Skip to main content

HYPOTHESIS AND THEORY article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 14 June 2023
Sec. Brain Health and Clinical Neuroscience
This article is part of the Research Topic Women In Brain Health and Clinical Neuroscience View all 7 articles

Sensory processing sensitivity and social pain: a hypothesis and theory

  • 1Faculty of Biomedical Sciences, Università della Svizzera italiana, Lugano, Switzerland
  • 2Department of Psychology, University of Turin, Turin, Italy
  • 3Neuroradiology Unit, Diagnostic and Technology Department, Fondazione Istituto di Ricovero e Cura a Carattere Scientifico (IRCCS), Istituto Neurologico Carlo Besta, Milan, Italy
  • 4Faculty of Communication, Culture and Society, Università della Svizzera italiana, Lugano, Switzerland

Sensory-processing sensitivity (SPS) defined, as a personality trait, seems to be characterized by emotional sensitivity, and stronger reactivity to both external and internal stimuli. SPS can represent a risk factor for developing clinical conditions during childhood and adolescence. This personality trait is not to be considered a pathological clinical condition, however, can expose to greater environmental vulnerability. In particular, the recent studies about SPS can be contextualized to social situations that evoke traumatic and stressful emotional responses such as social exclusion. We hypothesize that highly sensitive people (HSP) are more vulnerable to social exclusion and social pain. This hypothesis could help structure new educational and intervention models designed to improve coping strategies and promote HSP’s psychophysical and social well-being.

Introduction

Highly sensitive people: conceptualization and assessment methods

Highly sensitive people (HSP) were first studied and described by psychologist Aron (1996), who identified people that are high in a personality trait known as sensory-processing sensitivity (SPS). Individuals with high sensitivity exhibit abnormal reactivity and emotional sensitivity to both external and internal stimuli which, in turn, might interfere with daily life. High SPS is seen in 20% of the populations studied (Aron and Aron, 1997); recently Lionetti et al. (2019) indicate around 30% of the people (Lionetti et al., 2018, 2019; Pluess et al., 2018). It seems equally distributed in both genders (Kagan and Kagan, 1994; Kristal, 2005). Importantly, SPS was identified as a one-dimensional construct, differing from introversion, emotionality, neuroticism, and shyness. SPS seems only related to high sensitivity (Aron and Aron, 1997).

The sensitivity trait represents the result of a careful strategy of processing environmental information, carried out before taking an action (Aron et al., 2012). This strategy can lead to two outcomes - more adaptive or less adaptive depending on the circumstances: increased awareness of environmental details or overstimulation resulting in anxiety or avoidance conduct (Aron, 2018). Research on this topic states that deeper processing (i.e., a more meaningful analysis of information) of stimuli would not be stimulus-specific but is the result of high responsivity to salient environmental stimuli (Aron et al., 2012). The evolutionary purpose of this kind of information processing is to utilize information obtained in situation A (where more details are noticed) to better predict the future and enact more effective responses in situation B (the same as situation A, where people practise the behaviors previously learned) (Aron et al., 2012; Aron, 2018).

It is important to underline that SPS is not to be considered a pathological clinical condition, however high sensitivity could expose to an increased risk of developing psychopathologies, especially during childhood and adolescence (Aron, 2018; Greven et al., 2019; Lionetti et al., 2022). In particular, High SPS children who have had an unhappy childhood or have been exposed to harmful environments would be more vulnerable to anxiety, depression, and high levels of perceived stress with physical issues such as pain or fainting (Liss et al., 2005; Benham, 2006). In contrast, children who had a happy childhood and grew up in a supportive environment would develop several adaptive advantages, even to a greater extent than non-sensitive children (Pluess and Belsky, 2013).

In such a case, it is possible to detect advantageous sensitivity, that is the propensity to disproportionately benefit from positive experiences and contexts (Pluess and Belsky, 2013, 2015). Conversely, individuals made vulnerable may have a greater propensity to succumb to the negative effects of adversity, as explained by the diathesis-stress model. The two evolutionary directions are not mutually exclusive (Belsky and Pluess, 2009; Bakermans-Kranenburg and van Ijzendoorn, 2011). In fact, the SPS would likely be a predictor of an increased likelihood of benefiting from positive contexts and situations although it exposes a person to greater potential vulnerability (Belsky, 2005; Pluess and Belsky, 2009).

A recent study explored the vantage sensitivity hypothesis to understand possible individual differences in response to psychotherapist treatment (de Villiers et al., 2018). According to the Authors, individual differences in response to environmental exposure can be explained by the diathesis–stress model, which conceives psychological vulnerability as due to the interaction between an individual’s inherent propensity for vulnerability, and some sort of external life stressor. Endogenous markers of vantage sensitivity appear to fall into genetics (5-HTTLPR); physiology (cortisol reactivity), and psychological traits (childhood negative emotionality) with several factors of vulnerability (Obradović et al., 2010; Ramchandani et al., 2010; Drury et al., 2012; Pluess and Belsky, 2015; Bozzatello et al., 2019, 2021; Longobardi et al., 2020; Morese and Longobardi, 2020). Several higher-order cognitive processes involve individual sensitivity markers and represent potential facilitators of vantage sensitivity (or environmental sensitivity more generally): attentional processes, reward sensitivity, social sensitivity, and stress reactivity (Drury et al., 2012).

Importantly, the concept of environmental sensitivity can encompass differential susceptibility, vantage sensitivity, biological sensitivity to context, and SPS at the same time (de Villiers et al., 2018). The centrality of the nervous system in that context can be evaluated in terms of “neurosensitivity,” which in turn would manifest itself physiologically and at the psychological level. In addition, the quantifiable SPS neurobiological trait influences the degree of sensory stimuli processing (de Villiers et al., 2018). Authors through the review of literature indicate interesting results about a growing number of studies that report that some people with heightened sensitivity benefit more from psychological intervention than others.

The implication of the above is particularly important: it means that environmental sensitivity factors explain individual differences in response to both adverse and supportive experiences, such as psychotherapy. Therefore, knowing individual sensitivity could help in the strategy and the design of clinical practice according to a tailored patient-centered approach.

The topic has been studied empirically, but the ability to predict clinical outcomes remains limited (Pluess and Boniwell, 2015; Nocentini et al., 2018). It has been reported, however, that targeting interventions can have a number of practical advantages, both financially and in terms of improving clinical services. Indeed, individuals with higher levels of sensitivity and responsiveness may require shorter interventions, while those with higher levels of resilience may require longer, more intense interventions or different strategies. According to de Villiers et al. (2018), this assumption can also be applied to educational and social welfare plans.

On the other side, SPS may expose highly sensitive people to an increased risk of psychopathology. Indeed, Individuals with SPS report, for example, increased frequency and distress of nightmares (Carr et al., 2021). SPS has been previously found in autism spectrum disorder (Joosten and Bundy, 2010), attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (Panagiotidi et al., 2020), mild cognitive impairment, and dementia (Rhodus et al., 2022).

Assessment approach: diagnostic factors and proposed rating scales

In 2012, Aron published a meta-analysis in which she analyzed 4 typical features of HSP: inhibition of behavior, greater sensory awareness, deep information processing, and stronger and more emphasized emotional responses. These are summarized and conceptualized in the acronym DOES (Aron, 2018):

Depth of Processing: deep information processing usually involves elaboration rehearsal, requiring a more meaningful analysis of information and leading to better recall. HSP exposes each stimulus to more processing, making comparisons and/or connections with past, similar situations, or other objects. Processing is slower and more painstaking and can be either conscious or unconscious. In HSP brain areas devoted to deep information processing have been found to be more elicited (Jagiellowicz et al., 2011). The area most activated is the insula, a core area for awareness of emotional and inner states, and salient stimuli (Acevedo et al., 2014).

Overarousability: excessive attention to environmental details in HSPs leads to overstimulation and early and high fatigue (Gerstenberg, 2012). HSPs are usually faster and more accurate, but at the same time, more stressed and exhausted than non-sensitive people, especially in impulsive and risk-taking behaviors (Gerstenberg, 2012).

Emotional responsiveness/empathy (emotional reactivity): HSPs are more reactive than non-HSPs when exposed to images with positive valence (Jagiellowicz et al., 2011). This phenomenon is reinforced according to the emotional valence experienced during childhood (Pluess and Belsky, 2013). HSPs are also more empathic and have better skills in mentalization and Theory of Mind (Acevedo et al., 2014).

Sensitivity to subtle stimuli and details: HSPs can notice small details that escape others. This is a matter of complex and careful sensory processing (Aron, 2018).

Considering the above, a reliable assessment approach to HSP is the use of standardized questionnaires such as:

• The Highly Sensitive Person Scale (HSP; Aron and Aron, 1997) with 27-item is a self-report to detect environmental sensitivity in adults. Scores > 14 indicate the possible presence of HSP traits.

• The Highly Sensitive Person Scale – Brief Version (HSP-12; Pluess et al., 2023) is a self-report that uses only 12-items to assess Environmental Sensitivity in adults.

• The Highly Sensitive Child Scale (HSC; Pluess et al., 2018) scale is self-report with 12-item designed for children and adolescents (between 8 and 18 years) to evaluate Environmental Sensitivity.

In addition to the DOES (Aron, 2018), other typical characteristics of a highly sensitive individual are considered in these rating scales. Examples are conscientiousness, creativity, spirituality, and love of nature, but also, higher physical sensitivity, a more responsive immune system, and higher pain sensitivity (Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. List of distinguishing characteristics of highly sensitive persons considered childhood adulthood comparison.

Current evidence: HSP, physical and social pain

Sensory-processing sensitivity represents a trait that highlights interindividual differences in sensitivity to environments that can encompass a wide range of internal and external stimuli, either conditioned or unconditioned, as well as physical environments, sensory environments, social environments, and internal events (such as thoughts, feelings, and bodily sensations for example for hunger and pain) (Greven et al., 2019).

The anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), the anterior insula (AI), and the primary and secondary somatosensory cortex (S1 and S1) have been considered as hubs of physical pain (Eisenberger, 2012). More recently, neuroimaging and neurophysiological evidence reported that noxious stimuli activated neural correlates and connectivity in several cerebral areas: insular cortex, prefrontal cortex, somatosensory cortex, ACC, thalamus (Mercer Lindsay et al., 2021).

Research on the neural correlates of sensitivity to physical pain associates it with cortical thickening in S1, posterior cingulate cortex, and the orbitofrontal cortex (Hsiao et al., 2020). Nonetheless, (hyper)activation of some of these regions in pain-sensitive individuals than in pain-insensitive individuals has been also observed (Coghill et al., 2003; Erpelding et al., 2012).

The lingering effects of this pain sensitivity may exacerbate the effects of brain trophism resulting from chronic pain. Chronic pain causes both central and pathology-specific gray matter alterations in several cerebral networks (Cauda et al., 2014). Common alterations have been detected in the prefrontal regions, post and precentral gyri, thalamus, cingulate cortex, anterior insula, basal ganglia, periaqueductal gray, and inferior parietal lobule. As a highly salient stimulus, pain constantly taxes the salience and attentional processing systems, interfering with cognitive abilities and consuming emotional and cognitive resources. Most available studies report an overlapping contribution of salience and attention networks in subjects with various chronic pain conditions and pathologies (Cauda et al., 2014, Palermo, 2022).

Most of the cerebral responses are activated when physical pain is present, but they can also be elicited when pain is not triggered by a physical stimulus (Mouraux and Iannetti, 2018). For example, similar neural underpinnings can be observed in social pain (Eisenberger, 2012). While physical pain is related to actual or potential tissue damage, social pain is associated with psychological distance from other people or social groups (Xiao et al., 2020). Indeed, social pain is considered a strong experience of discomfort and distress caused by ostracism (i.e., social exclusion or loss such as rejection from the others or the own group) that involve affective, behavioral and cognitive iatrogenic consequences (Eisenberger and Lieberman, 2004; Eisenberger, 2012; Wesselmann et al., 2012; Sturgeon and Zautra, 2016; McIver et al., 2019; Kiefer et al., 2021; Schwarz et al., 2021; Jauch et al., 2022). Physical and social pain share not only physiological-adaptive characteristics to the environment, psychological and behavioral manifestation, but also a strong capacity for interaction and interpenetration (Xiao et al., 2020). Nevertheless, social pain neurobiology has been studied for much less time.

The “Cyberball Game,” a virtual task in which participants pass the ball with two virtual players, has been used in neuroimaging studies to understand the neurophysiological basis of social pain (or ostracism). At first, both players pass the ball normally, but after a while they begin passing it only to one another, excluding the real player (Hartgerink et al., 2015). The consequences of exclusion from the game evoke negative feelings of discomfort and unpleasantness and activate neural areas devoted to processing negative affect, and shared with neural substrates of physical pain: the dorsal ACC, the subgenual ACC, and the anterior insula (AI) (Coghill et al., 2003; Masten et al., 2009, 2011; Eisenberger, 2012; Erpelding et al., 2012; Cacioppo et al., 2013; Novembre et al., 2015; Rotge et al., 2015; Auriemma et al., 2020; Hsiao et al., 2020).

The possible overlap of physical pain with social pain leads us to hypothesize that highly sensitive individuals are hyper-reactive to both types of pain, but to date there is no evidence on the relationship between pain sensitivity and high HSP scores.

A recent study on ostracism was concerned with understanding how behavioral and neural correlates could be modulated by social support (Morese et al., 2019). The authors administered the Cyberball Game to their normal sample, as modified by Novembre et al. (2015). Through functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI), participants were observed to activate their brains in response to receiving the emotional, appraisal, or no social support from a friend (Lo Gerfo et al., 2019; Morese et al., 2019). At the neural level, social-emotional support (hand caresses) reduced feelings of unpleasantness and reduced AI recruitment, a core brain area associated with negative emotional states both in social pain and in physical pain (Morese et al., 2019; Palermo, 2022). In contrast, social appraisal support (text messages) would not attenuate the effects, but on the contrary, would increase feelings of unpleasantness (Morese et al., 2019). On a neural level, the right temporal parietal junction (TPJ) is less activated (a component of the ToM network that detects inconsistencies and distinguishes oneself from others), resulting in a reduced need to comprehend the social situation (Lo Gerfo et al., 2019; Morese et al., 2019). In line with this finding, an increase in subACC activity (i.e., an increase in negative affectivity) was also observed (Morese et al., 2019).

Although there are no relevant studies on this yet, we can speculate, given the wide range of evidence (both neural and behavioral) in support, that a highly sensitive individual is hyper-reactive not only to physical pain but also to social pain. Consequently, social-emotional support and not just social appraisal support might prove to be an effective supplement to traditional psychotherapies.

In support of this hypothesis, a recent study conducted by Ren et al. (2020) suggested a possible relationship between pain sensitivity and empathy. Authors found that individuals with high pain sensitivity would experience higher empathy for other people’s pain, manifesting it through stronger emotional reactions. Importantly, a significant correlation between pain sensitivity and emotional empathy in the context of a mediation-moderation induced by anxiety, catastrophizing, and fear was found. Those behavioral findings are consistent with meta-analytic connectivity analysis on experimental pain (Palermo et al., 2015). A highly distributed perceptual set of self-regulation can prompt brain areas to elaborate information in which emotion, action, and perception represents an important role (Palermo et al., 2015). Highly sensitive metacognitive-executive functions (such as ToM and mentalizing) and the intensity of emotional reactions normally associated with highly pain-sensitive individuals can therefore be considered prototypical characteristics of HSPs. What has been described leads us to speculate that social pain resulting from the SPS trait may be significantly perceived as greater in HPSs.

Hypotheses

Understanding how hypersensitivity, negative situations, and expectations influence nociception is important to understanding how nocebo responses are neuromodulated in HSPs. The implications that result are of great clinical-applicative importance since new approaches for the management of physical and social pain in these people are needed.

Nocebo hyperalgesia could represent the outcome of negative anticipations leading to extended pain experiences (Tracey, 2010). Acute psychological stress affects nocebo responses and, consequently, the pain threshold, through its action on serotonin, dopamine, and norepinephrine (Palermo, 2022). Because dopamine and norepinephrine specifically potentiate the ability to process and modulate pain, it is critical that these neurotransmitters are well-balanced (Palermo, 2022).

Highly sensitive people are more prone to stress (Gulla and Golonka, 2021). The reason could be attributed to genetic mutations found in HSPs in the three neurotransmitters directly related to stress and pain tolerance (Chen et al., 2011). In a stressed HSP, neurochemicals are dysregulated, deteriorating the nervous system’s ability to cope with both short-term and long-term pain (Chen et al., 2011).

Anxiety, stress, a sense of overload, and catastrophism impact on general well-being (Gulla and Golonka, 2021) and pain expectancy (Palermo et al., 2015) and trigger hyperalgesia phenomena (nocebo effects), easily verified in psychobiological responses associated with pain anticipation (Palermo et al., 2015; Amanzio and Palermo, 2019). Therefore, the psychological mechanisms of emotion management become fundamental. Indeed, modulatory cortical networks involved in placebo analgesia largely overlap with those involved in the regulation of emotional processes, while nociceptive brain networks are downregulated in parallel with behavioral analgesia (Amanzio et al., 2013).

According to Gulla and Golonka (2021), attention awareness can moderate the relationship between HSPs and resilience to psychophysical stress, including pain. Indeed, the positive relationship between sensing subtle emotions and tolerating negative emotions is strengthened by attentional awareness. That finding is in line with a conceptualization of executive-metacognitive functions as predictors of disease prognosis and treatment compliance (Morese et al., 2018).

Conclusion

Highly sensitive people are those who perceive internal and external stimuli more intensely and deeply. This occurs because the trait of High Sensitivity, which is proposed to be partly innate (Aron, 2004) and partly genetic (Aron et al., 2012; Acevedo et al., 2014; Assary et al., 2021), is determined by a different functioning of the neurological system, which is more active and susceptible. In our hypotheses, high SPS decreases well-being in situations of social pain. Proper attention awareness and executive-metacognitive functions may thus be of great importance in improving well-being and protecting HSP from various stressors and, consequently, from the relapses of hyperalgesia or (chronic) physical and social pain.

What is presented here lays the foundation for future studies on HSP and social pain, with use of advanced neuroimaging techniques and exploration of determinants through the use of the proposed rating scales for HSP.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in this study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding authors.

Author contributions

LM and AI conducted the literature search and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. AC participated in writing. SP and RM conceived the content of the manuscript, supervised the writing, and wrote the last version of the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Acevedo, B. P., Aron, E. N., Aron, A., Sangster, M. D., Collins, N., and Brown, L. L. (2014). The highly sensitive brain: an fMRI study of sensory processing sensitivity and response to others’ emotions. Brain Behav. 4, 580–594. doi: 10.1002/brb3.242

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Amanzio, M., Benedetti, F., Porro, C. A., Palermo, S., and Cauda, F. (2013). Activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis of brain correlates of placebo analgesia in human experimental pain. Hum. Brain Mapp. 34, 738–752. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21471

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Amanzio, M., and Palermo, S. (2019). Pain anticipation and nocebo-related responses: A descriptive mini-review of functional neuroimaging studies in normal subjects and precious hints on pain processing in the context of neurodegenerative disorders. Front. Pharmacol. 10:969. doi: 10.3389/fphar.2019.00969

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aron, E. N. (1996). The Highly Sensitive Person. New York City, NY: Harmony Books.

Google Scholar

Aron, E. N. (2004). Revisiting Jung’s concept of innate sensitiveness. J. Anal. Psychol. 49, 337–367. doi: 10.1111/j.1465-5922.2004.00465.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aron, E. N. (2018). Persone altamente sensibili. Milan: Edizioni Mondadori.

Google Scholar

Aron, E. N., and Aron, A. (1997). Sensory-processing sensitivity and its relation to introversion and emotionality. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 73, 345–368. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.73.2.345

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aron, E. N., Aron, A., and Jagiellowicz, J. (2012). Sensory processing sensitivity: a review in the light of the evolution of biological responsivity. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 16, 262–282. doi: 10.1177/1088868311434213

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Assary, E., Zavos, H. M. S., Krapohl, E., Keers, R., and Pluess, M. (2021). Genetic architecture of Environmental Sensitivity reflects multiple heritable components: a twin study with adolescents. Mol. Psychiatry 26, 4896–4904. doi: 10.1038/s41380-020-0783-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Auriemma, V., Iorio, G., Roberti, G., and Morese, R. (2020). Cyberbullying and empathy in the age of hyperconnection: An interdisciplinary approach. Front. Sociol. 5:551881. doi: 10.3389/fsoc.2020.551881

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., and van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (2011). Differential susceptibility to rearing environment depending on dopamine-related genes: new evidence and a meta-analysis. Dev. Psychopathol. 23, 39–52. doi: 10.1017/S0954579410000635

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belsky, J. (2005). “Differential susceptibility to rearing influences: an evolutionary hypothesis and some evidence,” in Origins of the social mind: evolutionary psychology and child development, eds B. Ellis and D. Bjorklund (New York, NY: Guilford).

Google Scholar

Belsky, J., and Pluess, M. (2009). Beyond diathesis stress: differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Psychol. Bull. 135, 885–908. doi: 10.1037/a0017376

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benham, G. (2006). The highly sensitive person: Stress and physical symptom reports. Pers. Individ. Differ. 40, 1433–1440. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2005.11.021

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bozzatello, P., Morese, R., Valentini, M. C., Rocca, P., and Bellino, S. (2021). How interpersonal psychotherapy changes the brain: A study of fMRI in borderline personality disorder. J. Clin. Psychiatry 83:21m13918. doi: 10.4088/JCP.21m13918

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bozzatello, P., Morese, R., Valentini, M. C., Rocca, P., Bosco, F., and Bellino, S. (2019). Autobiographical memories, identity disturbance and brain functioning in patients with borderline personality disorder: An fMRI study. Heliyon 5:e01323. doi: 10.1016/j.heliyon.2019.e01323

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cacioppo, S., Frum, C., Asp, E., Weiss, R. M., Lewis, J. W., and Cacioppo, J. T. (2013). A quantitative meta-analysis of functional imaging studies of social rejection. Sci. Rep. 3:2027. doi: 10.1038/srep02027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carr, M., Matthews, E., Williams, J., and Blagrove, M. (2021). Testing the theory of Differential Susceptibility to nightmares: The interaction of sensory processing sensitivity with the relationship of low mental wellbeing to nightmare frequency and nightmare distress. J. Sleep Res. 30:e13200. doi: 10.1111/jsr.13200

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cauda, F., Palermo, S., Costa, T., Torta, R., Duca, S., Vercelli, U., et al. (2014). Gray matter alterations in chronic pain: A network-oriented meta-analytic approach. Neuroimage 4, 676–686. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2014.04.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, C., Chen, C., Moyzis, R., Stern, H., He, Q., Li, H., et al. (2011). Contributions of dopamine-related genes and environmental factors to highly sensitive personality: a multi-step neuronal system-level approach. PLoS One 6:e21636. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0021636

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coghill, R. C., McHaffie, J. G., and Yen, Y. F. (2003). Neural correlates of interindividual differences in the subjective experience of pain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 100, 8538–8542. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1430684100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Villiers, B., Lionetti, F., and Pluess, M. (2018). Vantage sensitivity: A framework for individual differences in response to psychological intervention. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatric Epidemiol. 53, 545–554. doi: 10.1007/s00127-017-1471-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Drury, S. S., Gleason, M. M., Theall, K. P., Smyke, A. T., Nelson, C. A., Fox, N. A., et al. (2012). Genetic sensitivity to the caregiving context: The influence of 5httlpr and BDNF val66met on indiscriminate social behavior. Physiol. Behav. 106, 728–735. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.11.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eisenberger, N. I. (2012). The neural bases of social pain: evidence for shared representations with physical pain. Psychos. Med. 74, 126–135. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3182464dd1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eisenberger, N. I., and Lieberman, M. D. (2004). Why rejection hurts: a common neural alarm system for physical and social pain. Trends Cogn. Sci. 8, 294–300. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.05.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Erpelding, N., Moayedi, M., and Davis, K. D. (2012). Cortical thickness correlates of pain and temperature sensitivity. Pain 153, 1602–1609. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2012.03.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gerstenberg, F. X. (2012). Sensory-processing sensitivity predicts performance on a visual search task followed by an increase in perceived stress. Pers. Individ. Differ. 53, 496–500. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2012.04.019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greven, C. U., Lionetti, F., Booth, C., Aron, E. N., Fox, E., Schendan, H. E., et al. (2019). Sensory Processing Sensitivity in the context of Environmental Sensitivity: A critical review and development of research agenda. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 98, 287–305. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.01.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gulla, B., and Golonka, K. (2021). Exploring protective factors in wellbeing: How sensory processing sensitivity and attention awareness interact with resilience. Front. Psychol. 12:751679. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.751679

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hartgerink, C. H., van, I., Wicherts, J. M., and Williams, K. D. (2015). The ordinal effects of ostracism: a meta-analysis of 120 Cyberball studies. PLoS One 10:e0127002. doi: 10.1371/jour-nal.pone.0127002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hsiao, F. J., Chen, W. T., Liu, H. Y., Wang, Y. F., Chen, S. P., Lai, K. L., et al. (2020). Individual pain sensitivity is associated with resting-state cortical activities in healthy individuals but not in patients with migraine: a magnetoencephalography study. J. Headache Pain 21:133. doi: 10.1186/s10194-020-01200-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jagiellowicz, J., Xu, X., Aron, A., Aron, E., Cao, G., Feng, T., et al. (2011). The trait of sensory processing sensitivity and neural responses to changes in visual scenes. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 6, 38–47. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsq001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jauch, M., Rudert, S. C., and Greifeneder, R. (2022). Social pain by non-social agents: Exclusion hurts and provokes punishment even if the excluding source is a computer. Acta Psychol. 230:103753. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2022.103753

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Joosten, A. V., and Bundy, A. C. (2010). Sensory processing and stereotypical and repetitive behaviour in children with autism and intellectual disability. Austral. Occup. Therapy J. 57, 366–372. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1630.2009.00835.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kagan, S., and Kagan, S. (1994). Cooperative learning. San Clemente, CA: Kagan.

Google Scholar

Kiefer, M., Sim, E. J., Heil, S., Brown, R., Herrnberger, B., Spitzer, M., et al. (2021). Neural signatures of bullying experience and social rejection in teenagers. PLoS One 16:e0255681. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0255681

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kristal, J. (2005). The temperament perspective: Working with children’s behavioral styles. Baltimore, MA: Paul H Brookes Publishing.

Google Scholar

Lionetti, F., Aron, A., Aron, E. N., Burns, G. L., Jagiellowicz, J., and Pluess, M. (2018). Dandelions, tulips and orchids: evidence for the existence of low-sensitive, medium-sensitive and high-sensitive individuals. Transl. Psychiatry 8:24. doi: 10.1038/s41398-017-0090-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lionetti, F., Klein, D. N., Pastore, M., Aron, E. N., Aron, A., and Pluess, M. (2022). The role of environmental sensitivity in the development of rumination and depressive symptoms in childhood: a longitudinal study. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 31, 1815–1825. doi: 10.1007/s00787-021-01830-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lionetti, F., Pastore, M., Moscardino, U., Nocentini, A., Pluess, K., and Pluess, M. (2019). Sensory Processing Sensitivity and its association with personality traits and affect: A meta-analysis. J. Res. Pers. 81, 138–152. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2019.05.013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liss, M., Timmel, L., Baxley, K., and Killingsworth, P. (2005). Sensory processing sensitivity and its relation to parental bonding, anxiety, and depression. Pers. Individ. Differ. 39, 1429–1439. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2005.05.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lo Gerfo, E., Gallucci, A., Morese, R., Vergallito, A., Ottone, S., Ponzano, F., et al. (2019). The role of ventromedial prefrontal cortex and temporo-parietal junction in third-party punishment behavior. Neuroimage 200, 501–510. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.06.047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Longobardi, C., Morese, R., and Fabris, M. A. (2020). COVID-19 Emergency: Social Distancing and Social Exclusion as Risks for Suicide Ideation and Attempts in Adolescents. Front. Psychol. 11:551113. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.551113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Masten, C. L., Eisenberger, N. I., Borofsky, L. A., McNealy, K., Pfeifer, J. H., and Dapretto, M. (2011). Subgenual anterior cingulate responses to peer rejection: a marker of adolescents’ risk for depression. Dev. Psychopathol. 23, 283–292. doi: 10.1017/S0954579410000799

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Masten, C. L., Eisenberger, N. I., Borofsky, L. A., Pfeifer, J. H., McNealy, K., Mazziotta, J. C., et al. (2009). Neural correlates of social exclusion during adolescence: understanding the distress of peer rejection. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 4, 143–157. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsp007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McIver, T. A., Bosma, R. L., Goegan, S., Sandre, A., Klassen, J., Chiarella, J., et al. (2019). Functional connectivity across social inclusion and exclusion is related to peer victimization and depressive symptoms in young adults. J. Affect. Disord. 253, 366–375. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2019.04.085

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mercer Lindsay, N., Chen, C., Gilam, G., Mackey, S., and Scherrer, G. (2021). Brain circuits for pain and its treatment. Sci. Transl. Med. 13, eabj7360. doi: 10.1126/scitranslmed.abj7360

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morese, R., Lamm, C., Bosco, F. M., Valentini, M. C., and Silani, G. (2019). Social support modulates the neural correlates underlying social exclusion. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 14, 633–643. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsz033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morese, R., and Longobardi, C. (2020). Suicidal Ideation in Adolescence: A Perspective View on the Role of the Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex. Front. Psychol. 11:713. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00713

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morese, R., Stanziano, M., and Palermo, S. (2018). Commentary: Metacognition and perspective-taking in Alzheimer’s disease: a mini-review. Front. Psychol. 9:2010. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mouraux, A., and Iannetti, G. D. (2018). The search for pain biomarkers in the human brain. Brain 141, 3290–3307. doi: 10.1093/brain/awy281

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nocentini, A., Menesini, E., and Pluess, M. (2018). The personality trait of environmental sensitivity predicts children’s positive response to school-based antibullying intervention. Clin. Psychol. Sci. 6, 848–859. doi: 10.1177/2167702618782194

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Novembre, G., Zanon, M., and Silani, G. (2015). Empathy for social exclusion involves the sensory-discriminative component of pain: a within-subject fMRI study. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 10, 153–164. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsu038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Obradović, J., Bush, N. R., Stamperdahl, J., Adler, N. E., and Boyce, W. T. (2010). Biological sensitivity to context: the interactive effects of stress reactivity and family adversity on socioemotional behavior and school readiness. Child Dev. 81, 270–289. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01394.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Palermo, S. (2022). “Neurocognition and placebo analgesia: Linking in functional magnetic resonance imaging,” in The Neurobiology, Physiology, and Psychology of Pain, eds R. Patel and P. Martin (Oxford: Academic Press), doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-820589-1.00028-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Palermo, S., Benedetti, F., Costa, T., and Amanzio, M. (2015). Pain anticipation: an activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis of brain imaging studies. Hum. Brain Mapp. 36, 1648–1661. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22727

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Panagiotidi, M., Overton, P. G., and Stafford, T. (2020). The relationship between sensory processing sensitivity and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder traits: A spectrum approach. Psychiatry Res. 293:113477. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2020.113477

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pluess, M., Assary, E., Lionetti, F., Lester, K. J., Krapohl, E., Aron, E. N., et al. (2018). Environmental sensitivity in children: Development of the highly sensitive child scale and identification of sensitivity groups. Dev. Psychol. 54, 51–70. doi: 10.1037/dev0000406

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pluess, M., and Belsky, J. (2009). Differential susceptibility to rearing experience: the case of childcare. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry Allied Discipl. 50, 396–404. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01992.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pluess, M., and Belsky, J. (2013). Vantage sensitivity: individual differences in response to positive experiences. Psychol. Bull. 139, 901–916. doi: 10.1037/a0030196

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pluess, M., and Belsky, J. (2015). “Vantage sensitivity: genetic susceptibility to effects of positive experiences,” in Genetics of psychological well-being, ed. M. Pluess (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 193–120. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.927411

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pluess, M., and Boniwell, I. (2015). Sensory-Processing Sensitivity predicts treatment response to a school-based depression preven-tion program: evidence of vantage sensitivity. Pers. Individ. Differ. 82, 40–45. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2015.03.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pluess, M., Lionetti, F., Aron, E. N., and Aron, A. (2023). People differ in their sensitivity to the environment: An integrated theory, measurement and empirical evidence. J. Res. Pers. 104:104377. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2023.104377

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramchandani, P. G., Jzendoorn, M. V., and Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (2010). Differential susceptibility to fathers’ care and involvement: The moderating effect of infant reactivity. Fam. Sci. 1, 93–101. doi: 10.1080/19424621003599835

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ren, Q., Lu, X., Zhao, Q., Zhang, H., and Hu, L. (2020). Can self-pain sensitivity quantify empathy for others’ pain? Psychophysiology 57:e13637. doi: 10.1111/psyp.13637

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rhodus, E. K., Hunter, E. G., Rowles, G. D., Bardach, S. H., Parsons, K., Barber, J., et al. (2022). Sensory processing abnormalities in community-dwelling older adults with cognitive impairment: A mixed methods study. Gerontol. Geriatr. Med. 8:23337214211068290. doi: 10.1177/23337214211068290

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rotge, J. Y., Lemogne, C., Hinfray, S., Huguet, P., Grynszpan, O., Tartour, E., et al. (2015). A meta-analysis of the anterior cingulate contribution to social pain. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 10, 19–27. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsu110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwarz, S. M., Feike, M., and Stangier, U. (2021). Mental imagery and social pain in adolescents-analysis of imagery characteristics and perspective-a pilot study. Children 8:1160. doi: 10.3390/children8121160

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sturgeon, J. A., and Zautra, A. J. (2016). Social pain and physical pain: shared paths to resilience. Pain Manage. 6, 63–74. doi: 10.2217/pmt.15.56

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tracey, I. (2010). Getting the pain, you expect: mechanisms of placebo, nocebo and reappraisal effects in humans. Nat. Med. 16:1277. doi: 10.1038/nm.2229

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wesselmann, E. D., Nairne, J. S., and Williams, K. D. (2012). An evolutionary social psychological approach to studying the effects of ostracism. J. Soc. Evol. Cult. Psychol. 6, 309–328. doi: 10.1037/h0099249

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xiao, H., Zhang, Y., Kong, D., Li, S., and Yang, N. (2020). The effects of social support on sleep quality of medical staff treating patients with coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) in January and February 2020 in China. Med. Sci. Monit. 26:e923549–1. doi: 10.12659/MSM.923549

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: sensory-processing sensitivity, highly sensitive people, pain, social pain, Cyberball Game

Citation: Morellini L, Izzo A, Celeghin A, Palermo S and Morese R (2023) Sensory processing sensitivity and social pain: a hypothesis and theory. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 17:1135440. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2023.1135440

Received: 31 December 2022; Accepted: 24 May 2023;
Published: 14 June 2023.

Edited by:

Daniel Kamp, Heinrich Heine University Düsseldorf, Germany

Reviewed by:

Enrica Laura Santarcangelo, University of Pisa, Italy
Mark Blagrove, Swansea University, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2023 Morellini, Izzo, Celeghin, Palermo and Morese. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Rosalba Morese, rosalba.morese@usi.ch; Sara Palermo, sara.palermo@unito.it

These authors have contributed equally to this work and share last authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.