Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 13 December 2022
Sec. Cognitive Neuroscience
This article is part of the Research Topic What Can We Make of Theories of Embodiment and the Role of the Human Mirror Neuron System? An Enduring, Ever Larger Question View all 6 articles

Grasping the semantic of actions: a combined behavioral and MEG study

\r\nElisa Visani&#x;Elisa Visani1†Gioacchino Garofalo,&#x;Gioacchino Garofalo2,3†Davide Rossi SebastianoDavide Rossi Sebastiano4Dunja DuranDunja Duran1Laila CraigheroLaila Craighero5Lucia RiggioLucia Riggio6Giovanni Buccino,*Giovanni Buccino2,3*
  • 1Bioengineering Unit, Fondazione IRCCS Istituto Neurologico Carlo Besta, Milan, Italy
  • 2Division of Neuroscience, Universitity “Vita-Salute” San Raffaele, Milan, Italy
  • 3IRCCS San Raffaele, Milan, Italy
  • 4Neurophysiology Unit, Fondazione IRCCS Istituto Neurologico Carlo Besta, Milan, Italy
  • 5Department of Neuroscience and Rehabilitation, University of Ferrara, Ferrara, Italy
  • 6Unit of Neuroscience, Department of Medicine and Surgery, University of Parma, Parma, Italy

There is experimental evidence that the brain systems involved in action execution also play a role in action observation and understanding. Recently, it has been suggested that the sensorimotor system is also involved in language processing. Supporting results are slower response times and weaker motor-related MEG Beta band power suppression in semantic decision tasks on single action verbs labels when the stimulus and the motor response involve the same effector. Attenuated power suppression indicates decreased cortical excitability and consequent decreased readiness to act. The embodied approach forwards that the simultaneous involvement of the sensorimotor system in the processing of the linguistic content and in the planning of the response determines this language-motor interference effect. Here, in a combined behavioral and MEG study we investigated to what extent the processing of actions visually presented (i.e., pictures of actions) and verbally described (i.e., verbs in written words) share common neural mechanisms. The findings demonstrated that, whether an action is experienced visually or verbally, its processing engages the sensorimotor system in a comparable way. These results provide further support to the embodied view of semantic processing, suggesting that this process is independent from the modality of presentation of the stimulus, including language.

Introduction

Accumulating evidence suggests that during action perception the same neural structures necessary for the execution of that action are recruited (Jeannerod et al., 1995; Rizzolatti et al., 1998; Fogassi et al., 2001; Rizzolatti and Matelli, 2003; Buccino et al., 2004; Binkofski and Buccino, 2006; Hardwick et al., 2018; Borra and Luppino, 2019). A matching mechanism in which the visual processing of an action activates a corresponding motor representation has been forwarded, namely action re-enactment, in order to attribute meaning and decoding others’ behavior (Gallese et al., 1996; Rizzolatti and Craighero, 2004; Rizzolatti and Fogassi, 2014). At a neuronal level, this matching mechanism has its counterpart in the presence of the so-called mirror neurons (Di Pellegrino et al., 1992). In the monkey, these neurons were recorded in the ventral premotor area F5 and in the inferior parietal area PFG, and constitute a fronto-parietal network devoted to planning actions and understanding others’ motor acts (Rizzolatti and Fogassi, 2014).

In humans, the neural structures implicated in the execution and understanding of observed actions also appear involved in the understanding and processing of action-related language (Vigliocco et al., 2011; Marino et al., 2014; Borghi and Riggio, 2015; Buccino et al., 2016, 2018; García and Ibáñez, 2016; Sakreida et al., 2016; Zhang et al., 2016; Horoufchin et al., 2018; Garofalo et al., 2022; Visani et al., 2022). In this respect, accumulating empirical evidence suggested that the sensorimotor and even emotional systems involved in experiencing the content expressed by verbal material are causally involved in understanding the content of that linguistic stuff (Pulvermüller, 2002, 2010; Barsalou, 2008; Fischer and Zwaan, 2008; Gallese, 2008; Jirak et al., 2010; Buccino et al., 2016).

In addition, there is evidence that depending on the effector typically used in the expressed action, language that describes that action recruits the sector of the motor system where the effector is motorically represented (Hauk et al., 2004; Tettamanti et al., 2005; Aziz-Zadeh et al., 2006; Baumgaertner et al., 2007; Kemmerer et al., 2008; de Vega et al., 2014). In keeping with this, studies using neurophysiological techniques demonstrated that when processing verbs connected to concrete actions, the motor system is recruited quite early, just 150–170 ms after verbal stimuli are presented auditorily or visually (Pulvermüller et al., 2001; Pulvermüller and Shtyrov, 2005; Pulvermüller et al., 2005). The behavioral counterpart of this early activation is an interference effect. In detail, when participants are required to solve a hand-related semantic task and to give a motor response with the same effector, there is a slowing down of reaction times (Buccino et al., 2005; Boulenger et al., 2006; Sato et al., 2008; Dalla Volta et al., 2009; de Vega et al., 2013, 2014). This interference effect has been explained in terms of competition of neuronal resources (Buccino et al., 2005; de Vega et al., 2013). Specifically, in this context, the motor system is simultaneously processing the meaning of the action as well as preparing the motor response needed to complete the task, leading to a cost for the motor system that will be less prompt to give the motor output. In support of this interpretation, Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation (TMS) studies showed a decrease in Motor-Evoked Potentials (MEPs) during verb listening (Buccino et al., 2005). Furthermore, a weaker suppression of Beta band oscillations was found when motor responses are given with the same body part normally used to perform the action expressed by the verb (Klepp et al., 2015; Visani et al., 2022). Beta band oscillations are the main rhythm deriving from the motor cortex, and it is characterized by a pattern of suppression and rebound during movement (Pfurtscheller and Lopes da Silva, 1999). Beta suppression, or event-related desynchronization (ERD), starts several 100 ms before the start of the movement (both when the movement is internally or externally triggered) and becomes maximal around the time of movement. Hence, due to these features, the ERD is frequently used to investigate the brain correlates of action-related processes, such as action observation (Hari and Kujala, 2009; Moreno and de Vega, 2013), motor imagery (Schnitzler et al., 1997; de Lange and Roelofs, 2008; Brinkman et al., 2014), and the processing of action-related language (Weiss and Mueller, 2012; Klepp et al., 2015; Visani et al., 2022). In general terms, a weaker suppression (minor decrease of ERD) of Beta rhythm indicates that the motor system is less ready for generating a motor output, while an increased suppression (greater decrease of ERD) indicates that the motor system is more ready to generate a motor response.

A still open question is related to the degree of overlap among action execution, understanding of actions visually presented, and understanding of actions verbally described. In a very recent behavioral study of our group (Garofalo et al., 2022), the interference effect was found during the processing of visually presented actions (i.e., pictures depicting hand- and foot-related actions) and actions verbally described (i.e., verbs expressing hand- and foot-related actions). The results of a go/no-go task revealed that when hand actions and hand-related verbs were presented, hand motor responses were slower than when foot actions and foot-related-verbs were presented. We hypothesized that the same semantic mechanisms underlie the understanding of observed actions and verbs.

The aim of the current study was to investigate the neurophysiological underpinnings of the interference effect using oscillatory Magnetoencephalography (MEG) analysis to study Beta band power suppression. The MEG signals were recorded while the participants carried out the same go/no-go task used in a previous study (Garofalo et al., 2022). We expected a replica of the behavioral results and, according to the hypothesis of competition of neuronal resources (Buccino et al., 2005; de Vega et al., 2013), a weaker ERD in both visual and verbal presentation of hand-related actions and verbs as compared to foot-related actions and verbs.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Fifteen volunteers (eight females, age = 26.8 ± 5.1 years) were recruited for the experiment. All participants were adult (>18 years), right-handed, according to the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (Oldfield, 1971), had a normal or corrected-to-normal vision, and were native Italian speakers. Exclusion criteria were formal education in linguistics, the presence of neurological or psychiatric disorders, and the current use of drugs affecting the central nervous system. The experiment was carried out in accordance with the ethical standards laid down in the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki and its later amendments. The experiment was approved by the local Ethical Committee (approval number: 47/2012; date of approval: November 2012). Participants gave their written informed consent before being included in the study.

Task

Participants in the experiment were required to perform a go/no-go task in which they had to respond to words and pictures that represented actions involving either hands or feet, and to refrain from responding when presented stimuli were pseudowords (i.e., built by substituting one consonant and one vowel in two distinct syllables of each verb) or scrambled images (see also, Garofalo et al., 2022), during MEG signals acquisition. Participants had to respond with a flexion of the hand and reaction times (RTs) were collected. Each trial started with a black fixation cross displayed at the center of a gray background. After a random delay ranging from 1,000 to 1,500 ms (in order to avoid response habituation), the fixation cross was replaced by a stimulus item, either a hand or foot verb, or pseudoverbs, or a hand or foot image, or a scrambled image. Stimuli were centrally displayed and surrounded by a red frame. The red frame changed to green 150 ms after the stimulus onset. The “go” signal for the response was the change in the frame color. Participants were instructed to give the motor response (hand flexion). After the go signal, stimuli remained visible for 1,350 ms or until participants’ responses (see Figure 1). The experimental task was divided into two sessions, and each part included 48 go trials (12 hand action images, 12 foot action images, 12 hand action verbs, 12 foot action verbs) and 48 no-go trials (12 hand and 12 foot action pseudo-verbs, 12 hand and 12 foot action scrambled images). The stimuli selection procedure as well as their description are illustrated in previous articles of our group (Garofalo et al., 2022; Visani et al., 2022). Stimuli were randomly presented. No feedback was given to participants. Stimuli were delivered using the software package Stim2. Before starting the acquisition, participants underwent a short training session.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Experimental procedure. Participants were asked to fixate on the center of the screen placed in front of them. Each trial started with the presentation of the stimulus surrounded by a red frame. The stimulus could be either a hand or foot verb, a hand or foot action image, and a pseudoverb or a scrambled image. After 150 ms the frame turned green, and the participants were allowed to respond. Participants were instructed to respond only to words and pictures depicting hand- or foot-related actions. The trial ended when participants provided their responses or after 1,350 ms if no response was given. Stimuli examples: hand action verb (A), hand action image (B), pseudoverb (C), scrambled image (D).

MEG acquisition and pre-processing

A 306-channel whole head MEG system (Triux, MEGIN, Helsinki, Finland) was used to collect the MEG signals. Pairs of electrodes positioned bilaterally 2–3 cm apart over the belly of the right and left flexor and extensor of the wrist were used to simultaneously record surface EMG signals. Signals were sampled at 1 kHz. Moreover, bipolar electro-oculographic (EOG) and electrocardiographic signals (ECG) were acquired. Five head position identification (HPI) coils on the participant’s scalp continuously monitored the participant’s head position inside the MEG helmet. A 3D digitizer (FASTRAK, Polhemus, Colchester, VT, USA) was used to digitally capture the locations of these coils, three anatomical landmarks (the nasion, right and left preauriculars), and additional scalp points before the recording.

In order to remove external interference and correct for head motions, the raw MEG data were first pre-processed off-line using the spatio-temporal signal-space separation approach (Taulu and Simola, 2006) implemented in the Maxfilter 2.2 (MEGIN, Helsinki, Finland). The data were then band-pass filtered at 0.1–100 Hz. Cardiac and ocular movement artifacts were removed using ICA algorithm based on EEGLAB toolbox (Delorme and Makeig, 2004) implemented in a custom-made MATLAB code (R2021a, Mathworks Inc., Natick, MA, USA), using ECG and EOG as reference. MEG data were divided into epochs ranging from 1 s before to 3 s after the stimulus onset. Epochs with continuous muscular contraction identified on EMG signal and/or sensor jumps were excluded from further analysis. Finally, data epochs were grouped according to the experimental conditions: hand-related images, hand-related verbs, foot-related images, foot-related verbs. Movement onset was determined by manually tagging the onset of the EMG burst identified as the time point in which the EMG signal exceeded 30% of the maximal voluntary contraction.

MEG data analysis

Cortical source activations and time-series were estimated using Brainstorm software (Hari et al., 1998). A template brain MRI (MNI/ICBM152, 56), co-registered on MEG data by means of digitized scalp points, was used to generate a realistically shaped single-shell model as volume conductor (BEM model as implemented in OpenMEEG, 57) and ~15,000 dipoles distributed on the brain cortex were defined as source model. The dynamic statistical parametric mapping (dSPM) method (Nishitani, 2000) was employed for the estimation of brain activity at the source level. The noise covariance matrix was calculated using pre-stimulus baseline period data (−1,000–0 ms).

As the inferences were made at the group level, individual source maps were spatially smoothed with a Gaussian kernel with a Full-Width Half Maximum of 3 mm and were averaged for all conditions. Brain sources were grouped according to Destrieux’s atlas (Strafella, 2000) for further analysis.

To explore the language-motor interference effect, we chose the post-stimulus range of 150–350 ms as the period of interest. We selected this period since it has been proposed that concrete linguistic materials are semantically processed in the motor and premotor cortices within 150–170 ms after the stimulus onset (Pulvermüller et al., 2001; Pulvermüller and Shtyrov, 2005; Pulvermüller et al., 2005). Following the experimental hypothesis that understanding semantic of actions recruit brain regions also involved in the execution of those same actions, we selected as the region of interest (ROI) the precentral gyrus (preCG), within the period of interest for all conditions.

The source time series corresponding to each epoch (−2–2.5 s) was extracted from all vertices belonging to the ROI and PCA was used to obtain a single time series for each condition for all the successive comparisons (virtual channel). We used PCA to find the most representative signal in the activated areas since the selected ROI was quite large. Time–frequency representations of virtual channel epochs were computed across frequencies from 1 to 30 Hz (in 1 Hz steps) and time from −2 to 2.5 s (in 0.1 s steps) with a fixed frequency smoothing of 4 Hz by means of multitapers approach. The relative power change course band as compared to the mean of the baseline period was calculated for each epoch and each frequency in the β band (13–30 Hz) by applying the formula [(Epoch(t) − Baseline)/Baseline], where t indicates the time point, then averaged across frequencies and finally averaged for each condition separately. Finally, the Area under Curve (AuC) in the period of interest (150–350 ms) was calculated. Analyses were performed by means of custom Matlab (MATLAB 2021a, MathWorks, Inc., Natick, MA, USA) using functions from the Fieldtrip toolbox (Glenberg and Kaschak, 2002).

Statistical analysis

RTs and AuC of Beta rhythm were first checked for normality using the Shapiro-Wilk test. Both RTs and AuC of Beta rhythm were compared using repeated measures ANOVA (rmANOVA) with Effector (hand, foot) and Stimulus type (images, verbs) as within participants factors. Post-hoc test for stimulus type was performed by means of paired t-tests. The significance level was set to 0.05, and values are expressed as mean ± standard deviation.

Results

Reaction times

Two participants were excluded from the analysis due to technical problems with MEG signals (presence of sensors’ jumps or epochs with artifacts). The remaining participants performed well, and the overall mean error rate was 2.4%. In particular, the errors of commission (response to a scrambled image or pseudo-verb) were 1.8%, while the errors of omission (non-response to words and pictures depicting hand- or foot-related action) were only 0.5%. Given that the maximum error of omission was one for six subjects (one foot picture, two foot verbs, one hand picture and two hand verbs), all correct answers were analyzed. Shapiro-Wilk test indicates that data were normally distributed (see Table 1). rmANOVA showed main effects of Effector (F(1,12) = 24.52, p < 0.001) and Stimulus type (F(1,12) = 8.89, p = 0.011). Inspection of RTs revealed that participants gave slower responses to hand-related actions as compared to foot-related actions, regardless of the presentation modality (images or words). Furthermore, responses to visually presented actions were faster than responses to verbs (see Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive statistic of RTs analysis.

MEG data

We extracted and analyzed ROI from the pre CG, including the premotor and motor cortex (see Figure 2A). In Figure 2B the time frequency representation of the virtual channel obtained from the ROI for each condition is shown. Shapiro-Wilk test indicates that all AuC values were normally distributed. rmANOVA showed main effect of Effector (F(1, 12) = 9.169, p = 0.011). In general, the AuC was greater for the foot-related action stimuli as compared to hand-related action ones (Foot-related verbs: −0.57 ± 0.51; Hand-related verbs: −0.10 ± 0.77; Foot-related images: −0.45 ± 0.53; Hand-related images −0.10 ± 0.64). The paired t-tests confirmed the greater AuC for Foot-related stimuli in comparison to Hand-related stimuli, regardless of the presentation modalities [Verbs: t(12) = −2.31, p = 0.039); Images (t(12) = −2.47, p = 0.030) in pre CG (see Figure 2C)]. For the activation of areas different from pre CG see Supplementary Materials.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. (A) Grand average maps of responses to different categories of stimuli in the 150–350 ms period. The highlighted area represents the ROI used for the analysis. The color scale is the same for each map (0–0.3); for illustrative purposes, maps were thresholded at 50% of the maximum amplitude. (B) Time-frequency representation of the virtual channel obtained from the ROI for each condition. The highlighted area represents the time and frequency interval selected for the analysis. The color scale is the same for each image (−0.3–0.4). (C) Beta band modulation with respect to baseline (−1–0 s) for verbs (upper graph) and images (lower graph) stimuli. Shaded areas indicate the standard error of the mean. Dotted lines indicate the period for AuC calculation, gray dotted line refers to the go signal.

Discussion

The results of the present study showed a slowing down of hand responses to pictures showing hand-related actions, and to hand-related verbs, as compared to pictures and verbs describing foot-related actions, thus confirming the results of a previous behavioral study of our group (Garofalo et al., 2022). In addition, the analysis of the MEG signals provided the neurophysiological correlates of this effect by showing a modulation of Beta rhythm within the pre-central gyrus, coherent with the behavioral results.

Beta rhythm, as revealed by MEG, had a weaker decrease during the processing of hand-related actions, whatever the modality of presentation, as compared to foot-related actions. The so-called ERD, elicited by suppression of Beta rhythm, recorded in pre CG, occurs when motor areas are involved in the actual execution of an action or, to a less degree, when people watch or imagine performing an action (Hari et al., 1998). Our results are in keeping with pivotal studies, showing that this ERD also occurs, although to a less degree, not only during the observation of hand actions but also during the processing of hand-related verbs (Klepp et al., 2015). The presence of ERD for both visually and verbally presented stimuli supports the notion that the same mechanisms and neural structures are working when participants had to give meaning to actions (i.e., semantic processing), whatever the modality of presentation. These findings imply that the brain areas involved in the execution of actions are also recruited during the semantic processing of those actions. Hence, they support further the embodied approach to language processing, showing that the re-enactment of motor structures where actions are represented are crucial to attribute meanings to action words.

One could argue that these findings go counter to some important research that showed facilitation of motor activity during action observation (Fadiga et al., 1995; Cochin, 1999; Nishitani, 2000; Strafella, 2000). When actions are expressed by verbal labels (i.e., verbs), it has been forwarded that a double-stage processing occurs. The first stage occurs very early after stimulus presentation, and it seems to be crucial for understanding. This early processing results from a behavioral point of view in the slowing down of motor responses (Buccino et al., 2005; Boulenger et al., 2006; Sato et al., 2008; Dalla Volta et al., 2009; de Vega et al., 2013; Marino et al., 2014) and from a neurophysiological point of view in a reduction of MEPs amplitude (as revealed by TMS), and a weaker decrease of ERD as showed by MEG (Buccino et al., 2005; Klepp et al., 2015). The second stage is late and occurs when semantic processing has already been completed. In this situation, participants give faster responses (for example action-sentence compatibility effect, ACE; see Del Maschio et al., 2021) or show facilitation of different neurophysiological parameters elicited with different neurophysiological techniques (Watkins and Strafella, 2003; Chersi et al., 2010; de Vega et al., 2013; Klepp et al., 2017, 2019). Considering that, as revealed also by the present results, a substantial motor equivalence exists between observed actions and verbally described actions (Buccino et al., 2016; Hardwick et al., 2018; Garofalo et al., 2022), one may argue that the time course of motor activity during action observation overlaps the one found during the processing of verbally described actions. In detail, when participants engage in a hand motor response, as in our task, during the processing of a seen hand movement, there may be a cost at an early stage. However, when the motor task is completed after the observed action has been fully comprehended, there may be action facilitation. Note that for abstract actions, there may still be a substantial equivalence between sensorimotor experience and verbal description of actions, if it is assumed that they are different from concrete ones because they are anchored in more sophisticated sensory, motor, and emotional experiences than concrete actions (Buccino et al., 2019; Del Maschio et al., 2021) rather than because they are disentangled from experiences or learned through social interactions (Borghi et al., 2013). The complexity of abstract concepts can be defined by the number of biological effectors that can be involved in an abstract action; in the recruitment of different systems (sensory, motor, and emotional); and in the dynamic changes that an abstract action can undergo over time and across cultures, and hence in the different neural substrates subserving those dynamic changes (Buccino et al., 2019).

It is worth stressing that similar recruitment of the motor system may also occur during the processing of visually presented graspable objects and their corresponding nouns (Shinkareva et al., 2011; Gough et al., 2012; Devereux et al., 2013; Marino et al., 2013, 2014; Visani et al., 2022). Also for graspable objects, the neural structures where their motor properties are represented are also re-enacted during the processing of corresponding verbal labels, further supporting the notion of shared semantics.

Despite the fact that there are no single neuron studies supporting the presence of mirror neurons in the humans’ pre-motor and parietal cortices, it is most likely that attributing meaning to verbs has its neuronal counterpart in these neurons (Kemmerer and Gonzalez-Castillo, 2010; Buccino et al., 2016). In a similar vein, one may argue that nouns may have their neuronal counterpart in canonical neurons (Marino et al., 2014; Horoufchin et al., 2018). These neurons, in the monkey, discharge during the grasping of objects and also during the mere observation of those objects (Murata et al., 2000). Accordingly, evidence in humans indicate that during action observation the intrinsic characteristics of the to-be-grasped objects modulate corticospinal excitability and responses to the time-to-contact (Craighero et al., 2014).

It is interesting to observe that when combining verbs and nouns to construct sentences, people choose one precise approach to perform an action from a list of possible ones. For instance, when we say “I hold,” I express all possible ways to hold an object in my hands. However, if a word is added to the sentence, as in “I hold a cup,” then only one particular manner of grasping is re-enacted (Marino et al., 2012). By doing this, we restrict the potential actions and re-enact the action using the best suitable motor representation. In other words, the way a biological effector (such as a hand or foot) interacts with an object in the environment is reflected in the way we construct concrete sentences to depict what occurs in a particular context. Further evidence suggests that adverbs of place (far vs. near) are also rooted in the sensorimotor system since they are implicitly associated with functionally congruent actions (look at vs. grasp, Craighero and Marini, 2021), as well as adjectives denoting manipulative qualities are associated with the characteristics of the objects expressed by nouns (Gough et al., 2013; Garofalo et al., 2021).

Overall, these findings imply that rather than being based on a priori determined syntactic categories (Vigliocco et al., 2011; Buccino et al., 2016), the distinction between word categories may be anchored in the sensorimotor experience. This proposal offers a perspective for an embodied approach also to the way we combine linguistic words (i.e., syntax). In this respect, some authors have proposed that the syntactic representation of words can be rooted in the activation and interaction of specific neuronal populations (Feldman and Narayanan, 2004; Pulvermüller, 2010). We forward that at least for verbs and nouns this speculation most likely refers to mirror and canonical neurons.

Despite the increasing number of empirical evidence suggesting the common neural substrates for semantic processing, including the present findings, it is worth stressing that in the present study some methodological limitations need to be underlined. First, our sample size was rather small, so future studies including a greater number of participants are needed. Second, our analysis focused on the motor/premotor cortex. Since these areas are strictly connected with the parietal cortex, it could be interesting to study the modulation of brain rhythms also within the parietal lobe during the processing of observed and verbally described actions. This in order to assess whether the fronto-parietal network, known to be involved in action observation and understanding, is also involved in language processing.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Ethical Committee of Istituto Neurologico Carlo Besta. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

EV: resources, data curation, formal analysis, methodology, and writing—original draft. GG: methodology, visualization, formal analysis, and writing—original draft. DR: project administration, methodology, resources, and writing—original draft. DD: data curation and investigation. LC and LR: writing—original draft, and writing—review and editing. GB: conceptualization, methodology, supervision, writing—original draft, and writing—review and editing. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was partially supported by the Italian Ministry of Health (RRC).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnhum.2022.1008995/full#supplementary-material.

References

Aziz-Zadeh, L., Wilson, S. M., Rizzolatti, G., and Iacoboni, M. (2006). Congruent embodied representations for visually presented actions and linguistic phrases describing actions. Curr. Biol. 16, 1818–1823. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.07.060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 59, 617–645. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093639

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baumgaertner, A., Buccino, G., Lange, R., McNamara, A., and Binkofski, F. (2007). Polymodal conceptual processing of human biological actions in the left inferior frontal lobe. Eur. J. Neurosci. 25, 881–889. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05346.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Binkofski, F., and Buccino, G. (2006). The role of ventral premotor cortex in action execution and action understanding. J. Physiol. Paris 99, 396–405. doi: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2006.03.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borghi, A. M., and Riggio, L. (2015). Stable and variable affordances are both automatic and flexible. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9:351. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00351

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borghi, A. M., Scorolli, C., Caligiore, D., Baldassarre, G., and Tummolini, L. (2013). The embodied mind extended: using words as social tools. Front. Psychol. 4, 1–10. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00214

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borra, E., and Luppino, G. (2019). Large-scale temporo-parieto-frontal networks for motor and cognitive motor functions in the primate brain. Cortex 118, 19–37. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2018.09.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boulenger, V., Roy, A. C., Paulignan, Y., Deprez, V., Jeannerod, M., and Nazir, T. A. (2006). Cross-talk between language processes and overt motor behavior in the first 200 msec of processing. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 18, 1607–1615. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.10.1607

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brinkman, L., Stolk, A., Dijkerman, H. C., and de Lange, F. P. (2014). Toni distinct roles for alpha- and beta-band oscillations during mental simulation of goal-directed actions. J. Neurosci. 34, 14783–14792. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2039-14.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buccino, G., Binkofski, F., and Riggio, L. (2004). The mirror neuron system and action recognition. Brain Lang. 89, 370–376. doi: 10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00356-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buccino, G., Colagè, I., Gobbi, N., and Bonaccorso, G. (2016). Grounding meaning in experience: a broad perspective on embodied language. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 69, 69–78. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.07.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buccino, G., Colagè, I., Silipo, F., and D’Ambrosio, P. (2019). The concreteness of abstract language: an ancient issue and a new perspective. Brain Struct. Funct. 224, 1385–1401. doi: 10.1007/s00429-019-01851-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buccino, G., Dalla Volta, R., Arabia, G., Morelli, M., Chiriaco, C., Lupo, A., et al. (2018). Processing graspable object images and their nouns is impaired in Parkinson’s disease patients. Cortex 100, 32–39. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.03.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buccino, G., Riggio, L., Melli, G., Binkofski, F., Gallese, V., and Rizzolatti, G. (2005). Listening to action-related sentences modulates the activity of the motor system: a combined TMS and behavioral study. Cogn. Brain Res. 24, 355–363. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.02.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chersi, F., Thill, S., Ziemke, T., and Borghi, A. M. (2010). Sentence processing: linking language to motor chains. Front. Neurorobot. 4, 1–9. doi: 10.3389/fnbot.2010.00004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cochin, S. (1999). Observation and execution of movement: similarities demonstrated by quantified electroencephalography. Eur. J. Neurosci. 11, 1839–1842. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.1999.00598.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Craighero, L., and Marini, M. (2021). Implicit associations between adverbs of place and actions in the physical and digital space. Brain Sci. 11:1523. doi: 10.3390/brainsci11111523

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Craighero, L., Zorzi, V., Canto, R., and Franca, M. (2014). Same kinematics but different objects during action observation: detection times and motor evoked potentials. Vis. Cogn. 22, 653–671. doi: 10.1080/13506285.2014.904460

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dalla Volta, R., Gianelli, C., Campione, G. C., and Gentilucci, M. (2009). Action word understanding and overt motor behavior. Exp. Brain Res. 196, 403–412. doi: 10.1007/s00221-009-1864-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Lange, F. P., and Roelofs, K. (2008). Toni Motor imagery: a window into the mechanisms and alterations of the motor system. Cortex 44, 494–506. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2007.09.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Vega, M., León, I., Hernández, J. A., Valdés, M., Padrón, I., and Ferstl, E. C. (2014). Action sentences activate sensory motor regions in the brain independently of their status of reality. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 26, 1363–1376. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00559

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Vega, M., Moreno, V., and Castillo, D. (2013). The comprehension of action-related sentences may cause interference rather than facilitation on matching actions. Psychol. Res. 77, 20–30. doi: 10.1007/s00426-011-0356-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Del Maschio, N., Fedeli, D., Garofalo, G., and Buccino, G. (2021). Evidence for the concreteness of abstract language: a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Brain Sci. 12, 1–21. doi: 10.3390/brainsci12010032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delorme, A., and Makeig, S. (2004). EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J. Neurosci. Methods 134, 9–21. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Devereux, B. J., Clarke, A., Marouchos, A., and Tyler, L. K. (2013). Representational similarity analysis reveals commonalities and differences in the semantic processing of words and objects. J. Neurosci. 33, 18906–18916. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3809-13.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Di Pellegrino, G., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., and Rizzolatti, G. (1992). Understanding motor events: a neurophysiological study. Exp. Brain Res. 91, 176–180. doi: 10.1007/BF00230027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Pavesi, G., and Rizzolatti, G. (1995). Motor facilitation during action observation: a magnetic stimulation study. J. Neurophysiol. 73, 2608–2611. doi: 10.1152/jn.1995.73.6.2608

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feldman, J., and Narayanan, S. (2004). Embodied meaning in a neural theory of language. Brain Lang. 89, 385–392. doi: 10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00355-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischer, M. H., and Zwaan, R. A. (2008). Embodied language: a review of the role of the motor system in language comprehension. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 61, 825–850. doi: 10.1080/17470210701623605

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., Buccino, G., Craighero, L., Fadiga, L., and Rizzolatti, G. (2001). Cortical mechanism for the visual guidance of hand grasping movements in the monkey: a reversible inactivation study. Brain 124, 571–586. doi: 10.1093/brain/124.3.571

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gallese, V. (2008). Mirror neurons and the social nature of language: the neural exploitation hypothesis. Soc. Neurosci. 3, 317–333. doi: 10.1080/17470910701563608

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., and Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain 119, 593–609. doi: 10.1093/brain/119.2.593

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

García, A. M., and Ibáñez, A. (2016). A touch with words: dynamic synergies between manual actions and language. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 68, 59–95. doi: 10.1186/s12934-022-01952-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garofalo, G., Marino, B. F. M., Bellelli, S., and Riggio, L. (2021). Adjectives modulate sensorimotor activation driven by nouns. Cogn. Sci. 45, 1–36. doi: 10.1111/cogs.12953

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garofalo, G., Magliocco, F., Silipo, F., Riggio, L., and Buccino, G. (2022). What matters is the underlying experience: similar motor responses during processing observed hand actions and hand-related verbs. J. Neuropsychol. 16, 389–406. doi: 10.1111/jnp.12270

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glenberg, A. M., and Kaschak, M. P. (2002). Grounding language in action. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 9, 558–565. doi: 10.3758/bf03196313

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gough, P. M., Campione, G. C., and Buccino, G. (2013). Fine tuned modulation of the motor system by adjectives expressing positive and negative properties. Brain Lang. 125, 54–59. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2013.01.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gough, P. M., Riggio, L., Chersi, F., Sato, M., Fogassi, L., and Buccino, G. (2012). Nouns referring to tools and natural objects differentially modulate the motor system. Neuropsychologia 50, 19–25. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.10.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hardwick, R. M., Caspers, S., Eickhoff, S. B., and Swinnen, S. P. (2018). Neural correlates of action: comparing meta-analyses of imagery, observation and execution. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 94, 31–44. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hari, R., Forss, N., Avikainen, S., Kirveskari, E., Salenius, S., and Rizzolatti, G. (1998). Activation of human primary motor cortex during action observation: a neuromagnetic study. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 95, 15061–15065. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.25.15061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hari, R., and Kujala, M. V. (2009). Brain basis of human social interaction: from concepts to brain imaging. Physiol. Rev. 89, 453–479. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00041.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hauk, O., Johnsrude, I., and Pulvermüller, F. (2004). Somatotopic representation of action words in human motor and premotor cortex. Neuron 41, 301–307. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00838-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Horoufchin, H., Bzdok, D., Buccino, G., Borghi, A. M., and Binkofski, F. (2018). Action and object words are differentially anchored in the sensory motor system - a perspective on cognitive embodiment. Sci. Rep. 8:6583. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-24475-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jeannerod, M., Arbib, M. A., Rizzolatti, G., and Sakata, H. (1995). Grasping objets: the cortical mechanisms of visuomotor transformation. Trends Neurosci. 18, 314–320.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Jirak, D., Menz, M. M., Buccino, G., Borghi, A. M., and Binkofski, F. (2010). Grasping language—a short story on embodiment. Conscious. Cogn. 19, 711–720. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2010.06.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kemmerer, D., Castillo, J. G., Talavage, T., Patterson, S., and Wiley, C. (2008). Neuroanatomical distribution of five semantic components of verbs: evidence from fMRI. Brain Lang. 107, 16–43. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2007.09.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kemmerer, D., and Gonzalez-Castillo, J. (2010). The Two-Level Theory of verb meaning: an approach to integrating the semantics of action with the mirror neuron system. Brain Lang. 112, 54–76. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2008.09.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klepp, A., Niccolai, V., Buccino, G., Schnitzler, A., and Biermann-Ruben, K. (2015). Language-motor interference reflected in MEG beta oscillations. Neuroimage 109, 438–448. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.12.077

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klepp, A., Niccolai, V., Sieksmeyer, J., Arnzen, S., Indefrey, P., Schnitzler, A., et al. (2017). Body-part specific interactions of action verb processing with motor behaviour. Behav. Brain Res. 328, 149–158. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2017.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klepp, A., van Dijk, H., Niccolai, V., Schnitzler, A., and Biermann-Ruben, K. (2019). Action verb processing specifically modulates motor behaviour and sensorimotor neuronal oscillations. Sci. Rep. 9:15985. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-52426-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marino, B. F. M., Gallese, V., Buccino, G., and Riggio, L. (2012). Language sensorimotor specificity modulates the motor system. Cortex 48, 849–856. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2010.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marino, B. F. M., Gough, P. M., Gallese, V., Riggio, L., and Buccino, G. (2013). How the motor system handles nouns: a behavioral study. Psychol. Res. 77, 64–73. doi: 10.1007/s00426-011-0371-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marino, B. F. M., Sirianni, M., Volta, R. D., Magliocco, F., Silipo, F., Quattrone, A., et al. (2014). Viewing photos and reading nouns of natural graspable objects similarly modulate motor responses. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:968. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00968

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moreno, I., and de Vega, M. (2013). León Understanding action language modulates oscillatory mu and beta rhythms in the same way as observing actions. Brain Cogn. 82, 236–242. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2013.04.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murata, A., Gallese, V., Luppino, G., Kaseda, M., and Sakata, H. (2000). Selectivity for the shape, size and orientation of objects for grasping in neurons of monkey parietal area AIP. J. Neurophysiol. 83, 2580–2601. doi: 10.1152/jn.2000.83.5.2580

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nishitani, N. (2000). Hari temporal dynamics of cortical representation for action. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 97, 913–918. doi: 10.1073/pnas.97.2.913

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: the edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9, 97–113. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pfurtscheller, G., and Lopes da Silva, F. H. (1999). Event-related EEG/MEG synchronization and desynchronization: basic principles. Clin. Neurophysiol. 110, 1842–1857. doi: 10.1016/s1388-2457(99)00141-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pulvermüller, F. (2002). A brain perspective on language mechanisms: from discrete neuronal ensembles to serial order. Prog. Neurobiol. 67, 85–111. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0082(02)00014-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pulvermüller, F. (2010). Brain embodiment of syntax and grammar: discrete combinatorial mechanisms spelt out in neuronal circuits. Brain Lang. 112, 167–179. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2009.08.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pulvermüller, F., Assadollahi, R., and Elbert, T. (2001). Neuromagnetic evidence for early semantic access in word recognition. Eur. J. Neurosci. 13, 201–205. doi: 10.1046/j.0953-816x.2000.01380.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pulvermüller, F., Hauk, O., Nikulin, V. V., and Ilmoniemi, R. J. (2005). Functional links between motor and language systems. Eur. J. Neurosci. 21, 793–797. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.03900.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pulvermüller, F., and Shtyrov, Y. (2005). Ilmoniemi brain signatures of meaning access in action word recognition. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 17, 884–892. doi: 10.1162/0898929054021111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rizzolatti, G., and Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron System. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 27, 169–192. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144230

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rizzolatti, G., and Fogassi, L. (2014). The mirror mechanism: recent findings and perspectives. Philos. Trans. R Soc. B. Biol. Sci. 369:20130420. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2013.0420

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rizzolatti, G., Luppino, G., and Matelli, M. (1998). The organization of the cortical motor system: new concepts. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 106, 283–296. doi: 10.1016/s0013-4694(98)00022-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rizzolatti, G., and Matelli, M. (2003). Two different streams form the dorsal visual system: anatomy and functions. Exp. Brain Res. 153, 146–157. doi: 10.1007/s00221-003-1588-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sakreida, K., Effnert, I., Thill, S., Menz, M. M., Jirak, D., Eickhoff, C. R., et al. (2016). Affordance processing in segregated parieto-frontal dorsal stream sub-pathways. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 69, 89–112. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.07.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sato, M., Mengarelli, M., Riggio, L., Gallese, V., and Buccino, G. (2008). Task related modulation of the motor system during language processing. Brain Lang. 105, 83–90. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2007.10.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schnitzler, A., Salenius, S., Salmelin, R., and Jousmäki, V. (1997). Hari involvement of primary motor cortex in motor imagery: a neuromagnetic study. Neuroimage 6, 201–208. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1997.0286

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shinkareva, S. V., Malave, V. L., Mason, R. A., Mitchell, T. M., and Just, M. A. (2011). Commonality of neural representations of words and pictures. Neuroimage 54, 2418–2425. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.10.042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strafella, A. P. (2000). Paus Modulation of cortical excitability during action observation: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Neuroreport 11, 2289–2292. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200007140-00044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taulu, S., and Simola, J. (2006). Spatiotemporal signal space separation method for rejecting nearby interference in MEG measurements. Phys. Med. Biol. 51, 1759–1768. doi: 10.1088/0031-9155/51/7/008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tettamanti, M., Buccino, G., Saccuman, M. C., Gallese, V., Danna, M., Scifo, P., et al. (2005). Listening to action-related sentences activates fronto-parietal motor circuits. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 17, 273–281. doi: 10.1162/0898929053124965

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vigliocco, G., Vinson, D. P., Druks, J., Barber, H., and Cappa, S. F. (2011). Nouns and verbs in the brain: a review of behavioural, electrophysiological, neuropsychological and imaging studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 407–426. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.04.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Visani, E., Magliocco, F., Sebastiano, D. R., Duran, D., Garofalo, G., Magliocco, F., et al. (2022). The semantics of natural objects and tools in the brain: a combined behavioural and MEG study. Brain Sci. 12:97. doi: 10.3390/brainsci12010097

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watkins, K. E., and Strafella, A. P. (2003). Paus seeing and hearing speech excites the motor system involved in speech production. Neuropsychologia 41, 989–994. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(02)00316-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weiss, S., and Mueller, H. (2012). “Too many betas do not spoil the broth”: the role of beta brain oscillations in language processing. Front. Psychol. 3:201. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00201

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, Z., Sun, Y., and Humphreys, G. W. (2016). Perceiving object affordances through visual and linguistic pathways: a comparative study. Sci. Rep. 6, 1–9. doi: 10.1038/srep26806

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: semantics, embodiment, language processing, motor responses, beta rhythm, MEG (magnetoencephalography), sensorimotor system

Citation: Visani E, Garofalo G, Rossi Sebastiano D, Duran D, Craighero L, Riggio L and Buccino G (2022) Grasping the semantic of actions: a combined behavioral and MEG study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 16:1008995. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2022.1008995

Received: 01 August 2022; Accepted: 22 November 2022;
Published: 13 December 2022.

Edited by:

Analí-a Arévalo, University of Sao Paulo Medical School, Brazil

Reviewed by:

Jan Kujala, Aalto University, Finland
Anna Pavlova, Moscow State University of Psychology and Education, Russia

Copyright © 2022 Visani, Garofalo, Rossi Sebastiano, Duran, Craighero, Riggio and Buccino. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Giovanni Buccino, buccino.giovanni@hsr.it

These authors have contributed equally to this work

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.