100797. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2020.100797

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peen, J., Schoevers, R. A., Beekman, A. T., and Dekker, J. (2010). The current status of urban-rural differences in psychiatric disorders. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 121, 84–93. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2009.01438.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Phelps, E. A. (2004). Human emotion and memory: Interactions of the amygdala and hippocampal complex. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 14, 198–202. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2004.03.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Qin, S., Young, C. B., Duan, X., Chen, T., Supekar, K., and Menon, V. (2014). Amygdala subregional structure and intrinsic functional connectivity predicts individual differences in anxiety during early childhood. Biol. Psychiatry 75, 892–900. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.10.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rakesh, D., Seguin, C., Zalesky, A., Cropley, V., and Whittle, S. (2021). Associations between neighborhood disadvantage, resting-state functional connectivity, and behavior in the adolescent brain cognitive development study®: The moderating role of positive family and school environments. Biol. Psychiatry Cogn. Neurosci. Neuroimaging. 6, 877–886. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2021.03.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rakesh, D., Zalesky, A., and Whittle, S. (2022). Assessment of parent income and education, neighborhood disadvantage, and child brain structure. JAMA Netw. Open 5:e2226208. doi: 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2022.26208

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sampson, R. J., Raudenbush, S. W., and Earls, F. (1997). Neighborhoods and violent crime: A multilevel study of collective efficacy. Science 277, 918–924.

Google Scholar

Saragosa-Harris, N. M., Chaku, N., MacSweeney, N., Guazzelli Williamson, V., Scheuplein, M., Feola, B., et al. (2022). A practical guide for researchers and reviewers using the ABCD Study and other large longitudinal datasets. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 55:101115.

Google Scholar

Schaefer, E. S. (1965). A configurational analysis of children’s reports of parent behavior. J. Consult. Psychol. 29, 552–557. doi: 10.1037/h0022702

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sherman, L. E., Rudie, J. D., Pfeifer, J. H., Masten, C. L., McNealy, K., and Dapretto, M. (2014). Development of the default mode and central executive networks across early adolescence: A longitudinal study. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 10, 148–159. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2014.08.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Silvers, J. A., Shu, J., Hubbard, A. D., Weber, J., and Ochsner, K. N. (2014). Concurrent and lasting effects of emotion regulation on amygdala response in adolescence and young adulthood. Dev. Sci. 18, 771–784. doi: 10.1111/desc.12260

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sudimac, S., Sale, V., and Kühn, S. (2022). How nature nurtures: Amygdala activity decreases as the result of a one-hour walk in nature. Mol. Psychiatry 27, 4446–4452.

Google Scholar

Tang, S., Lu, L., Zhang, L., Hu, X., Bu, X., Li, H., et al. (2018). Abnormal amygdala resting-state functional connectivity in adults and adolescents with major depressive disorder: A comparative meta-analysis. Ebiomedicine 36, 436–445. doi: 10.1016/j.ebiom.2018.09.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Toazza, R., Franco, A. R., Buchweitz, A., Molle, R. D., Rodrigues, D. M., Reis, R. S., et al. (2016). Amygdala-based intrinsic functional connectivity and anxiety disorders in adolescents and young adults. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 257, 11–16. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2016.09.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Townsend, J., and Altshuler, L. L. (2012). Emotion processing and regulation in bipolar disorder: A review. Bipolar Disord. 14, 326–339. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2012.01021.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uban, K. A., Horton, M. K., Jacobus, J., Heyser, C., Thompson, W. K., Tapert, S. F., et al. (2018). Biospecimens and the ABCD study: Rationale, methods of collection, measurement and early data. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 32, 97–106. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2018.03.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Veer, I. M., Beckmann, C. F., van Tol, M. J., Ferrarini, L., Milles, J., Veltman, D. J., et al. (2010). Whole brain resting-state analysis reveals decreased functional connectivity in major depression. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 4:41. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2010.00041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, J. L. (2004). Rural–urban differences in the prevalence of major depression and associated impairment. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 39, 19–25. doi: 10.1007/s00127-004-0698-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wiggins, J. L., Brotman, M. A., Adleman, N. E., Kim, P., Oakes, A. H., Reynolds, R. C., et al. (2016). Neural correlates of irritability in disruptive mood dysregulation and bipolar disorders. Am. J. Psychiatry 173, 722–730. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2015.15060833

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xu, Y., Lindh, C. H., Jönsson, B. A. G., Broberg, K., and Albin, M. (2018). Occupational exposure to asphalt mixture during road paving is related to increased mitochondria DNA copy number: A cross-sectional study. Environ. Health 17:29. doi: 10.1186/s12940-018-0375-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, T. T., Simmons, A. N., Matthews, S. C., Tapert, S. F., Frank, G. K., Max, J. E., et al. (2010). Adolescents With Major Depression Demonstrate Increased Amygdala Activation. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 49, 42–51. doi: 10.1097/00004583-201001000-00008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zucker, R. A., Gonzalez, R., Feldstein Ewing, S. W., Paulus, M. P., Arroyo, J., Fuligni, A., et al. (2018). Assessment of culture and environment in the Adolescent Brain and Cognitive Development Study: Rationale, description of measures, and early data. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 32, 107–120. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2018.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: fMRI, resting state, gray space, amygdala, default mode network

Citation: Harris JC, Liuzzi MT, Cardenas-Iniguez C, Larson CL and Lisdahl KM (2023) Gray space and default mode network-amygdala connectivity. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 17:1167786. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2023.1167786

Received: 16 February 2023; Accepted: 09 August 2023;
Published: 30 August 2023.

Edited by:

Luigi Alberto Pini, University of Modena and Reggio Emilia, Italy

Reviewed by:

Mohammad Mofatteh, Queen’s University Belfast, United Kingdom
Brendon M. Nacewicz, Accelerated Psychiatry SC, United States

Copyright © 2023 Harris, Liuzzi, Cardenas-Iniguez, Larson and Lisdahl. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Julia C. Harris, aGFycmk1ODVAdXdtLmVkdQ==

These authors share first authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.