Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 09 January 2023
Sec. Brain Health and Clinical Neuroscience
This article is part of the Research Topic Women In Brain Health and Clinical Neuroscience View all 7 articles

Electrophysiological biomarkers and age characterize phenotypic heterogeneity among individuals with major depressive disorder

\r\nAlexandra P. Key*Alexandra P. Key1*Tricia A. Thornton-WellsTricia A. Thornton-Wells2Daniel G. SmithDaniel G. Smith2
  • 1Department of Hearing and Speech Sciences, Vanderbilt University Medical Center, Nashville, TN, United States
  • 2Translational Medicine, Pharmaceutical and Early-Stage Clinical Development, Alkermes, Inc., Waltham, MA, United States

Introduction: Despite the high need for effective treatments for major depressive disorder (MDD), the development of novel medicines is hampered by clinical, genetic and biological heterogeneity, unclear links between symptoms and neural dysfunction, and tenuous biomarkers for clinical trial contexts of use.

Methods: In this study, we examined the International Study to Predict Optimized Treatment in Depression (iSPOT-D) clinical trial database for new relationships between auditory event-related potential (ERP) responses, demographic features, and clinical symptoms and behavior, to inform strategies for biomarker-driven patient stratification that could be used to optimize future clinical trial design and drug development strategy in MDD.

Results: We replicate findings from previous analyses of the classic auditory oddball task in the iSPOT-D sample showing smaller than typical N1 and P300 response amplitudes and longer P300 latencies for target and standard stimuli in patients with MDD, suggesting altered bottom-up sensory and top-down attentional processes. We further demonstrate that age is an important contributor to clinical group differences, affecting both topographic distribution of the clinically informative ERP responses and the types of the stimuli sensitive to group differences. In addition, the observed brain-behavior associations indicate that levels of anxiety and stress are major contributing factors to atypical sensory and attentional processing among patients with MDD, particularly in the older subgroups.

Discussion: Our novel findings support the possibility of accelerated cognitive aging in patients with MDD and identify the frontal P300 latency as an additional candidate biomarker of MDD. These results from a large, well-phenotyped sample support the view that heterogeneity of the clinical population with MDD can be systematically characterized based on age and neural biomarkers of sensory and attentional processing, informing patient stratification strategies in the design of clinical trials.

Introduction

Major depressive disorder (MDD) is a chronic and recurrent condition with a high and rising prevalence that is among the most burdensome global illnesses (Proudman et al., 2021). It negatively affects daily living, quality of life, cognition, employment, and productivity and is associated with myriad medical and mental health conditions. Antidepressant medications and cognitive-behavioral therapies are effective for some, but many patients (estimates range from 20% to more than 50%; Joffe et al., 1996) do not respond, are treatment-resistant, or have persistent deficits that detract from daily functioning (Kessler et al., 2003; Rush et al., 2006). A high degree of heterogeneity in clinical presentation, an expansive diversity of genetic, neuropathological, or neurophysiological underpinnings, and a lack of informative biomarkers hamper the certainty of diagnosis of MDD and its successful treatment (Fried, 2015). It is widely recognized that expanding the range of objective measures to improve MDD diagnosis, stratify patients, and predict treatment responsiveness or long-term outcomes may increase the probability of success in drug development and effective management of MDD (Buch and Liston, 2021).

Greater affordability and accessibility of functional neuroimaging techniques have led to a significant increase in the interest to identify objective brain-based measures that could predict risk, assist with a diagnosis, or evaluate treatment effects in clinical populations. Event-related potentials (ERP)—brief changes in ongoing EEG following a stimulus event—offer an effective means to noninvasively monitor brain functioning with millisecond-level precision. Differences in the amplitude, latency, and scalp distribution of various ERP responses are interpreted to index neural activity underlying sensory, perceptual, and higher-order cognitive processes and can be used to detect alterations associated with clinical diagnoses, including MDD (Mumtaz et al., 2015; Kangas et al., 2022).

To date, the auditory P300 ERP response has received the most attention as a possible biomarker of MDD-related alterations in neural function (e.g., Mumtaz et al., 2015). It is a positive peak with a centro-parietal scalp maximum occurring 300 ms after the stimulus onset and characterized by a larger amplitude in response to an infrequent target than the frequent standard stimulus in an oddball paradigm. The P300 is interpreted to reflect active attention allocation and working memory (Polich, 2012), both of which may be disrupted in MDD (e.g., Landrø et al., 2001; Hasler et al., 2004; Bruder et al., 2012; Rock et al., 2014). Several prior auditory P300 studies in MDD have reported reduced amplitudes to target tones (Gangadhar et al., 1993; Röschke and Wagner, 2003; Urretavizcaya et al., 2003; Kawasaki et al., 2004; Bruder et al., 2009; Kemp et al., 2009, 2010; Zhou et al., 2019) or a longer P300 peak latency (Bruder et al., 1991; Vandoolaeghe et al., 1998; Urretavizcaya et al., 2003; Houston et al., 2004). Yet, others noted no statistically significant differences in the P300 response characteristics between participants with MDD and healthy controls (Giedke et al., 1981; Sara et al., 1994; Ancy et al., 1996; Bruder et al., 1998; Kaustio et al., 2002; Nan et al., 2018). Studies examining the P300’s ability to predict treatment response also yielded inconsistent results: larger pre-treatment amplitudes or prolonged latencies were associated with better outcomes for antidepressant and electroconvulsive therapy treatments (Gangadhar et al., 1993; Bruder et al., 1995; Ancy et al., 1996; Vandoolaeghe et al., 1998) but did not predict treatment response to yoga (Murthy et al., 1998). Amplitude and latency of the P300 response were related to increased suicidal behavior (Hansenne et al., 1996), heightened anhedonia associated with the melancholic subtype (Schlegel et al., 1991; Ancy et al., 1996; Urretavizcaya et al., 2003), and the presence of psychotic features (Karaaslan et al., 2003) or cognitive impairment (Bruder et al., 1991; Vandoolaeghe et al., 1998). Direct associations between the P300 metrics and the severity of depression symptoms were detected in some (Nan et al., 2018) but not in other studies (Schlegel et al., 1991).

The auditory stimuli in the oddball paradigm elicit other ERP responses in addition to the P300, including the N1, a negative peak with a frontal or central scalp maximum occurring around 100 ms after sound onset and modulated by its physical characteristics. It reflects stimulus detection and early perceptual feature analysis (Näätänen and Picton, 1987). The N1 is generated in the primary auditory cortex (Pantev et al., 1990; Key et al., 2005), which is densely innervated by serotonergic neurons and characterized by a high serotonin synthesis rate (Brown et al., 1979; Gallinat et al., 2000). To date, examination of the N1 response using the auditory oddball paradigm in MDD yielded inconsistent findings. Several studies have reported that MDD patients elicit smaller or slower than typical N1 responses (Burkhart and Thomas, 1993; Urretavizcaya et al., 2003; Kemp et al., 2009, 2010), while others found no significant group differences (Sara et al., 1994; Greimel et al., 2015). Delayed latencies of the N1 or P300 also did not correlate with the behavioral ratings of depression symptoms (Urretavizcaya et al., 2003).

This variability in the N1 and P300 findings could be due to the small sample sizes and discrepant consideration of individual differences in the MDD group. In a large sample of MDD patients (n = 1,008), van Dinteren et al. (2015) observed smaller than typical P300 amplitudes to targets. Depression severity (Kemp et al., 2009; Jandl et al., 2010), years since diagnosis, and the duration of medication use (Houston et al., 2004), as well as age, have been identified as potentially important factors contributing to the conflicting ERP findings (Greimel et al., 2015). For example, only females with MDD younger than 46 years of age had slower P300 latencies compared to female healthy controls of the same age, while smaller than typical N1 amplitudes were observed only in young vs. older patients with MDD and in male non-responders vs. responders to venlafaxine XR (van Dinteren et al., 2015). Thus, it remains unclear whether the P300 or N1 responses reflect more general deficits associated with MDD (e.g., reduced motivation to engage with a task, Kähkönen et al., 2007) or are related to specific demographic or clinical features.

In the current study, we re-analyzed the large sample of well-characterized adults with MDD and age- and sex-matched healthy controls originally used by van Dinteren et al. (2015) to systematically examine whether the N1 and P300 responses to both target and standard stimuli in an auditory oddball task differentiate participants with MDD from healthy controls and whether they are sensitive to individual differences in age, biological sex, depression symptoms and severity, and factors related to daily functioning (anxiety, negativity bias, etc.).

Materials and Methods

Participants

The international multi-center, randomized, prospective open-label trial dataset (International Study to Predict Optimized Treatment Response in Depression, iSPOT-D; Williams et al., 2011) was previously described in van Dinteren et al. (2015) and included adults 18–65 years of age: 954 MDD patients and 328 healthy controls matched on age and sex. Data from the baseline (pre-treatment) study visits are included in the current analysis. Usable ERP data were available for 794 MDD patients and 307 healthy controls. A summary of the participant characteristics is presented in Table 1. The participants were not taking medication at the time of data collection.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of the iSPOT-D study participants used in the current analysis.

Clinical assessments

All participants completed the 17-item Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression (HRSD; Hamilton, 1986), a clinician-administered self-report assessment of symptoms of depression experienced over the past week. A score of 0–7 is considered to be within the normal range (or in clinical remission), while a score of 20 or higher indicates at least moderate severity of depressive symptoms.

The Social and Occupational Functioning Assessment Scale (SOFAS; American Psychiatric Association, 1994) is a clinician-completed global rating measure of current functioning ranging from 0 to 100, with lower scores representing lower functioning (Goldman et al., 1992).

A self-report on the Quick Inventory of Depressive Symptomatology (QIDS-SR16; Rush et al., 2003) documented depressive symptom severity with the total score ranging from 0 to 27, where scores of 5 or lower indicate no depression, scores from 6 to 10 correspond to mild depression, 11–15—moderate depression, 16–20—severe depression, and 21–270—very severe depression.

The Depression Anxiety and Stress Scales (DASS; Lovibond and Lovibond, 1995) included 42 self-report items measuring depression, anxiety, and stress, with higher scores indicating greater severity of symptoms.

Participants also completed the Brain Resource Inventory of Social Cognition (BRISC; Gordon et al., 2008) for assessment of self-regulation processes that included 45 self-report items related to Negativity Bias, Emotional Resilience, and Social Skills. Lower scores indicate less efficient functioning.

ERP procedures

Detailed data acquisition and processing procedures are described in van Dinteren et al. (2015). Briefly, the auditory event-related potentials (ERPs) were recorded using an oddball paradigm with a 500 Hz tone as the standard condition (280 trials) and a 1,000 Hz tone as the target (60 trials). All tones were 50 ms long (5 ms rise/fall time), presented binaurally in random order at 75 dB sound pressure level, with 1,000 ms interstimulus interval. Two targets could not appear consecutively. Participants pressed two buttons simultaneously with the index fingers of each hand in response to the target tones. Speed and accuracy of response were both equally emphasized, and a brief practice session familiarized the participants with the stimuli and the task.

ERPs were acquired using 26 Ag/AgCl electrodes (Quikcap with the extended International 10–20 electrode placement system: Fp1, Fp2, F7, F3, Fz, F4, F8, FC3, FCz, FC4, T3, C3, Cz, C4, T4, CP3, CPz, CP4, T5, P3, Pz, P4, T6, O1, Oz, O2) and Neuroscan NuAmps DC amplifier with 500 Hz sampling rate. Additional electrodes placed at the outer canthus of each eye as well as above and below the left eye monitored horizontal and vertical eye movements with a ground at AFz. Impedances were kept at or below 5 kOhms. Data preprocessing was completed by the original iSPOT-D study team using BrainVision Analyzer 2 and custom automated pipelines validated against manual review (for specific details see van Dinteren et al., 2015; Arns et al., 2016). Briefly, data were re-referenced offline to averaged mastoids, low-pass filtered at 35 Hz with a Tukey (tapered cosine) window, and baseline corrected using the 300-ms prestimulus interval. Ocular artifacts were corrected using a regression-based technique similar to that by Gratton et al. (1983).

Statistical analyses

Statistical analysis included only the correct response trials. In the original report aiming to identify neural predictors of pharmacological treatment outcomes in MDD (van Dinteren et al., 2015), the target stimulus waveforms at frontal (Fz) and parietal (Pz) locations were analyzed relative to a 300 ms pre-stimulus baseline. In the current analysis, peak amplitude and latency measures for the N1 (70–120 ms) and P300 (300–500 ms) were examined for both stimulus conditions (standard and target tones) for a more detailed evaluation of auditory processing and to assess stimulus discrimination effects in MDD. In line with the published guidelines for clinical research using ERPs (Duncan et al., 2009), amplitude and latency measures were obtained at frontal, central, and parietal midline electrodes (Fz, Cz, Pz) to provide a more detailed evaluation of the topographic distribution of the N1 (fronto-central maximum, Näätänen and Picton, 1987) and P300 (parietal maximum for the goal-directed attention, frontal maximum for the involuntary orienting; Polich, 2007) as well as to account for possible differences in neural processing between the clinical groups. The time window for the P300 response was adjusted from the wide interval (220–550 ms) in van Dinteren et al. (2015) to the more traditional 300–500 ms window to minimize the potential overlap with the preceding auditory P200 response, which may also be affected in MDD patients (e.g., Vandoolaeghe et al., 1998; Chen et al., 2002).

Examination of the age distribution in the study sample revealed a non-normal distribution, with greater representation of the younger age range (18–30 years), and a median of 46 years. Therefore, for the purpose of analyses, the sample was divided into three age subgroups: 18–30 years, 31–45 years, and 46–65 years. These subgroups represent commonly used divisions in the field as well as reflect known age-related differences. Previously, age 30 has been reported as the end point of neural development, followed by a plateau and then degeneration (van Dinteren et al., 2014), and aging-related alterations in neural activity can be seen after age 45 years (Handy, 2005).

The statistical analyses focused on Group differences (MDD vs. healthy controls) in the auditory ERP characteristics using repeated measures ANOVA with Stimulus (2: standard, target) × Electrode (3: Fz, Cz, Pz) within-subject factors and Group (2: Control, MDD) × Sex (2: female, male) × Age subgroup (3: 18–30, 31–45, 46–65 years) as the between-subject factors. Huynh-Feldt correction was used for sphericity violations. Only main effects and interactions involving Group or Stimulus factors were considered for interpretation. Brain-behavior associations were examined in the combined sample representing the full continuum of depressive symptoms using correlations between the ERP measures sensitive to group differences and the clinical data related to depression presence and severity.

Results

The averaged ERP waveform for each group at the three analyzed scalp locations is presented in Figure 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Grand-average auditory ERPs for standard (blue) and target (red) tones recorded during the baseline visit in healthy controls (left column) and MDD patients (right column) at frontal, central, and parietal midline scalp locations. Standard errors are plotted as thin lines around each waveform. MDD, major depressive disorder; ERPs, event-related potentials.

N1 amplitude

There were main effects of Group, F(1,1089) = 18.236, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.016; Sex, F(1,1089) = 8.04, p = 0.005, ηp2 = 0.007; and Stimulus, F(1,1089) = 284.458, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.207. Significant interactions included Stimulus × Group, F(1,1089) = 11.411, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.001; Stimulus × Electrode, F(2,2178) = 54.303, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.047; and Stimulus × Electrode × Group × Age, F(4,2178) = 3.742, p = 0.005, ηp2 = 0.007.

Further analyses of the interactions indicated that both study groups differentiated between the standard and target stimuli, with larger amplitudes for the latter (p < 0.001; controls d = 0.780, MDD d = 0.448). However, the MDD group generated smaller (less negative) N1 responses than the healthy controls for both standard, F(1,1203) = 4.698, p = 0.030, and target stimuli, F(1,1116) = 368.581, p < 0.001. The overall magnitude of stimulus discrimination quantified as the amplitude difference between the target and standard stimuli was also significantly smaller in the MDD group than in the healthy controls, F(1,1100) = 14.674, p < 0.001 (M = 1.2 μV vs. 1.9 μV).

Consideration of the age-related effects noted that within each age subgroup, all participants differentiated between the standard and target tones (p < 0.001). In the 18–30-year-olds, clinical group differences were observed in response to target but not to standard tones with larger amplitudes in the healthy controls than the MDD group at all three analyzed scalp locations: Fz: F(1,461) = 7.950, p = 0.005; Cz: F(1,486) = 17.132, p < 0.001; Pz: F(1,492) = 12.257, p < 0.001. A similar pattern was observed in the 31–45-year-olds, Fz: F(1,314) = 11.068, p < 0.001; Cz: F(1,334) = 6.311, p = 0.012; Pz: F(1,339) = 4.059, p = 0.021. In the oldest subgroup, 46–65 years, clinical group differences included the N1 amplitude to targets, Fz: F(1,354) = 4.354, p = 0.038; Cz: F(1,364) = 4.465, p = 0.035; Pz: F(1,366) = 8.902, p = 0.003, as well as the N1 amplitude to standard tones at Pz: F(1,370) = 5.997, p = 0.015, all of which were lower in the patients with MDD compared to the healthy controls. Overall, the healthy controls showed no age-related differences in the N1 amplitude, while in the MDD group, participants in the oldest subgroup elicited the smallest frontal N1 responses to standard tones compared to the other two subgroups (p < 0.001).

N1 latency

There were no significant Group, Age, or Sex main effects for the N1 latency. However, the main effect of Stimulus, F(1,1089) = 8.809, p = 0.003, ηp2 = 0.008, and the interactions of Stimulus × Electrode, F(2,2198) = 7.625, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.007, and Stimulus × Sex, F(1,1089) = 4.176, p = 0.041, ηp2 = 0.004, were significant. Follow-up tests noted that all participants elicited faster N1 responses for target than standard stimuli at Cz, t(1,169) = 3.014, p = 0.003, d = 0.088; and Pz, t(1,187) = 2.423, p = 0.016, d = 0.070. Stimulus differences in the timing of the N1 response did not reach significance at Fz.

Sex differences were present in the form of slightly faster N1 latencies in response to the standard tones in females vs. males, F(1,1202) = 5.672, p = 0.017 (107.66 vs. 109.56 ms). No sex differences were observed in response to the target tones. Furthermore, only males demonstrated faster N1 latencies to targets than standards, t(485) = 3.041, p = 0.002, d = 0.138 (107.74 vs. 109.56 ms).

P300 amplitude

There were main effects of Group, F(1,1089) = 7.529, p = 0.006, ηp2 = 0.007; Sex, F(1,1089) = 19.459, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.018; and Stimulus, F(1,1089) = 942.029, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.464, as well as Stimulus × Group, F(1,1089) = 9.139, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.028; Stimulus × Sex, F(1,1089) = 31.468, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.028; Stimulus × Age Groups × Sex, F(2,1089) = 3.284, p = 0.038, ηp2 = 0.006; Stimulus × Electrode, F(2,2178) = 779.650, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.417; Stimulus × Electrode × Group, F(2,2178) = 3.643, p = 0.026, ηp2 = 0.003; Stimulus × Electrode × Sex, F(2,2178) = 3.445, p = 0.032, ηp2 = 0.003; Stimulus × Electrode × Age interactions; F(4,2178) = 50.304, p = 0.003, ηp2 = 0.085; and Stimulus × Electrode × Group × Age interactions, F(4,2178) = 2.519, p = 0.039, ηp2 = 0.005.

Further testing of the Stimulus × Electrode × Sex interaction revealed that both males and females elicited larger P300 amplitudes to targets than standards at each of the three analyzed locations (p < 0.001). Larger P300 amplitudes in response to targets were recorded in males vs. females at the Pz, F(1,1199) = 29.614, p < 0.001; Cz, F(1,1186) = 29.397, p < 0.001; and Fz, F(1,1131) = 12.461, p < 0.001. Females also elicited smaller P300 amplitudes than males in response to standards at Cz, F(1,1204) = 12.053, p < 0.001.

Analysis of the Stimulus × Electrode × Group × Age interaction identified no age-related differences in the P300 response of the healthy controls. In the MDD group, age differences were observed for the standard tone responses at the Fz site, F(2,903) = 7.294, p < 0.001, where the oldest subgroup (46–65 years) elicited a larger response compared to the younger subgroups (vs. 18–30: F(1,639) = 11.549, p < 0.001, vs. 31–45: F(1,539) = 6.950, p = 0.009).

Clinical group differences were observed among 18–30-year-olds in response to targets at the Pz, F(1,492) = 21.287, p < 0.001, with larger amplitudes in healthy controls than in the MDD group. A similar pattern was evident in the oldest subgroup, 46–65 years, F(1,366) = 4.235, p = 0.040. No clinical group differences reached significance in the middle-age subgroup, 31–45 years. Further exploratory analyses revealed that brain responses to targets of patients in the youngest MDD subgroup were not statistically different from those of healthy controls age 31 years or older.

P300 latency

There were main effects of Group, F(1,1089) = 15.174, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.014; Age, F(2,1089) = 13.310, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.024; and Stimulus, F(1,1089) = 30.445, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.027, as well as interactions of Stimulus × Age, F(2,1089) = 36.538, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.063; Stimulus × Sex, F(1,1089) = 8.397, p = 0.004, ηp2 = 0.008; Stimulus × Electrode, F(2,2178) = 89.322, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.076; Stimulus × Electrode × Age, F(4,2178) = 8.774, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.016; and Stimulus × Electrode × Group × Age, F(4,2178) = 3.045, p = 0.016, ηp2 = 0.006. Follow-up analyses noted sex differences in response to targets only, F(1,1116) = 15.162, p < 0.001, with faster latencies in females than males (356.72 vs. 366.77 ms).

The MDD group produced P300 responses with longer latencies than the healthy controls in response to both standard and target stimuli. The interaction effect in the combined sample was due to faster P300 responses to target vs. standard stimuli at Cz, t(1169) = 6.532, p < 0.001, d = 0.191; and Fz, t(1120) = 14.310, p < 0.001, d = 0.427. No significant stimulus differences in the timing of the P300 response were observed at Pz (p = 0.385).

Age-related differences were observed in the healthy control group for the responses to targets at Pz, F(2,307) = 4.382, p = 0.013, due to the longer latency in the oldest (46–65 years) subgroup compared to the younger ages, and for the standard stimuli at Fz, F(2,309) = 30.049, p < 0.001, and Cz, F(2,309) = 10.737, p < 0.001, due to the shorter latencies in the oldest subgroup. In the MDD group, age-related differences were present for all stimuli and electrode locations, with the increasing age resulting in progressive shortening of the latency to standards and an increase in latency to targets.

Brain-behavior associations with clinical measures

The large sample size in the current study afforded high statistical power to detect multiple significant correlations among the ERP measures sensitive to the clinical group differences (N1 amplitude, P300 amplitude and latency) and the behavioral measures of depression symptoms. However, most of the correlations were exceedingly small, with values less than 0.1, raising questions about their practical significance. Therefore, we considered only the results with at least a small effect size (r = 0.15 or greater).

N1 response

Brain-behavior correlations for the N1 response at the Cz location are presented in Table 2. The N1 is commonly observed to be maximal at the Cz and is thought to index the automatic processing of stimulus physical characteristics and contribute to preattentive change detection (Key et al., 2005). Reduced N1 amplitudes to target tones at Cz in the 18–30-year-olds were associated with higher depressive symptoms as measured by the HDRS, SOFAS, QIDS, DASS Depression subscale, and BRISC Negativity Bias scale. In the 31–45-year-olds, reduced N1 amplitudes to targets at Cz were related to HDRS and SOFAS scores only. A different pattern was observed in the oldest subgroup, where reduced central N1 responses to standard tones were related to higher DASS scores on anxiety and stress subscales. In the combined sample, only the association between the smaller central N1 responses to targets and lower SOFAS scores reached the small effect size threshold.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Correlations between the N1 amplitude at the Cz location in response to standard and target sounds and behavioral measures.

P300 response

For the P300 responses indexing involuntary (frontal) and goal-directed (centro-parietal) attention, brain-behavior correlations are presented in Table 3 (amplitude) and Table 4 (latency). The larger parietal P300 amplitudes to target tones were related to lower depression symptoms (HDRS, QIDS, SOFAS, DASS Depression, BRISC Negativity Bias) and lower DASS anxiety and stress levels in the youngest subgroup. No consistent associations were observed with the P300 latency measures for that age range.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Correlations between the P300 amplitude at the Fz, Cz, and Pz locations in response to standard and target sounds and behavioral measures.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Correlations between the P300 latency at the Fz, Cz, and Pz locations in response to standard and target sounds and behavioral measures.

In the middle-age subgroup, no correlations with the P300 amplitude reached the small effect threshold, while longer frontal and central P300 latency to standard tones were associated with higher DASS stress scores.

In the oldest subgroup, larger frontal and parietal P300 amplitude to targets were related to lower DASS anxiety scores. Furthermore, consistent with the pattern observed for the N1 amplitude, responses to standard tones were more informative about the depressive symptoms in the oldest subgroup: longer latency of the frontal P300 was related to higher scores on HDRS, QIDS, DASS Depression and DASS Anxiety, as well as lower scores on BRISC Negativity Bias and Social Skills.

In the combined sample, only the associations between the higher parietal P300 amplitude to targets and lower depressive symptoms (HDRS, SOFAS, QIDS) reached the small effect size threshold.

Discussion

The primary goal of this reanalysis of an existing large data set was to systematically examine whether combinations of neural biomarkers, demographic characteristics, and clinical features could be used to define subtypes of MDD patients to inform sample selection and stratification strategies for MDD drug development. A key objective was to determine whether the N1 and P300 components of ERP responses are sensitive to neural pathophysiology in MDD and whether they may serve as informative neural biomarkers of MDD. The findings from the classic auditory oddball task indicated that all participants, regardless of age, clinical group membership, or depression symptom severity, could discriminate between the two tones varying in frequency as reflected by the larger N1 and P300 amplitudes in response to the target compared to the standard stimuli. However, compared to the healthy controls, the patients with MDD generated smaller N1 and P300 response amplitudes and longer P300 latencies for both stimulus conditions, suggesting alterations in sensory and goal-directed attentional processes. Age was a significant modulator of these neural differences between patients with MDD and healthy controls.

Analysis of the N1 responses reflecting early sensory auditory processing (encoding of acoustic features and preattentive change detection: Martin et al., 1999; Näätänen et al., 2011) noted smaller than typical N1 amplitudes to targets in patients with MDD at all three analyzed scalp locations. The magnitude of stimulus discrimination (target-standard amplitude difference) was also reduced in the MDD group compared to the healthy controls, suggesting lower than typical sensitivity to acoustic frequency differences and/or reduced top-down allocation of neural processing resources to the sensory stimuli. Larger N1 amplitudes have been previously reported in studies requiring active attention to the stimuli compared to passive exposure (Hillyard et al., 1973; Näätänen et al., 1978; Coch et al., 2005). Conversely, no significant clinical group differences were observed for the speed of early auditory processing, as indexed by the N1 latency.

Brain-behavior correlations allowed us to investigate the functional significance of the N1 amplitude reduction in the MDD group. The results revealed consistent associations between the reduction in the central N1 amplitudes to target tones and the increased severity of depression symptoms, as measured by clinician observations and by self-report in the 18–30-year-old subgroup. In the 31–45-year-olds, such associations were observed only with clinician ratings. Of note, in the oldest participants, the central N1 response to targets did not relate to any clinical symptoms of MDD. Instead, reduced N1 amplitudes to standard stimuli at central sites, indicating more general tuning out of sensory inputs, were related to increased anxiety and stress, as measured by the DASS. Previously, increased state and trait anxiety had been associated with reduced sensory registration in non-MDD populations (Engel-Yeger and Dunn, 2011). Similarly, increased auditory sensory difficulties have been related to greater depression and anxiety symptoms in older adults (Simning et al., 2019).

In combination, these results replicate prior findings of N1 amplitude reductions in MDD (e.g., Burkhart and Thomas, 1993) and suggest that patients of all ages with increased depression symptoms may exhibit reduced bottom-up sensory processing and/or less effective top-down attention allocation needed to detect unexpected changes in the auditory environment. In addition, age appears to be an important contributor to the MDD phenotype and should be considered in the context of treatment planning.

Analysis of the P300 responses, indexing goal-directed (central, parietal) and involuntary (frontal) attention allocation to the auditory target detection task revealed the expected larger amplitudes to the infrequent targets compared to the standard tones in all participants. Replicating prior findings of the reduced P300 amplitudes in patients with MDD (see Mumtaz et al., 2015 for review), the MDD group elicited smaller than typical responses to targets, reflecting reduced attention allocation to task performance. No group differences were observed for the standard stimuli.

Furthermore, clinical group differences were most clearly observed for the youngest (18–30 years) and oldest (46–65 years) subgroups. Brain-behavior associations noted that reduced parietal P300 amplitudes in response to target tones in the youngest age subgroup were related to increased symptoms of depression (both clinician-observed and self-reported) as well as to higher anxiety and stress scores, consistent with prior reports of high comorbidity of anxiety and depression (e.g., Lamers et al., 2011). In the oldest age subgroup, a similar association was observed between the self-reported anxiety and P300 amplitudes to targets at parietal and frontal locations. The reduction in the P300 amplitude was recently reported to be predictive of future depression severity in patients with MDD (Santopetro et al., 2021). Previously, smaller P300 responses were observed in patients with depression and anxiety compared to patients with anxiety alone (Bruder et al., 2002; Enoch et al., 2008); thus, the current results are likely driven by the depression features.

In addition, exploratory follow-up analyses noted that the parietal P300 response amplitudes to targets in the youngest MDD patients (18–30-year-olds) were not significantly different from those in the older healthy controls. These results extend previous reports of associations between accelerated cognitive decline in aging and depression symptoms (Panza et al., 2009; Oi, 2017; Sacchet et al., 2017), and similar to the N1 findings, highlight the importance of considering the participants’ age.

Analyses of the P300 response latencies noted the expected faster responses to targets than standard tones in all participants, as well as prolonged latencies in the MDD group compared to the healthy controls for both stimulus types. The latter is consistent with the generalized slowing in information processing and reduced sustained attention performance (e.g., Nebes et al., 2000; van der Meere et al., 2007), which have been reported to explain some of the key symptoms in MDD (Piani et al., 2022). In the healthy controls, age-related differences were observed only for the target P300 latency with longer values in the oldest compared to the younger two subgroups. In the MDD group, the latency for the rare targets progressively increased across the three age subgroups while the latency for frequent standard stimuli decreased with increasing age.

A novel contribution of this study is the identification of the anterior P300 response latency, which indexes the speed of involuntary attentional responses, as a candidate biomarker sensitive to MDD symptom severity in middle- and older-age adults. Brain-behavior associations indicated that the timing of the frontal P300 response to standard stimuli was related to the severity of depression symptoms only in the oldest subgroup (46–65 years), suggesting that increased depression was associated with less efficient processing of the frequent stimuli. It also correlated with stronger negativity bias and lower social skills. In the 31–45-year-olds, prolonged frontal and central P300 latencies to standard tones were related to increased scores on the stress subscale, while no systematic relationships with the behavioral measures were observed for the youngest subgroup. The anterior scalp distribution of the P300 latencies involved in these brain-behavior associations suggests that MDD symptoms may interfere with the speed of involuntary attention allocation to regularly presented stimuli, possibly indicating slower memory trace formation for the standard tones, resulting in less efficient stimulus categorization. These observations are consistent with prior reports of a compensatory frontal shift in the P300 topography in older adults (van Dinteren et al., 2018) and serve as additional evidence of potentially accelerated cognitive aging in MDD (see also Oi, 2017; Sacchet et al., 2017; Lorenzo et al., 2022).

Similar auditory ERP alterations, especially for the P300 response, have been previously observed in other clinical groups, such as schizophrenia and dementia (e.g., Hedges et al., 2016; Hamilton et al., 2019). Of note, those populations also have high rates (up to 50%) of comorbid depression diagnoses or preclinical depression symptoms that are often present early in the disease progression (e.g., Häfner et al., 2005; Cipriani et al., 2015). In the current study, the participants with MDD as well as the healthy controls were free of schizophrenia, bipolar, obsessive-compulsive, or posttraumatic stress disorders, and substance abuse (see Williams et al., 2011). Furthermore, the two groups were matched on age, sex, and education. Therefore, we attribute the observed group differences in the auditory ERPs to the presence of MDD.

Our results extended the original report by van Dinteren et al. (2015) that identified different alterations of neural responses in the chronologically younger vs. older subgroups of patients with MDD compared to the healthy controls. These findings are also consistent with the observation that symptoms of depression vary with age (Brown et al., 2016). Time since birth is the most readily available and therefore commonly used objective measure of age. However, prior studies (e.g., Han et al., 2018), as well as our results, suggest that patients with MDD may be biologically older than their chronological age, and the latest reports note a significant overlap between the biological alterations in MDD and those commonly observed in aging (Lorenzo et al., 2022). The lack of an objectively quantified biological age is a limitation of the current analysis. Thus, future studies should consider including an estimate of biological age based on the clinical tests of multiple physiological systems (e.g., immune, metabolic, cardiovascular, renal; Belsky et al., 2015; Jazwinski and Kim, 2019). Although correlated, biological and chronological age measures would contribute distinct information. Indeed, in older adults, biological age had been a stronger predictor of depressive symptoms than chronological age (Brown et al., 2018). Identifying the unique contributions of biological and chronological age could facilitate treatment planning.

Despite observing several significant differences in the auditory ERP responses between the healthy controls and participants with MDD, as well as the numerous brain-behavior associations, it is important to note that the observed effect sizes are small. In other words, although our large, well-characterized sample provided sufficient statistical power to detect these differences, similar findings may not be observable in smaller samples. Furthermore, the small effect sizes suggest that the reported clinical group differences in neural activity recorded during the performance of an active auditory oddball task, while informative, may not be sufficient on their own to fully characterize individual differences related to MDD. Considering additional subject characteristics such as age (e.g., chronological, biological, etc.) can increase the utility of neural data.

In conclusion, the large iSPOT-D sample allowed us sufficient sensitivity to conclude that auditory N1 and P300 responses in an oddball task reflect patterns of neural functioning that differ between individuals with MDD and healthy controls. In addition, age emerged as a critical contributor to the observed results, as reflected by a shift from clinical group differences specific to target- or change-related processing (observed in all age groups) to more broad differences involving both target and standard stimuli (observed mainly in the older participants). The patterns of brain-behavior correlations confirmed functional associations between the depression symptoms and the N1 and P300 response characteristics, as well as noted the increasing contributions of anxiety and stress measures in the older subgroups. Future studies may explore the role of DASS Anxiety and Stress scale scores as mediators between the altered sensory and attentional neural processes and MDD severity (Wigham et al., 2015; Engel-Yeger et al., 2016). Overall, these findings support the use of auditory N1 and P300 responses along with age as an effective means of stratifying the clinical population with MDD, with the goal of reducing heterogeneity and therefore facilitating a more efficient design of future treatment studies.

Data Availability Statement

The data analyzed in this study is subject to the following licenses/restrictions: the data analyzed in this study were obtained from Total Brain Ltd. Requests to access these datasets should be directed to Evian Gordon, evian.gordon@totalbrain.com.

Author Contributions

AK designed the analysis approach, performed statistical tests and functional interpretation of the results, and contributed to writing this report. TT-W and DS conceived the study and contributed to interpretation of the results and writing this report. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

Brain Resource, Ltd. was the sponsor for the original iSPOT-D study. The sponsor was not involved in the current data analysis, interpretation of data, the writing of this article or the decision to submit it for publication.

Conflict of Interest

TT-W and DS are employed by Alkermes. AK received consulting fees from Alkermes for contributions to the current study.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnhum.2022.1055685/full#supplementary-material.

References

American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic And Statistical Manual Of Mental Disorders, 4th edition. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

Google Scholar

Ancy, J., Gangadhar, B. N., and Janakiramaiah, N. (1996). ‘Normal’ P300 amplitude predicts rapid response to ECT in melancholia. J. Affect. Disord. 41, 211–215. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0327(96)00090-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arns, M., Bruder, G., Hegerl, U., Spooner, C., Palmer, D. M., Etkin, A., et al. (2016). EEG alpha asymmetry as a gender-specific predictor of outcome to acute treatment with different antidepressant medications in the randomized iSPOT-D study. Clin. Neurophysiol. 127, 509–519. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2015.05.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belsky, D. W., Caspi, A., Houts, R., Cohen, H. J., Corcoran, D. L., Danese, A., et al. (2015). Quantification of biological aging in young adults. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 112, E4104–E4110. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1506264112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, R. M., Crane, A. M., and Goldman, P. S. (1979). Regional distribution of monoamines in the cerebral cortex and subcortical structures of the rhesus monkey: concentrations and in vivo synthesis rates. Brain Res. 168, 133–150. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(79)90132-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, P. J., Rutherford, B. R., Yaffe, K., Tandler, J. M., Ray, J. L., Pott, E., et al. (2016). The depressed frail phenotype: the clinical manifestation of increased biological aging. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 24, 1084–1094. doi: 10.1016/j.jagp.2016.06.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, P. J., Wall, M. M., Chen, C., Levine, M. E., Yaffe, K., Roose, S. P., et al. (2018). Biological age, not chronological age, is associated with late-life depression. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 73, 1370–1376. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glx162

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bruder, G. E., Kayser, J., and Tenke, C. E. (2012). “Event-related brain potentials in depression: clinical, cognitive and neurophysiological implications,” in The Oxford Handbook of Event-Related Potential Components, (Vol. 168) eds S. J. Luck, and E. S. Kappenman (Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press), 563–592).

Google Scholar

Bruder, G. E., Kayser, J., Tenke, C. E., Leite, P., Schneier, F. R., Stewart, J. W., et al. (2002). Cognitive ERPs in depressive and anxiety disorders during tonal and phonetic oddball tasks. Clin. Electroencephalogr. 33, 119–124. doi: 10.1177/155005940203300308

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bruder, G. E., Kroppmann, C. J., Kayser, J., Stewart, J. W., McGrath, P. J., and Tenke, C. E. (2009). Reduced brain responses to novel sounds in depression: P3 findings in a novelty oddball task. Psychiatry Res. 170, 218–223. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2008.10.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bruder, G. E., Tenke, C. E., Stewart, J. W., Towey, J. P., Leite, P., Voglmaier, M., et al. (1995). Brain event-related potentials to complex tones in depressed patients: relations to perceptual asymmetry and clinical features. Psychophysiology 32, 373–381. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1995.tb01220.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bruder, G. E., Tenke, C. E., Towey, J. P., Leite, P., Fong, R., Stewart, J. E., et al. (1998). Brain ERPs of depressed patients to complex tones in an oddball task: relation of reduced P3 asymmetry to physical anhedonia. Psychophysiology 35, 54–63. doi: 10.1017/S0048577298961157

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bruder, G. E., Towey, J. P., Stewart, J. W., Friedman, D., Tenke, C., and Quitkin, F. M. (1991). Event-related potentials in depression: influence of task, stimulus hemifield and clinical features on P3 latency. Biol. Psychiatry 30, 233–246. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(91)90108-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buch, A. M., and Liston, C. (2021). Dissecting diagnostic heterogeneity in depression by integrating neuroimaging and genetics. Neuropsychopharmacology 46, 156–175. doi: 10.1038/s41386-020-00789-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burkhart, M. A., and Thomas, D. G. (1993). Event-related potential measures of attention in moderately depressed subjects. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 88, 42–50. doi: 10.1016/0168-5597(93)90027-m

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, T.-J., Yu, Y. W.-Y., Chen, M.-C., Tsai, S.-J., and Hong, C.-J. (2002). Association analysis for serotonin transporter promoter polymorphism and auditory evoked potentials for major depression. Neuropsychobiology 46, 57–60. doi: 10.1159/000065412

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cipriani, G., Lucetti, C., Carlesi, C., Danti, S., and Nuti, A. (2015). Depression and dementia. A review. Eur. Geriatr. Med. 6, 479–486. doi: 10.1016/j.eurger.2015.07.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coch, D., Sanders, L. D., and Neville, H. J. (2005). An event-related potential study of selective auditory attention in children and adults. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 17, 605–622. doi: 10.1162/0898929053467631

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duncan, C. C., Barry, R. J., Connolly, J. F., Fischer, C., Michie, P. T., Näätänen, R., et al. (2009). Event-related potentials in clinical research: guidelines for eliciting, recording and quantifying mismatch negativity, P300 and N400. Clin. Neurophysiol. 120, 1883–1908. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2009.07.045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Engel-Yeger, B., and Dunn, W. (2011). The relationship between sensory processing difficulties and anxiety level of healthy adults. Br. J. Occupation. Ther. 74, 210–216. doi: 10.4276/030802211X13046730116407

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Engel-Yeger, B., Muzio, C., Rinosi, G., Solano, P., Geoffroy, P. A., Pompili, M., et al. (2016). Extreme sensory processing patterns and their relation with clinical conditions among individuals with major affective disorders. Psychiatry Res. 236, 112–118. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2015.12.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Enoch, M.-A., White, K. V., Waheed, J., and Goldman, D. (2008). Neurophysiological and genetic distinctions between pure and comorbid anxiety disorders. Depress. Anxiety 25, 383–392. doi: 10.1002/da.20378

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fried, E. I. (2015). Problematic assumptions have slowed down depression research: why symptoms, not syndromes are the way forward. Front. Psychol. 6:309. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00309

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gallinat, J., Bottlender, R., Juckel, G., Munke-Puchner, A., Stotz, G., Kuss, H. J., et al. (2000). The loudness dependency of the auditory evoked N1/P2-component as a predictor of the acute SSRI response in depression. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 148, 404–411. doi: 10.1007/s002130050070

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gangadhar, B. N., Ancy, J., Janakiranaiah, N., and Umapathy, C. (1993). P300 amplitude in non-bipolar, melancholic depression. J. Affect. Disord. 28, 57–60. doi: 10.1016/0165-0327(93)90077-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giedke, H., Thier, P., and Bolz, J. (1981). The relationship between P3-latency and reaction time in depression. Biol. Psychol. 13, 31–49. doi: 10.1016/0301-0511(81)90026-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldman, H. H., Skodol, A. E., and Lave, T. R. (1992). Revising axis V for DSM-IV: a review of measures of social functioning. Am. J. Psychiatry 149, 1148–1156. doi: 10.1176/ajp.149.9.1148

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gordon, E., Barnett, K. J., Cooper, N., Tran, N., and Williams, L. M. (2008). An “integrative neuroscience” platform: application to profiles of negativity and positivity. J. Integr. Neurosci. 7, 345–366. doi: 10.1142/S0219635208001927

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gratton, G., Coles, M. G., and Donchin, E. (1983). A new method for off-line removal of ocular artifact. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 55, 468–484. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(83)90135-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greimel, E., Trinkl, M., Bartling, J., Bakos, S., Grossheinrich, N., and Schulte-Körne, G. (2015). Auditory selective attention in adolescents with major depression: an event-related potential study. J. Affect. Disord. 172, 445–452. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2014.10.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Häfner, H., Maurer, K., Trendler, G., an der Heiden, W., Schmidt, M., and Könnecke, R. (2005). Schizophrenia and depression: challenging the paradigm of two separate diseases—a controlled study of schizophrenia, depression and healthy controls. Schizophr. Res. 77, 11–24. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2005.01.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamilton, H. K., Roach, B. J., Bachman, P. M., Belger, A., Carrion, R. E., Duncan, E., et al. (2019). Association between P300 responses to auditory oddball stimuli and clinical outcomes in the psychosis risk syndrome. JAMA Psychiatry 76, 1187–1197. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2019.2135

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamilton, M. (1986). “The Hamilton rating scale for depression,” in Assessment of Depression, eds Norman Sartorius and Thomas A. Ban (Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer), 76, 143–152.

Google Scholar

Han, L. K., Aghajani, M., Clark, S. L., Chan, R. F., Hattab, M. W., Shabalin, A. A., et al. (2018). Epigenetic aging in major depressive disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 175, 774–782. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2018.17060595

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Handy, T. C. (2005). Event-Related Potentials: A Methods Handbook. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Google Scholar

Hansenne, M., Pitchot, W., Moreno, A. G., Zaldua, I. U., and Ansseau, M. (1996). Suicidal behavior in depressive disorder: an event-related potential study. Biol. Psychiatry 40, 116–122. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(95)00372-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hasler, G., Drevets, W. C., Manji, H. K., and Charney, D. S. (2004). Discovering endophenotypes for major depression. Neuropsychopharmacology 29, 1765–1781. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300506

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hedges, D., Janis, R., Mickelson, S., Keith, C., Bennett, D., and Brown, B. L. (2016). P300 amplitude in Alzheimer’s disease: a meta-analysis and meta-regression. Clin. EEG Neurosci. 47, 48–55. doi: 10.1177/1550059414550567

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hillyard, S. A., Hink, R. F., Schwent, V. L., and Picton, T. W. (1973). Electrical signs of selective attention in the human brain. Science 182, 177–180. doi: 10.1126/science.182.4108.177

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Houston, R. J., Bauer, L. O., and Hesselbrock, V. M. (2004). P300 evidence of cognitive inflexibility in female adolescents at risk for recurrent depression. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 28, 529–536. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2004.01.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jandl, M., Steyer, J., and Kaschka, W. P. (2010). Suicide risk markers in major depressive disorder: a study of electrodermal activity and event-related potentials. J. Affect. Disord. 123, 138–149. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2009.09.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jazwinski, S. M., and Kim, S. (2019). Examination of the dimensions of biological age. Front. Genet. 10:263. doi: 10.3389/fgene.2019.00263

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Joffe, R., Sokolov, S., and Streiner, D. (1996). Antidepressant treatment of depression: a metaanalysis. Can. J. Psychiatry 41, 613–616. doi: 10.1177/070674379604101002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kähkönen, S., Yamashita, H., Rytsälä, H., Suominen, K., Ahveninen, J., and Isometsä, E. (2007). Dysfunction in early auditory processing in major depressive disorder revealed by combined MEG and EEG. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 32, 316–322. doi: 10.1016/j.genhosppsych.2022.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kangas, E. S., Vuoriainen, E., Lindeman, S., and Astikainen, P. (2022). Auditory event-related potentials in separating patients with depressive disorders and non-depressed controls: a narrative review. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 179, 119–142. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2022.07.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karaaslan, F., Gonul, A. S., Oguz, A., Erdinc, E., and Esel, E. (2003). P300 changes in major depressive disorders with and without psychotic features. J. Affect. Disord. 73, 283–287. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0327(01)00477-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaustio, O., Partanen, J., Valkonen-Korhonen, M., Viinamäki, H., and Lehtonen, J. (2002). Affective and psychotic symptoms relate to different types of P300 alteration in depressive disorder. J. Affect. Disord. 71, 43–50. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0327(01)00410-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kawasaki, T., Tanaka, S., Wang, J., Hokama, H., and Hiramatsu, K. (2004). Abnormalities of P300 cortical current density in unmedicated depressed patients revealed by LORETA analysis of event-related potentials. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 58, 68–75. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1819.2004.01195.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kemp, A. H., Benito, L. P., Quintana, D. S., Clark, C. R., McFarlane, A., Mayur, P., et al. (2010). Impact of depression heterogeneity on attention: an auditory oddball event related potential study. J. Affect. Disord. 123, 202–207. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2009.08.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kemp, A. H., Hopkinson, P. J., Hermens, D. F., Rowe, D. L., Sumich, A. L., Clark, C. R., et al. (2009). Fronto-temporal alterations within the first 200 ms during an attentional task distinguish major depression, non-clinical participants with depressed mood and healthy controls: a potential biomarker? Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 602–614. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20528

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kessler, R. C., Berglund, P., Demler, O., Jin, R., Koretz, D., Merikangas, K. R., et al. (2003). The epidemiology of major depressive disorder: results from the national comorbidity survey replication (NCS-R). JAMA 289, 3095–3105. doi: 10.1001/jama.289.23.3095

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Key, A. P., Dove, G. O., and Maguire, M. J. (2005). Linking brainwaves to the brain: an ERP primer. Dev. Neuropsychol. 27, 183–215. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn2702_1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lamers, F., van Oppen, P., Comijs, H. C., Smit, J. H., Spinhoven, P., van Balkom, A. J., et al. (2011). Comorbidity patterns of anxiety and depressive disorders in a large cohort study: the Netherlands study of depression and anxiety (NESDA). J. Clin. Psychiatry 72, 341–348. doi: 10.4088/JCP.10m06176blu

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Landrø, N. I., Stiles, T. C., and Sletvold, H. (2001). Neuropsychological function in nonpsychotic unipolar major depression. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol. Behav. Neurol. 14, 233–240.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Lorenzo, E. C., Kuchel, G. A., Kuo, C. L., Moffitt, T. E., and Diniz, B. S. (2022). Major depression and the biological hallmarks of aging. Ageing Res. Rev. 3:101805. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2022.101805

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lovibond, P. F., Lovibond, S. H. (1995). Manual for the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales, 2nd edition. Sydney: Psychological Foundation.

Google Scholar

Martin, B. A., Kurtzberg, D., and Stapells, D. R. (1999). The effects of decreased audibility produced by high-pass noise masking on N1 and the mismatch negativity to speech sounds/ba/and/da/. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 42, 271–286. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4202.271

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mumtaz, W., Malik, A. S., Yasin, M. A. M., and Xia, L. (2015). Review on EEG and ERP predictive biomarkers for major depressive disorder. Biomed. Signal Process. Control 22, 85–98. doi: 10.1016/j.bspc.2015.07.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murthy, P. N. V., Janakiramaiah, N., Gangadhar, B. N., and Subbakrishna, D. K. (1998). P300 amplitude and antidepressant response to Sudarshan Kriya Yoga (SKY). J. Affect. Disord. 50, 45–48. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0327(98)00029-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Näätänen, R., Gaillard, A. W., and Mäntysalo, S. (1978). Early selective-attention effect on evoked potential reinterpreted. Acta Psychol. (Amst) 42, 313–329. doi: 10.1016/0001-6918(78)90006-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Näätänen, R., Kujala, T., and Winkler, I. (2011). Auditory processing that leads to conscious perception: a unique window to central auditory processing opened by the mismatch negativity and related responses. Psychophysiology 48, 4–22. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.01114.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Näätänen, R., and Picton, T. (1987). The N1 wave of the human electric and magnetic response to sound: a review and an analysis of the component structure. Psychophysiology 24, 375–425. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1987.tb00311.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nan, C., Wang, G., Wang, H., Wang, X., Liu, Z., Xiao, L., et al. (2018). The P300 component decreases in a bimodal oddball task in individuals with depression: an event-related potentials study. Clin. Neurophysiol. 129, 2525–2533. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2018.09.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nebes, R. D., Butters, M. A., Mulsant, B. H., Pollock, B. G., Zmuda, M. D., Houck, P. R., et al. (2000). Decreased working memory and processing speed mediate cognitive impairment in geriatric depression. Psychol. Med. 30, 679–691. doi: 10.1017/s0033291799001968

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oi, K. (2017). Inter-connected trends in cognitive aging and depression: evidence from the health and retirement study. Intelligence 63, 56–65. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2017.05.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pantev, C., Hoke, M., Lehnertz, K., Lütkenhöner, B., Fahrendorf, G., and Stöber, U. (1990). Identification of sources of brain neuronal activity with high spatiotemporal resolution through combination of neuromagnetic source localization (NMSL) and magnetic resonance imaging (MRI). Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 75, 173–184. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(90)90171-f

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Panza, F., D’Introno, A., Colacicco, A. M., Capurso, C., Del Parigi, A., Caselli, R. J., et al. (2009). Temporal relationship between depressive symptoms and cognitive impairment: the Italian longitudinal study on aging. J. Alzheimers Dis. 17, 899–911. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2009-1111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Piani, M. C., Maggioni, E., Delvecchio, G., and Brambilla, P. (2022). Sustained attention alterations in major depressive disorder: a review of fMRI studies employing Go/No-Go and CPT tasks. J. Affect. Disord. 303, 98–113. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2022.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Polich, J. (2007). Updating P300: an integrative theory of P3a and P3b. Clin. Neurophysiol. 118, 2128–2148. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2007.04.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Polich, J. (2012). “Neuropsychology of P300,” in The Oxford Handbook Of Event-Related Potential Components, (Vol. 118) eds S. J. Luck and E. S. Kappenman (Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press), 159–188. doi: 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195374148.013.0089

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Proudman, D., Greenberg, P., and Nellesen, D. (2021). The growing burden of major depressive disorders (MDD): implications for researchers and policy makers. Pharmacoeconomics 39, 619–625. doi: 10.1007/s40273-021-01040-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rock, P. L., Roiser, J. P., Riedel, W. J., and Blackwell, A. (2014). Cognitive impairment in depression: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol. Med. 44, 2029–2040. doi: 10.1017/S0033291713002535

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Röschke, J., and Wagner, P. (2003). A confirmatory study on the mechanisms behind reduced P300 waves in depression. Neuropsychopharmacology 28, S9–S12. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300139

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rush, A. J., Trivedi, M. H., Ibrahim, H. M., Carmody, T. J., Arnow, B., Klein, D. N., et al. (2003). The 16-item quick inventory of depressive symptomatology (QIDS), clinician rating (QIDS-C) and self-report (QIDS-SR): a psychometric evaluation in patients with chronic major depression. Biol. Psychiatry 54, 573–583. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01866-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rush, A. J., Trivedi, M. H., Wisniewski, S. R., Stewart, J. W., Nierenberg, A. A., Thase, M. E., et al. (2006). Bupropion-SR, sertraline, or venlafaxine-XR after failure of SSRIs for depression. N Eng J. Med. 354, 1231–1242. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa052963

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sacchet, M. D., Camacho, M. C., Livermore, E. E., Thomas, E. A., and Gotlib, I. H. (2017). Accelerated aging of the putamen in patients with major depressive disorder. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 42, 164–171. doi: 10.1503/jpn.160010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Santopetro, N. J., Brush, C. J., Bruchnak, A., Klawohn, J., and Hajcak, G. (2021). A reduced P300 prospectively predicts increased depressive severity in adults with clinical depression. Psychophysiology 58:e13767. doi: 10.1111/psyp.13767

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sara, S. J., Vankov, A., and Hervé, A. (1994). Locus coeruleus-evoked responses in behaving rats: a clue to the role of noradrenaline in memory. Brain Res. Bull. 35, 457–465. doi: 10.1016/0361-9230(94)90159-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schlegel, S., Nieber, D., Herrmann, C., and Bakauski, E. (1991). Latencies of the P300 component of the auditory event-related potential in depression are related to the Bech-rafaelsen melancholia scale but not to the hamilton rating scale for depression. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 83, 438–440. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1991.tb05571.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simning, A., Fox, M. L., Barnett, S. L., Sorensen, S., and Conwell, Y. (2019). Depressive and anxiety symptoms in older adults with auditory, vision and dual sensory impairment. J. Aging Health 31, 1353–1375. doi: 10.1177/0898264318781123

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Urretavizcaya, M., Moreno, I., Benlloch, L., Cardoner, N., Serrallonga, J., Menchon, J. M., et al. (2003). Auditory event-related potentials in 50 melancholic patients: increased N100, N200 and P300 latencies and diminished P300 amplitude. J. Affect. Disord. 74, 293–297. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0327(02)00016-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van der Meere, J., Börger, N., and Van Os, T. (2007). Sustained attention in major unipolar depression. Percept. Motil. Skills 104, 1350–1354. doi: 10.2466/pms.104.4.1350-1354

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Dinteren, R., Arns, M., Jongsma, M. L. A., and Kessels, R. P. C. (2014). P300 development across the lifespan: a systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS One 9:e87347. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0087347

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Dinteren, R., Arns, M., Kenemans, L., Jongsma, M. L., Kessels, R. P., Fitzgerald, P., et al. (2015). Utility of event-related potentials in predicting antidepressant treatment response: an iSPOT-D report. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 25, 1981–1990. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2015.07.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Dinteren, R., Huster, R. J., Jongsma, M. L. A., Kessels, R. P. C., and Arns, M. (2018). Differences in cortical sources of the event-related P3 potential between young and old participants indicate frontal compensation. Brain Topogr. 31, 35–46. doi: 10.1007/s10548-016-0542-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vandoolaeghe, E., van Hunsel, F., Nuyten, D., and Maes, M. (1998). Auditory event related potentials in major depression: prolonged P300 latency and increased P200 amplitude. J. Affect. Disord. 48, 105–113. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0327(97)00165-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wigham, S., Rodgers, J., South, M., McConachie, H., and Freeston, M. (2015). The interplay between sensory processing abnormalities, intolerance of uncertainty, anxiety and restricted and repetitive behaviours in autism spectrum disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 45, 943–952. doi: 10.1007/s10803-014-2248-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, L. M., Rush, A. J., Koslow, S. H., Wisniewski, S. R., Cooper, N. J., Nemeroff, C. B., et al. (2011). International study to predict optimized treatment for depression (iSPOT-D), a randomized clinical trial: rationale and protocol. Trials 12:4. doi: 10.1186/1745-6215-12-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhou, L., Wang, G., Nan, C., Wang, H., Liu, Z., and Bai, H. (2019). Abnormalities in P300 components in depression: an ERP-sLORETA study. Nord. J. Psychiatry 73, 1–8. doi: 10.1080/08039488.2018.1478991

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: biomarker, event-related potential, depression, age, N1, oddball, P300

Citation: Key AP, Thornton-Wells TA and Smith DG (2023) Electrophysiological biomarkers and age characterize phenotypic heterogeneity among individuals with major depressive disorder. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 16:1055685. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2022.1055685

Received: 28 September 2022; Accepted: 02 December 2022;
Published: 09 January 2023

Edited by:

Jessica A. Turner, The Ohio State University, United States

Reviewed by:

XiXi Zhao, Beijing Anding Hospital, Capital Medical University, China
Kazuhiko Yamamuro, Nara Medical University, Japan
Jamie O’Reilly, King Mongkut’s Institute of Technology Ladkrabang, Thailand

Copyright © 2023 Key, Thornton-Wells and Smith. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Alexandra P. Key, c2FzaGEua2V5QHZ1bWMub3Jn

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.