Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 18 January 2023
Sec. Cognitive Neuroscience

Oscillatory brain responses to emotional stimuli are effects related to events rather than states

  • 1Behavioural Science Institute (BSI), Radboud University, Nijmegen, Netherlands
  • 2Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University, Nijmegen, Netherlands
  • 3School of Psychology, Centre for Human Brain Health, University of Birmingham, Birmingham, United Kingdom
  • 4Department of Clinical Psychology, Utrecht University, Utrecht, Netherlands

Emotional cues draw attention, thereby enabling enhanced processing. Electrophysiological brain research in humans suggests that increased gamma band activity and decreased alpha band activity over posterior brain areas is associated with the allocation of attention. However, emotional events can alternate quickly, like rapidly changing news items and it remains unknown whether the modulation of brain oscillations happens in a stimulus induced manner, changing with each individual stimulus, or whether the events lead to prolonged, state-like changes. To investigate this, we measured the electroencephalogram (EEG) during a passive viewing task (N = 32) while emotional pictures International Affective Picture System (IAPS) were presented in blocks containing either pleasant and neutral or unpleasant and neutral pictures. As predicted, we found decreased alpha and increased gamma power over posterior areas in response to unpleasant compared to pleasant pictures (and also compared to neutral pictures for gamma power). When testing the neutral pictures of the unpleasant and pleasant block against each other, we found no significant difference, which speaks to a stimulus induced effect of alpha and gamma power rather than a state effect. In addition, the inter-trial interval (ITI) between the pictures did not differ between the unpleasant and pleasant block either, corroborating this conclusion. Since emotional pictures can at the same time elicit a freezing-like response and we were interested in whether this freezing-like response co-occurs with enhanced attention, we also collected postural sway data. However, within this EEG-setup, postural analyses indicated no stimulus-related effects nor a correlation with EEG-data. We interpret the alpha and gamma band results as reflecting event-related attention toward unpleasant compared to pleasant (and neutral) pictures and discuss this finding in light of previous EEG research and in combination with behavioral research on threat-induced reductions in body sway (freezing-like response).

Introduction

Emotional content is a strong determinant of the degree of attention that we direct toward a given visual object. Brain oscillations are known to be modulated in different frequency bands as a consequence of visual gating as for instance modulated by selective attention (Klimesch et al., 2007; Jensen and Mazaheri, 2010; Foxe and Snyder, 2011). The alpha rhythm (8–14 Hz) has been shown to reflect inhibition of task-irrelevant brain areas during the allocation of spatial attention (Klimesch et al., 2007; Haegens et al., 2012; Okazaki et al., 2015; Zazio et al., 2020). While this relation between alpha band activity and attention is known for spatial attention tasks, we set out to investigate how alpha band activity as a read-out measure of attention is modulated by the emotional valence of pictures. In several recent studies using electroencephalogram (EEG) it was shown that alpha power decreases more for emotional compared to less salient stimuli (Keil et al., 2001; Balconi et al., 2009; Popov et al., 2012). Keil et al. (2001) demonstrated a stronger decrease of alpha power (i.e., alpha suppression) for unpleasant compared to both pleasant and neutral pictures during right hemifield presentations. Interestingly, they found the opposite effect for left hemifield presentations (a stronger alpha power decrease for pleasant pictures). Popov et al. (2012) also found posterior alpha suppression for unpleasant compared to neutral pictures. However, Balconi et al. (2015) did not find effects of either valence or arousal of affective pictures on alpha power in a passive viewing task.

Besides alpha activity, gamma band activity has been shown to reflect active processing and to increase with attention (Fries et al., 2001). Furthermore, there is evidence for increased gamma activity in response to emotional compared to neutral stimuli, albeit the reported frequency band of gamma varies across the studies. Balconi et al. (2009) found wide-spread increased gamma power (40–50 Hz) in response to highly arousing pictures (pleasant and unpleasant) compared to low arousing pictures. Keil et al. (2001) also found 45–65 Hz gamma power increases over the right hemisphere for arousing (unpleasant and pleasant) compared to neutral pictures. Aftanas et al. (2004) found a similar effect over parieto-occipital areas in the 30–45 Hz band. Mueller et al. (1999) reported increased gamma power (30–50 Hz) over right frontal and temporal sites for arousing compared to neutral pictures, but no effect was observed for gamma power between 50 and 90 Hz. In a block design, Güntekin and Tülay (2014) found higher gamma power in response to unpleasant pictures compared to pleasant and neutral pictures over occipital electrodes.

In sum, previous studies showed modulation of alpha and gamma power by emotional stimuli, although the results for alpha power are somewhat inconsistent. However, all of these studies used statistical approaches based on pre-defined time windows, frequency ranges, and/or topographical sites. We set out to investigate the power modulations during emotional picture viewing by using a non-parametric statistical test, free from a priori defined time periods and frequencies (Maris and Oostenveld, 2007) for two reasons. First, this test assures an adequate statistical sensitivity to detect condition differences. Second, by not pre-defining the frequency and time ranges, and including all channels in the analysis, we allow for bottom-up discovery of effects in terms of frequency range and time window, and the topographical pattern of activity will reveal in a bottom-up manner the sites that form part of the largest cluster of activation difference due to condition. Using such an approach, Popov et al. (2012) found gamma power increases (60–80 Hz) for unpleasant compared to neutral pictures; however, they did not have arousing pictures of pleasant valence in their design.

The encounter of aversive information such as unpleasant pictures not only affects attention, but has also been shown to elicit freezing-like responses in humans (Lang et al., 1997; Hagenaars et al., 2012, 2014), i.e., a state of immobility in the face of threat. This can be detected by measuring postural movement (body sway) using a stabilometric platform (Azevedo et al., 2005; Facchinetti et al., 2006; Stins and Beek, 2007; Hagenaars et al., 2012; Niermann et al., 2015) and is usually found in a state-like fashion (i.e., on block-level). Animal models posit that freezing is a state of enhanced attention (Lang et al., 1997), but the link between allocation of attention and freezing is unknown in humans. Therefore, the brain oscillations associated with freezing deserve further investigation. In particular, we were interested if the power modulations by emotional pictures, as described above, reflect the immediate (i.e., stimulus induced) modulation of attention or whether longer lasting (i.e., state-like) effects of emotion also contribute to this attention process, as was found for body sway.

With these considerations in mind, we conducted an EEG study in which participants were shown pictures of unpleasant, pleasant and neutral valence while standing on a stabilometric platform (balance board). We explored whether these oscillatory modulations are state-like (as freezing is a state-like response) or stimulus induced (as most EEG research on emotional pictures finds stimulus induced effects). Further, we hypothesized that in particular unpleasant pictures would elicit a stronger alpha decrease and gamma power increase compared to the other pictures. Lastly, we posited that these oscillatory modulations would relate to freezing (a reduction in body sway for unpleasant compared to pleasant and neutral pictures), i.e., that people who froze more would also show a stronger alpha power decrease and gamma power increase toward unpleasant pictures as compared to pleasant and neutral pictures.

Materials and methods

Participants

Forty-four healthy volunteers (18 male) were recruited from Radboud University Nijmegen, via an experiment participation system, flyers, and personal contacts. Due to technical problems leading to incomplete data sets, data from 4 participants were excluded for the EEG analysis, and 7 for the analysis of stabilometric data (with an overlap in participants). Additionally, 3 more participants dropped out during the course of the experiment due to dizziness or feeling unwell. The mean age of the 34 remaining participants (12 male) was 24.16 (SD = 7.16). The study was approved by the local ethics committee (Ethische Commissie Gedragswetenschappelijk onderzoek, ECG) and participants gave written informed consent prior to the experiment.

Materials

Stimuli

Each participant was presented 208 pictures from the International Affective Picture System (IAPS; Lang et al., 2001) of which 52 were pleasant, 52 unpleasant, and 104 neutral pictures (see Appendix). Pictures were selected based on the normative valence ratings provided by the IAPS manual (cut-off values: unpleasant <3.2; neutral: 4–6; pleasant >6.8). The valence ratings were further constrained not to differ more than 0.8 points between men’s and women’s ratings. The pictures were matched in terms of luminance and size with a custom written program (MatLab script). Each category included the same ratio of pictures including vs. excluding humans (including the face).

Each picture appeared only once during the passive viewing task. Pictures were presented in 8 blocks consisting of 26 pictures. Each block contained 13 neutral pictures and 13 emotional (either pleasant or unpleasant) pictures. This amounted to 4 blocks of pleasant and neutral pictures, and 4 blocks of unpleasant and neutral pictures. This arrangement lead to four conditions on the picture level: (1) pleasant, (2) neutral-in-pleasant-block, (3) unpleasant, and (4) neutral-in-unpleasant-block.

Procedure and experimental paradigm

Participants took part in a larger study, of which the third part consisted of the passive viewing task of the IAPS pictures, the data of which are presented here. The first part consisted of the same study design as described here (passive viewing task) but with emotional faces and the second part of a memory task on those faces. Recordings took place in a specialized EEG lab which was quiet, without daylight and undisturbed. Participants were standing on a stabilometric force platform while the EEG was simultaneously recorded. Stimuli were presented on a Samsung 2233SW screen with a refresh-rate of 60 or 120 Hz differing between subjects, which was adjusted to eye-height. The refresh rate differed due to a change in the technical circumstances in the laboratory but did not show in the data nor affect stimulus presentation times. The pictures and blocks were pseudo-randomized being constrained to not more than two blocks of one valence (pleasant or unpleasant) sequentially, and within each block not more than four pictures of one valence (neutral or pleasant, or neutral or unpleasant, respectively) sequentially. Pictures were presented for 2 s with inter-trial intervals (ITIs) of 1.5–2 s, leading to a total duration of around 1 min 36 s per block. The blocks were separated by 20 s breaks. After four blocks followed a 3 min break allowing participants to sit down and rest their legs. This design represents the best current combination between EEG and balance board designs, compromising between the requirements of each method (i.e., block designs used in balance board studies and event-related designs as used in EEG studies). A white fixation cross stayed on the screen and the pictures were presented on top (Figure 1). During the break this cross turned into a countdown to give the participants an orientation as to how long they still had time to relax and move their eyes before resuming the picture presentation.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Experimental paradigm. Passive viewing task during which participants were gazing at the fixation cross while perceiving the picture around the cross. Neutral pictures were presented among either pleasant or unpleasant pictures.

Participants were asked to fixate on the cross while perceiving the picture around it. Furthermore, they were asked to blink after each picture in order to reduce blinking during stimulus presentation. Their posture was required to be relaxed with arms hanging alongside the body.

Data acquisition

Electroencephalography

A 64 channel EEG system (ActiCap64 system, Brain Products GmbH, Gilching, Germany) based on the extended 10-10 system was applied. The signals were referenced to linked mastoids and later re-referenced to a common average reference. The data were sampled at 500 Hz following band-pass filtering between 0.016 and 150 Hz.

Stabilometric platform

The recordings of the body sway took place on a custom build stabilometric square platform (50 × 50 cm; 19.7 inches) with a force sensor in each corner (distance between sensors: 42.5 cm; Niermann et al., 2015). The stabilometric data were acquired together with the EEG using four additional E × G channels.

Data analysis

Electroencephalography sensor level analyses

Electroencephalography data were analyzed using MatLab and the MatLab-based FieldTrip toolbox (Oostenveld et al., 2011). Artifacts were detected in a semiautomatic manner using visual inspection and selecting a threshold for each subject (Horschig et al., 2015). Trials containing ocular or muscle-artifacts were excluded from analysis (on average 31%). This was done for each subject individually since the absolute values of power differ largely between subjects and hence a general cut-off value would not have been useful. Datasets with too many lost trials (more than 1 SD above the average amount of lost trials) were excluded from the EEG analysis (5 data sets were excluded). This lead to 32 included data sets for EEG analysis (44 recorded subjects, –3 drop outs, –4 data sets with technical problems, –5 data sets with too few trials for statistical analysis), leading to 32 included data sets for the EEG analysis. The remaining EEG data looked as clean as in a sitting position.

Sensor level time-frequency analysis of power was performed using a sliding time-window of 500 ms and Hanning taper for the lower frequencies (2–30 Hz). This resulted in, e.g., a 5 cycle window for 10 Hz. For the higher frequencies (30–120 Hz) we used a 400 ms sliding time window and a multi-taper approach [discrete prolate spheroidal sequences (DPSS), Percival and Walden, 1993]. This resulted in, e.g., a 20 cycle window for 50 Hz. To aim for a spectral smoothing of 10 Hz we used 7 DPSS tapers. The difference in parameters between low and high frequencies is due to the different spectral smoothing requirements of, e.g., the alpha and gamma band oscillations (see Ferrante et al., 2022).

Electroencephalography statistical analyses

Statistical analysis was performed on the whole brain sensor level, i.e., including all sensors and frequencies. For each subject, trials were averaged per picture condition (pleasant, neutral-in-pleasant-block, unpleasant, and neutral-in-unpleasant-block) from −1 to 2 s around stimulus onset. To assess whether sensor level power was significantly different between conditions, a cluster based, non-parametric permutation test was used within subjects (Maris and Oostenveld, 2007). This test clusters neighboring sensors and time-frequency points which show the same effect and thereby controls for multiple comparisons. For each grid point (space by time by frequency point) a dependent samples F-test (four picture categories) was computed. For each point the F-value was calculated and clusters on the basis of spatial adjacency were formed where these values exceeded the critical threshold of p = 0.05 (uncorrected). The sum of the F-values within a cluster was used as cluster-level statistic and the cluster with the maximum sum was used for the test statistic. To create a reference distribution to obtain a maximum cluster F-value for statistical comparison with the present data, 1,000 permutations were performed in which the data of the four conditions was randomized and the test statistic recalculated. The same principle was applied for t-tests for block-comparison and post-hoc analyses. The test was first conducted on the inter trial interval (1 s before picture onset) between the two block types (pleasant vs. unpleasant block) to test for state effects. After this test, the pre-stimulus interval (−800 to −200 ms; for each of the four picture conditions separately) was used as baseline interval like in classic EEG designs (Aftanas et al., 2002; De Cesarei and Codispoti, 2011; Haegens et al., 2012) and the cluster-based permutation test was conducted for the full window of −1000 to 2000 ms around stimulus to test for stimulus induced effects.

Stabilometric platform

Offline, the data were low-pass filtered at 10 Hz. Trials with a z-score >4 (scores computed for each participant over all trials) were regarded as outliers and removed from analyses (on average 4%). The standard deviation of the movement in anterior-posterior direction (SD-AP), medio-lateral direction (SD-ML), and the sway path length (SPL) were calculated per trial over the stimulus period (0–2 s), normalized to the baseline period (−1–0 s), z-scored, and then averaged per picture category (Stins and Beek, 2007; Hagenaars et al., 2012). Statistical analysis was conducted in SPSS 23 (SPSS, IBM Corp, 2015).

Results

Electroencephalography data were acquired during a passive viewing task in which subjects watched blocks of either unpleasant and neutral or pleasant and neutral pictures while standing on a stabilometric platform.

Electroencephalography stimulus induced rather than state effects

We first tested for stimulus induced vs. state/block effects by contrasting the inter trial interval (1 s prior to picture onset) between the two block types. The t-test over this period yielded no significant differences between the pleasant and the unpleasant block, neither in the lower (p = 0.28; Figure 2A) nor the higher frequencies (p = 0.53; Figure 2B). From this part of the analysis we conclude that the emotional pictures did not induce longer lasting effects on EEG power (beyond the time window of stimulus presentation).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. (A,B) Comparing the inter trial interval (red box). Time-frequency representations of power when comparing blocks (t-statistics), masked for significance. No significant difference in power neither in the low nor the high frequencies during the pre-stimulus interval was observed.

Electroencephalography stimulus induced effects: The alpha band

Next we investigated the modulation in the lower frequencies (2–30 Hz) by the different picture categories. As seen in Figure 3A, the cluster based permutation F-test revealed robust differences in the in alpha and beta band (8–20 Hz) with respect to the four conditions over posterior sensors (Figure 3B; p < 0.001). Follow-up post-hoc pair wise comparisons (t-tests) showed a stronger alpha-beta power decrease for the unpleasant compared to the pleasant pictures (Figure 3C; p < 0.01) over all sensors with the strongest effect over posterior sensors (Figure 3D). The unpleasant compared to the neutral-in-unpleasant-block pictures showed a significant difference (p = 0.01), but when inspecting the according figure (Figure 3E) it becomes apparent that this contrast is clearly not dominated by alpha band activity. When excluding the evoked part (i.e., frequencies up to 5 Hz, see Karakaş et al., 2000; Herrmann et al., 2014) from the test, the difference was no longer significant (p = 0.17; Figures 3E,F). The contrasts between the pleasant and neutral-in-pleasant-block (p = 0.16; Figures 3I,J) and between the neutral pictures of the two block types did not yield significant differences (p = 0.07; Figures 3G,H). In short, our findings demonstrate that the suppression of the alpha and beta band activity increased with unpleasant pictures and this effect seems to be most pronounced between 0.5 and 1.5 s after stimulus onset.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Power modulations at lower frequencies with respect to valence of the pictures. Time-frequency representations of power when comparing conditions (F/t-statistics), masked for significance, at sensor PO3. Topographical representations are based on averaged data for the latency 0.5–1.5 s and a frequency range from 8 to 20 Hz. Significant sensors are displayed with an asterisk in the topographical representation. (A) The F-test between the four conditions revealed that alpha and beta power is modulated in a condition specific manner (p < 0.05; controlled for multiple comparisons). (B) Topographical representation for the effect shown in panel (A) revealed posterior modulation. (C,D) A stronger decrease in alpha and beta power was observed posteriorly for unpleasant compared to pleasant pictures. (E,F) Apart from the evoked activity (the low frequencies <5 Hz) there was no significant difference between the unpleasant and neutral-in-unpleasant-block pictures. (G,H) There was no difference when comparing neutral pictures from the unpleasant and pleasant blocks, (I,J) neither for pictures of the pleasant vs. neutral-in-unpleasant-block.

Electroencephalography stimulus induced effects: The gamma band

Next, we set out to investigate the modulation in the higher frequencies (30–120 Hz), applying the same cluster based F/t-test procedure as to the lower frequencies. We found a main effect in the gamma band (60–80 Hz; at latency >1 s for an even broader band) when comparing the four picture categories using an F-test (p < 0.001; Figures 4A,B). A post-hoc t-test revealed a significant difference between the unpleasant and the pleasant pictures (p < 0.01; Figures 4C,D) with stronger gamma band activity for the unpleasant pictures. Also when compared to the neutral pictures in the unpleasant block, the unpleasant pictures elicited significantly more gamma activity (p < 0.01; Figures 4E,F). The contrast between the pleasant and neutral-in-pleasant-block did not yield any significant differences (p = 1; Figures 4I,J). The contrast between the neutral pictures in the unpleasant and the pleasant block did not yield any significant difference in the higher gamma band, but did show a difference in the 30–40 Hz band with gamma power being stronger for the neutral-in-pleasant-block pictures (p = 0.04; see Figures 4G,H).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Modulations at higher frequencies with respect to valence of the pictures. Time-frequency representations of power when comparing conditions (F/t-statistics), masked for significance, at sensor Pz. Topographical representations are based on averaged data for the latency 0.5–1.5 s and a frequency range from 60 to 80 Hz. Significant sensors are displayed with an asterisk in the topographical representation. (A) The F-test revealed that gamma power was differently modulated by conditions (p < 0.05; controlled for multiple comparison). (B) Topographical representation for the effect shown in panel (A) revealed posterior and anterior modulation. (C,D) A stronger increase in gamma power was observed posteriorly and anteriorly for unpleasant compared to pleasant pictures. (E,F) Similarly, for unpleasant compared to neutral-in-unpleasant-block a strong increase in gamma power was observed, mainly over posterior sensors. (G,H) When comparing neutral pictures of the unpleasant and pleasant blocks, a gamma decrease at the lower frequencies around 30–40 Hz for neutral-in-unpleasant block became apparent. (I,J) No significant difference for pictures of the pleasant vs. neutral-in-pleasant-block was found.

In short, our findings demonstrate a stimulus induced increase in the higher gamma band activity for unpleasant pictures, while the lower gamma band activity (30–40 Hz) showed a block effect for neutral pictures (unpleasant vs. pleasant block) over temporo-posterior sites. This effect seems to be most pronounced between 0 and 2.0 s.

Body sway effects: Stabilometric data

A repeated measures MANOVA with the factor emotion (four levels: pleasant, unpleasant, neutral-in-pleasant-block, neutral-in-unpleasant-block) was carried out on the three dependent variables (SD-ML, SD-AP, and SPL) yielding no significant effect [Wilks’ Lambda = 0.64, F(9,25) = 1.55, p = 0.19]. Means (SDs) are listed in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Body sway measures per condition.

Correlations between electroencephalography and stabilometric data

In order to investigate the link between freezing and attention, we correlated the stabilometric (body sway) and EEG measures. To this end, EEG alpha power was averaged in the 0.5–1.5 s time window, over the parieto-occipital channels in the 8–18 Hz band (where we observed the strongest effect between conditions). We also averaged the gamma power (60–80 Hz) in the same time window and over the same sensors. These values were then log transformed. The body sway data were averaged over the full stimulus presentation window from 0 to 2 s. The resulting values were then z-scored and averaged per subject per condition.

A canonical correlation analysis (Sherry and Henson, 2005) evaluating the multivariate shared relationship between the EEG measures (alpha and gamma band activity) with those of the body sway measures (SD-AP, SD-ML, and SPL) for the difference scores between the conditions (unpleasant–pleasant, unpleasant–neutral-in-unpleasant-block, pleasant–neutral-in-pleasant-block, and neutral-in-unpleasant-block–neutral-in-pleasant-block) was set up. The canonical correlation did not yield any significant results [F(5,70.88) = 1.16, p = 0.25 for the strongest correlation].

Discussion

In the current study we observed that pictures of unpleasant valence elicited a stimulus induced decrease in alpha/beta power (around 8–18 Hz) over posterior sensors and an increase in gamma power (around 60–80 Hz) over posterior and anterior sensors when compared to pleasant pictures, as well as a gamma increase when compared to neutral pictures within the unpleasant block. Pleasant compared to neutral pictures did not show a difference in either alpha or gamma power. Furthermore, we explored whether these attention effects pertain to a state effect over a whole block or are related to single pictures (i.e., stimulus induced). As the ITI did not show a difference between the pleasant and unpleasant blocks, and furthermore the contrast between the neutral pictures from the two block types did not show a difference (except for a relative decrease in power around 30 Hz for neutral pictures from the unpleasant block), we conclude that the effects of emotional attention are stimulus induced rather than state effects.

Finally, we tested whether this emotional modulation of brain responses was linked to decreased body sway as a measure of freezing. The body sway data showed neither a main effect of emotion condition nor any correlation with alpha or gamma power. In light of the results from previous research on selective attention (Fries et al., 2001; Jensen and Mazaheri, 2010; Foxe and Snyder, 2011; Haegens et al., 2012) we interpret our findings of alpha/beta power decreases and gamma increases as enhanced attention toward unpleasant stimuli. These findings will be discussed in detail below.

Alpha power

Attention toward unpleasant pictures

The decrease in alpha power toward unpleasant compared to pleasant pictures found in the present study is generally in line with previous research (Keil et al., 2001; Popov et al., 2012). However, there are a few distinctions between our study and previous studies. Popov et al. (2012) found a significant alpha decrease for unpleasant compared to neutral pictures in a passive viewing task. Our data did not show this effect in the alpha band, yet, we did find increased gamma power for unpleasant compared to neutral pictures, pointing in the direction of an attention increase for unpleasant compared to neutral pictures. The difference in the alpha band finding may be due to differences in study designs.

An effect related to events

While balance board studies involving emotions mostly found effects using block designs (Azevedo et al., 2005; Facchinetti et al., 2006; Hagenaars et al., 2012, 2014; Bastos et al., 2016) our study showed emotion effects related to events on a short time-scale (stimulus induced), at least in the EEG (and no effects in the balance board). The absence of a block effect may be due to the semi-blocked design of our study, which was a hybrid between a classic (blocked) balance board design and a classic (fully randomized) EEG design. Stins and Beek (2007) already found that a passive viewing task with a fully randomized design induces too weak effects of emotion on body sway to be measured by the balance board. This also seems to be true for our semi-blocked design.

Our EEG results suggest that even in a semi-blocked design (as opposed to a fully randomized design), the increases in attention to unpleasant pictures are stimulus induced rather than state (i.e., block) effects, which is in line with previous research (Schupp et al., 2013). Future studies investigating concurring effects of body sway and brain activity may want to take a step closer toward the classic balance board designs, for example by leaving out ITIs between stimuli while keeping a semi-blocked design, or by keeping the ITIs but using a fully blocked design in terms of valence, in order to be able to find freezing-like effects in body sway and potentially link them to attentional effects of emotion.

In principle, the effects of the pictures can represent a short lasting effect or carry on for longer. Our data of the ITI seems to speak to the shorter lasting effect. Yet, the significant difference between neutral pictures from unpleasant vs. pleasant blocks in the gamma band may indicate a state-related oscillatory brain response as well. It may be the case that short lasting effects play out in the alpha band while longer lasting effects play out in the gamma band. This may pose an interesting avenue for future research.

A central-posterior effect

While the effects of emotion on alpha or gamma power in other studies were often either hemifield, hemisphere or site specific, or including interactions between these factors (Mueller et al., 1999; Keil et al., 2001; Balconi et al., 2009), we found a central posterior alpha/beta (and frontal gamma) effect. While previous studies mostly analyzed pre-defined sites, we used the cluster-based permutation test over the whole brain. This difference in analysis method may have led to the difference of results in terms of the location of the emotion effects; however, the differences might also be explained by the details of the experiments not being the same.

Role of arousal

Aftanas et al. (2002) and De Cesarei and Codispoti (2011) found more alpha suppression for arousing pictures than for non-arousing pictures, independent of valence. In our design we did not see arousal modulating alpha/beta suppression, i.e., p = 0.09 for the high arousing emotional pictures (pleasant and unpleasant taken together) compared to the low arousing neutral pictures (of both blocks taken together). The mean arousal rating of our emotional pictures, based on the normative ratings of the IAPS database, was 5.55 (SD = 0.97) and of the neutral pictures 3.39 (SD = 0.70). The difference between our result and the one of De Cesarei and Codispoti (2011) could be explained by a difference in stimuli, as they used also pictures of other origin than the IAPS which might have been even more arousing than the ones we used from the IAPS database. To better address this issue, future studies would ideally include individual arousal ratings to be able to control for possible differences in arousal perception.

Uusberg et al. (2013) found less alpha power decrease over dorsal and central sites for emotional and in particular unpleasant (aversive) pictures, i.e., the opposite of De Cesarei and Codispoti (2011) and the present study. Uusberg et al. (2013) reason that their results are due to the difference in task: In their study the subjects had to make evaluations (affective and non-affective), whereas in the study by De Cesarei and Codispoti (2011) the instruction was to just perceive the picture (passive viewing task). Since we also used a passive viewing task, our results support this interpretation. It seems that conducting a task (e.g., evaluating aspects of a picture) leads to guarding the attention from distracting input like, e.g., too strong emotional input. Unpleasant pictures seem to require more attentional resources to ignore, than pleasant and neutral ones in the case where they interfere with task goals. This is in line with the findings by Haegens et al. (2012) who showed that stronger distracting input needs more alpha power in order to attenuate the distracting input than weak distracting input. This is in line with attention models differentiating between top-down (e.g., instructed) and bottom-up (stimulus-driven) attention (see e.g., Compton, 2003).

Beta band

Some of the activation differences we see may stretch into the beta band (ca. 13–23 Hz). Other studies have found results in this range with similar images, namely, Güntekin and Tülay (2014) who showed higher beta power in response to unpleasant pictures compared to pleasant and neutral pictures and Güntekin and Başar (2010) who indicated greater beta responses for negative images compared to positive images. Our results are thus in line with these findings.

Lower frequencies

We did find an effect at lower frequencies (<5 Hz; see Figures 2, 3). For unpleasant vs. positive this effect speaks to enhanced attention for unpleasant pictures, similar to the decrease in alpha activity in the same contrast. We also observed a small effect for unpleasant vs. neutral pictures which would indicate increased attention for the unpleasant pictures as well. Due to the small size of this cluster, we remain conservative in interpreting this finding.

Gamma power

Our result of increased gamma power for (high arousing) unpleasant pictures compared to (low arousing) neutral pictures is in line with the literature, though often these effects are found in the 30–60 Hz range (Mueller et al., 1999; Keil et al., 2001; Aftanas et al., 2004; Balconi et al., 2009; Güntekin and Tülay, 2014) while our effects were most pronounced in the 60–90 Hz band. Our study distinguishes itself from those studies in allowing the cluster-based permutation test to identify effects up to 120 Hz rather than pre-defining and hence limiting the frequency range in the analysis, which may explain this difference in findings. Another difference in findings is that those studies found an effect of arousal: they found increased gamma toward high arousing as opposed to low arousing pictures (both valences together). Our findings in contrast show an effect of valence: the unpleasant pictures elicited enhanced gamma activity compared to neutral but also compared to (high arousing) pleasant pictures. This could possibly be linked to the difference in the frequency range in which the effects were found.

Comparing alpha and gamma results

Our alpha and gamma findings support each other, being in line with computational frameworks which explain that a decrease in alpha band activity would support active processing which then is reflected by increased activity in the gamma band (Jensen and Mazaheri, 2010; Jensen et al., 2014; Hyafil et al., 2015; Schalk, 2015). Taken together, our results of increased gamma and decreased alpha power suggest a clear increase of attention toward unpleasant pictures.

Body sway

Our measure of freezing, as reflected by body sway, did not yield any significant effects of emotion in the current design; neither main effects of picture condition, nor when correlating the condition contrasts of the balance board with the brain activity in the alpha and gamma band. We strongly believe that this is due to the task setup. Compared to the classic design of balance board studies (Azevedo et al., 2005; Roelofs et al., 2010; Hagenaars et al., 2012; Noordewier et al., 2020), we undertook a number of alterations, the sum of which may have altered the participant’s automatic postural response: First, there were two unnatural constraints during the passive viewing task: (1) participants had to fixate their gaze on the fixation cross and hence suppress the natural impulse to explore the picture, and (2) they were required not to blink during the presentation of the picture (and we would remind them if they did, so participants who blinked might have felt observed). Secondly, our design may have induced a less intense emotion, since we had a shorter picture presentation time (2 s instead of 3 s), used ITIs, and used a semi-blocked design. Thirdly, the passive viewing task took longer (20 min rather than 3–5 min) and thus far, it is not known what effect prolonged standing has on body sway. Fourth, wearing the EEG cap (with cables attached to the amplifier) may have affected their posture, though we asked whether they felt constrained by the cap and most subjects reported that they did not. Yet, it would be interesting to overcome these issues and be able to link attention effects in brain activity to freezing in order to disclose whether freezing is indeed a state of enhanced attention, as the animal models posit (Lang et al., 1997). In future studies, we recommend to keep the design closer to classic balance board designs, e.g., use a fully blocked design (perhaps not even with an inter trial-interval between stimuli within a block) and use a pre-experimental period, or a short interval between blocks, as baseline for the EEG.

Conclusion

Our findings reveal an attentional increase toward unpleasant pictures as identified by a robust increase of gamma activity for unpleasant as compared to pleasant and neutral pictures, as well as alpha suppression for unpleasant compared to neutral stimuli, speaking to increased attention toward unpleasant pictures given the functional role of these oscillatory activities. In addition, the lack of a difference in brain oscillatory activity during the inter trial interval between pictures and between the neutral pictures presented within the unpleasant and pleasant blocks suggest that attention to emotional pictures is directed in a stimulus induced manner rather than reflecting a state that is maintained throughout a block. In our semi-blocked design we observed no emotion related changes in body sway. In future research, an adapted design may allow researchers to combine measures of brain activity and body sway, in order to elucidate the neural correlates of freezing and the predicted relation between freezing and enhanced attention.

Data availability statement

The datasets presented in this article are not readily available because the data are archived at the Donders Institute, Nijmegen, Netherlands. The involved authors have moved away from this university. Theoretically the data can be handed out, but one would need to get to Nijmegen and find a person who can hand them out. Requests to access the datasets should be directed to YWRtaW5AZG9uZGVycy5ydS5ubA==.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Ethische Commissie Gedragswetenschappelijk onderzoek, ECG, of the Behavioural Science Institute at the Radboud University Nijmegen, Netherlands. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

LL was responsible for the data collection and drafted the manuscript. LL performed the EEG data analysis and interpretation under the supervision of OJ with input by JH. LL performed the body sway data analysis and interpretation under supervision of OJ, KR, and MH. All authors contributed to the study concept and design, provided revisions, and approved the final version of the manuscript prior to submission.

Acknowledgments

We thank Isabel Woyke and Anne Schwenzfeier for their help with stimulus preparation and data acquisition and Joanneke Weerdmeester for her help with data acquisition. Additionally, we are grateful for help from Thomas Gladwin with balance board data analysis.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Author disclaimer

The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the funding agencies.

References

Aftanas, L. I., Reva, N. V., Varlamov, A. A., Pavlov, S. V., and Makhnev, V. P. (2004). Analysis of evoked EEG synchronization and desynchronization in conditions of emotional activation in humans: Temporal and topographic characteristics. Neurosci. Behav. Physiol. 34, 859–867. doi: 10.1023/B:NEAB.0000038139.39812.eb

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aftanas, L. I., Varlamov, A. A., Pavlov, S. V., Makhnev, V. P., and Reva, N. V. (2002). Time-dependent cortical asymmetries induced by emotional arousal: EEG analysis of event-related synchronization and desynchronization in individually defined frequency bands. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 44, 67–82. doi: 10.1016/S0167-8760(01)00194-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Azevedo, T. M., Volchan, E., Imbiriba, L. A., Rodrigues, E. C., Oliveira, J. M., Oliveira, L. F., et al. (2005). A freezing-like posture to pictures of mutilation. Psychophysiology 42, 255–260. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2005.00287.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balconi, M., Brambilla, E., and Falbo, L. (2009). Appetitive vs. defensive responses to emotional cues. Autonomic measures and brain oscillation modulation. Brain Res. 1296, 72–84. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.08.056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balconi, M., Grippa, E., and Vanutelli, M. E. (2015). What hemodynamic (fNIRS), electrophysiological (EEG) and autonomic integrated measures can tell us about emotional processing. Brain Cogn. 95, 67–76. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2015.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bastos, A. F., Vieira, A. S., Oliveira, J. M., Oliveira, L., Pereira, M. G., Figueira, I., et al. (2016). Stop or move: Defensive strategies in humans. Behav. Brain Res. 302, 252–262. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2016.01.043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Compton, R. J. (2003). The interface between emotion and attention: A review of evidence from psychology and neuroscience. Behav. Cogn. Neurosci. Rev. 2, 115–129. doi: 10.1177/1534582303255278

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Cesarei, A., and Codispoti, M. (2011). Affective modulation of the LPP and α-ERD during picture viewing. Psychophysiology 48, 1397–1404. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2011.01204.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Facchinetti, L. D., Imbiriba, L. A., Azevedo, T. M., Vargas, C. D., and Volchan, E. (2006). Postural modulation induced by pictures depicting prosocial or dangerous contexts. Neurosci. Lett. 410, 52–56. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2006.09.063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferrante, O., Liu, L., Minarik, T., Gorska, U., Ghafari, T., Luo, H., et al. (2022). FLUX: A pipeline for MEG analysis. Neuroimage 253:119047. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2022.119047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Foxe, J. J., and Snyder, A. C. (2011). The role of alpha-band brain oscillations as a sensory suppression mechanism during selective attention. Front. Psychol. 2:154. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00154

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fries, P., Reynolds, J. H., Rorie, A. E., and Desimone, R. (2001). Modulation of oscillatory neuronal synchronization by selective visual attention. Science 291, 1560–1563. doi: 10.1126/science.291.5508.1560

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Güntekin, B., and Başar, E. (2010). Event-related beta oscillations are affected by emotional eliciting stimuli. Neurosci. Lett. 483, 173–178. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2010.08.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Güntekin, B., and Tülay, E. (2014). Event related beta and gamma oscillatory responses during perception of affective pictures. Brain Res. 1577, 45–56.

Google Scholar

Haegens, S., Luther, L., and Jensen, O. (2012). Somatosensory anticipatory alpha activity increases to suppress distracting input. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 24, 677–685. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00164

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hagenaars, M. A., Roelofs, K., and Stins, J. F. (2014). Human freezing in response to affective films. Anxiety Stress Coping 27, 27–37. doi: 10.1080/10615806.2013.809420

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hagenaars, M. A., Stins, J. F., and Roelofs, K. (2012). Aversive life events enhance human freezing responses. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 141, 98–105. doi: 10.1037/a0024211

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herrmann, C. S., Rach, S., Vosskuhl, J., and Strüber, D. (2014). Time-frequency analysis of event-related potentials: A brief tutorial. Brain Topogr. 27, 438–450. doi: 10.1007/s10548-013-0327-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Horschig, J. M., Smolders, R., Bonnefond, M., Schoffelen, J. M., Van Den Munckhof, P., Schuurman, P. R., et al. (2015). Directed communication between nucleus accumbens and neocortex in humans is differentially supported by synchronization in the theta and alpha band. PLoS One 10:e0138685. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0138685

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hyafil, A., Giraud, A. L., Fontolan, L., and Gutkin, B. (2015). Neural cross-frequency coupling: Connecting architectures, mechanisms, and functions. Trends Neurosci. 38, 725–740.

Google Scholar

IBM Corp (2015). IBM SPSS statistics for windows, version 23.0. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp.

Google Scholar

Jensen, O., and Mazaheri, A. (2010). Shaping functional architecture by oscillatory alpha activity: Gating by inhibition. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 4:186. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00186

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jensen, O., Gips, B., Bergmann, T. O., and Bonnefond, M. (2014). Temporal coding organized by coupled alpha and gamma oscillations prioritize visual processing. Trends Neurosci. 37, 357–369. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2014.04.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karakaş, S., Erzengin, ÖU., and Başar, E. (2000). The genesis of human event-related responses explained through the theory of oscillatory neural assemblies. Neurosci. Lett. 285, 45–48. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3940(00)01022-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keil, A., Mueller, M. M., Gruber, T., Wienbruch, C., Stolarova, M., and Elbert, T. (2001). Effects of emotional arousal in the cerebral hemispheres: A study of oscillatory brain activity and event-related potentials. Clin. Neurophysiol. 112, 2057–2068.

Google Scholar

Klimesch, W., Sauseng, P., and Hanslmayr, S. (2007). EEG alpha oscillations: The inhibition-timing hypothesis. Brain Res. Rev. 53, 63–88. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2006.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., and Cuthbert, B. (1997). “Motivated attention: affect, activation and action,” in Attention and orienting: Sensory and motivational processes, eds M. T. B. P. J. Lang and R. F. Simons (Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum), 97–135.

Google Scholar

Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., and Cuthbert, B. N. (2001). International affective picture system (IAPS): Instruction manual and affective ratings. Gainesville, FL: University of Florida.

Google Scholar

Maris, E., and Oostenveld, R. (2007). Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG-data. J. Neurosci. Methods 164, 177–190. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2007.03.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mueller, M. M., Keil, A., Gruber, T., and Elbert, T. (1999). Processing of affective pictures modulates right-hemispheric gamma band EEG activity. Clin. Neurophysiol. 110, 1913–1920. doi: 10.1016/s1388-2457(99)00151-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Niermann, H. C., Ly, V., Smeekens, S., Figner, B., Riksen-Walraven, J. M., and Roelofs, K. (2015). Infant attachment predicts bodily freezing in adolescence: Evidence from a prospective longitudinal study. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 9:263. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00263

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Noordewier, M. K., Scheepers, D. T., and Hilbert, L. P. (2020). Freezing in response to social threat: A replication. Psychol. Res. 84, 1890–1896.

Google Scholar

Okazaki, Y. O., Horschig, J. M., Luther, L., Oostenveld, R., Murakami, I., and Jensen, O. (2015). Real-time MEG neurofeedback training of posterior alpha activity modulates subsequent visual detection performance. Neuroimage 107, 323–332. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.12.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oostenveld, R., Fries, P., Maris, E., and Schoffelen, J. M. (2011). FieldTrip: Open source software for advanced analysis of MEG, EEG, and invasive electrophysiological data. Comput. Intell. Neurosci. 2011:156869. doi: 10.1155/2011/156869

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Percival, D. B., and Walden, A. T. (1993). Spectral analysis for physical applications. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511622762

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Popov, T., Steffen, A., Weisz, N., Miller, G. A., and Rockstroh, B. (2012). Cross-frequency dynamics of neuromagnetic oscillatory activity: Two mechanisms of emotion regulation. Psychophysiology 49, 1545–1557. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2012.01484.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roelofs, K., Hagenaars, M. A., and Stins, J. (2010). Facing freeze: Social threat induces bodily freeze in humans. Psychol. Sci. 21, 1575–1581. doi: 10.1177/0956797610384746

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schalk, G. (2015). A general framework for dynamic cortical function: The function-through-biased-oscillations (FBO) hypothesis. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9:352. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00352

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schupp, H. T., Schmälzle, R., Flaisch, T., Weike, A. I., and Hamm, A. O. (2013). Reprint of “affective picture processing as a function of preceding picture valence: An ERP analysis”. Biol. Psychol. 92, 520–525. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.02.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sherry, A., and Henson, R. K. (2005). Conducting and interpreting canonical correlation analysis in personality research: A user-friendly primer. J. Pers. Assess. 84, 37–48. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa8401_09

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stins, J. F., and Beek, P. J. (2007). Effects of affective picture viewing on postural control in healthy male subjects. BMC Neurosci. 8:83. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-8-83

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uusberg, A., Uibo, H., Kreegipuu, K., and Allik, J. (2013). EEG alpha and cortical inhibition in affective attention. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 89, 26–36. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2013.04.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zazio, A., Schreiber, M., Miniussi, C., and Bortoletto, M. (2020). Modelling the effects of ongoing alpha activity on visual perception: The oscillation-based probability of response. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 112, 242–253. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.01.037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Appendix

Numbers of the International Affective Picture System pictures.

Neutral
2215 1675 7004 7034 9070 7175 5510 7205 2880 7040 7031 2280 7493
2191 2305 1670 7036 7490 2489 7100 7080 5395 7224 7033 2635 7000
7504 7236 2595 2211 2410 9210 7011 3550.2 2038 7001 7060 7090 2480
2870 7041 2273 7300 2493 2484 2411 2840 7030 1903 2026 7035 2441
5471 2002 7512 7640 2383 7500 5740 5040 5520 4605 8325 2382 7487
7950 7081 2597 7161 7026 2495 7057 2780 2381 7025 2221 7285 7513
7595 2749 7550 2720 5731 7187 2102 7179 7002 8121 7497 7044 2210
2385 2020 2272 7050 2830 2104 7190 2514 2200 7061 2390 7037 2397
2487 2101 7290 8312 7010 1616 7705 7009 2230 7217 2351 2393 7006
2396 2579 7235 7546 9700 9260 9468 2512 7207 2357 2575 2518 7150
2372 8010 2440 2377 2506 7491 6150 7185 7062 2890 5534 1935 2214
2235 2485 2499 2107 7140 2190 1313 2570 7211 1350 2516 7020 7506
Pleasant
8041 4641 5600 8161 5210 1750 8186 8350 7330 8192 4626 8620 8400
1460 4599 2310 8190 5825 1731 5760 2045 4623 2071 5700 8210 1440
8461 8032 5621 8470 2216 1463 7200 2550 1811 5982 1999 8490 1920
2158 8120 2540 8370 5829 2170 8300 8460 2154 1722 8034 8540 8040
2332 5833 5480 8021 2306 2340 7508 1610 5631 5594 5200 2035 2391
8200 8090 1441 2311 1710 2373 8380 2208 8465 5780 2398 5910 5010
Unpleasant
9419 3100 2375.1 9006 3180 3069 9904 9410 3150 3010 3102 6540 9428
9414 3053 9040 3130 9000 3181 9903 9340 9920 6212 9322 9570 3131
9901 3225 3266 9830 3016 3051 3195 9342 9940 3063 2703 3060 2900.1
3160 7380 9183 9560 9325 9520 6230 2352.2 9254 3400 3191 3080 9185
9184 3000 3110 9140 3015 3030 9181 9413 3061 9810 6311 9331 6838
3062 2717 2799 9405 3140 2205 9290 2095 3170 3261 9611 3064 9250

Keywords: IAPS, EEG, alpha band activity, gamma band activity, stimulus induced, state effect, balance board, body sway

Citation: Luther L, Horschig JM, van Peer JM, Roelofs K, Jensen O and Hagenaars MA (2023) Oscillatory brain responses to emotional stimuli are effects related to events rather than states. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 16:868549. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2022.868549

Received: 02 February 2022; Accepted: 20 July 2022;
Published: 18 January 2023.

Edited by:

Viktor Müller, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Germany

Reviewed by:

Emine Elif Tülay, Muğla University, Turkey
Agnese Zazio, San Giovanni di Dio Fatebenefratelli Center (IRCCS), Italy

Copyright © 2023 Luther, Horschig, van Peer, Roelofs, Jensen and Hagenaars. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Lisa Luther, bC5sdXRoZXJAZG9uZGVycy5ydS5ubA==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.