Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 12 July 2022
Sec. Brain Health and Clinical Neuroscience
This article is part of the Research Topic Nutrients and Brain: From Bench to the Bedside View all 5 articles

Motor Cortex Response to Pleasant Odor Perception and Imagery: The Differential Role of Personality Dimensions and Imagery Ability

\r\nCarmenrita InfortunaCarmenrita Infortuna1Francesca Gualano,Francesca Gualano2,3David FreedbergDavid Freedberg4Sapan P. PatelSapan P. Patel4Asad M. SheikhAsad M. Sheikh5Maria Rosaria Anna MuscatelloMaria Rosaria Anna Muscatello1Antonio BrunoAntonio Bruno1Carmela MentoCarmela Mento1Eileen ChusidEileen Chusid5Zhiyong HanZhiyong Han2Florian P. ThomasFlorian P. Thomas3Fortunato Battaglia,*Fortunato Battaglia2,3*
  • 1Department of Biomedical and Dental Sciences and Morphofunctional Imaging, Policlinico Universitario “G. Martino”, University of Messina, Messina, Italy
  • 2Department of Medical Sciences, Hackensack Meridian School of Medicine, Nutley, NJ, United States
  • 3Department of Neurology, Hackensack Meridian School of Medicine, Nutley, NJ, United States
  • 4Department of Art History and Archeology, Italian Academy for Advanced Studies, Columbia University, New York, NY, United States
  • 5Department of Pre-Clinical Sciences, New York College of Podiatric Medicine, New York, NY, United States

Background: Neuroimaging studies have shown a complex pattern of brain activation during perception of a pleasant odor and during its olfactory imagery. To date, little is known regarding changes in motor cortex excitability during these tasks. Bergamot essential oil (BEO) is extensively used in perfumes and cosmetics for its pleasantness. Therefore, to further our understanding of the human sense of smell, this study aimed to investigate the effect of perception and imagery of a pleasant odor (BEO) on motor cortex using Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS).

Materials and Methods: We examined the primary motor cortex (M1) excitability during perception of a pleasant odor (BEO) or perception of odorless saline (experiment 1). Furthermore, we tested the effect of olfactory imagery (OI) of BEO on corticospinal excitability (experiment 2). The increase in motor evoked potential (MEP) amplitude was correlated with personality dimensions scores, pleasantness, vividness, and general imagery ability.

Results: The results indicate that the corticospinal excitability changed after both perception and imagery of a pleasant odor (BEO). The correlation analysis shows an association with neuroticism personality trait (experiment 1) and with general olfactory imagery ability (experiment 2).

Conclusion: Both perception of a pleasant odor and its olfactory imagery modulate motor cortex excitability. The enhanced brain activation is affected by specific individual characteristics. Overall, our findings provide physiological evidence for a complex interaction between the olfactory and motor systems.

Introduction

The olfactory system has ancestral purposes, and it is integrated with other neural structures influencing attention, emotion, memory, and motor control (Riera and Dillin, 2016). For instance, hedonic tagging during olfactory perception guides behaviors such as social interaction, food consumption, reward, and emotional reactivity (Low et al., 2021; Faour et al., 2022; Han et al., 2022). Previous studies investigating brain activation during exposure to pleasant and unpleasant odors reported the presence of a hedonic map with activation of the medial region of the rostral orbitofrontal cortex (Grabenhorst et al., 2007), the posterior part of insula (Wicker et al., 2003), and the anterior cingulate cortex (Callara et al., 2021) after exposure to pleasant odors. Yet, little is known regarding the activation of motor related areas. The investigation of changes of corticospinal excitability during odor perception are of pivotal importance in understanding adaptive motor behaviors during perception of odors (either getting close to or moving away from the odor source according to the hedonic valence). It was demonstrated that some scented essential oils increase cortical excitability and animal studies have demonstrated that they may induce tonic-clonic seizures through the modulation of GABAergic neurotransmission (Bahr et al., 2019). In a study using transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS), it was suggested that pleasant odors associated with food facilitated the activation of the human motor system (Rossi et al., 2008). Moreover, the interplay between olfactory system and motor system during olfactory hedonic perception were not yet fully investigated. A previous study indicates a functional link between the olfactory bulb and the motor cortex with a specific pattern of oscillations associated with both odor’ valence and avoidance response (Iravani et al., 2021). In addition, the processing of odor pleasantness engages a large network including the precentral gyrus (Ruser et al., 2021). Hence, the use of TMS may provide additional information regarding the functional connectivity between olfactory and motor areas during exposure to a pleasant odor not associated with food. Bergamot Essential Oil (BEO) is derived from Citrus bergamia, which is grown primarily in Calabria, Italy. The oil triggers a sensation of pleasantness not strictly associated with food and for this reason is widely used in perfumery, cosmetics, and aromatherapy. Studies indicate that BEO has neuroactive components and may improve mild symptoms of anxiety, depression, and chronic pain (Rombolà et al., 2016; Scuteri et al., 2019). Motor responses to perception of pleasant odor not associated with food might be affected by different factors. For instance, personality traits affect sensory processing and perceptions of one’s environment. Studies indicate that both personality dimensions and cognitive factors modulate odor perception, discrimination, and identification (Larsson et al., 2000; Hedner et al., 2010; Seo et al., 2013). Thus, personality factors need to be considered in studies assessing brain activation and olfactory pleasantness.

Olfactory imagery (OI) is defined as the individual ability to generate the sensations associated with perception without direct olfactory stimulation (Arshamian and Larsson, 2014). Neuroimaging studies indicate that OI engages similar neuronal networks to odor perception (McNorgan, 2012). While neural responses to food odor have been shown to trigger a visual imagery which is an integral part of the “appetizing” aspect of food (Rossi et al., 2008) it remains to be demonstrated whether the OI of a pleasant odor unrelated to food/feeding may activate sensorimotor networks.

Considering the aforementioned research, we sought to ascertain the following: (1) is there an increase in MEP size during pleasant odor perception? (2) Is there an increase in MEP size during olfactory imagery of pleasant odors? and (3) Are these effects associated with specific personality dimensions? We hypothesized that both perception of pleasant odor and olfactory imagery will activate the corticospinal system, and that these effects will be associated with specific personality traits.

Materials and Methods

Subjects

Twenty-five right-handed (Oldfield, 1971) healthy subjects (12 women, mean age ± SD: 28.8 ± 7.2) were enrolled in the study. We followed recommended safety guidelines (Rossi et al., 2021) and excluded smokers, those with a history of neuropsychiatric diseases, under treatment with neuroactive drugs, and using alcohol. Furthermore, we excluded subjects who reported an impaired sense of smell due to infection, allergies, sinus pathology, neurodegenerative diseases, and trauma. The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the New York College of Podiatric Medicine, New York, NY, United States and conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. All subjects completed a written informed consent.

Experimental Procedure

This was a randomized, placebo-controlled, crossover study. The experimental design consisted of three sessions. During day 1, we assessed whether the participants met the inclusion criteria. The participants enrolled into the study underwent personality assessment (Big 5) and the self-assessment of olfaction Self-reported Mini Olfactory Questionnaire (Self-MOQ). Each subject anonymously completed the 44 items Big Five Inventory (BFI) (John et al., 1991). The personality assessment consisted of 44 short phrases with each response being recorded on a five-point Likert scale (1 = Disagree strongly, 2 = Disagree a little, 3 = Neither agree nor disagree, 4 = Agree a little, 5 = Agree strongly). The surveys were graded by experiments and five psychometrics categories including Extraversion, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, Neuroticism, and Openness were scored for each subject. The Self-MOQ is a 14 items instrument used for participants’ self-assessment of olfaction. Each of the 14 statements is scored as “totally disagree” score 0, “partially disagree” score 1, “partially agree” score 2, and “totally agree” score 3. Scores below 3.5 indicates a normosmic subject (Rubel et al., 2020; Zou et al., 2020; Fisicaro et al., 2021). We included in the study only participants that scored below this cut-off.

During day 2 (“perception”), we investigated the effect of bergamot essential oil (BEO) perception on Resting Motor Threshold (rMT) and Motor Evoked Potentials (MEP) amplitude. Subjects in the experimental group were required to nasally inhale the scent emitted from prepared fragrance blotter strips with BEO. To prepare these scented strips, two drops (0.1 mL) of BEO were placed on sterile fragrance blotter strips positioned on a metal holder (placed 1 cm below the nose). In the control condition we used the same amount of saline solution (Figure 1). According to the manufacturer (Essenze Bova 1997, Melito di Porto Salvo, Reggio Calabria, Italy), the oil was extracted via cold-pressed methods from Bergamot (Citrus bergamia) from Italy’s Calabria region. The active molecules in the BEO were characterized in a previous study (Sicari et al., 2016). The dose of BEO used in this study was in keeping with dosages used in clinical trials (Peng et al., 2009). After the experiment the participant were asked to rate pleasantness on a Visual Analog Scale (VAS) scale (from 0 = “extremely unpleasant” to 100 = “extremely pleasant”).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Study design and experimental procedure. (A) Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) stimuli were delivered over the left motor cortex. Olfactory stimulation was carried out by using paper strips soddened with Bergamot essential oil (BEO) or with saline. (B) In experiment 1 (perception), the participants were asked to inhale two drops of bergamot essential oil of saline positioned on a paper located on metal holder below the nose. The two interventions were randomized and separated by a 2-h break. During experiment 2 (Odor imagery), the participant was asked to mentally rehearse (odor imagery) the two conditions experienced the in the previous day. At rest, we recorded resting motor threshold (RMT) and twenty motor evoked potentials (MEPs). The same parameters were tested during the continuous exposure/imagery of BEO/saline.

On day 3 (“olfactory imagery), we first assessed the vividness of olfactory imagery using the vividness of olfactory imagery questionnaire (VOIQ) (Gilbert et al., 1988). Briefly, the VOIQ is a 16-items instrument split into four categories of odors: personal hygiene, food-related, tobacco, and vehicles. For each category, the participants were asked to image 4 different related odors and rate them on a Likert scale: (1) perfectly realistic and as vivid as the actual odor; (2) realistic and reasonably vivid; (3) moderately realistic and vivid; (4) vague and dim; (5) no odor at all, you only “know” that you are thinking of the odor. The mean VOIQ score was calculated for each participant (sum of answers/16). The vividness of BEO imagery was tested by using the same Likert scale. We then assessed the effect of BEO OI and Saline OI on rMT and MEP amplitude using the same experimental design.

Electromyographic Recording

We recorded motor evoked potentials (MEPs) using surface electrodes positioned in a tendon-belly arrangement on the right abductor pollicis brevis (APB) muscle at rest. Surface EMG was monitored simultaneously to ensure muscle relaxation. The signal was amplified, filtered (band-pass 2 Hz to 5 kHz), digitized at 5 kHz (Micro1401, Cambridge Electronics Design, Cambridge, United Kingdom), and collected for analysis.

Cortical Excitability

Participants were seated comfortably in an armchair with a comfortable arm support. TMS was delivered using a Magstim 200 stimulator and a figure-of-eight coil (9 cm) (The Magstim Company, Dyfed, United Kingdom). The coil was optimally positioned over the primary motor cortex at a 45° angle away from the midline. The rMT was determined according to the recommendations of the International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology (Rossini et al., 2015) and defined as the minimum stimulation intensity capable of generating resting MEP of at least 50 μV in amplitude in 5 of 10 trials. Upon determination of each subject’s rMT, twenty MEPs were collected, measured from peak-to-peak, and averaged using a stimulation intensity that was 120% of the rMT (Cantone et al., 2019). Each subject was tested in the morning (9 AM–10 PM) in two different consecutive days. During day 2, we assessed changes in rMT and MEP amplitude at rest and during olfactory stimulation with either saline or BEO. During day 3, we tested the same electrophysiological parameter before and after OI of the same conditions (saline/BEO). The control and experimental conditions were randomized. RMT and MEP amplitude were first recorded at rest (without exposure to BEO/Saline) and then during the exposure or imagery to BEO/saline.

Statistical Analysis

The data distribution was assessed using the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test. T-test was used to compare means at rest. Differences in rMT and MEP amplitudes were assessed using a repeated-measure ANOVA (main effect: Condition and Time). When variables were not normally distributed values log(x + 1) transformed for the repeated-measure ANOVA. If the assumption of sphericity was violated in the Mauchly’s sphericity test, the Greenhouse–Geisser correction was used. Post hoc comparisons using paired-samples t-test were used to evaluate the effects of perception and olfactory imagery on MEP amplitude. Spearman’ Rho’ correlation coefficient was used to assess the correlation between variables. We used SPSS version 23 Software (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, United States) for statistical analysis and p-values were considered statistically significant when <0.05.

Results

The experiments were well-tolerated without any adverse effect. Participants’ pleasantness of BEO score, VOIQ score, vividness of BEO OI, and personality dimensions’ scores are reported in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Mean ± SD scores for pleasantness of bergamot essential oil (BEO) perception, vividness of olfactory imagery questionnaire (VOIQ), vividness of BEO olfactory imagery (OI), and Big 5 personality dimensions.

Motor evoked potential values were not normally distributed and were log transformed for the analysis. We first tested the effect of perception of a pleasant odor on rMT and MEP amplitude (Experiment 1). At baseline, both rMT and MEP amplitude were not different in the two conditions (Saline: rMT = 40.28 ± 6.48%, BEO rMT: 40.42 ± 6.1%, p = 0.03; Saline logMEP: 0.28 ± 0.08 V, BEO logMEP: 0.31 ± 0.09 V, p = 0.14). Exposure to BEO and saline did not change rMT [main effect Time: Wilks’ lambda: 0.97, F(1,48) = 1.39, p = 0.24, η2 = 0.02; Group× Time interaction: Wilks’ lambda: 0.97, F(1,48) = 1.04, p = 0.31, η2 = 0.02]. Regarding the MEP amplitude, the two-way RMANOVA results indicate a statistically significant main effect of Time, sphericity assumed [Wilks’ lambda: 0.57, F(1,48) = 35.99, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.429] and a significant Group× Time interaction, sphericity assumed [Wilks’ lambda: 0.63, F(1,48) = 327.57, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.365]. Post hoc analysis indicated that MEPs were significantly larger during perception of BEO compared to saline (t = −7.14, p < 0.0001) (Figure 2).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Experiment 1 (perception): average amplitudes of logMEP before and after perception of either Saline or BEO. Perception of BEO significantly increased the amplitude of logMEP. Data are mean ± SD. **p < 0.01.

We next examined corticospinal activation during BEO OI (experiment 2). No changes in rMT were detected [main effect Time: Wilks’ lambda: 0.9, F(1,48) = 2.33, p = 0.71 η2 = 0.03; Group× Time interaction: Wilks’ lambda: 0.99, F(1,48) = 0.13, p = 0.71, η2 = 0.03]. On the contrary, olfactory imagery of BEO increased MEP size [main effect of Time, sphericity assumed, Wilks’ lambda: 0.52, F(1,48) = 43.1, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.47; and a significant Group× Time interaction, sphericity assumed: Wilks’ lambda: 0.72, F(1,48) = 18.18, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.315]. Post hoc analysis indicated that MEPs were significantly larger during imagery of BEO compared to saline (t = −5.24, p < 0.0001) (Figure 3). RMT and MEP amplitude were not different at baseline (Saline OI: rMT = 39.8 ± 7%, BEO OI: rMT: 40 ± 6.2%, p = 0.43; Saline logMEP: 0.29 ± 0.1, BEO logMEP: 0.31 ± 0.1, p = 0.17).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Experiment 2 (odor imagery): average amplitudes of logMEP before and after imagery of either Saline or BEO. Imagery of BEO significantly increased the amplitude of logMEP. Data are mean ± SD. **p < 0.01.

We next correlated the increase in logMEP size after perception of BEO (Δ BEO perception: 26 ± 16%) with pleasantness of BEO (r = −0.07, p = 0.72), extraversion score (r = −0.04, p = 0.824), agreeableness score (r = −0.016, p = 0.44) conscientiousness score (r = 0.1, p = 0.1), neuroticism score (p = 0.73, p < 0.01), openness score (r = 011, p = 0.58). Also the increase in logMEP amplitude after BEO OI (Δ BEO imagery: 36 ± 25%) was correlated with the VOIQ score (r = −0.47, p = 0.02), vividness of BEO OI (r = 0.01, p = 0.95), extraversion score (r = −0.31, p = 0.12), agreeableness score (r = 0.09, p = 0.64), conscientiousness score (r = 0.07, p = 0.71), neuroticism score (r = −0.27, p = 0.18), openness scores (r = −0.21, p = 0.3). Thus, the correlation analysis indicates that the neuroticism traits are associated with larger MEPs after BEO perception (Figure 4A) while the facilitatory effect of BEO OI on motor cortex is linked to the individual general olfactory imagery ability (Figure 4B).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Correlation analysis between changes in logMEP amplitude after perception of BEO and Neuroticism score (A) and changes in logMEP amplitude after odor imagery and vividness of olfactory imagery questionnaire (VOIQ) score (B). Lower VOIQ scores indicate better imagery ability.

Discussion

In this study, we analyzed relationships amongst olfactory perception, olfactory imagery, personality dimensions, and motor system. We found that both perception of a pleasant odor (BEO) and its mental rehearsal increase MEP amplitude. Furthermore, the increase in corticospinal excitability is correlated with specific personality dimensions and with imagery ability.

The increase of MEP after exposure to a pleasant odor we observed is in keeping with a previous report (Rossi et al., 2008) showing similar findings after sniffing alimentary odorants indicating that pleasant smells (both food and non-food related) activate the motor system. Exposure to food odors can trigger physiological (visual imagery and grasping) (Rossi et al., 2008), and biochemical (hormones, enzymes, and neurotransmitters) adaptations (Smeets et al., 2010) in anticipation of food consumption. By contrast, since the participants did not associate BEO to food, we can assume that the reported cortical activation after sensory stimulation depends upon the hedonic nature of the odor. There are several possible mechanisms underlying our results. It was demonstrated that systemic administration of BEO elevates the concentration of extracellular levels of aspartate, glycine, and taurine in the hippocampus of freely moving rats (Morrone et al., 2007). Furthermore, this effect is Ca2+-dependent suggesting that BEO may increase the presynaptic release of the excitatory amino acids (Morrone et al., 2007). BEO can induce behavioral arousal in rats (increased sniffing, grooming, and exploratory activity) which is associated with a cortical increase of power density in the alpha and beta frequency (Rombolà et al., 2009). Although these preclinical results were obtained using intraperitoneal BEO injections, evidence indicates that BEO administered via inhalation is pharmacologically active on the rodent brain (Scuteri et al., 2022) and that volatile components of BEO can reach the limbic system directly through the olfactory system or passing through the alveoli into the capillaries in humans (Brooker et al., 1997). Thus, it is conceivable that the effect on MEP amplitude may be due to the modulation of excitatory and inhibitory circuits in M1. In addition, it was demonstrated that occurrence of EEG beta rhythm before a magnetic stimulus is associated with the manifestation of MEP of a larger amplitude (Hussain et al., 2019). These data support the hypothesis that the increase in mean beta power induced by BEO could inhibit M1 layer II/III interneurons and excite layer V corticospinal neurons inducing, in this way, corticospinal gain output (Khademi et al., 2018). Future studies using different TMS and EEG paradigms are needed to further investigate this research hypothesis. A recent meta-analysis investigating the brain activation during perception of pleasant odor across studies indicated consistent activation of bilateral amygdala, orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), piriform cortex, insula, pallidum, putamen, and the central operculum (Torske et al., 2022). According to this activation pattern, it is likely that the hedonic encoding activates both the reward system (Okuda et al., 2003) and the corticostriatal loops subserving the stimulus-action-dependent reward circuits (Haruno and Kawato, 2006; Smith et al., 2009) which may explain the corticospinal activation to pleasantness we recorded. We can also speculate that the pleasantness of the sensory experience might convey a sense of beauty and consequent aesthetic processing which has been previously associated with limbic system activation, increases in motor cortex excitability, changes in connectivity, and sensorimotor integration (Battaglia et al., 2011; Concerto et al., 2015, 2016; Mineo et al., 2018b) indicating that motor cortex integrates information about rewards and prepares to performance (Galaro et al., 2019; Di Lazzaro et al., 2021).

We found that OI of BEO had a facilitatory effect on the corticospinal system. During mental imagery we simulate perceptual experiences. Previous works indicate that OI (like olfactory perception) activates primary olfactory cortex alongside limbic structures such as hippocampus, anterior cingulate cortex, and insula (McNorgan, 2012). Thus, the increases in MEP amplitude after BEO OI might rely upon the modulation of the same networks we have previously discussed explaining MEP gain during BEO perception. Studies have consistently revealed that the human imagery system comprises both modality-specific and modality-independent components (Daselaar et al., 2010). Regarding imagery of pleasant odors, it has been hypothesized that the olfactomotor activity during OI mimics that during perception (Bensafi et al., 2003) and that the process by which an olfactory image is created might depend upon the induction of nasal inhalation. Thus, the greater airflow during olfactory imagery (sniffing) became an essential component of the cortical activation necessary for odor encoding (Bensafi et al., 2003) and indicates activation of motor areas.

Although correlation does not mean causal link, our results point to a role of neuroticism traits and general olfactory imagery capability as important factors associated with motor cortex activation during the tasks. Nonetheless, it is to be explained why neuroticism traits during perception of BEO would induce stronger motor cortex activation. Given that research has shown that chemosensory sensitivity is linked to higher neuroticism traits scores (Kärnekull et al., 2011), it is possible that the higher emotional and cognitive reactivity associated with this trait might underlie this effect. For instance, during odor perception participants with high neuroticism trait display stronger activation in cingulate cortex (Hillert et al., 2007), a brain area thought to play a pivotal role in the interplay between emotion, cognition, and motor control (Paus, 2001). Hence, activation of limbic structure during perception and imagery of a peasant odor might trigger emotional arousal and consequently increase MEP amplitude (Borgomaneri et al., 2021). The strong connections between the olfactory nerve, the amygdala, and the hippocampus (Herz, 2016) support the evidence indicating that odor triggers autobiographical memories (El Haj, 2022), which have been associated with increased motor cortex activation (Mineo et al., 2018a). Thus, we can hypothesize a contribution of memory networks to the activation of the motor cortex during BEO perception and imagery. Regarding OI, the correlation of the MEP gain with imagery abilities we observed is consistent with the literature. Neuroimaging studies indicate that the participant’s difference in OI ability is mirrored in a different pattern of brain activation (Plailly et al., 2012). Future studies should address the role of expertise, odor expectation, and rehearsal of information previously associated with odor in OI facilitation of motor areas (Royet et al., 2013).

Furthermore, we cannot entirely exclude that some participants might associate BEO to food. Thus, the gut-brain interactions and their contribution to hyperexcitability (Pennisi et al., 2017; Lanza et al., 2018) should be further investigated to determine cortical responses to olfactory stimulation. Furthermore, the study of neural responses to odors perception and imagery with non-invasive brain stimulation techniques may provide useful information to better understand olfactory dysfunction and olfactory imagery deficits associated with COVID-19 infections (Tomasino et al., 2022).

Limitations

The current study has limitations. First, the study design did not allow us to perform blind experiments. Future studies using different experimental conditions (dose-response, different essential oils, and different TMS paradigms) are needed to further explore the effects of olfactory perception and imagery on motor cortex excitability. In addition, there are important variables that need to be considered to avoid biases. For instance, all our participants were college students. It was demonstrated that academic stress is highly prevalent in students (Infortuna et al., 2020a) and affects motor cortex plasticity (Concerto et al., 2017b,2018) and metaplasticity (Infortuna et al., 2021). Furthermore, health-risk behaviors in college students may affect cortical excitability (Concerto et al., 2017a; Mineo et al., 2018c; Infortuna et al., 2020b), and sleep quality (Sawah et al., 2015). We believe that the cross-over design of our study minimized possible biases; nonetheless, future studies should consider these factors.

An important aspect that was not investigated in our study is the contribution of subcortical sites (brainstem and spinal cord) to corticospinal facilitation. We used a rather strong olfactory stimulation that might have induced sniffing and consequent teeth clenching. A previous study indicates that teeth clenching facilitates corticospinal excitability at different levels: cortical, brainstem, and spinal (Boroojerdi et al., 2000). Thus, future studies should consider temporalis muscle and masseter muscle EMG monitoring to exclude trials with teeth clenching and include brainstem and spinal excitability protocols to ascertain the subcortical contribution to MEP facilitation. The use of paired-pulse paradigms could further our understanding regarding the role of intracortical excitatory and inhibitory circuits in facilitating MEPs during olfactory imagery and perception.

Conclusion

The study was well-powered to detect medium effect sizes. Our results provide novel evidence for a modulatory effect of pleasant odor perception and imagery on motor networks. Furthermore, our data point toward a pivotal role of personality traits and individual imagery abilities in mediating such effects. Future studies should address the clinical relevance of our results.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Institutional Review Board of the New York College of Podiatric Medicine, New York, NY, United States. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

CI, FG, SP, AS, and FB contributed to the data acquisition and analysis. DF, MM, AB, CM, EC, ZH, FT, and FB contributed to the interpretation of the data. CI and FB designed the experiments and drafted the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article, and reviewed and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Arshamian, A., and Larsson, M. (2014). Same same but different: the case of olfactory imagery. Front. Psychol. 5:34. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bahr, T. A., Rodriguez, D., Beaumont, C., and Allred, K. (2019). The effects of various essential oils on epilepsy and acute seizure: a systematic review. Evid. Based Complement. Alternat. Med. 2019:6216745.

Google Scholar

Battaglia, F., Lisanby, S. H., and Freedberg, D. (2011). Corticomotor excitability during observation and imagination of a work of art. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 5:79. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00079

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bensafi, M., Porter, J., Pouliot, S., Mainland, J., Johnson, B., Zelano, C., et al. (2003). Olfactomotor activity during imagery mimics that during perception. Nat. Neurosci. 6, 1142–1144. doi: 10.1038/nn1145

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borgomaneri, S., Vitale, F., Battaglia, S., and Avenanti, A. (2021). Early right motor cortex response to happy and fearful facial expressions: a TMS motor-evoked potential study. Brain Sci. 11:1203.. doi: 10.3390/brainsci11091203

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boroojerdi, B., Battaglia, F., Muellbacher, W., and Cohen, L. G. (2000). Voluntary teeth clenching facilitates human motor system excitability. Clin. Neurophysiol. 111, 988–993. doi: 10.1016/s1388-2457(00)00279-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brooker, D. J., Snape, M., Johnson, E., Ward, D., and Payne, M. (1997). Single case evaluation of the effects of aromatherapy and massage on disturbed behaviour in severe dementia. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 36, 287–296. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8260.1997.tb01415.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Callara, A. L., Greco, A., Frasnelli, J., Rho, G., Vanello, N., and Scilingo, E. P. (2021). Cortical network and connectivity underlying hedonic olfactory perception. J. Neural Eng. 18:056050. doi: 10.1088/1741-2552/ac28d2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cantone, M., Lanza, G., Vinciguerra, L., Puglisi, V., Ricceri, R., Fisicaro, F., et al. (2019). Age, height, and sex on motor evoked potentials: translational data from a large italian cohort in a clinical environment. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 13:185. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00185

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Concerto, C., Al Sawah, M., Infortuna, C., Freedberg, D., Chusid, E., Aguglia, E., et al. (2015). Neural circuits underlying motor facilitation during observation of implied motion. Somatosens. Motor Res. 32, 207–210. doi: 10.3109/08990220.2015.1048329

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Concerto, C., Patel, D., Infortuna, C., Chusid, E., Muscatello, M. R., Bruno, A., et al. (2017b). Academic stress disrupts cortical plasticity in graduate students. Stress 20, 212–216. doi: 10.1080/10253890.2017.1301424

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Concerto, C., Infortuna, C., Chusid, E., Coira, D., Babayev, J., Metwaly, R., et al. (2017a). Caffeinated energy drink intake modulates motor circuits at rest, before and after a movement. Physiol. Behav. 1, 361–368. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2017.07.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Concerto, C., Infortuna, C., Mineo, L., Pereira, M., Freedberg, D., Chusid, E., et al. (2016). Observation of implied motion in a work of art modulates cortical connectivity and plasticity. J. Exerc. Rehabil. 12, 417–423. doi: 10.12965/jer.1632656.328

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Concerto, C., Infortuna, C., Muscatello, M. R. A., Bruno, A., Zoccali, R., Chusid, E., et al. (2018). Exploring the effect of adaptogenic Rhodiola Rosea extract on neuroplasticity in humans. Complement. Therap. Med. 41, 141–146. doi: 10.1016/j.ctim.2018.09.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daselaar, S. M., Porat, Y., Huijbers, W., and Pennartz, C. M. (2010). Modality-specific and modality-independent components of the human imagery system. Neuroimage 52, 677–685. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.04.239

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Di Lazzaro, V., Bella, R., Benussi, A., Bologna, M., Borroni, B., Capone, F., et al. (2021). Diagnostic contribution and therapeutic perspectives of transcranial magnetic stimulation in dementia. Clin. Neurophysiol. 132, 2568–2607. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2021.05.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

El Haj, M. (2022). ODor-evoked Autobiographical Memory in Alzheimer’s disease? Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 37, 513–520.

Google Scholar

Faour, M., Magnan, C., Gurden, H., and Martin, C. (2022). Olfaction in the context of obesity and diabetes: insights from animal models to humans. Neuropharmacology 206:108923. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2021.108923

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fisicaro, F., Di Napoli, M., Liberto, A., Fanella, M., Di Stasio, F., Pennisi, M., et al. (2021). Neurological sequelae in patients with COVID-19: a histopathological perspective. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18:1415. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18041415

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Galaro, J. K., Celnik, P., and Chib, V. S. (2019). Motor cortex excitability reflects the subjective value of reward and mediates its effects on incentive-motivated performance. J. Neurosci. 39, 1236–1248. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1254-18.2018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gilbert, A. N., Crouch, M., and Kemp, S. E. (1988). Olfactory and visual mental imagery. J. Ment. Imagery 22, 137–146.

Google Scholar

Grabenhorst, F., Rolls, E. T., Margot, C., da Silva, M. A., and Velazco, M. I. (2007). How pleasant and unpleasant stimuli combine in different brain regions: odor mixtures. J. Neurosci. 27, 13532–13540. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3337-07.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Han, P., Su, T., and Hummel, T. (2022). Human odor exploration behavior is influenced by olfactory function and interest in the sense of smell. Physiol. Behav. 249:113762.

Google Scholar

Haruno, M., and Kawato, M. (2006). Heterarchical reinforcement-learning model for integration of multiple cortico-striatal loops: fMRI examination in stimulus-action-reward association learning. Neural Netw. 19, 1242–1254. doi: 10.1016/j.neunet.2006.06.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hedner, M., Larsson, M., Arnold, N., Zucco, G. M., and Hummel, T. (2010). Cognitive factors in odor detection, odor discrimination, and odor identification tasks. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 32, 1062–1067.

Google Scholar

Herz, R. S. (2016). The role of odor-evoked memory in psychological and physiological health. Brain Sci. 6:22. doi: 10.3390/brainsci6030022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hillert, L., Musabasic, V., Berglund, H., Ciumas, C., and Savic, I. (2007). Odor processing in multiple chemical sensitivity. Hum. Brain Mapp. 28, 172–182.

Google Scholar

Hussain, S. J., Cohen, L. G., and Bönstrup, M. (2019). Beta rhythm events predict corticospinal motor output. Sci. Rep. 9:18305. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-54706-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Infortuna, C., Gratteri, F., Benotakeia, A., Patel, S., Fleischman, A., Muscatello, M. R. A., et al. (2020a). Exploring the gender difference and predictors of perceived stress among students enrolled in different medical programs: a cross-sectional study. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 17:6647. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17186647

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Infortuna, C., Mineo, L., Buffer, S., Thomas, F. P., Muscatello, M. R. A., Aguglia, E., et al. (2021). Acute social and somatic stress alters cortical metaplasticity probed with non-invasive brain stimulation in humans. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 170, 1–5. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2021.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Infortuna, C., Silvestro, S., Crenshaw, K., Muscatello, M. R. A., Bruno, A., Zoccali, R. A., et al. (2020b). Affective temperament traits and age-predicted recreational cannabis use in medical students: a cross-sectional study. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 17:4836. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17134836

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iravani, B., Schaefer, M., Wilson, D. A., Arshamian, A., and Lundström, J. N. (2021). The human olfactory bulb processes odor valence representation and cues motor avoidance behavior. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 118:e2101209118. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2101209118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

John, O. P., Donahue, E. M., and Kentle, R. L. (1991). The “Big Five” Inventory: Versions 4a and 5. Berkeley, CA: University of California.

Google Scholar

Kärnekull, S. C., Jönsson, F. U., Larsson, M., and Olofsson, J. K. (2011). Affected by smells? Environmental chemical responsivity predicts odor perception. Chem. Sens. 36, 641–648.

Google Scholar

Khademi, F., Royter, V., and Gharabaghi, A. (2018). Distinct beta-band oscillatory circuits underlie corticospinal gain modulation. Cereb. Cortex 28, 1502–1515. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhy016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lanza, G., Bella, R., Cantone, M., Pennisi, G., Ferri, R., and Pennisi, M. (2018). Cognitive impairment and celiac disease: Is transcranial magnetic stimulation a trait d’union between gut and brain? Int. J. Mol. Sci. 19:2243. doi: 10.3390/ijms19082243

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Larsson, M., Finkel, D., and Pedersen, N. L. (2000). Odor identification: influences of age, gender, cognition, and personality. J. Gerontol. Ser. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 55, 304–310.

Google Scholar

Low, J., Seet, M., Hamano, J., Saba, M., Thakor, N. V., and Dragomir, A. (2021). Community analysis of brain functional networks reveals systems-level integration in olfactory hedonic perception. Annu. Int. Conf. IEEE Eng. Med. Biol. Soc. 2021, 5995–5998. doi: 10.1109/EMBC46164.2021.9629919

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McNorgan, C. (2012). A meta-analytic review of multisensory imagery identifies the neural correlates of modality-specific and modality-general imagery. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:285. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00285

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mineo, L., Concerto, C., Patel, D., Mayorga, T., Chusid, E., Infortuna, C., et al. (2018a). Modulation of sensorimotor circuits during retrieval of negative Autobiographical Memories: exploring the impact of personality dimensions. Neuropsychologia 110, 190–196. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.04.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mineo, L., Fetterman, A., Concerto, C., Warren, M., Infortuna, C., Freedberg, D., et al. (2018b). Motor facilitation during observation of implied motion: evidence for a role of the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 128, 47–51. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2018.04.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mineo, L., Sarraf, Y., Ingram, C., Hanauer, S., Infortuna, C., Chusid, E., Coira, D., et al. (2018c). Affective temperaments and stimulant medications misuse for neuroenhancement in graduate students. J. Subst. Use 23, 124–129.

Google Scholar

Morrone, L. A., Rombolà, L., Pelle, C., Corasaniti, M. T., Zappettini, S., Paudice, P., et al. (2007). The essential oil of bergamot enhances the levels of amino acid neurotransmitters in the hippocampus of rat: implication of monoterpene hydrocarbons. Pharmacol. Res. 55, 255–262. doi: 10.1016/j.phrs.2006.11.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Okuda, J., Fujii, T., Ohtake, H., Tsukiura, T., Tanji, K., Suzuki, K., et al. (2003). Thinking of the future and past: the roles of the frontal pole and the medial temporal lobes. Neuroimage 19, 1369–1380. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00179-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9, 97–113.

Google Scholar

Paus, T. (2001). Primate anterior cingulate cortex: where motor control, drive and cognition interface. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2, 417–424. doi: 10.1038/35077500

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peng, S. M., Koo, M., and Yu, Z. R. (2009). Effects of music and essential oil inhalation on cardiac autonomic balance in healthy individuals. J. Alternat. Complement. Med. 15, 53–57. doi: 10.1089/acm.2008.0243

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pennisi, M., Lanza, G., Cantone, M., Ricceri, R., Ferri, R., D’Agate, C. C., et al. (2017). Cortical involvement in celiac disease before and after long-term gluten-free diet: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study. PLoS One 12:e0177560. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0177560

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Plailly, J., Delon-Martin, C., and Royet, J. P. (2012). Experience induces functional reorganization in brain regions involved in odor imagery in perfumers. Hum. Brain Mapp. 33, 224–234. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21207

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riera, C. E., and Dillin, A. (2016). Emerging role of sensory perception in aging and metabolism. Trends Endocrinol. Metab. 27, 294–303.

Google Scholar

Rombolà, L., Amantea, D., Russo, R., Adornetto, A., Berliocchi, L., Tridico, L., et al. (2016). Rational basis for the use of bergamot essential oil in complementary medicine to treat chronic pain. Mini Rev. Med. Chem. 16, 721–728. doi: 10.2174/1389557516666160321113913

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rombolà, L., Corasaniti, M. T., Rotiroti, D., Tassorelli, C., Sakurada, S., Bagetta, G., et al. (2009). Effects of systemic administration of the essential oil of bergamot (BEO) on gross behaviour and EEG power spectra recorded from the rat hippocampus and cerebral cortex. Func. Neurol. 24, 107–112.

Google Scholar

Rossi, S., Antal, A., Bestmann, S., Bikson, M., Brewer, C., Brockmöller, J., et al. (2021). Safety and recommendations for TMS use in healthy subjects and patient populations, with updates on training, ethical and regulatory issues: expert Guidelines. Clin. Neurophysiol. 132, 269–306. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2020.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rossi, S., De Capua, A., Pasqualetti, P., Ulivelli, M., Fadiga, L., Falzarano, V., et al. (2008). Distinct olfactory cross-modal effects on the human motor system. PLoS One 3:e1702. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0001702

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rossini, P. M., Burke, D., Chen, R., Cohen, L. G., Daskalakis, Z., Di Iorio, R., et al. (2015). Non-invasive electrical and magnetic stimulation of the brain, spinal cord, roots and peripheral nerves: basic principles and procedures for routine clinical and research application. an updated report from an I.F.C.N. Committee. Clin. Neurophysiol. 126, 1071–1107. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2015.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Royet, J. P., Delon-Martin, C., and Plailly, J. (2013). Odor mental imagery in non-experts in odors: A paradox? Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:87. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00087

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rubel, K., Sharma, D., Campiti, V., Yedlicka, G., Burgin, S. J., Illing, E. A., et al. (2020). COVID-19 status differentially affects olfaction: a prospective case-control study. OTO Open 4:2473974x20970176. doi: 10.1177/2473974X20970176

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruser, P., Koeppel, C. J., Kitzler, H. H., Hummel, T., and Croy, I. (2021). Individual odor hedonic perception is coded in temporal joint network activity. Neuroimage 229:117782. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.117782

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sawah, M. A., Ruffin, N., Rimawi, M., Concerto, C., Aguglia, E., Chusid, E., et al. (2015). Perceived stress and coffee and energy drink consumption predict poor sleep quality in podiatric medical students a cross-sectional study. J. Am. Podiatr. Med. Assoc. 5, 429–434. doi: 10.7547/14-082

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scuteri, D., Rombolà, L., Crudo, M., Watanabe, C., Mizoguchi, H., Sakurada, S., et al. (2022). Preclinical characterization of antinociceptive effect of bergamot essential oil and of its fractions for rational translation in complementary therapy. Pharmaceutics 14:312. doi: 10.3390/pharmaceutics14020312

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scuteri, D., Rombolà, L., Morrone, L. A., Bagetta, G., Sakurada, S., Sakurada, T., et al. (2019). Neuropharmacology of the neuropsychiatric symptoms of dementia and role of pain: essential oil of bergamot as a novel therapeutic approach. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 20:3327. doi: 10.3390/ijms20133327

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seo, H. S., Lee, S., and Cho, S. (2013). Relationships between personality traits and attitudes toward the sense of smell. Front. Psychol. 4:901. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00901

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sicari, V., Loizzo, M. R., Branca, V., and Pellicanò, T. M. (2016). Bioactive and Antioxidant Activity from Citrus bergamia Risso (Bergamot) Juice Collected in Different Areas of Reggio Calabria Province, Italy. Int. J. Food Propert. 19, 1962–1971.

Google Scholar

Smeets, P. A., Erkner, A., and de Graaf, C. (2010). Cephalic phase responses and appetite. Nutr. Rev. 68, 643–655.

Google Scholar

Smith, K. S., Tindell, A. J., Aldridge, J. W., and Berridge, K. C. (2009). Ventral pallidum roles in reward and motivation. Behav. Brain Res. 196, 155–167.

Google Scholar

Tomasino, B., Pellitteri, G., Bax, F., Marini, A., Surcinelli, A., Gigli, G. L., et al. (2022). Multisensory mental representation in covid-19 patients and the possibility of long-lasting gustatory and olfactory dysfunction in the CNS. Sci. Rep. 12:7340. doi: 10.1038/s41598-022-11119-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Torske, A., Koch, K., Eickhoff, S., and Freiherr, J. (2022). Localizing the human brain response to olfactory stimulation: A meta-analytic approach. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 134:104512. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.12.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wicker, B., Keysers, C., Plailly, J., Royet, J. P., Gallese, V., and Rizzolatti, G. (2003). Both of us disgusted in My insula: the common neural basis of seeing and feeling disgust. Neuron 40, 655–664. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00679-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zou, L. Q., Linden, L., Cuevas, M., Metasch, M. L., Welge-Lüssen, A., Hähner, A., et al. (2020). Self-reported mini olfactory questionnaire (Self-MOQ): a simple and useful measurement for the screening of olfactory dysfunction. Laryngoscope 130, E786–E790. doi: 10.1002/lary.28419

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: corticospinal excitability, pleasant odor perception, odor imagery, personality traits, transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS)

Citation: Infortuna C, Gualano F, Freedberg D, Patel SP, Sheikh AM, Muscatello MRA, Bruno A, Mento C, Chusid E, Han Z, Thomas FP and Battaglia F (2022) Motor Cortex Response to Pleasant Odor Perception and Imagery: The Differential Role of Personality Dimensions and Imagery Ability. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 16:943469. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2022.943469

Received: 13 May 2022; Accepted: 17 June 2022;
Published: 12 July 2022.

Edited by:

Mariagiovanna Cantone, ASP Caltanissetta, Italy

Reviewed by:

Giuseppe Lanza, University of Catania, Italy
George M. Opie, University of Adelaide, Australia

Copyright © 2022 Infortuna, Gualano, Freedberg, Patel, Sheikh, Muscatello, Bruno, Mento, Chusid, Han, Thomas and Battaglia. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Fortunato Battaglia, fortunato.battaglia@hmhn.org

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.