Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 25 November 2021
Sec. Cognitive Neuroscience
This article is part of the Research Topic Bridging Cognitive Neuroscience and Neurosurgery for Effective Brain Mapping View all 9 articles

Language Assessment in Multilingualism and Awake Neurosurgery

  • 1Department of Political and Communication Sciences (POLICOM), University of Salerno, Fisciano, Italy
  • 2San Giovanni Addolorata Hospital, Rome, Italy
  • 3Brain Associati, Rome, Italy
  • 4Department of Neurosciences, Biomedicine and Movement Sciences, University of Verona, Verona, Italy
  • 5Center for Mind/Brain Sciences, University of Trento, Trento, Italy

Multilingualism has become a worldwide phenomenon that poses critical issues about the language assessment in patients undergoing awake neurosurgery in eloquent brain areas. The accuracy and sensitivity of multilingual perioperative language assessment procedures is crucial for a number of reasons: they should be appropriate to detect deficits in each of the languages spoken by the patient; they should be suitable to identify language-specific cortical regions; they should ensure that each of the languages of a multilingual patient is tested at an adequate and comparable level of difficulty. In clinical practice, a patient-tailored approach is generally preferred. This is a necessary compromise since it is impossible to predict all the possible language combinations spoken by individuals and thus the availability of standardized testing batteries is a potentially unattainable goal. On the other hand, this leads to high inconsistency in how different neurosurgical teams manage the linguistic features that determine similarity or distance between the languages spoken by the patient and that may constrain the neuroanatomical substrate of each language. The manuscript reviews the perioperative language assessment methodologies adopted in awake surgery studies on multilingual patients with brain tumor published from 1991 to 2021 and addresses the following issues: (1) The language selected for the general neuropsychological assessment of the patient. (2) The procedures adopted to assess the dimensions that may constrain language organization in multilingual speakers: age and type of acquisition, exposure, proficiency, and use of the different languages. (3) The type of preoperative language assessment used for all the languages spoken by the patient. (4) The linguistic tasks selected in the intraoperative setting. The reviewed data show a great heterogeneity in the perioperative clinical workup with multilingual patients. The only exception is the task used during language mapping, as the picture naming task is highly preferred. The review highlights that an objective and accurate description of both the linguistic profile of multilingual patients and the specific properties of the languages under scrutiny can profitably support clinical management and decision making in multilingual awake neurosurgery settings.

Introduction

In a broad and inclusive sense multilingualism can be defined as the acquisition and use in everyday life of two or more languages (Butler, 2013; Grosjean, 2013).1 In order to cope with challenges resulting from migration and globalization, current human societies support multilingualism since it promotes education, cognitive health (Antoniou and Wright, 2017; Baumgart and Billick, 2018; Calabria et al., 2020), cultural, social, and economic inclusion (Aronin and Singleton, 2008). Worldwide, multilingual people are actually the rule rather than the exception, mostly if one considers that, beyond the official and standardized languages, many people use dialect for communication in everyday life (Grosjean, 1992; EuroStat, 2015; Hartsuiker et al., 2016). However, the monolingual brain and the monolingual language processing system are still considered as the norm both in neurocognitive models of language and in clinical practice. This is probably due to mixed and inconsistent findings and to several extant controversies on the functioning, architecture, and neural underpinnings of language processing in multilinguals.

Starting from late ’70, aphasiology, neurosurgery, and neuroimaging studies have provided evidence about the multilingual brain (Albert and Obler, 1978; Ojemann and Whitaker, 1978; Paradis, 2004; Bhatia and Ritchie, 2006). Two major issues have been addressed:

(1). Whether multilingual speakers recruit the same regions as monolinguals during linguistic tasks or multilingualism requires recruiting additional brain regions.

(2). Whether or not different languages require the support of specific cortical regions.

In general, clinical observations on multilingual aphasic individuals documented different patterns of impairment and of post-insult recovery in each of the languages spoken by the patients and described complex correlations between language and brain sites (Albert and Obler, 1978; Aglioti and Fabbro, 1993; Paradis, 2000; Giussani et al., 2007). Consistent findings were reported in neurosurgical settings where multilingual patients showed language-specific2 responses to brain stimulation (see Połczyñska and Bookheimer, 2020 for a recent review). These data have often been used as evidence that different languages are represented in different brain regions. However, neuroimaging studies in healthy multilinguals provided evidence that the neural representation of L1 converges with that of additional spoken languages (Green, 2003; Abutalebi and Green, 2007; García-Pentón et al., 2016). The contrasting results that emerge from studies on brain-damaged patients and from neuroimaging investigations on neurotypical individuals do not yet have a straightforward explanation. Reliable accounts will require substantial progress in at least two areas of investigation. In the first place, a finer-grained knowledge of the neural representation of linguistic knowledge and domain-general resources is mandatory. So far studies focused on single-word processing (mostly nouns) but largely steered clear of language-specific aspects of syntax and morphosyntax, and of their interactions with processing resources. Obviously, studies should be carried out in more languages than currently available. Secondly, results should be interpreted based on an in-depth knowledge of experimental methods. To mention but one issue, neuroimaging investigations analyze BOLD signal changes in macroareas regions of interest (ROIs) during exposure to a relatively large number of stimuli, whereas direct electrical stimulation (DES) is delivered over very small areas of the brain, each of which may occupy a minimal fraction of said ROIs, and results are inferred based on a necessarily limited number of stimuli.

At present, despite the growing amount of evidence, it is hard to draw clear and firm conclusions about the neural and cognitive organization of multilingualism. As a matter of fact, multilingualism poses a number of critical questions on both theoretical and methodological grounds. The linguistic profiles of multilingual speakers are very heterogeneous, since a multitude of experience-related factors determines the multilingual competence: age (early vs. late) and type of acquisition (formal vs. informal education; simultaneous vs. sequential acquisition), amount of exposure to the different languages, modality (oral vs. written or both) and context (familiar vs. professional or both) of use, proficiency level, and degree of similarity/distance between languages. Recent reviews and meta-analyses (Cargnelutti et al., 2019; Połczyñska and Bookheimer, 2020) have shown that those factors affect the performance of multilinguals in linguistic tasks and have an impact on the neural organization of languages. In addition, the interaction among the spoken languages has been shown to modulate their neural underpinnings (De Bot, 2004; Kroll and Bialystok, 2013; Kroll and Ma, 2018; Del Maschio and Abutalebi, 2019). However, there are no standardized objective measures or procedures to operationalize these variables. From a research/academic perspective, this is conducive to results that are not comparable across studies and thus hampers an adequate comprehension of the multilingual system and of its cerebral organization. From a clinical standpoint, a potential underestimation of the role of those factors during language assessment may produce skewed profiles of the pattern of compromised/preserved linguistic abilities in multilingual patients. For instance, an incorrect estimation of the proficiency or of the frequency of usage of the languages spoken by a multilingual speaker may produce confounds when assessing the presence of linguistic deficits. In other words, it is crucial to distinguish a true anomia, or a true semantic/phonological paraphasia, from errors due to inaccurate knowledge or infrequent use of a given language. Similarly, increased latencies in reading or naming tasks could indicate either difficulties in lexical access, or cross-language interference, or even a language-switching cost in speakers with strong dominance of one language over the others. Such a problem has relevant consequences especially for the procedures adopted during awake glioma surgery in language-sensitive brain regions. The surgical procedure with the patient in awake state has been introduced in brain tumor treatment in the ‘90s and requires the patient to perform cognitive tasks while specific parts of the brain are stimulated. If stimulation interferes with the task, the stimulated area should not be resected to prevent post-surgery deficits. This technique offers two primary advantages: it allows enrolling in surgical treatment patients who were previously excluded because their tumors were located in brain areas critical for specific cognitive functions, such as language; furthermore, it preserves full functionality while allowing maximal resection of pathological tissue. The potential variation of both linguistic competence and anatomical differences in the cortical representation of the different languages in multilingual patients may induce additional post-surgical deficits if all the languages are not comprehensively and adequately assessed preoperatively and if an appropriate intraoperative testing has not been prepared.

This manuscript analyses the perioperative language assessments adopted in awake surgery studies on brain tumors in multilingual patients published from 1991 to 2021. Albeit awake surgery procedures are usually adopted to treat severe epilepsy while preserving language functions, we focused only on brain tumor surgery. The reason is that epilepsy frequently has a childhood onset and may affect language acquisition, thus adding a possible confound in the analysis of language assessment procedures in multilingual speakers. The aim of the present manuscript is to verify to what extent the variables that affect linguistic processing in multilingual speakers have been considered during planning and decision making in awake surgery for brain tumors. The following main issues will be addressed: whether and how AoA and proficiency in each language are evaluated; how language skills are assessed in all the languages; whether the distance/similarity across languages is kept under control in the direct comparison of language-specific performance accuracy; how the language for the global neuropsychological assessment of the multilingual patient is selected and which tasks are used in the intraoperative setting. In the following paragraphs, each of the variables analyzed in the manuscript is described. Results are summarized in separate sections. Finally, strengths and weaknesses of the most frequently used clinical approaches to multilingualism in awake surgery for brain tumors are discussed.

Materials and Methods

A literature search using PubMed and Web of Science databases was performed between March and May 2021. The following terms were used: plurilingual*, multilingual*, bilingual*, trilingual*, quadrilingual*, polyglot, brain tumor, brain tumour, brain cancer, cerebral cancer, glioma, glioblastoma, meningioma, awake surgery, craniotomy, neurosurgery, direct electrical stimulation, and electrocorticography. The asterisk indicates that terms used to enter the bibliographic databases were abbreviated. This allowed us to broaden the search by finding words that started with the same letters. Manuscripts published between 1991 and 2021 were considered.

We found 1,113 peer-reviewed manuscripts, removed duplications (746 manuscripts) and among the remaining 367 manuscripts we only focused on those that included multilingual individuals who underwent awake surgery. A flowchart of the research strategy is reported in Figure 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Flowchart of the search process. Numbers show how many studies were included at each stage.

The following exclusion criteria were applied: review manuscript, sign languages, not in English, not a manuscript, not brain tumor, not multilingual, not adult, not intraoperative language mapping in awake surgery, not multilingual intraoperative testing, insufficient details about the preoperative and/or the intraoperative multilingual language testing.

Ultimately, 22 manuscripts were selected and reviewed. The list of the selected manuscripts is reported in the first column on the left of Tables 16.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Patient information*.

The following data were extracted from the reviewed manuscripts: demographic and clinical information about patients3 (Table 1), languages (number and types of studied languages, multilingual profiles and language distance/similarity, Table 2), AoA (Table 3), proficiency (Table 4), language assessment and intraoperative tasks (Table 5), and general neuropsychological assessment (Table 6).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Number and types of studied languages and language distance/similarity.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Age of acquisition*.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Language proficiency.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Language assessment and Intraoperative tasks.

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Preoperative and postoperative neuropsychological assessment and preoperative language mapping*.

Analyzed Variables

Language Distance

The concept of distance between languages has to do with qualitative and quantitative differences that may involve many domains: phonetics (tonal vs. non-tonal languages, e.g., Mandarin Chinese vs. English), orthographic systems (alphabetic vs. logographic, e.g., English vs. Japanese Kanji; direction of writing, e.g., right to left vs. left to right vs. top to bottom, as in Arabic vs. French vs. traditional Japanese; deep vs. transparent orthography, e.g., English vs. Italian), vocabulary (e.g., presence/absence of cognates4), grammar (e.g., presence/absence of determiners, grammatical gender), morphology (e.g., inflectional systems, agreement patterns, auxiliaries), and syntax (e.g., word order, phrase structure). There is no well-established method to quantify the similarity between languages and it is hard to reduce all these parameters to a single distance score (Gamallo et al., 2017). Usually, linguistic distance is determined by measuring the number of branches between two languages on the language family tree model (Dimmendaal, 1995; Połczyñska and Bookheimer, 2020). This system is based on the possibility to identify common ancestors of languages and to define broad categories of language families (e.g., Romance, Germanic, Scandinavian, African, Slavic, Semitic, Asiatic, and isolated). However, and even beyond its theoretical limitations, such an approach is scarcely useful when attempting to understand how language distance affects cognitive aspects of language processing in multilinguals. Conversely, the problem has been dealt with repeatedly in neuroscience and psycholinguistics (Vaid, 1983; Gleitman, 1985; Odlin, 1989; Cenoz et al., 2001; Koda, 2005; Bassetti, 2008; Kim et al., 2016; Schepens et al., 2016; Zawiszewski and Laka, 2020; Shinozuka et al., 2021). Models of multilingual processing have tried to define how the cognitive system manages shared and distinctive features between languages. In general, it is assumed that conceptual information on words is represented in a language-independent fashion, as it is related to the semantic properties of the word’s referents (Francis, 2005). Other aspects are controversial. On the “shared syntax” approach, syntactic-grammatical properties common to different languages are represented only once in the multilingual language system, thus reducing redundancy and increasing efficiency of language processes (Hartsuiker et al., 2004). On the other hand, the structural similarity across languages modulates the functioning of the hypothesized unified syntax (Runnqvist et al., 2013). Similarly, Peeters et al. (2013) demonstrated that identical interlingual cognates are stored as a single orthographic representation but as two distinct phonological and morphological representations, and that the activation of each representation can vary with the linguistic operation to be performed. In a study on the behavioral and neural correlates of naming in L2 in healthy speakers, Ghazi-Saidi and Ansaldo (2017) found that naming in L2 is more effortful and demanding in distant language pairs than in close language pairs.

To sum up, despite the difficulty operationalizing the distance/similarity across languages and even though the mechanisms of cerebral-cognitive assimilation and accommodation during the processing of multiple languages are still largely unknown (Kim et al., 2016), it is reasonable to conclude that specific properties of each language impose different cognitive demands on multilingual speakers. The obvious implication for researchers and clinicians is to adapt the perioperative procedures used when selecting tasks and stimuli for language assessment so as to properly address the language distance issue.

Age of Acquisition

The AoA generally indicates the age of exposure to a language and is taken as an indication of the moment in life when that language is acquired. The AoA parameter provides indirect information on the way a language is acquired: for instance, it can be used to make inferences about whether or not the speaker received any kind of formal education in his/her additional languages. Qualitatively different multilingual conditions have been described by using the AoA parameter (Kim et al., 1997). Simultaneous multilingualism applies to children who are exposed to two or more languages from birth or shortly after birth. In this case, there is no chronological gap between the first language (L1) and other languages (L2 and L3, etc.); thus, it is assumed that simultaneous multilingual speakers acquire all their languages through similar developmental trajectories and learning mechanisms. Early-sequential multilinguals begin to acquire additional languages after acquiring the basic grammatical structures of L1; this happens from ages 3 to 5–7 years. Late-sequential multilinguals acquire additional languages by the age of 5–7 years and often, albeit not always, receive formal education in these other languages (Perani et al., 2003; Leonard et al., 2011; Połczyñska et al., 2016). In the literature, different age ranges have been proposed to distinguish simultaneous, early and late bilingualism and it is plausible that AoA should be thought of as a continuous rather than a categorical parameter. Several studies demonstrated that people who learn a language in infancy generally achieve greater proficiency than late learners (Johnson and Newport, 1989; Birdsong, 1999, 2014; Perani et al., 2003), that AoA affects several language-specific skills like lexical access, phonology, grammar and syntax (Weber-Fox and Neville, 1996; Mahendra et al., 2003; Perani et al., 2003; Wartenburger et al., 2003; Frenck-Mestre et al., 2005; Hernandez et al., 2007; Isel et al., 2010; Waldron and Hernandez, 2013; Wei et al., 2015) as well as domain-general cognitive control mechanisms (Luk et al., 2011; Tao et al., 2011). Moreover, AoA has an actual role in shaping multilingual brain networks (Perani et al., 1996, 1998; Fabbro, 1998, 2001; Wartenburger et al., 2003; Mechelli et al., 2004; Abutalebi et al., 2013; Klein et al., 2014; Wei et al., 2015; Liu and Cao, 2016; Del Maschio and Abutalebi, 2019).

Proficiency

Proficiency indicates how well a language is known either in production or in comprehension and denotes the level of competence attained in each language (Del Maschio and Abutalebi, 2019). It is strictly related both to fluency, which refers to the speed and automaticity of linguistic behavior (Segalowitz, 2010), and to the context and amount of use and exposure to a given language. Proficiency is a multidimensional construct (Treffers-Daller, 2019), which can be differently related to specific aspects of linguistic competence such as modality (oral vs. written), task (single word/sentence or discourse production, word recognition, and language comprehension), domain (syntax, semantics, morphology, phonology, and vocabulary). Proficiency in a language can change over time: for instance, multilingual speakers can become more proficient in later-acquired languages than in their mother tongue if they stop using the latter in everyday life or only use it occasionally. For similar reasons, they can be very proficient in a specific modality, or achieve better vocabulary than grammatical skills, or vice versa.

Even if the relative relevance of the many features that define language proficiency remains unclear, the measures of proficiency used to assess the linguistic competence of multilingual speakers should be spelled out in published reports. Recent findings indicate that proficiency and frequency of use of additional languages are key factors in the organization of language networks in the multilingual brain (Stowe and Sabourin, 2005; Kotz, 2009; Consonni et al., 2013; Sugiura et al., 2015).

Preoperative Language Assessment

Preoperative language testing for patients undergoing awake surgery should provide detailed information on all aspects of their linguistic competence to detect aphasic deficits, identify the functional locus of damage to the language system and select the most suitable tasks/stimuli for intraoperative testing (Miceli et al., 2012). Usually, this goal is accomplished by employing standardized language batteries that provide tasks for the evaluation of different modalities (written and oral), functions (production, comprehension, transcoding, and verbal memory), and levels of language organization (materials controlled for distributional, phonological, lexical, grammatical/morphological, syntactic, and semantic features). The language assessment of multilingual patients eligible for awake surgery has specific requirements but suffers from the lack of standard procedures. Few standardized tests include multilingual materials and provide normative data from multilingual individuals (Goral and Conner, 2013; Fernández-Coello et al., 2021; Gisbert-Muñoz et al., 2021). Even when such data exist, it is practically impossible to find tests that are adequately matched in all the possible language combinations of the multilingual population5. In clinical settings, the standard practice consists of adapting the tests available in one of the languages spoken by a multilingual person to the other languages.

General Neuropsychological Assessment

Extensive neuropsychological investigations are indispensable in the clinical work-up of brain tumors. More in detail, executive functions, working memory, attention, and emotional status, at the minimum, must be assessed since they impact on linguistic performance and on the ability to tolerate the brain stimulation procedure (Talacchi et al., 2013). The preoperative assessment provides critical information about the cognitive deficits induced by the tumor so that results can be used to plan the surgical approach and define a baseline for subsequent evaluations. The postoperative assessment allows identifying the short-term and long-term outcomes of treatment and provides indications for rehabilitation (Miceli et al., 2012).

Neuropsychological assessment in multilinguals suffers from several biases. The main bias is related to the socio-cultural background of multilingual speakers, especially in immigration contexts (Ardila et al., 1994; Fortuny and Mullaney, 1997; Gasquoine, 1999). Additional biases stem from the properties of testing tools: low scores in a test obtained by a person belonging to an ethnical/linguistic group different from that in which normative data were collected, may be due to reasons other than neurological or cognitive factors (Anastasi, 1988). Potentially unpredictable biases may depend on the unique characteristics of the individual, such as the number, type, and combination of the spoken languages.

Informal testing and translated test materials are frequently used in clinical settings and may be the best possible compromise when a balance between acceptability and adequacy is warranted. However, a crucial issue concerns the selection of the language for the neuropsychological assessment. This decision is strictly related to the linguistic profile of the patient, as s/he must be able to complete the clinical interview and to understand test instructions with as little difficulty as possible. Multilinguals who are equally proficient in all their languages collaborate without difficulty during interviews and testing, as their competence is not dissimilar from that of monolingual speakers. Under the same circumstances, however, so-called functional multilinguals, who use different languages depending on the context (e.g., at work vs. in the family) may face serious difficulties. This is frequently the case of newly arrived immigrants.

Intraoperative Testing

Language is by far the cognitive process tested most frequently in awake surgery for brain tumors. However, intraoperative tasks and/or batteries show an extreme variability across studies. Automatic speech (e.g., counting and reciting word series) and object naming, especially adaptations of the Test de Dénomination Orale D’Images (DO80, Deloche and Hannequin, 1997) and of the Boston Naming Test (BNT, Kaplan et al., 1983), are the most commonly used tasks. Standardized tests and many other tasks are also employed (see Rofes and Miceli, 2014; Ruis, 2018; Papatzalas et al., 2021, for recent reviews). In general, intraoperative paradigms must meet specific criteria that allow both to perform an accurate and sensitive language assessment and to minimize risks in the surgical procedure. Rofes and Miceli (2014) suggested that intraoperative tasks specific for language mapping should be adapted to different kind of constraints. Some constraints are imposed by the requirements of language mapping techniques: thus, tasks should be short, allow fast stimulus-response cycles and require simple responses that can be easily scored. Other constraints depend on the language under scrutiny: relevant language-specific properties should be tapped. Furthermore, clinical constraints require that tasks and stimuli should be sufficiently sensitive as to identify fine-grained deficits, should tap specific components of the language system and should be appropriately related to the brain areas associated with the assessed language processes.

These criteria are even more stringent when multilingual people must be assessed intraoperatively.

Recently, a multilingual naming task has been standardized in nine different languages (Spanish, Basque, Catalan, Italian, French, English, German, Mandarin Chinese, and Arabic) with the specific aim to minimize linguistic distance between different groups of items. It includes colored drawings of objects and actions; stimulus words are controlled for name agreement, frequency, length, and substitution neighbors. Depending on the language combination, the test includes between 25 and 30 items and can be administered in a maximum of 5 min per language (Gisbert-Muñoz et al., 2021).

Results

Patient Characteristics

Overall, the studies reviewed here included 127 multilingual patients with brain tumors who underwent awake surgery. The histology of the tumor was reported in 93 cases (56 gliomas, 6 metastases, and 31 other tumors). Lesions were predominantly located in the left hemisphere (LH) but nine cases with right hemisphere (RH) lesions were reported (see Table 1 and Figure 2).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Patient information: lesion site, number of languages spoken, sex, handedness, and aetiology.

Language Assessment

Number and Types of Studied Languages, Multilingual Profiles, and Language Distance/Similarity

The selected manuscripts investigated 31 languages and reported on different multilingual profiles. The majority of patients were bilingual (100), 15 were trilingual, 9 were quadrilingual, and 3 patients spoke five languages. A great heterogeneity in the number and type of language combinations was observed, ranging from very close (e.g., Spanish/Catalan; Mandarin Chinese/Cantonese Chinese) to very distant pairs (e.g., Arabic/French; Japanese/English).

In the reviewed manuscripts, the issue of language distance was either not considered (13 studies out of 22) or poorly addressed. A possible reason is that it is taken for granted that some language combinations have higher levels of mutual intelligibility than others and that the processing of a given language is influenced by the properties it shares with other languages (Jeong et al., 2007; Gooskens et al., 2018). In two studies (Sierpowska et al., 2013, 2018) special attention was paid to the selection of stimuli for the intraoperative task, where a possible confound due to high language similarity was avoided by excluding cognates. In one study (Fernández-Coello et al., 2017) languages were classified in different families but authors did not describe whether and how they exploited such information in surgical planning. Five studies (Roux and Trémoulet, 2002; Lubrano et al., 2004; Cervenka et al., 2011; Chan et al., 2019; Jain et al., 2019) reported only post hoc considerations on how across-language variations of different factors may have affected performance. One study (Połczyñska et al., 2016) explicitly investigated if language similarity (Swiss-German and German) was associated to similarity in neural representation and found this not to be the case (see Table 2).

Age of Acquisition

With one exception (Lubrano et al., 2004), all the selected studies reported data about the AoA of all the languages spoken by the patients (see Table 3). Most studies distinguished between early and late acquired languages but operationalized the variable differently; four studies only distinguished languages acquired during childhood vs. adulthood (Bilotta et al., 2011; Chan et al., 2019; Jain et al., 2019; de Macêdo Filho et al., 2020). The remaining studies either reported AoA or distinguished between early-acquired and late acquired multilingualism. In these cases, 7 and 5 years were the most used cut-offs between early and late AoA.

The variable was assessed differently across studies (see Figure 3). Wang et al. (2013) used a Bilingual History Questionnaire [BHQ, Li et al., 2006]. Two studies indicated the age at which patients started receiving formal education in languages other than L1 (Borius et al., 2012; Lubrano et al., 2012). Two studies (Roux and Trémoulet, 2002; Roux et al., 2004) used a qualitative classification of multilingualism where measures of AoA, proficiency and frequency of usage were collapsed into a unique score. The remaining studies (16 out of 22) collected information on AoA through patients’ or family reports.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Methods used to assess proficiency and age of acquisition.

The only study (Fernández-Coello et al., 2017) that focused specifically on how AoA influences the cortical organization of language in multilinguals undergoing awake surgery procedures for glioma resection reported more early-specific than late-specific cortical language sites, irrespective of the location of the stimulated area.

Proficiency

In our sample, three studies (Lubrano et al., 2004; Bilotta et al., 2011; de Macêdo Filho et al., 2020) did not report any information about how proficiency was assessed.

Two studies (Sierpowska et al., 2013; Jain et al., 2019) chose self-ratings on Likert-like scales to obtain information on language proficiency. In one case (Sierpowska et al., 2013) proficiency was evaluated also from a receptive point of view through the judgment provided by a certified translator.

Two studies (Roux and Trémoulet, 2002; Roux et al., 2004) used a qualitative measure of linguistic performance classification which collapsed AoA, proficiency and frequency of usage. In two studies (Wang et al., 2013; Gao et al., 2016) the proficiency in L2 was assessed by using the level achieved in the formal education of L2. Two studies (Walker et al., 2004; Fernández-Coello et al., 2017) used the scores obtained in naming tasks, while two (Bello et al., 2006; Lubrano et al., 2012) used the results of formal language assessment as indexes of proficiency in the different languages. The remaining nine studies used patient self-reports as the only indicator of proficiency in all the languages (see Figure 3).

Across the selected studies, additional information was occasionally provided about the amount of use/exposure and the context of use of the languages spoken by the patients. However, only qualitative information was provided, in the absence of objective measures. All these results are reported in Table 4. It is worth noting that only in one case (Lubrano et al., 2012) information on proficiency, context and amount of language use was considered in surgical planning, in order to decide which languages should be tested intraoperatively.

Preoperative Language Assessment

Of the studies considered in this manuscript, four did not describe the procedures used for language assessment (Bilotta et al., 2011; Wang et al., 2013; de Macêdo Filho et al., 2020; ReFaey et al., 2020). In one case (Pouratian et al., 2000) the authors reported that extensive testing was performed in all the languages of the patient but did not describe it. Six studies out of 22 directly compared performance accuracy in the languages spoken by the patient (Roux and Trémoulet, 2002; Lubrano et al., 2004; Roux et al., 2004; Bello et al., 2006; Borius et al., 2012; Fernández-Coello et al., 2017). In these studies, a variety of standardized tests was administered in all the relevant languages. When this was not feasible, ad hoc translations of the materials were used. Walker et al. (2004) used only a naming task. Połczyñska et al. (2016) assessed only one of the languages used most frequently by the patient (L3) via a composite test battery; for L1, L2, and L4 only a naming test was used. In two studies (Kin et al., 2013; Sierpowska et al., 2018) L1 was tested in a variety of tasks, while other languages were either not assessed at all (Kin et al., 2013), or were assessed only through the tasks that were going to be used intraoperatively (Sierpowska et al., 2018). In two studies (Cervenka et al., 2011; Sierpowska et al., 2013) authors only assessed the language for which standardized tests were available. In one of these studies (Cervenka et al., 2011) L1 was not assessed at all, while in the other (Sierpowska et al., 2013) it was assessed only through a picture naming task. Gao et al. (2015) tested word counting (from 1 to 100), reading aloud and naming in all the languages, while Gao et al. (2016) only tested word counting (from 1 to 100) and naming. Jain et al. (2019) administered the BAT and a naming task in the languages they studied. Chan et al. (2019) tested L2 by means of counting, naming and a semantic association task, and L1 and L2 only through a naming task that was planned to be used intraoperatively. Language assessment procedures are summarized in Table 5 and Figure 4.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Tests used in preoperative language assessment in each language.

In eight studies out of 22, pre-surgical language mapping methodologies were employed: Walker et al. (2004) used the WADA test to determine language laterality, while seven studies employed fMRI procedures (Pouratian et al., 2000; Bello et al., 2006; Gao et al., 2016; Połczyñska et al., 2016; Fernández-Coello et al., 2017; Sierpowska et al., 2018; ReFaey et al., 2020).

General Neuropsychological Assessment

In our sample, three studies (Pouratian et al., 2000; Walker et al., 2004; Bilotta et al., 2011) did not report details about the general neuropsychological assessment. The remaining studies provided very heterogeneous selections of tests in both pre and postoperative assessments (see Table 6).

Seven studies provided details on pre and postoperative neuropsychological assessments (Sierpowska et al., 2013, 2018; Gao et al., 2015; Fernández-Coello et al., 2017; Chan et al., 2019; Jain et al., 2019; ReFaey et al., 2020). Four studies did not report any information on postoperative assessment (Bello et al., 2006; Cervenka et al., 2011; Wang et al., 2013; Połczyñska et al., 2016). In seven studies the postoperative assessment was restricted to language (Roux and Trémoulet, 2002; Lubrano et al., 2004; 2012; Roux et al., 2004; Borius et al., 2012; Kin et al., 2013; Gao et al., 2016). In one study (de Macêdo Filho et al., 2020) a complete neurologic examination and the language assessment were performed only after surgery but no information was provided.

The procedures adopted for preoperative assessment in the reviewed manuscripts can be thus summarized. Two studies (Wang et al., 2013; ReFaey et al., 2020) reported only the Karnofsky Performance Scale (KPS, Karnofsky and Burchenal, 1949). Three studies (Roux and Trémoulet, 2002; Lubrano et al., 2004; Roux et al., 2004) reported the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (EHI, Oldfield, 1971). Seven studies used the EHI and additional tests for apraxia, working memory and global screening scales (Bello et al., 2006; Lubrano et al., 2012; Sierpowska et al., 2013, 2018; Gao et al., 2015, 2016; Fernández-Coello et al., 2017). Three studies provided global screening scales and a few additional tests (Cervenka et al., 2011; Kin et al., 2013; Jain et al., 2019). Połczyñska et al. (2016) assessed attention, working memory, and verbal executive abilities but did not report the employed tests. Chan et al. (2019) administered a composite battery for neuropsychological assessment.

Seven studies out of 17 provided additional information about the language selected for neuropsychological assessment: Sierpowska et al. (2018) performed neuropsychological assessment in L1; Gao et al. (2015) and Jain et al. (2019) in both L1 and L2; Cervenka et al. (2011), Sierpowska et al. (2013), Połczyñska et al. (2016), and Chan et al. (2019) used the language spoken most frequently by their patients and for which standardized tests were available.

Intraoperative Tasks

A variety of tasks and of task combinations were employed for intraoperative testing in the studies reviewed here: seven studies used one task, six studies used two tasks, and nine studies used three tasks (see Table 5 and Figure 5).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Number and type of intraoperative tasks.

A picture naming task was used in all the studies. Number and type of stimuli varied (objects, actions, and famous faces) as well as the naming context (single word vs. short sentence (“This is a…”). Six studies used a counting task (Roux and Trémoulet, 2002; Bilotta et al., 2011; Gao et al., 2015, 2016; Chan et al., 2019; Jain et al., 2019), three studies used a sentence reading task (Roux and Trémoulet, 2002; Lubrano et al., 2004; Borius et al., 2012), two studies a word reading task (Gao et al., 2015, 2016), three studies used a specific language switching task (Wang et al., 2013; Sierpowska et al., 2013, 2018), and two studies (Chan et al., 2019; de Macêdo Filho et al., 2020) used the Pyramids and Palm Trees Test (PPT, Howard and Patterson, 1992) in order to test the patient’s ability to access meaning from words. The following tasks were also employed: writing to dictation, translation form L2 to L1, naming orally described objects, word comprehension, repetition (see Table 5 for details). In addition to linguistic tasks, one study (Jain et al., 2019) included a mentalizing test in the intraoperative protocol (The reading the mind in the eyes test (RME, Baron-Cohen, 1997); however, the test was only attempted but not concluded by the patient during the surgical session.

None of the selected studies reported on how stimuli were matched across languages, nor provided information on the specialty and linguistic competence of the clinician who conducted the linguistic evaluations. The tasks described in these studies tapped different aspects of the functional architecture of language, but authors did not specify the criterion followed in task selection, except for the studies focusing on voluntary language-switching6 (Sierpowska et al., 2013, 2018; Wang et al., 2013).

Discussion

This manuscript aimed at describing the state of the art in the perioperative language assessment of multilingual patients undergoing awake surgery for brain tumor. Twenty-two studies, published over the last 30 years, were reviewed. Special attention was devoted to the procedures employed to describe the patients’ multilingual profiles for their crucial role in determining the neuroanatomical organization of multiple languages and effects on cognitive functioning (Cargnelutti et al., 2019; Połczyñska and Bookheimer, 2020). Among the linguistic experience-related factors, AoA and proficiency were analyzed. Almost all the reviewed studies provided scores for those variables but assessed them differently. Noteworthy, no strong statement was reported about whether and to what extent AoA and proficiency scores helped planning intraoperative procedures (e.g., selecting languages, tasks, stimuli, and stimulation sites) nor if they had an impact on the outcome of surgery. This finding alone shows that information on AoA and proficiency has not been properly used to shed light on the cerebral organization of multiple languages. Such a bias could be neutralized if multilingual patients eligible for awake surgery were systematically questioned to obtain objective measures of their multilingualism. As for multilingualism history, the following data should be collected: AoA of L1 and of other languages; setting in which languages were acquired/learned; primary language used in school education; formal education received in each language; global amount of exposure to each language. Where and how languages are used should be ascertained through questions about context (familiar, social, and professional), linguistic profiles of interlocutors (native vs. non-native speakers), modality (spoken, written, formal, and informal), language-related media preferences (television, radio, newspaper, and internet), and frequency of use of each language in each modality in recent months.

Proficiency should be assessed preferably through subjective and objective ratings along several dimensions: proficiency in different contexts, modalities and linguistic domain, perceived accent in different languages, probability of spontaneous language switching, cross-linguistic flexibility, amount of engagement in translation activity, skills associated with effective communication, and family/friends and patient’s sense of impairment in the different languages. It is worth to underline here that all these variables related to multilingualism should be operationalized and treated comparably in awake surgery settings, in order to obtain reliable findings that could be additionally supported by formal statistical analyses in cross-linguistic studies. This might significantly improve the understanding of cerebral organization of multiple languages.

A precise and objective description of the patient’s multilingual profile should be efficiently used also for the general neuropsychological assessment. So far, seven studies reviewed in this manuscript specifically addressed this issue and reported that language used for testing was carefully chosen. Language selection depended on the availability of standardized neuropsychological tools in the language most frequently used by the patient at the moment of surgery. Such an approach should be encouraged as it prevents misinterpretation deriving from patients’ unbalanced proficiency or mastery of one language over the others, which may in turn reduce compliance with the evaluation setting, produce inaccurate comprehension of task requirements and, consequently, induce unreliable performances in neuropsychological tests (Rosselli et al., 2002; Gasquoine et al., 2007; Bender, 2015). Obviously, when the linguistic competence of the neuropsychologist is not sufficient to conduct the evaluation in the selected language, the support of (psycho)linguists and interpreters is needed. This is often difficult to afford, but it holds the obvious advantage that it allows collecting reliable information on the cognitive status of the patient.

A final aspect of the general neuropsychological assessment is worth considering. Given that the requirement of well-matched normative data is unlikely to be ever met due to the heterogeneity and variability of multilingual populations, an effective approach would consist in relying less on quantitative information (comparison of the patient’s score with that of a normative sample) and more on qualitative information about the patient’s performance in various tests.

A further crucial feature addressed in this review is the type of preoperative language assessment to be used for all the languages spoken by multilingual patients. The reviewed manuscripts showed great variability with respect to this dimension but, due to the lack of standardized multilingual tests, they did not prevent the possibility that languages may be accidentally assessed by non-equivalent modalities and at different levels of difficulty. The risk here is to underestimate or overestimate language difficulties in one language over the others, and consequently to miss the specific goal of intraoperative testing. Most studies reviewed in this manuscript suggest that, in principle, all languages should be assessed across functions (reading, writing, repetition of words and non-words, comprehension and production of words and sentences) and domains (lexicon, semantics, phonology, grammar, morphology, and syntax). In those manuscripts, the use of translated materials is common but only few details are provided about the implementation of the tasks and of the lists of stimuli.

Noteworthy, caveats should be considered. Items and tasks selected for language assessment must respect the culture standards of the languages under scrutiny; thus, culturally biased items should be avoided (Luke et al., 2002; Cheung et al., 2006). When translated from one language into another, test items should also undergo an independent back-translation in order to avoid phenomena of lexical ambiguity or synonymy. Conversely, when translation does not achieve the purpose of obtaining well-matched testing materials across different languages, additional criteria should be respected. Some examples may help clarify this point. The English verb ‘‘to knit’’ can be translated to Italian by using the multi-word expression ‘‘lavorare a maglia.’’ Since the two items are not equivalent on a lexical ground, they should be replaced by alternative pairs. In other words, it is not necessary to include the same items in all the languages under scrutiny but, rather, it is recommended that the words used in each language be matched for the main variables that affect linguistic processing (length, phonological complexity, frequency, AoA, imageability, grammatical class, semantic category, syntactic features, and morphological structure).7 This allows a good control on the cross-linguistic difficulty of tasks. Similarly, in comprehension tasks (e.g., word/picture matching or verification), the selected stimuli should be associated with appropriate semantic and phonological foils. Semantic foils usually do not suffer from translation biases, but phonological foils do. For instance, in Italian, “sarta” (seamstress) is a good phonological distractor for “carta” (manuscript); the same pair does not work when translated to English and should be changed by an equivalent pair (e.g., “boy/toy”). Again, language-specific critical features (e.g., presence/absence of case-marking, specific morphological rules, word-order constraints, and pro-drop patterns, etc.) might preclude the possibility to build perfectly matched lists of materials. For instance, the sentence “mangio la mela” is not fully equivalent to its English version “I eat the apple.” Italian is a pro-drop language where independent clauses may lack an explicit subject/pronoun since it is grammatically inferable by verbal inflection that, in turns, provides information on person and number. In English, an explicit subject is normally needed in sentence structure. Moreover, in Italian, in order to select the appropriate determiner for “mela,” speakers must retrieve information about grammatical gender and perform an operation of determiner + noun agreement while English speakers do not. This also holds for naming tasks when a minimal sentential context is required: “This is the boy,” noun + determiner (gender) agreement not required, vs. “Questo è il ragazzo,” noun + determiner (gender) agreement required. These few examples are a strong reminder that the specific properties of each language under evaluation should be carefully considered so that their distance and similarities are clearly and “objectively” defined. This is indispensable for a reliable language assessment in multilinguals as shared properties impact their language-specific cerebral organization (Połczyñska and Bookheimer, 2020). Nevertheless, the present review shows that this problem has been almost totally neglected in multilingual awake surgery settings.

Similar considerations hold for materials to be included in the tasks for intraoperative testing. In addition, in this latter case, the issue of how to overcome the problem of language-specific properties intersects other critical concerns. The goal of language testing during awake surgery in multilinguals is to find shared/distinct areas and networks related to different languages, so as to minimize the likelihood of postoperative (multi)linguistic disorders. On the other hand, it is necessary to find an optimal trade-off between the duration of the intraoperative testing and the neurosurgical procedure (Mandonnet et al., 2020). Thus, the number of languages to be tested and the range of intraoperative linguistic tasks must comply both with the time constraints and with the (multi)linguistic needs of the patient. The patient should be asked which language is most important to him/her and in which language(s) s/he would like to be tested during surgery. On the other hand, s/he should feel comfortable and not overwhelmed throughout the operation and should be aware of the benefits and risks of testing or not all his/her languages. In the reviewed studies, 20 surgical teams out of 22 tested all languages and agree that the ideal testing should include all the languages or at least all the most relevant spoken by the patient. However, they used different (combinations of) tasks and did not report on how they dealt with the problems linked to direct comparison between languages, language distance, and lack of standardized tools.

An accurate analysis of the linguistic behavior of the patient is crucial to optimize intraoperative procedures and, consequently, to evaluate postoperative outcomes. When standardized multilingual instruments are not available, the preparation of sufficiently specific and sensitive patient-tailored intraoperative testing should include the following steps. A picture naming task should be included, since tasks of this type have been extensively employed in awake surgery and meet the main requirements of the stimulation setting: fast presentation, easy scoring, and good patient compliance (Miceli et al., 2012; De Witte and Mariën, 2013). Nouns (objects) and finite verbs (actions) inserted in a minimal phrasal context (“This is the….”; “He/she/it ….”) should be selected as stimuli for each language. This paradigm affords the opportunity to tap semantic processing and lexical retrieval while manipulating and keeping under control the main language-specific features, e.g., morphology (nominal and verbal inflection) and syntax (determiner + noun agreement; subject + verb agreement). A third task should be dedicated either to specific properties of the languages spoken by the patients (e.g., relevant differences in the orthography) or to other language abilities (e.g., language switching or cross-linguistic translation) relevant for the linguistic needs and quality of life of patients (e.g., simultaneous translators, people living in multilingual countries like the Basque Country, Singapore, Switzerland, Italian autonomous provinces). When possible, the materials to be used in the intraoperative assessment of a multilingual patient should be tested on control groups composed of healthy speakers from the same environment (family members, friends, and multilingual speakers with similar linguistic profiles) in which reaction times and response accuracy are in principle roughly comparable with those of the patient. A summary of recommended perioperative assessment procedures to be used in awake neurosurgery settings with multilingual patients is provided in Table 7.

TABLE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Table 7. Recommended perioperative assessment procedures in multilingual awake neurosurgery settings.

Concluding Remarks

There is a critical need for a structured, theory-driven and evidence-based approach to multilingual patients in several clinical settings (neurological, neuropsychological, psychological, neurosurgical, and rehabilitative) since the number of people who use more than one language in everyday life is steadily increasing. For multilingual patients with tumors in language areas, awake brain surgery is used ever more often, as it allows maximizing the extent of resection while minimizing the functional risk.

However, this review shows that there is no consensus on the rationale that should underlie the selection of the neuropsychological tests to be included in the preoperative clinical work-up, and of the language paradigms to be used during language mapping procedures. The patient-tailored approach for perioperative assessment is still, necessarily, the preferred method due to the impossibility to predict all the combinations of languages spoken by multilinguals and to the difficulty in matching language-specific properties. The lack of such criteria may have serious implications and weaken the potential clinical benefit of awake surgery. For example, it could induce biases in deciding whether the outcome of intraoperative stimulation is due to interference with linguistic knowledge shared by all languages or specific for the language tested during stimulation. Moreover, given that neuroanatomical findings are highly inconsistent across studies, the present review highlights that the outcome of the neurosurgical procedure relies on an accurate planning of preoperative and intraoperative testing. Especially, this review illustrates the relevance of an objective and accurate description of both the linguistic profile of multilingual patients and the specific properties of the languages under scrutiny.

Author Contributions

MD, AT, and GM contributed to conception and design of the study. MD and AM collected the data. MD, RC, and GM analyzed and interpreted the data. MD wrote the first draft of the manuscript. GM revised the text. GM and AT supervised the study. All authors discussed the results, contributed, and approved the final version of the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Footnotes

  1. ^ For the sake of simplicity, in this manuscript we consider bilingualism (and literature on bilingualism) as an instance of multilingualism.
  2. ^ In this manuscript the term language-specific designates mechanisms and properties associated with each language spoken by a multilingual individual.
  3. ^ Data about monolingual patients or about patients with brain pathologies other than tumors, although they were included in the selected manuscripts, were not considered in this review.
  4. ^ Cognate words are pairs that are phonologically similar in the translation from one language to another (e.g., origin in English and origine, in Italian).
  5. ^ An extensive bilingual battery that provides more than 30 tasks matched across various language combinations is available for the evaluation of aphasic disorders: BAT (Bilingual Aphasia Test, https://www.mcgill.ca/linguistics/research/bat). When the language combination of a brain tumor patient corresponds to those included in the BAT (e.g., Italian-Slovenian or Italian-Spanish), its tests could be adapted and profitably used.
  6. ^ Involuntary language-switching phenomena induced by electrocortical stimulation are described in Tomasino et al. (2014).
  7. ^ Several databases can be used to obtain distributional measures in different languages (a list of some existing materials can be found at http://crr.ugent.be/archives/2141).

References

Abutalebi, J., Della Rosa, P. A., Ding, G., Weekes, B., Costa, A., and Green, D. W. (2013). Language proficiency modulates the engagement of cognitive control areas in multilinguals. Cortex 49, 905–911. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.08.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Abutalebi, J., and Green, D. (2007). Bilingual language production: the neurocognition of language representation and control. J. Neurolinguistics. 20, 242–275. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2006.10.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aglioti, S., and Fabbro, F. (1993). Paradoxical selective recovery in a bilingual aphasic following subcortical lesions. Neuroreport 4, 1359–1362. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199309150-00019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Albert, M. L., and Obler, L. K. (1978). The Bilingual Brain: Neuropsychological and Neurolinguistic Aspects of Bilingualism. Perspectives in Neurolinguistics and Psycholinguistics. New York, NY: Academic Press Inc.

Google Scholar

Anastasi, A. (1988). Psychological Testing, 6th Edn. New York, NY: Macmillan.

Google Scholar

Antoniou, M., and Wright, S. M. (2017). Uncovering the mechanisms responsible for why language learning may promote healthy cognitive aging. Front. Psychol. 8:2217. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02217

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ardila, A., Rosselli, M., and Puente, A. E. (1994). Neuropsychological Evaluation of the Spanish Speaker. Boston: Springer. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4899-1453-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aronin, L., and Singleton, D. (2008). Multilingualism as a new linguistic dispensation. Int. J. Multiling 5, 1–16. doi: 10.2167/ijm072.0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baron-Cohen, S. (1997). Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test. Available online at: http://www.as-tt.de/assets/applets/Augentest_Erwachsene (accessed May 04, 2014)

Google Scholar

Bassetti, B. (2008). ““Orthographic input and second language phonology”,” in Input Matters in SLA, eds T. Piske and M. Young-Scholten (Bristol: Multilingual Matters), 191–206. doi: 10.21832/9781847691118-013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baumgart, C. Q., and Billick, S. B. (2018). Positive cognitive effects of bilingualism and multilingualism on cerebral function: a review. Psychiatr. Q. 89, 273–283. doi: 10.1007/s11126-017-9532-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bello, L., Acerbi, F., Giussani, C., Baratta, P., Taccone, P., and Songa, V. (2006). Intraoperative language localization in multilingual patients with gliomas. Neurosurgery 59, 115–125. doi: 10.1227/01.NEU.0000219241.92246.FB

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bender, H. A. (2015). “The neuropsychological assessment of culturally and linguistically diverse epilepsy patients,” in Handbook on the Neuropsychology of Epilepsy, eds W. B. Barr and C. Morrison (New York, NY: Springer), 317–344. doi: 10.1007/978-0-387-92826-5_15

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bhatia, T. K., and Ritchie, W. C. (eds) (2006). The Handbook of Bilingualism. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing Ltd.

Google Scholar

Bilotta, F., Stazi, E., Delfini, R., and Rosa, G. (2011). Language testing during awake “anesthesia” in a bilingual patient with brain lesion adjacent to Wernicke’s area. Anesth. Analg. 112, 938–939. doi: 10.1213/ANE.0b013e31820bd1a4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Birdsong, D. (2014). Dominance and age in bilingualism. Appl. Linguist. 35, 374–392. doi: 10.1093/applin/amu031

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Birdsong, D. (ed.) (1999). Second Language Acquisition and the Critical Period Hypothesis. New York, NY: Routledge. doi: 10.4324/9781410601667

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borius, P. Y., Giussani, C., Draper, L., and Roux, F. E. (2012). Sentence translation in proficient bilinguals: a direct electrostimulation brain mapping. Cortex 48, 614–622. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2011.01.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Butler, Y. G. (2013). “Bilingualism/multilingualism and second-language acquisition,” in The Handbook of Bilingualism and Multilingualism, eds T. K. Bhatia and W. C. Ritchie (Oxford: Blackwell Publishing Ltd), 109–136. doi: 10.1002/9781118332382.ch5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Calabria, M., Hernández, M., Cattaneo, G., Suades, A., Serra, M., Juncadella, M., et al. (2020). Active bilingualism delays the onset of mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychologia 146:107528. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2020.107528

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cargnelutti, E., Tomasino, B., and Fabbro, F. (2019). Language brain representation in bilinguals with different age of appropriation and proficiency of the second language: a meta-analysis of functional imaging studies. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 13:154. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00154

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cenoz, J., Hufeisen, B., and Jessner, U. (2001). Towards trilingual education. Int. J. Biling. Educ. Biling 4, 1–10. doi: 10.1080/13670050108667714

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cervenka, M. C., Boatman-Reich, D., Ward, J., Franaszczuk, P. J., and Crone, N. (2011). Language mapping in multilingual patients: electrocorticography and cortical stimulation during naming. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 5:13. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chan, H. M., Loh, W. N. H., Yeo, T. T., and Teo, K. (2019). Awake craniotomy and excision of a diffuse low-grade glioma in a multilingual patient: neuropsychology and language. World Neurosurg. 128, 91–97. doi: 10.1016/j.wneu.2019.04.181

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheung, M. C., Chan, A. S., Chan, Y. L., and Lam, J. M. (2006). Language lateralization of Chinese-English bilingual patients with temporal lobe epilepsy: a functional MRI study. Neuropsychology 20:589. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.20.5.589

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Consonni, M., Cafiero, R., Marin, D., Tettamanti, M., Iadanza, A., Fabbro, F., et al. (2013). Neural convergence for language comprehension and grammatical class production in highly proficient bilinguals is independent of age of acquisition. Cortex 49, 1252–1258. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.04.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Bot, K. (2004). The multilingual lexicon: modelling selection and control. Int. J. Multiling 1, 17–32. doi: 10.1080/14790710408668176

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Macêdo Filho, L. J., Aguiar, G. C., Pessoa, F. C., Diógenes, G. S., Borges, F. S., Joaquim, A. F., et al. (2020). Intraparenchymal epidermoid cyst close to broca area-awake craniotomy and gross total resection. World Neurosurg. 141, 367–372. doi: 10.1016/j.wneu.2020.06.135

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Witte, E., and Mariën, P. (2013). The neurolinguistic approach to awake surgery reviewed. Clin. Neurol. Neurosurg. 115, 127–145. doi: 10.1016/j.clineuro.2012.09.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Del Maschio, N., and Abutalebi, J. (2019). “Language organization in the bilingual and multilingual brain,” in The Handbook of the Neuroscience of Multilingualism, eds J. W. Schwieter and M. Paradis (Hoboken: John Wiley and sons), 197–213. doi: 10.1002/9781119387725.ch9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deloche, G., and Hannequin, D. (1997). Test de Dénomination Orale D’images (DO 80). Paris: Ed. du Centre de Psychologie Appliquée.

Google Scholar

Dimmendaal, G. (1995). “Do some languages have a multi-genetic or non-genetic origin? an exercise in taxonomy,” in Proceedings of the 5th Nilo-Saharan Conference, Nice 1992, eds R. Nicolai and F. Rottland (Keulen: Rüdiger Koppe), 354–369.

Google Scholar

EuroStat (2015). Foreign Language Skills Statistics. Statistics Explained. Luxembourg: EuroStat.

Google Scholar

Fabbro, F. (1998). Bilingual aphasia research is not a tabula rasa. Aphasiology 12, 138–141. doi: 10.1080/02687039808250470

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fabbro, F. (2001). The bilingual brain: cerebral representation of languages. Brain Lang. 79, 211–222. doi: 10.1006/brln.2001.2481

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernández-Coello, A., Gil-Robles, S., and Carreiras, M. (2021). “Multilingual naming,” in Intraoperative Mapping of Cognitive Networks, eds E. Mandonnet and G. Herbert (New York, NY: Springer), 219–231. doi: 10.1007/978-3-030-75071-8_14

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernández-Coello, A., Havas, V., Juncadella, M., Sierpowska, J., Rodríguez-Fornells, A., and Gabarrós, A. (2017). Age of language acquisition and cortical language organization in multilingual patients undergoing awake brain mapping. J. Neurosurg. 126, 1912–1923. doi: 10.3171/2016.5.JNS152791

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fortuny, L. A. I., and Mullaney, H. A. (1997). Neuropsychology with Spanish speakers: language use and proficiency issues for test development. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 19, 615–622. doi: 10.1080/01688639708403747

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Francis, W. S. (2005). Bilingual Semantic and Conceptual Representation. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Frenck-Mestre, C., Anton, J. L., Roth, M., Vaid, J., and Viallet, F. (2005). Articulation in early and late bilinguals’ two languages: evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging. Neuroreport 16, 761–765. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200505120-00021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gamallo, P., Pichel, J. R., and Alegria, I. (2017). From language identification to language distance. Physica A 484, 152–162. doi: 10.1016/j.physa.2017.05.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gao, H., Bai, H., Han, L., Li, T., Wang, W., Liu, Y., et al. (2016). Language-associated cortical regions in non-proficient Chinese-English bilinguals with glioma. J. Neurolinguist. 39, 49–56. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2016.01.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gao, H., Bai, H. M., Han, L. X., Li, T. D., Wang, G. L., and Wang, W. M. (2015). Brain cancer surgery in the language areas of Mandarin-Cantonese bilinguals. J. Cancer Res. Ther. 11:415. doi: 10.4103/0973-1482.151932

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

García-Pentón, L., Fernandez Garcia, Y., Costello, B., Duñabeitia, J. A., and Carreiras, M. (2016). The neuroanatomy of bilingualism: how to turn a hazy view into the full picture. Lang. Cogn. Neurosci. 31, 303–327. doi: 10.1080/23273798.2015.1068944

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gasquoine, P. G. (1999). Variables moderating cultural and ethnic differences in neuropsychological assessment: the case of Hispanic Americans. Clin. Neuropsychol. 13, 376–383. doi: 10.1076/clin.13.3.376.1735

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gasquoine, P. G., Croyle, K. L., Cavazos-Gonzalez, C., and Sandoval, O. (2007). Language of administration and neuropsychological test performance in neurologically intact Hispanic American bilingual adults. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 22, 991–1001. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2007.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ghazi-Saidi, L., and Ansaldo, A. I. (2017). The neural correlates of semantic and phonological transfer effects: language distance matters. Biling. (Camb. Engl.) 20, 1080–1094. doi: 10.1017/S136672891600064

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gisbert-Muñoz, S., Quiñones, I., Amoruso, L., Timofeeva, P., Geng, S., Boudelaa, S., et al. (2021). MULTIMAP: multilingual picture naming test for mapping eloquent areas during awake surgeries. Behav. Res. Methods 53, 918–927. doi: 10.3758/s13428-020-01467-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giussani, C., Roux, F. E., Lubrano, V., Gaini, S. M., and Bello, L. (2007). Review of language organisation in bilingual patients: what can we learn from direct brain mapping? Acta Neurochir. 149, 1109–1116. doi: 10.1007/s00701-007-1266-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gleitman, L. R. (1985). Orthographic resources affect reading acquisition—If they are used. Remedial. Spec. Educ. 6, 24–36. doi: 10.1177/074193258500600606

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gooskens, C., van Heuven, V. J., Goluboviæ, J., Schüppert, A., Swarte, F., and Voigt, S. (2018). Mutual intelligibility between closely related languages in Europe. Int. J. Multilin 15, 169–193. doi: 10.1080/14790718.2017.1350185

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goral, M., and Conner, P. S. (2013). Language disorders in multilingual and multicultural populations. Annu. Rev. Appl. Linguist. 33, 128–161. doi: 10.1017/S026719051300010X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Green, D. W. (2003). “The neural basis of the lexicon and the grammar,” in L2 Acquisition. The Lexicon-syntax Interface in Second Language Acquisition, eds R. van Hout, A. Hulk, F. Kuiken, and R. J. Towell (Amsterdam: John Benjamins), 197–208. doi: 10.1075/lald.30.10gre

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grosjean, F. (1992). Another view of bilingualism. Adv. Psychol. 83, 51–62. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4115(08)61487-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grosjean, F. (2013). “Bilingualism: a short introduction,” in The Psycholinguistics of Bilingualism, eds P. Li and F. Grosjean (Hoboken: John Wiley), 5–25.

Google Scholar

Hartsuiker, R. J., Beerts, S., Loncke, M., Desmet, T., and Bernolet, S. (2016). Cross-linguistic structural priming in multilinguals: further evidence for shared syntax. J. Mem. Lan. 90, 14–30. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2016.03.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hartsuiker, R. J., Pickering, M. J., and Veltkamp, E. (2004). Is syntax separate or shared between languages? Cross-linguistic syntactic priming in Spanish-English bilinguals. Psychol. Sci. 15, 409–414. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00693.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hernandez, A. E., Hofmann, J., and Kotz, S. A. (2007). Age of acquisition modulates neural activity for both regular and irregular syntactic functions. Neuroimage 36, 912–923. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.02.055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Howard, D., and Patterson, K. E. (1992). The Pyramids and Palm Trees Test. Bury St Edmunds: Thames Val Test Co Ltd.

Google Scholar

Isel, F., Baumgaertner, A., Thrän, J., Meisel, J. M., and Büchel, C. (2010). Neural circuitry of the bilingual mental lexicon: effect of age of second language acquisition. Brain Cogn. 72, 169–180. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2009.07.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jain, S., Chan, H. M., Yeo, T. T., and Teo, K. (2019). Language mapping of hindi and english in a bilingual patient during resection of a right frontal glioma. World Neurosurg. 125, 106–110. doi: 10.1016/j.wneu.2019.01.153

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jeong, H., Sugiura, M., Sassa, Y., Yokoyama, S., Horie, K., Sato, S., et al. (2007). Cross-linguistic influence on brain activation during second language processing: an fMRI study. Biling. (Camb. Engl.) 10, 175–187. doi: 10.1017/S1366728907002921

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, J. S., and Newport, E. L. (1989). Critical period effects in second language learning: the influence of maturational state on the acquisition of English as a second language. Cogn. Psychol. 21, 60–99. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(89)90003-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaplan, E., Goodglass, H., and Weintraub, S. (1983). The Boston Naming Test. Philadelphia, PA: Lea and Febiger.

Google Scholar

Karnofsky, D. A., and Burchenal, J. H. (1949). “The clinical evaluation of chemotherapeutic agents in cancer,” in Evaluation of Chemotherapeutic Agents, ed. C. M. MacLeod (New York, NY: Columbia University Press), 199–205.

Google Scholar

Kim, K. H., Relkin, N. R., Lee, K. M., and Hirsch, J. (1997). Distinct cortical areas associated with native and second languages. Nature 388, 171–174. doi: 10.1038/40623

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, S. Y., Qi, T., Feng, X., Ding, G., Liu, L., and Cao, F. (2016). How does language distance between L1 and L2 affect the L2 brain network? An fMRI study of Korean–Chinese–English trilinguals. Neuroimage 129, 25–39. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.11.068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kin, H., Ishikawa, E., Takano, S., Ayuzawa, S., Matsushita, A., Muragaki, Y., et al. (2013). Language areas involving the inferior temporal cortex on intraoperative mapping in a bilingual patient with glioblastoma-case report. Neurol. Med. Chir. (Tokyo) 53, 256–258. doi: 10.2176/nmc.53.256

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klein, D., Mok, K., Chen, J. K., and Watkins, K. E. (2014). Age of language learning shapes brain structure: a cortical thickness study of bilingual and monolingual individuals. Brain Lang. 131, 20–24. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2013.05.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koda, K. (2005). Insights into Second Language Reading: A Cross-linguistic Approach. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, doi: 10.1017/CBO9781139524841

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kotz, S. A. (2009). A critical review of ERP and fMRI evidence on L2 syntactic processing. Brain Lang. 109, 68–74. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2008.06.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kroll, J. F., and Bialystok, E. (2013). Understanding the consequences of bilingualism for language processing and cognition. J. Cogn. Psychol. 25, 497–514. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2013.799170

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kroll, J. F., and Ma, F. (2018). The Bilingual Lexicon. Hoboken: Wiley Blackwell, doi: 10.1002/9781118829516.ch13

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leonard, M. K., Torres, C., Travis, K. E., Brown, T. T., Hagler, D. J. Jr., Dale, A. M., et al. (2011). Language proficiency modulates the recruitment of non-classical language areas in bilinguals. PLoS One 6:e18240. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0018240

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, P., Sepanski, S., and Zhao, X. (2006). Language history questionnaire: a web-based interface for bilingual research. Behav. Res. Methods 38, 202–210. doi: 10.3758/BF03192770

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, H., and Cao, F. (2016). L1 and L2 processing in the bilingual brain: a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Brain Lang. 159, 60–73. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2016.05.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lubrano, V., Prod’homme, K., Démonet, J. F., and Köpke, B. (2012). Language monitoring in multilingual patients undergoing awake craniotomy: a case study of a German-English-French trilingual patient with a WHO grade II glioma. J. Neurolinguistics. 25, 567–578. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2011.08.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lubrano, V., Roux, F. E., and Démonet, J. F. (2004). Writing-specific sites in frontal areas: a cortical stimulation study. J. Neurosurg. 101, 787–798. doi: 10.3171/jns.2004.101.5.0787

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luk, G., Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I., and Grady, C. L. (2011). Lifelong bilingualism maintains white matter integrity in older adults. J. Neurosci. 31, 16808–16813. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4563-11.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luke, K. K., Liu, H. L., Wai, Y. Y., Wan, Y. L., and Tan, L. H. (2002). Functional anatomy of syntactic and semantic processing in language comprehension. Hum. Brain Mapp. 16, 133–145. doi: 10.1002/hbm.10029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mahendra, N., Plante, E., Magloire, J., Milman, L., and Trouard, T. P. (2003). fMRI variability and the localization of languages in the bilingual brain. NeuroReport 14, 1225–1228. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200307010-00007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mandonnet, E., Herbet, G., and Duffau, H. (2020). Introducing new tasks for intraoperative mapping in awake glioma surgery: clearing the line between patient care and scientific research. Neurosurgery 86, E256–E257. doi: 10.1093/neuros/nyz447

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mechelli, A., Crinion, J. T., Noppeney, U., O’Doherty, J., Ashburner, J., Frackowiak, R. S., et al. (2004). Structural plasticity in the bilingual brain. Nature 431, 757–757. doi: 10.1038/431757a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miceli, G., Capasso, R., Monti, A., Santini, B., and Talacchi, A. (2012). Language testing in brain tumor patients. J. Neurooncol. 108, 247–252. doi: 10.1007/s11060-012-0810-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Odlin, T. (1989). Language Transfer: Cross-linguistic Influence in Language Learning. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, doi: 10.2307/3587183

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ojemann, G. A., and Whitaker, H. A. (1978). The bilingual brain. Arch. Neurol. 35, 409–412. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1978.00500310011002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9, 97–113. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Papatzalas, C., Fountas, K., Kapsalaki, E., and Papathanasiou, I. (2021). The use of standardized intraoperative language tests in awake craniotomies: a scoping review. Neuropsychol. Rev. doi: 10.1007/s11065-021-09492-6 Online ahead of print.

CrossRef Full Text | PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Paradis, M. (2000). Cerebral representation of bilingual concepts. Biling (Camb. Engl.) 3, 22–24. doi: 10.1017/S1366728900270119

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paradis, M. (2004). A Neurolinguistic Theory of Bilingualism. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing. doi: 10.1075/sibil.18

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peeters, D., Dijkstra, T., and Grainger, J. (2013). The representation and processing of identical cognates by late bilinguals: RT and ERP effects. J. Mem. Lang. 68, 315–332. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2012.12.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perani, D., Abutalebi, J., Paulesu, E., Brambati, S., Scifo, P., Cappa, S. F., et al. (2003). The role of age of acquisition and language usage in early, high-proficient bilinguals: an fMRI study during verbal fluency. Hum. Brain Mapp. 19, 170–182. doi: 10.1002/hbm.10110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perani, D., Dehaene, S., Grassi, F., Cohen, L., Cappa, S. F., Dupoux, E., et al. (1996). Brain processing of native and foreign languages. Neuroreport 7, 2439–2444. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199611040-00007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perani, D., Paulesu, E., Galles, N. S., Dupoux, E., Dehaene, S., Bettinardi, V., et al. (1998). The bilingual brain. proficiency and age of acquisition of the second language. Brain 121, 1841–1852. doi: 10.1093/brain/121.10.1841

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Połczyñska, M. M., Benjamin, C. F., Japardi, K., Frew, A., and Bookheimer, S. Y. (2016). Language system organization in a quadrilingual with a brain tumor: implications for understanding of the language network. Neuropsychologia 86, 167–175. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.04.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Połczyñska, M. M., and Bookheimer, S. Y. (2020). Factors modifying the amount of neuroanatomical overlap between languages in Bilinguals—a systematic review of neurosurgical language mapping studies. Brain Sci. 10, 983. doi: 10.3390/brainsci10120983

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pouratian, N., Bookheimer, S. Y. O’Farrell, A. M., Sicotte, N. L., Cannestra, A. F., Becker, D., et al. (2000). Optical imaging of bilingual cortical representations: Case report. J. Neurosurg. 93, 676–681. doi: 10.3171/jns.2000.93.4.0676

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

ReFaey, K., Tripathi, S., Bhargav, A. G., Grewal, S. S., Middlebrooks, E. H., Sabsevitz, D. S., et al. (2020). Potential differences between monolingual and bilingual patients in approach and outcome after awake brain surgery. J. Neuroonc. 148, 587–598. doi: 10.1007/s11060-020-03554-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rofes, A., and Miceli, G. (2014). Language mapping with verbs and sentences in awake surgery: a review. Neuropsychol. Rev. 24, 185–199. doi: 10.1007/s11065-014-9258-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosselli, M., Ardila, A., Salvatierra, J., Marquez, M., Luis, M., and Weekes, V. A. (2002). A cross-linguistic comparison of verbal fluency tests. Int. J. Neurosci. 112, 759–776. doi: 10.1080/00207450290025752

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roux, F. E., Lubrano, V., Lauwers-Cances, V., Trémoulet, M., Mascott, C. R., and Démonet, J. F. (2004). Intraoperative mapping of cortical areas involved in reading in mono-and bilingual patients. Brain 127, 1796–1810. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh204

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roux, F. E., and Trémoulet, M. (2002). Organization of language areas in bilingual patients: a cortical stimulation study. J. Neurosurg. 97, 857–864. doi: 10.3171/jns.2002.97.4.0857

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruis, C. (2018). Monitoring cognition during awake brain surgery in adults: a systematic review. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 40, 1081–1104. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2018.1469602

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Runnqvist, E., Gollan, T. H., Costa, A., and Ferreira, V. S. (2013). A disadvantage in bilingual sentence production modulated by syntactic frequency and similarity across languages. Cognition 129, 256–263. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2013.07.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schepens, J. J., Van der Slik, F., and Van Hout, R. (2016). L1 and L2 distance effects in learning L3 Dutch Lang. Learning 66, 224–256. doi: 10.1111/lang.12150

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Segalowitz, N. (2010). Cognitive Bases of Second Language Fluency. New York, NY: Routledge. doi: 10.4324/9780203851357

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shinozuka, K., Niioka, K., Tokuda, T., Kyutoku, Y., Okuno, K., Takahashi, T., et al. (2021). Language familiarity and proficiency leads to differential cortical processing during translation between distantly related languages. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 15:593108. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2021.593108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sierpowska, J., Fernandez-Coello, A., Gomez-Andres, A., Camins, À, Castaner, S., Juncadella, M., et al. (2018). Involvement of the middle frontal gyrus in language switching as revealed by electrical stimulation mapping and functional magnetic resonance imaging in bilingual brain tumor patients. Cortex 99, 78–92. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.10.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sierpowska, J., Gabarrós, A., Ripollés, P., Juncadella, M., Castañer, S., Camins, Á, et al. (2013). Intraoperative electrical stimulation of language switching in two bilingual patients. Neuropsychologia 51, 2882–2892. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.09.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stowe, L. A., and Sabourin, L. (2005). Imaging the processing of a second language: effects of maturation and proficiency on the neural processes involved. IRAL 43:329.

Google Scholar

Sugiura, L., Ojima, S., Matsuba-Kurita, H., Dan, I., Tsuzuki, D., Katura, T., et al. (2015). Effects of sex and proficiency in second language processing as revealed by a large-scale fNIRS study of school-aged children. Hum. Brain Mapp. 36, 3890–3911. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22885

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Talacchi, A., Santini, B., Casartelli, M., Monti, A., Capasso, R., and Miceli, G. (2013). Awake surgery between art and science. Part II: language and cognitive mapping. Funct. Neurol. 28, 223–239. doi: 10.11138/FNeur/2013.28.3.223

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tao, L., Marzecová, A., Taft, M., Asanowicz, D., and Wodniecka, Z. (2011). The efficiency of attentional networks in early and late bilinguals: the role of age of acquisition. Front. Psychol. 2:123. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00123

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomasino, B., Marin, D., Canderan, C., Maieron, M., Budai, R., Fabbro, F., et al. (2014). Involuntary switching into the native language induced by electrocortical stimulation of the superior temporal gyrus: a multimodal mapping study. Neuropsychologia 62, 87–100. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.07.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Treffers-Daller, J. (2019). What defines language dominance in bilinguals? Annu. Rev. Linguist. 5, 375–393. doi: 10.1146/annurev-linguistics-011817-045554

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vaid, J. (1983). “Bilingualism and brain lateralization,” in Language Functions and Brain Organization, ed. S. Segalowitz (Cambridge MA: Academic Press), 315–339. doi: 10.1016/b978-0-12-635640-3.50023-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Waldron, E. J., and Hernandez, A. E. (2013). The role of age of acquisition on past tense generation in Spanish-English bilinguals: an fMRI study. Brain Lang. 125, 28–37. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2013.01.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walker, J. A., Quiñones-Hinojosa, A., and Berger, M. S. (2004). Intraoperative speech mapping in 17 bilingual patients undergoing resection of a mass lesion. Neurosurgery 54, 113–118. doi: 10.1227/01.NEU.0000097270.95721.3B

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, X., Wang, Y. Y., Jiang, T., Wang, Y. Z., and Wu, C. X. (2013). Direct evidence of the left caudate’s role in bilingual control: an intraoperative electrical stimulation study. Neurocase 19, 462–469. doi: 10.1080/13554794.2012.701635

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wartenburger, I., Heekeren, H. R., Abutalebi, J., Cappa, S. F., Villringer, A., and Perani, D. (2003). Early setting of grammatical processing in the bilingual brain. Neuron 37, 159–170. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)01150-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weber-Fox, C. M., and Neville, H. J. (1996). Maturational constraints on functional specializations for language processing: ERP and behavioral evidence in bilingual speakers. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 8, 231–256. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1996.8.3.231

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wei, M., Joshi, A. A., Zhang, M., Mei, L., Manis, F. R., He, Q., et al. (2015). How age of acquisition influences brain architecture in bilinguals. J. Neurolinguist. 36, 35–55. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2015.05.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zawiszewski, A., and Laka, I. (2020). Bilinguals processing noun morphology: evidence for the language distance hypothesis from event-related potentials. J. Neurolinguist. 55:100908. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2020.100908

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: multilingualism, language assessment, brain tumor, awake surgery, intraoperative testing

Citation: De Martino M, Talacchi A, Capasso R, Mazzotta A and Miceli G (2021) Language Assessment in Multilingualism and Awake Neurosurgery. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 15:750013. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2021.750013

Received: 30 July 2021; Accepted: 28 September 2021;
Published: 25 November 2021.

Edited by:

Carlo Semenza, University of Padua, Italy

Reviewed by:

Domenico D’Avella, University of Padua, Italy
Barbara Tomasino, Eugenio Medea (IRCCS), Italy
Roelien Bastiaanse, University of Groningen, Netherlands

Copyright © 2021 De Martino, Talacchi, Capasso, Mazzotta and Miceli. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Maria De Martino, bWRlbWFydGlub0B1bmlzYS5pdA==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.