Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 12 April 2022
Sec. Brain Imaging and Stimulation
This article is part of the Research Topic Neuroimaging of Non-Motor Deficits in Movement Disorders View all 5 articles

Does Prefrontal Glutamate Index Cognitive Changes in Parkinson’s Disease?

\r\nIsabelle Buard*Isabelle Buard1*Natalie Lopez-EsquibelNatalie Lopez-Esquibel1Finnuella J. CareyFinnuella J. Carey2Mark S. BrownMark S. Brown3Luis D. MedinaLuis D. Medina4Eugene KronbergEugene Kronberg1Christine S. MartinChristine S. Martin1Sarah RogersSarah Rogers1Samantha K. HoldenSamantha K. Holden1Michael R. GreherMichael R. Greher5Benzi M. Kluger\r\nBenzi M. Kluger6
  • 1Department of Neurology, University of Colorado, Denver, Aurora, CO, United States
  • 2Department of Medicine, University of Wisconsin–Madison, Madison, WI, United States
  • 3Department of Psychiatry, University of Colorado, Denver, Aurora, CO, United States
  • 4Department of Psychology, University of Houston, Houston, TX, United States
  • 5Department of Neurosurgery, University of Colorado, Denver, Aurora, CO, United States
  • 6Department of Neurology, University of Rochester Medical Center, Rochester, NY, United States

Introduction: Cognitive impairment is a highly prevalent non-motor feature of Parkinson’s disease (PD). A better understanding of the underlying pathophysiology may help in identifying therapeutic targets to prevent or treat dementia. This study sought to identify metabolic alterations in the prefrontal cortex (PFC), a key region for cognitive functioning that has been implicated in cognitive dysfunction in PD.

Methods: Proton Magnetic Resonance Spectroscopy was used to investigate metabolic changes in the PFC of a cohort of cognitively normal individuals without PD (CTL), as well as PD participants with either normal cognition (PD-NC), mild cognitive impairment (PD-MCI), or dementia (PDD). Ratios to Creatine (Cre) resonance were obtained for glutamate (Glu), glutamine and glutamate combined (Glx), N-acetylaspartate (NAA), myoinositol (mI), and total choline (Cho), and correlated with cognitive scores across multiple domains (executive function, learning and memory, language, attention, visuospatial function, and global cognition) administered to the PD participants only.

Results: When individuals retain cognitive capabilities, the presence of Parkinson’s disease does not create metabolic disturbances in the PFC. However, when cognitive symptoms are present, PFC Glu/Cre ratios decrease with significant differences between the PD-NC and PPD groups. In addition, Glu/Cre ratios and memory scores were marginally associated, but not after Bonferroni correction.

Conclusion: These preliminary findings indicate that fluctuations in prefrontal glutamate may constitute a biomarker for the progression of cognitive impairments in PD. We caution for larger MRS investigations of carefully defined PD groups.

Highlights

- Prefrontal alterations in PD may be relevant to cognitive decline

- Decreased prefrontal glutamate is observed in PD-related cognitive dysfunction

- Prefrontal glutamate could be a biomarker of cognitive changes in PD

Introduction

In addition to motor features, cognitive decline affects a large proportion of Parkinson’s disease (PD) patients (Saredakis et al., 2019). PD with mild cognitive impairment (PD-MCI) is a transitional state between normal cognition “PD-NC” and Parkinson’s disease dementia “PDD” (Litvan et al., 2012), characterized by increased difficulty in performing executive function, verbal memory, and object recognition tasks. PD-MCI is strongly associated with conversion to PDD (Nicoletti et al., 2019), often resulting in poor patient and caregiver quality of life (Obeso et al., 2017), and nursing home placement (Buchanan et al., 2002).

Cognitive impairment in PD is associated with disturbances in frontostriatal circuits influenced by dopaminergic abnormalities (Leh et al., 2010). However, glutamatergic abnormalities are also observed in frontal areas, such as changes in glutamatergic transporters homeostasis (Kashani et al., 2007), but focal metabolic changes may only be relevant to specific functions. Glutamatergic pathways are therefore suspected to play a major role in the structural and functional organization of these circuits (Albin et al., 1989) and possibly in cognitive changes in PD. Functional and connectivity alterations in the prefrontal cortex (PFC) are specifically associated with PD-related impairment in executive function (Isaacs et al., 2019). However, people with PD experience difficulties in other cognitive domains (Watson and Leverenz, 2010) including memory, language, attention, and visuospatial abilities. Both anatomical and functional neuroimaging provide important insights into prefrontal abnormalities associated with cognitive decline in PD.

Proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy (1H-MRS) identifies focal metabolic and neurodegenerative changes (Martin, 2007). Relevant metabolites that can be accessed include: N acetyl-aspartate (NAA), a putative marker of neuronal viability and function; creatine, a marker of energy reserves in the tissue; myo-Inositol (mI), a glial marker (Oz et al., 2014), choline and choline containing compounds (Cho), an indicator of glial health and also inflammation (Rovira and Alonso, 2013); and glutamate, the primary excitatory neurotransmitter in the brain, along with its precursor glutamine. The glutamate-glutamine cycle maintains an adequate supply of glutamate. Investigating various metabolic patterns may therefore shed the light on neuropathological changes associated with a neurologic disorder. Metabolic changes in the brain are a hallmark of other neurodegenerative disorders with cognitive dysfunction such as frontotemporal dementia (Chawla et al., 2010) and dementia with Lewy bodies (Graff-Radford et al., 2014). In PD, abnormal metabolic ratios of NAA have been observed in the substantia nigra as compared to normal controls (Metarugcheep et al., 2012). Regarding glutamate, one study reports a reduction in the posterior cingulate of non-demented patients (Griffith et al., 2008). In PD-MCI, prior MRS studies have observed reductions in NAA in the occipital lobe (as compared to controls) (Nie et al., 2013) and right PFC (as compared to PD-NC) (Pagonabarraga et al., 2012). In PDD, studies have observed reductions in NAA in the occipital region (Summerfield et al., 2002) and the left hippocampus (Pagonabarraga et al., 2012).

Though Pagonabarraga et al. (2012) correlated prefrontal NAA with frontal subcortical tasks, to our knowledge, no studies have examined glutamate and glutamine in PD patients across the full spectrum of cognitive impairment. Glutamate and glutamine do increase from rest during cognitive tasks (Huang et al., 2015), suggesting that cognitive dysfunction may result from an inability to appropriately increase glutamate and glutamine. Indeed, reduction of glutamate in the PFC occurs in age-related changes in thinking and memory (Grachev et al., 2001) so we hypothesized that PFC glutamate decreases along with cognitive decline. We were also interested as to whether these changes would track changes in other neurometabolites accessed with MRS such as NAA and to provide a more complete metabolic profile. Thus, the aim of this study was to expand on prior research by examining a broader array of metabolic compounds in the PFC using 1H-MRS, including for specific neurotransmitters (i) to determine if our cohort of cognitively normal PD patients differ from cognitively normal individuals without PD (CTL) to provide a baseline and confirm/refute previous published studies, and (ii) to determine if varying degrees of cognitive impairment causes metabolic changes in the PFC.

Methods

Participants and Cognitive Classification

Participants were recruited from the University of Colorado Hospital Movement Disorders clinics (for the PD groups) as well as fliers posted on campus and signed informed consent to participate in the study approved by the Colorado Multiple Institution Review Board. Inclusion criteria included a diagnosis of probable PD according to the UK Brain Bank Criteria (Hughes et al., 1992). Classification of normal cognition (PD-NC), mild cognitive impairment (PD-MCI), or dementia (PDD), was determined by consensus conference, attended by at least one neurologist (B.M.K., S.K.H.) and one neuropsychologist (M.R.G., L.D.M.) with expertise in PD cognition. Prior to consensus conference meetings, raw scores were transformed to z-scores based on normative data for each of the individual neuropsychological tests, drawn from either testing manuals (Smith, 1973; Benton et al., 1978; Wechsler, 1987; Reitan and Wolfson, 1993; Delis et al., 2000; Schmidt et al., 2005) or additional normative studies (Ruff and Parker, 1993; Tombaugh and Hubley, 1997; Tombaugh et al., 1999; Drane et al., 2002). MDS Task Force Level II diagnostic guidelines for PDD (Emre et al., 2007) and PD-MCI (Litvan et al., 2012) were applied, requiring impairment on at least two tests in one cognitive domain or one test in each of two cognitive domains for classification as cognitively impaired. Of note, we diverged from the MDS recommendations in that we had two neuropsychological tests for all domains tested but one (e.g., visuospatial abilities). Impairment was defined as performance of ≥1.5 standard deviations below age- and education-matched norms (Goldman et al., 2013). PD-MCI was differentiated from PDD based on the results of the neurologist’s functional interview: if significant functional impairment related to cognitive symptoms was present based on clinical impression, a participant was classified as PDD. Exclusion criteria included features suggestive of other neurological disorders and contraindications of MRI, including deep brain stimulation, implanted medical devices, intracranial metal, and motor symptoms expected to interfere with MRI scanning. All study visits were performed in the PD subjects’ best dopaminergic “ON” state. Healthy controls were screened for absence of cognitive impairment (MoCA scores > 26 were included in the study).

Neuropsychological Battery

PD-NC, PD-MCI, and PDD participants, but not CTL subjects, underwent extensive neuropsychological assessments: Mattis Dementia Rating Scale (DRS-2) (Mattis, 2002); Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) (Nasreddine et al., 2005); Trail Making Test-A (TMT-A) and Trail Making Test-B (TMT-B) (Army Individual Test Battery, 1944); Symbol Digit Modality Test (SDMT) Oral (Pereira et al., 2015); Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System (DKEFS) Verbal Fluency/Letter Fluency task (Delis et al., 2001); California Verbal Learning Test, Second Edition (CVLT-II) (Fine and Delis, 2011); Boston Naming Test (BNT) (Kaplan et al., 1983); Brief Test of Attention (BTA) (Schretlen, 1997); and Judgment of Line Orientation task (JLO) (Benton et al., 1978).

Cognitive data were checked for missingness and found to violate the assumption that data were missing completely at random (MCAR): Little’s MCAR Test, p = 0.007. Therefore, data were imputed to create five multiple imputations. The imputed data did not significantly differ from the original data (all ps > 0.05). For data reduction purposes, cognitive composite scores were created using principal components analyses for each set of imputed data separately. Summary statistics for the creation of the composite scores are displayed in Supplementary Table 1. Composites were created from two variables per domain as follows:

- Global cognition: MoCA and DRS-2 total scores

- Attention: TMT-A and BTA

- Language: BNT and verbal fluency

- Learning & memory: CVLT trials 1–5 total score and CVLT long delay free recall

- Executive function: TMT-B and SDMT oral

The Judgment of Line Orientation Test was the only individual test of visuospatial abilities, so a composite score was not created for it.

MR and MRS Acquisition

MR data were obtained using a Siemens Skyra 3 Tesla wide bore whole-body MRI scanner, equipped with high performance gradient coils (maximum gradient amplitude of 45 mT/m and maximum slew rate of 200 T/m/s) and a 12-channel phased-array head/neck coil. The MRS voxels were placed over PFC (Figure 1A) as described previously (de la Vega et al., 2014) and metabolites were measured using a Siemens’ standard svs-se (PRESS) sequence with parameters TR/TE = 30/2,000 ms, number of averages (NAV) = 128, with out-of-voxel saturation bands placed to minimize artifact from subdural fat. To estimate metabolites ratios in the PFC, LCModel software version 6.3-1L (Provencher, 1993) was used, based on a Bayesian analysis starting with solution spectra basis sets that provide estimates of metabolite ratios without operator bias. Five metabolites were analyzed (Figure 1B): glutamate (Glu), Glx (combined glutamate + glutamine, precursor/product of glutamate), NAA, myoinositol (mI; a glial marker), and total choline (Cho, combined signals from glycerophosphocholine and phosphocholine). Metabolite were expressed as the ratios of their peak areas relative to the peak area of the creatine (Cre) resonance. The averages signal to noise ratio (SNR) and full width half maximum (FWHM) of the spectra in the study (from LCModel output) were, respectively, 49.04 ± 8.9 and 0.049 ± 0.01. Poorly fitted metabolite peaks were excluded from further analysis. The average voxel size was 2.45 ± 0.18 mL.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Position of the MRS voxel in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of a cognitively normal participant without PD on a sagittal slice of the 3D T1 weighted MRI data set (A). Subplot (B) presents the corresponding 1H-MR spectrum (black) and fit (red).

T1-weighted images were used to determined voxel gray matter (GM), white matter (WM) and cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) fractions in the PFC. CSF, GM and WM of the voxels were segmented via calls from Gannet to SPM12 (Functional Imaging Laboratory, The Wellcome Trust Centre for NeuroImaging, in the Institute of Neurology at University College London, United Kingdom). Metabolite ratios were corrected for the voxel CSF content using standardized methods (Chowdhury et al., 2015), since CSF is considered to provide negligible contribution to the Glu and Gln signals.

Statistical Analysis

All statistical analyses were conducted at p < 0.05 for significance using SPSS (IBM Corp. Released 2017. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 25.0. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp.). Demographic data were compared between groups by one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA). Chi-squared test was used for categorical data (sex, handedness). CSF-corrected metabolite data were compared between groups by t-tests for the PD-NC vs. CTL comparison and one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) for the PD-NC vs. PD-MCI vs. PDD comparisons. Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons was used to investigate significant differences. Neuropsychological performance of the PD-NC, PD-MCI, and PDD groups were compared using one-way ANOVA. Bonferroni post hoc tests were performed to assess differences between each group. Stepwise linear regression analysis was computed to assess correlations between 1H-MRS metabolites and pooled composite scores from the five imputations, as well as scores for the JLO test with age of subjects as a covariate. Two models were run. Corrected Glu/Cre was used as a dependent variable, and age as well as composite score were used as explanatory variables. In addition, a False Discovery Rate correction (Benjamini, 1995) was used to control for multiple comparisons.

Results

Demographic/Clinical Characteristics

Data from nineteen CTL subjects, eleven PD-NC participants, twenty-four PD-MCI participants, and eleven PDD participants were used in the 1H-MRS study. Participant demographics and neuropsychological performance of the PD group are summarized in Supplementary Table 2 and Table 1, respectively. Groups were not statistically different in terms of demographics or baseline measures with the exception of sex, age, and neuropsychological testing. There was an association between sex and group, χ2 (3) = 8.315, p = 0.040. Age did not differ significantly between CTL, PD-NC, and PD-MCI participants, but PDD participants were significantly older than the other groups (F = 4.498, p = 0.006), which is inherently associated with dementia occurrence in PD patients. Therefore, age was used as a covariate for subsequent analyses. Sex was not used as a covariate given that there was no female in one of the PD groups (PDD). Motor severity was also not used as a covariate to avoid multicollinearity issues.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Neuropsychological test scores by cognitive classification for the PD groups.

MRS

CSF-corrected Glx/Cre ratio was increased in the PD-NC compared to the CTL group, however, Bonferroni correction for pairwise comparisons indicated this increase was not statistically significant (Supplementary Table 3). Univariate analysis of CSF-corrected metabolite ratios showed the ratio Glu/Cre [F(2,43) = 3.444, p = 0.041] in the PFC to be significantly different between the PD groups (Supplementary Table 4). Post hoc comparisons showed glutamate to decrease between PD-NC and PD-MCI and also between PD-MCI and PDD groups, but significant differences were only observed between PD-NC and PDD groups (p = 0.044; Supplementary Table 4 and Figure 2). Comparisons of NAA, Glx, mI, and Cho ratios among PD groups demonstrated no differences.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. CSF-corrected metabolite ratios to Creatine for the PD groups. Mean ± SD of metabolite ratio to Cre of Glu, Glx, NAA, mI, and Cho in the PD-NC, PD-MCI and PDD groups. Asterisk indicates significant group difference at p < 0.05.

Linear Regression Analysis

Averaged Glu/Cre ratios and pooled memory composite scores across PD groups were 0.93 and 1.49 10–16, respectively. When controlling for age, the two variables were marginally linearly related (R2 = 0.047; p = 0.062, Supplementary Table 5 and Figure 3) indicating that low PFC glutamate may be associated with poor memory (lower scores). However, given that our p-values were above the critical values created by the FDR procedure (Supplementary Table 4) this result was considered as statistically non-significant. No significant linear relationships were observed between Glu/Cre and other neuropsychology composite scores or JLO scores. In addition, no significant linear relationships were observed between any other PFC metabolite ratio and any other neuropsychology composite or JLO scores (Supplementary Table 5).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Linear dependance of Glu/Cre on the Memory composite score. The solid line is the line of best fit in the linear regression model. The dashed lines represent the 95% confidence interval (CI) for the upper and lower bounds of the slope parameter.

Discussion

Our results suggest that before cognitive symptoms appear, older people with or without PD exhibit similar neurometabolic profiles in the PFC. But, when cognitive symptoms have appeared, prefrontal glutamate is decreasing along with cognitive worsening.

Glutamate abnormalities at the subcortical level have been previously noted in animal models of PD (Albin et al., 1989; Greenamyre et al., 1994; Phillips et al., 2006) but not in humans (Clarke et al., 1997; Taylor-Robinson et al., 1999; Clarke and Lowry, 2000; Kickler et al., 2007; Emir et al., 2012). In cortical regions, however, glutamate contents are either unchanged when comparing adults with and without PD (Kickler et al., 2007; O’Gorman Tuura et al., 2018), or decreased in the posterior cingulate cortex when the PD group was selected with a strict non-dementia criteria (Griffith et al., 2008). Despite the exclusion of patients with suspected dementia, dementia with Lewy bodies, or atypical PD, it is unclear if patients with PD-MCI were included. In a neuropathological study, vesicular glutamate transporters VGLUT1 and two immunoreactivity was found significantly decreased in the prefrontal and temporal cortices, suggesting a diminution of Glu neuronal expression (Kashani et al., 2007). These findings and ours indicate that cortical glutamate may be perturbed in a regional manner in people with PD, and that it may be related to their cognitive status. Given an evident link between glutamate receptors activation and memory processes (Riedel et al., 2003), pharmacological studies have started to investigate the use of glutamate ion channels (AMPA receptors) potentiators for enhancing cognition in rodent models of PD (O’Neill et al., 2005). This compound appears to enhance AMPA-mediated responses in cortical and subcortical areas affected by the disease, which is encouraging given the extent of the damage PD causes to the large cortico-basal-thalamo-cortical loops. Further studies are, however, warranted to see if restoring glutamate levels does slow cognitive decline progression in PD.

We also observed that PFC Glu/Cre ratios were marginally associated with memory tasks indicating that worsening memory skills may be associated with decreasing glutamate contents in the PFC. This finding is not surprising given an extensive literature related to glutamate and memory, embracing human and animal models. An animal study at 14.1T showed task- or stimulation-induced increase in glutamate reflects enhanced excitatory neurotransmission as well as increased metabolic activity (Sonnay et al., 2016). Modulation of glutamate measured via 1H fMRS is therefore a practical method to investigate in vivo dynamics of excitatory neural activity driven by task-related cognitive demand. In healthy adults, elevated dorso-lateral PFC glutamate during a working memory task has also been shown in vivo (Woodcock et al., 2018). This suggests prefrontal glutamate may be a viable biomarker for memory-related changes, in relationship with diseases affecting cognition or normal aging. Regarding our study, it is possible that investigating glutamate at rest may be subthreshold and therefore not adequately address whether memory impairments are due to dysregulation of local glutamatergic homeostasis.

Conclusion

In summary, this exploratory study supports the relationship between cognitive dysfunction in PD and metabolite alteration in PFC, suggesting that dysregulation of frontal glutamate may index cognitive changes in PD patients. Limitations of the current study include a relatively small sample size, cross-sectional nature of our dataset, and possibly the lack of activation of glutamatergic networks during rest.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Colorado Multiple Institution Review Board. Inclusion. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

IB conceptualized and conducted the study, drafted, and reviewed the manuscript. NL-E and FC performed the analyses, drafted, and reviewed the manuscript. MB provided expertise with MRS data acquisition, analyses and interpretation, and reviewed the manuscript. LM helped conducting neuropsychological tests, performed the behavioral analyses, and reviewed the manuscript. EK helped with MRS data analyses and reviewed the manuscript. CM and SR conducted the study and reviewed the manuscript. SH helped with participants’ recruitment and neuropsychological testing, as well as reviewed the manuscript. MG helped with neuropsychological testing battery design and with the consensus on cognitive diagnosis, as well as reviewed the manuscript. BK conceptualized the study and reviewed the manuscript. All authors have reviewed the final version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was funded by three NIH grants 1K02NS080885-01A1 01A1 (PI: BK), 1R21NS093266-01A1 (PI: BK), and K01 AT009894-01 (PI: IB), and also a Michael J. Fox Foundation for Parkinson’s Research (Grant Number 10879; PI: SH).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank MRI technologist Deb Singel for her assistance with MR data collection via the Brain Imaging Center at the University of Colorado Denver, and Candace Ellman for assistance with manuscript submission. Last, this project used the University of Colorado Anschutz Brain Imaging Center, which is supported by an NIH High-End Instrumentation Grant S10OD018435 (Tregellas, PI).

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnhum.2022.809905/full#supplementary-material

Supplementary Table 1 | Summary statistics for the creation of the composite scores. KMO, Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin Measure of Sampling adequacy; BTS, Bartlett’s Test of Sphericity.

Supplementary Table 2 | Participant demographics. Data presented as mean (SD), range or fraction of participants. The high levodopa equivalence standard deviation indicates that the participants were spread out over a wide range of dosages. UPDRS, Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale. Bold text indicates statistically significant group differences.

Supplementary Table 3 | CSF-corrected metabolite measures for CTL and PD-NC. Values are expressed as means ± SD. Using the Bonferroni correction for three comparisons, the p value has to be below 0.05/1 = 0.01 for an effect to be significant at the 0.05 level. For these data, all p values are far below that, and therefore all pairwise differences are not significant.

Supplementary Table 4 | Corrected metabolite measures for the presence of CSF by cognitive classification. Means, SDs and f-values calculated using SPSS; Post hoc group comparisons using Bonferroni correction for each corrected metabolite level; bold text indicates statistically significant values at p < 0.05. Glu, Glutamate; Glx, Glutamate + Glutamine combined; NAA, N-Acetyl aspartate; Cho, Choline.

Supplementary Table 5 | Pearson product-moment correlation coefficient related to metabolite measures. Means, SDs, R2-values and p-values calculated using SPSS; * indicates statistically significant values at p < 0.05. JLO, Judgment of Line Orientation; B-H, Benjamini-Hochberg. False Discovery Rate correction using the B-H method was used to calculate Critical Values for the Glu/Cre linear regression analyses. P-values over the B-H Critical Values are interpreted as statistically non-significant.

Abbreviations

MoCA, Montreal Cognitive Assessment; SDMT, Symbol Digit Modalities Test; UPDRS, Unified Parkinson’s disease Rating Scale; DRS, Dementia Rating Scale; CVLT, California Verbal Learning Test; Glu, glutamate; Glx, glutamate + glutamine; NAA, N-acetyl-aspartate; mI, myoinositol; Cho, Choline; Cre, creatine; PFC, Prefrontal Cortex; PD-NC, Parkinson’s disease – Normal Cognition; PD-MCI, Parkinson’s disease – Mild Cognitive Impairment; PDD, Parkinson’s disease – Dementia.

References

Albin, R. L., Young, A. B., and Penney, J. B. (1989). The functional anatomy of basal ganglia disorders. Trends Neurosci. 12, 366–375. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(89)90074-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Army Individual Test Battery (1944). Army Individual Test Battery (1944) Manual of Directions and Scoring. Washington, DC: War Department, Adjutant General’s Office.

Google Scholar

Benjamini, Y. H. Y. (1995). Controlling the False Discovery Rate: A Practical and Powerful Approach to Multiple Testing. J. R. Stat. Soc. Ser. B 57, 289–300. doi: 10.1111/j.2517-6161.1995.tb02031.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benton, A. L., Varney, N. R., and Hamsher, K. D. (1978). Visuospatial Judgment - Clinical Test. Arch Neurol. 35, 364–367. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1978.00500300038006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buchanan, R. J., Wang, S., Huang, C., Simpson, P., and Manyam, B. V. (2002). Analyses of nursing home residents with Parkinson’s disease using the minimum data set. Parkinsonism Relat. Disord. 8, 369–380. doi: 10.1016/s1353-8020(02)00004-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chawla, S., Wang, S., Moore, P., Woo, J. H., Elman, L., McCluskey, L. F., et al. (2010). Quantitative proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy detects abnormalities in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and motor cortex of patients with frontotemporal lobar degeneration. J. Neurol. 257, 114–121. doi: 10.1007/s00415-009-5283-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chowdhury, F. A., O’Gorman, R. L., Nashef, L., Elwes, R. D., Edden, R. A., Murdoch, J. B., et al. (2015). Investigation of glutamine and GABA levels in patients with idiopathic generalized epilepsy using MEGAPRESS. J. Magn. Reson. Imag. 41, 694–699. doi: 10.1002/jmri.24611

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clarke, C. E., and Lowry, M. (2000). Basal ganglia metabolite concentrations in idiopathic Parkinson’s disease and multiple system atrophy measured by proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy. Eur. J. Neurol. 7, 661–665. doi: 10.1046/j.1468-1331.2000.00111.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clarke, C. E., Lowry, M., and Horsman, A. (1997). Unchanged basal ganglia N-acetylaspartate and glutamate in idiopathic Parkinson’s disease measured by proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy. Mov. Disord. 12, 297–301. doi: 10.1002/mds.870120306

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de la Vega, A., Brown, M. S., Snyder, H. R., Singel, D., Munakata, Y., and Banich, M. T. (2014). Individual differences in the balance of GABA to glutamate in pFC predict the ability to select among competing options. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 26, 2490–2502. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00655

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delis, D. C., Kaplan, E., and Kramer, J. H., and Psychological Corporation. (2001). Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System. San Antonio: Psychological Corp.

Google Scholar

Delis, D. C., Kramer, J. H., Kaplan, E., and Ober, B. A. (2000). California Verbal Learning Test - second edition. Adult Version. San Antonio: Psychological Corporation.

Google Scholar

Drane, D. L., Yuspeh, R. L., Huthwaite, J. S., and Klingler, L. K. (2002). Demographic characteristics and normative observations for derived-trail making test indices. Neuropsychiatr. Neuropsychol. Behav. Neurol. 15, 39–43.

Google Scholar

Emir, U. E., Tuite, P. J., and Oz, G. (2012). Elevated pontine and putamenal GABA levels in mild-moderate Parkinson disease detected by 7 tesla proton MRS. PLoS One 7:e30918. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0030918

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Emre, M., Aarsland, D., Brown, R., Burn, D. J., Duyckaerts, C., Mizuno, Y., et al. (2007). Clinical diagnostic criteria for dementia associated with Parkinson’s disease. Mov. Disord. 22, 1689–1707. doi: 10.1002/mds.21507

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fine, E. M., and Delis, D. C. (2011). “California Verbal Learning Test – Children’s Version,” in Encyclopedia of Clinical Neuropsychology, eds J. S. Kreutzer, J. DeLuca, and B. Caplan (New York: Springer New York), 476–479. doi: 10.1007/978-0-387-79948-3_1538

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldman, J. G., Holden, S., Bernard, B., Ouyang, B., Goetz, C. G., and Stebbins, G. T. (2013). Defining optimal cutoff scores for cognitive impairment using Movement Disorder Society Task Force criteria for mild cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease. Mov. Disord. 28, 1972–1979. doi: 10.1002/mds.25655

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grachev, I. D., Swarnkar, A., Szeverenyi, N. M., Ramachandran, T. S., and Apkarian, A. V. (2001). Aging alters the multichemical networking profile of the human brain: an in vivo (1)H-MRS study of young versus middle-aged subjects. J. Neurochem. 77, 292–303. doi: 10.1046/j.1471-4159.2001.t01-1-00238.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Graff-Radford, J., Boeve, B. F., Murray, M. E., Ferman, T. J., Tosakulwong, N., Lesnick, T. G., et al. (2014). Regional proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy patterns in dementia with Lewy bodies. Neurobiol. Aging 35, 1483–1490. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2014.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenamyre, J. T., Eller, R. V., Zhang, Z., Ovadia, A., Kurlan, R., and Gash, D. M. (1994). Antiparkinsonian effects of remacemide hydrochloride, a glutamate antagonist, in rodent and primate models of Parkinson’s disease. Ann. Neurol. 35, 655–661. doi: 10.1002/ana.410350605

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Griffith, H. R., Okonkwo, O. C., O’Brien, T., and Hollander, J. A. D. (2008). Reduced brain glutamate in patients with Parkinson’s disease. NMR Biomed. 21, 381–387. doi: 10.1002/nbm.1203

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, Z., Davis, H. I., Yue, Q., Wiebking, C., Duncan, N. W., Zhang, J., et al. (2015). Increase in glutamate/glutamine concentration in the medial prefrontal cortex during mental imagery: A combined functional mrs and fMRI study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 36, 3204–3212. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22841

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hughes, A. J., Daniel, S. E., Kilford, L., and Lees, A. J. (1992). Accuracy of clinical diagnosis of idiopathic Parkinson’s disease: a clinico-pathological study of 100 cases. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 55, 181–184. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.55.3.181

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Isaacs, M. L., McMahon, K. L., Angwin, A. J., Crosson, B., and Copland, D. A. (2019). Functional correlates of strategy formation and verbal suppression in Parkinson’s disease. Neuroimag. Clin. 22:101683. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2019.101683

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaplan, E. F., Goodglass, H., and Weintraub, S. (1983). The Boston Naming Test. Philadelphia: Lea and Febiger.

Google Scholar

Kashani, A., Betancur, C., Giros, B., Hirsch, E., and El Mestikawy, S. (2007). Altered expression of vesicular glutamate transporters VGLUT1 and VGLUT2 in Parkinson disease. Neurobiol. Aging 28, 568–578. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2006.02.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kickler, N., Krack, P., Fraix, V., Lebas, J. F., Lamalle, L., Durif, F., et al. (2007). Glutamate measurement in Parkinson’s disease using MRS at 3 T field strength. NMR Biomed. 20, 757–762. doi: 10.1002/nbm.1141

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leh, S. E., Petrides, M., and Strafella, A. P. (2010). The neural circuitry of executive functions in healthy subjects and Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychopharmacology 35, 70–85. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.88

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Litvan, I., Goldman, J. G., Troster, A. I., Schmand, B. A., Weintraub, D., Petersen, R. C., et al. (2012). Diagnostic criteria for mild cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease: movement Disorder Society Task Force guidelines. Mov. Disord. 27, 349–356. doi: 10.1002/mds.24893

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin, W. R. (2007). MR spectroscopy in neurodegenerative disease. Mol. Imag. Biol. 9, 196–203. doi: 10.1007/s11307-007-0087-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mattis, S. (2002). Dementis Rating Scale-2 (DRS-2). Lutz: Psychological Assessment Resources Inc.

Google Scholar

Metarugcheep, P., Hanchaiphiboolkul, S., Viriyavejakul, A., and Chanyawattiwongs, S. (2012). The usage of proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy in Parkinson’s disease. J. Med. Assoc. Thai. 95, 949–952.

Google Scholar

Nasreddine, Z. S., Phillips, N. A., Bedirian, V., Charbonneau, S., Whitehead, V., Collin, I., et al. (2005). The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 53, 695–699. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nicoletti, A., Luca, A., Baschi, R., Cicero, C. E., Mostile, G., Davi, M., et al. (2019). Incidence of Mild Cognitive Impairment and Dementia in Parkinson’s Disease: the Parkinson’s Disease Cognitive Impairment Study. Front. Aging Neurosci. 11:21. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2019.00021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nie, K., Zhang, Y., Huang, B., Wang, L., Zhao, J., Huang, Z., et al. (2013). Marked N-acetylaspartate and choline metabolite changes in Parkinson’s disease patients with mild cognitive impairment. Parkinsonism Relat. Disord. 19, 329–334. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2012.11.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Obeso, J. A., Stamelou, M., Goetz, C. G., Poewe, W., Lang, A. E., Weintraub, D., et al. (2017). Past, present, and future of Parkinson’s disease: A special essay on the 200th Anniversary of the Shaking Palsy. Mov. Disord. 32, 1264–1310. doi: 10.1002/mds.27115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Gorman Tuura, R. L., Baumann, C. R., and Baumann-Vogel, H. (2018). Beyond Dopamine: GABA, Glutamate, and the Axial Symptoms of Parkinson Disease. Front. Neurol. 9:806. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2018.00806

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Neill, M. J., Murray, T. K., Clay, M. P., Lindstrom, T., Yang, C. R., and Nisenbaum, E. S. (2005). LY503430: pharmacology, pharmacokinetics, and effects in rodent models of Parkinson’s disease. CNS Drug Rev. 11, 77–96. doi: 10.1111/j.1527-3458.2005.tb00037.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oz, G., Alger, J. R., Barker, P. B., Bartha, R., Bizzi, A., Boesch, C., et al. (2014). Clinical proton MR spectroscopy in central nervous system disorders. Radiology 270, 658–679. doi: 10.1148/radiol.13130531

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pagonabarraga, J., Gomez-Anson, B., Rotger, R., Llebaria, G., Garcia-Sanchez, C., Pascual-Sedano, B., et al. (2012). Spectroscopic changes associated with mild cognitive impairment and dementia in Parkinson’s disease. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 34, 312–318. doi: 10.1159/000345537

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pereira, D. R., Costa, P., and Cerqueira, J. J. (2015). Repeated Assessment and Practice Effects of the Written Symbol Digit Modalities Test Using a Short Inter-Test Interval. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 30, 424–434. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acv028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Phillips, J. M., Lam, H. A., Ackerson, L. C., and Maidment, N. T. (2006). Blockade of mGluR glutamate receptors in the subthalamic nucleus ameliorates motor asymmetry in an animal model of Parkinson’s disease. Eur. J. Neurosci. 23, 151–160. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04550.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Provencher, S. W. (1993). Estimation of metabolite concentrations from localized in vivo proton NMR spectra. Magn. Reson. Med. 30, 672–679. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910300604

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reitan, R. M., and Wolfson, D. (1993). The Trail-Making Test. Tuscon: Neuropsychology Press.

Google Scholar

Riedel, G., Platt, B., and Micheau, J. (2003). Glutamate receptor function in learning and memory. Behav. Brain Res. 140, 1–47. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(02)00272-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rovira, A., and Alonso, J. (2013). 1H magnetic resonance spectroscopy in multiple sclerosis and related disorders. Neuroimag. Clin. N. Am. 23, 459–474. doi: 10.1016/j.nic.2013.03.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruff, R. M., and Parker, S. B. (1993). Gender- and age-specific changes in motor speed and eye-hand coordination in adults: normative values for the Finger Tapping and Grooved Pegboard Tests. Percept. Mot. Skills 76, 1219–1230. doi: 10.2466/pms.1993.76.3c.1219

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saredakis, D., Collins-Praino, L. E., Gutteridge, D. S., Stephan, B. C. M., and Keage, H. A. D. (2019). Conversion to MCI and dementia in Parkinson’s disease: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Parkinsonism Relat. Disord. 65, 20–31. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2019.04.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmidt, K. S., Mattis, P. J., Adams, J., and Nestor, P. (2005). Test-retest reliability of the dementia rating scale-2: alternate form. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 20, 42–44. doi: 10.1159/000085073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schretlen, D. (1997). Brief Test of Attention Professional Manual. Odessa: Psychological Assessment Resources.

Google Scholar

Smith, A. (1973). Symbol Digits Modality Test. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.

Google Scholar

Sonnay, S., Duarte, J. M., Just, N., and Gruetter, R. (2016). Compartmentalised energy metabolism supporting glutamatergic neurotransmission in response to increased activity in the rat cerebral cortex: A 13C MRS study in vivo at 14.1 T. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 36, 928–940. doi: 10.1177/0271678X16629482

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Summerfield, C., Gomez-Anson, B., Tolosa, E., Mercader, J. M., Marti, M. J., Pastor, P., et al. (2002). Dementia in Parkinson disease: a proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy study. Arch. Neurol. 59, 1415–1420. doi: 10.1001/archneur.59.9.1415

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor-Robinson, S. D., Turjanski, N., Bhattacharya, S., Seery, J. P., Sargentoni, J., Brooks, D. J., et al. (1999). A proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy study of the striatum and cerebral cortex in Parkinson’s disease. Metab. Brain Dis. 14, 45–55. doi: 10.1023/a:1020609530444

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tombaugh, T. N., and Hubley, A. M. (1997). The 60-item Boston Naming Test: norms for cognitively intact adults aged 25 to 88 years. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 19, 922–932. doi: 10.1080/01688639708403773

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tombaugh, T. N., Kozak, J., and Rees, L. (1999). Normative data stratified by age and education for two measures of verbal fluency: FAS and animal naming. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 14, 167–177. doi: 10.1016/s0887-6177(97)00095-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watson, G. S., and Leverenz, J. B. (2010). Profile of cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease. Brain Pathol. 20, 640–645.

Google Scholar

Wechsler, D. (1987). Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised Manual. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation.

Google Scholar

Woodcock, E. A., Anand, C., Khatib, D., Diwadkar, V. A., and Stanley, J. A. (2018). Working Memory Modulates Glutamate Levels in the Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex during (1)H fMRS. Front. Psychiatry 9:66. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00066

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: Parkinson’s disease, mild cognitive impairment, magnetic resonance spectroscopy, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, glutamate

Citation: Buard I, Lopez-Esquibel N, Carey FJ, Brown MS, Medina LD, Kronberg E, Martin CS, Rogers S, Holden SK, Greher MR and Kluger BM (2022) Does Prefrontal Glutamate Index Cognitive Changes in Parkinson’s Disease? Front. Hum. Neurosci. 16:809905. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2022.809905

Received: 05 November 2021; Accepted: 18 March 2022;
Published: 12 April 2022.

Edited by:

Max Kurz, Boys Town National Research Hospital, United States

Reviewed by:

David Arpin, University of Florida, United States
Rachel K. Spooner, Heinrich Heine University Düsseldorf, Germany

Copyright © 2022 Buard, Lopez-Esquibel, Carey, Brown, Medina, Kronberg, Martin, Rogers, Holden, Greher and Kluger. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Isabelle Buard, Isabelle.Buard@CUAnschutz.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.