Skip to main content

METHODS article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 30 September 2021
Sec. Cognitive Neuroscience
This article is part of the Research Topic Cognitive Reserve, Cognitive Functioning, and Mental Health in Elderly People View all 7 articles

The Performance Index Identifies Changes Across the Dual Task Timed Up and Go Test Phases and Impacts Task-Cost Estimation in the Oldest-Old

  • 1Graduate Program in Biomedical Gerontology, School of Medicine, Pontifical Catholic University of Rio Grande do Sul, PUCRS, Porto Alegre, Brazil
  • 2Neuroplasticity and Neural Repair Research Group, Health and Life Sciences School, Pontifical Catholic University of Rio Grande do Sul, PUCRS, Porto Alegre, Brazil
  • 3KITE - Toronto Rehabilitation Institute, University Health Network, Toronto, ON, Canada

Introduction: Dual tasking is common in activities of daily living (ADLs) and the ability to perform them usually declines with age. While cognitive aspects influence dual task (DT) performance, most DT-cost (DT-C) related metrics include only time- or speed- delta without weighting the accuracy of cognitive replies involved in the task.

Objectives: The primary study goal was to weight the accuracy of cognitive replies as a contributing factor when estimating DT-C using a new index of DT-C that considers the accuracy of cognitive replies (P-index) in the instrumented timed up and go test (iTUG). Secondarily, to correlate the novel P-index with domains of the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE).

Methods: Sixty-three participants (≥85 years old) took part in this study. The single task (ST) and DT iTUG tests were performed in a semi-random order. Both the time taken to complete the task measured utilizing an inertial measurement unit (IMU), and the accuracy of the cognitive replies were used to create the novel P-index. Clinical and sociodemographic data were collected.

Results: The accuracy of the cognitive replies changed across the iTUG phases, particularly between the walk 1 and walk 2 phases. Moreover, weighting 0.6 for delta-time (W1) and 0.4 for cognitive replies (W2) into the P-index enhanced the prediction of the MMSE score. The novel P-index was able to explain 37% of the scores obtained by the fallers in the “spatial orientation” and “attention” domains of the MMSE. The ability of the P-index to predict MMSE scores was not significantly influenced by age, schooling, and number of medicines in use. The Bland-Altman analysis indicated a substantial difference between the time-delta-based DT-C and P-index methods, which was within the limits of agreement.

Conclusions: The P-index incorporates the accuracy of cognitive replies when calculating the DT-C and better reflects the variance of the MMSE in comparison with the traditional time- or speed-delta approaches, thus providing an improved method to estimate the DT-C.

Introduction

Fall-related injuries are a public health problem worldwide (Sherrington et al., 2017) and falling while performing activities of daily living (ADLs) is common in older adults (Tomas-Carus et al., 2019). Bone fractures, hospitalization, depression, and sedentary behavior are among the fall-triggered health issues that contribute to reducing the quality of life and autonomy (Ambrose et al., 2013). Understanding the interaction between fall-related factors is important to detect and prevent falls in older adults, which is of utmost clinical importance.

ADLs require different levels of cognitive demands and may involve dual tasks (DTs; e.g., walking in the supermarket while remembering what goods should be bought) (Herold et al., 2018). Commonly, the DT approach is used in laboratory settings to assess the ability to deal with DTs during simulated ADLs. DT approaches often utilize a cognitive factor to interfere with motor performance. In such cognitive-motor DT, the between-systems interplay may reduce the performance in the task, and the interference level depends on DT type, complexity, and individual skills (Bayot et al., 2018; Yiu et al., 2018). Cognitive-motor DTs, may require good levels of executive function, working memory, as well as attention, language, among other neurocognitive functions while performing a simultaneous motor gesture (Ehsani et al., 2019; Rhodes et al., 2019). For instance, attention is needed to perceive the environmental context and may be shared, alternated, or concentrated in one or more targets (Shalev et al., 2016). Thus, attention constraints may influence the cognitive load by increasing the working memory demands. In some cases, it hampers the ability to retain, retrieve, and recall information in a short period (Rodrigues and Pandeirada, 2015). By contrast, an optimal executive function may mitigate DT interference during walking (Bridenbaugh and Kressig, 2015). These are examples of how the cognitive load may affect multi-task performance, which would be reflected in the time taken to complete the task [i.e., conventional DT-cost (DT-C) metrics].

Older adults frequently experience both neurocognitive and neuromuscular decline, which contributes to reducing performance in walking-based DTs, with respectively, greater risk of falls (Rosso et al., 2018). Hence, we can assume an increased risk of falls within increased age in older adults (Kafri et al., 2017). Several studies have addressed functional mobility in older adults using different DT interferences (Yogev-Seligmann et al., 2012; Patel et al., 2014; Brustio et al., 2018; Commandeur et al., 2018) and most of these studies employed the instrumented timed up and go test (iTUG) to assess functional mobility (Zaferiou et al., 2017; Howcroft et al., 2018; Patel et al., 2019; Figueiredo et al., 2020).

Despite considerable efforts, identifying the best DT to assess the risk of falls in the oldest-old (individuals aged ≥ 80 years) remains a challenge (Montero-Odasso et al., 2019) and several types of cognitive-motor tasks have been investigated in fall-risk assessment trials (Barbosa et al., 2008; Fatori et al., 2015; Yeo, 2017; Asai et al., 2018; Zirek et al., 2018; Gillain et al., 2019; Allahverdipour et al., 2020; Figueiredo et al., 2020). For example, repeating the weekdays in reverse order during a cognitive-motor DT requires dividing attention and temporal organization to select the correct days while walking (Yeo, 2017). All these cognitive-motor tasks may lead to increased time- or speed- delta between single task (ST) and DT, captured by the traditional equations to estimate the DT-C (Kelly et al., 2010; Asai et al., 2018; Hunter et al., 2018).

Selecting the most appropriate equation to estimate the DT-C is challenging (Venema et al., 2019). Usually, the calculation of the DT-C considers the time- or speed-delta between the ST and DT (Kelly et al., 2010; Asai et al., 2018; Hunter et al., 2018). Nonetheless, most of the equations disregard the accuracy of the cognitive replies, which may influence time or speed differences in the test (Bherer et al., 2008; Tomas-Carus et al., 2019). The accuracy of cognitive replies can be obtained by counting the number of total, correct and wrong replies during the DT. The incorporation of the accuracy of cognitive replies into the DT-C calculation (P-index) may enhance the association of the DT assessment with neurocognitive decline, and better reflect the engagement and performance during the task. Primarily, the present study sought to weight the accuracy of cognitive replies as a contributing factor to estimate DT-C in the iTUG test using a novel modified P-index. We also tested the association of the P-index with neurocognitive performance by exploring correlations between the weighted P-index and mini-mental state examination (MMSE) cognitive domains. Secondarily, we compared the traditional time- or speed- delta DT-C and P-index in the oldest-old with and without a history of falls.

Materials and Methods

Participants

This is an observational, cross-sectional study. Participants were recruited (fallers and non-fallers) by convenience in the city of Porto Alegre, Rio Grande do Sul–Brazil. After an initial telephone call, a home visit was scheduled (lasting approximately 1 h and 30 min) to collect the data. The inclusion criteria were: volunteers of any gender; aged ≥ 85 years, who were able to walk independently (walking-assistant devices allowed), and understood the verbal commands necessary to complete the assessment test battery. The exclusion criteria were: participant uncertainty regarding their history of falls in the 6 months prior to the assessment (information was cross-checked with relatives or caregivers); hospitalization for more than 7 days in the previous 3 months; and a diagnosis of neurological (including major cognitive decline or dementia), severe respiratory, cardiovascular, visual, or auditory diseases (self-reported and confirmed by medical contact or report). Clinical and sociodemographic information (age, gender, blood pressure, schooling, marital status, medications in use, ethnicity, smoking, drinking, and history of falls) were collected during the assessment interview. This research was approved by the local Research Ethics Committee (number 2.278.707). All volunteers signed a free and informed consent form.

Falls

The volunteers who self-reported having fallen in the 6 months prior to the assessment were classified as “fallers” and those who did not report this experience were considered “non-fallers” (Hughes et al., 2020). A fall was defined as an involuntary and unexpected event in which the participant comes to rest on the ground (Deandrea et al., 2010). This definition was used because minor falls, i.e., unintentional changes in body level, without impacting on the ground, are more likely to be forgotten.

Instrumented Timed Up and Go Test (iTUG)

An inertial measurement unit (IMU) with a sampling rate of 100 Hz was used during the ST and DT (Bluetooth-compatible IMU, G-Walk, BTS Bioengineering, MA, United States). The IMU was positioned between the L5 and S1 vertebrae using an elastic belt provided by the manufacturer (Kleiner et al., 2017; Pau et al., 2018). Two types of iTUG tests were performed in a semi-random order (ST—DT—DT—ST—DT). We used three trials of each iTUG type to obtain a representative performance while minimizing fatigue in the oldest-old volunteers (Witchel et al., 2018; Figueiredo et al., 2020). Participants were asked to walk as fast as possible during the iTUG tests. To begin, the participants were seated on a standard chair provided by the researcher (43 cm in height, without armrests). The verbal command “get up and go” was given and participants were expected to walk for 6 m (3 m in the forward gait phase—walk 1, perform a 180° turn—turn phase, and return 3 m—walk 2 phase, and sit in the same chair from which they had started—turn and sit phase) (Figure 1A). During the DT iTUG, participants followed the same above-mentioned protocol while vocalizing the weekdays in reverse order (e.g., Wednesday, Tuesday, Monday, and so on until the completion of the test) (Barbosa et al., 2008; Fatori et al., 2015; Allahverdipour et al., 2020; Figueiredo et al., 2020). Participants were instructed to start speaking, as quickly as possible, while still sitting. At each DT iTUG trial, the initial day of the week was changed to reduce the chances of automatizing the sequence (Figueiredo et al., 2020). Each volunteer was previously informed that the correct/wrong answers would be recorded.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The instrumented timed up and go (iTUG) test, the dual task cost (DT-C), and the novel performance index (P-index). (A) The iTUG test. (B) The accuracy of the cognitive replies during the DT (orange–defined as the ratio of correct vocalizations and total vocalizations) was included in the time-delta equation for calculating the cost of the dual task (DT-C) (Asai et al., 2018). The P-index is a novel index adjusted by the accuracy of the DT. Higher P-index values indicate greater DT performance. We tested the influence of the cognitive task (i.e., correct/total vocalizations) by increasing W2 and decreasing W1 at 0, 0.1, 0.2, 0.3, 0.4, 0.5, 0.6, 0.7, 0.8, 0.9, and 1, respectively.

Cognition

The MMSE was used as a screening test for cognitive decline (Folstein et al., 1975; Bertolucci et al., 1994). The MMSE was developed by Folstein et al. (1975) and was adapted to Brazilian Portuguese by Bertolucci et al. (1994). The total MMSE score and the temporal orientation, spatial orientation, attention, memory, and language domains were assessed (Bertolucci et al., 1994; Kochhann and Lisboa, 2010).

Weighting the Accuracy of Cognitive Replies in the DT iTUG Performance

The influence of weighting cognitive replies to estimate DT-C was tested using an adapted version of the traditional time-delta equation (Asai et al., 2018) (Eq. 1), as follows (Eq. 2):

D T - C = [ ( T ( D T ) - T ( S T ) ) ( T ( S T ) +    T ( D T ) ) 2 ] X 100 (1)

where: DT-C is the cost of the dual task; T(DT) is the DT duration (in sec), T(ST) is the ST duration (in sec).

P - i n d e x = [ 1 - [ ( T ( D T ) - T ( S T ) ) ( T ( S T ) + T ( D T ) ) 2 ] ] × W 1 + ( C o r r e c t T o t a l ) × W 2 (2)

where: P-index is the DT performance index from 0 (poor) to 1 (high); T(DT) is the DT duration (in sec), T(ST) is the ST duration (in sec); W1 is the weight attributed to time-delta (DT-ST) in the equation (from 0 to 1); Correct is the number of correct cognitive replies; Total is the number of total cognitive replies (correct and wrong); W2 is the weight attributed to the accuracy (accuracy = correct/total replies) in the equation (from 0 to 1).

In this study, the accuracy of cognitive replies was defined by the ratio between the correct number of cognitive replies to the total replies during each DT iTUG phase (Figures 1A,B). We tested the weight of cognitive replies (i.e., correct/total answers) by increasing W2 and decreasing W1 at 0, 0.1, 0.2, 0.3, 0.4, 0.5, 0.6, 0.7, 0.8, 0.9, and 1, respectively. We have also compared the DT-C estimated by previously published equations (Kelly et al., 2010; Asai et al., 2018; Hunter et al., 2018).

Statistical Analysis

Data are expressed as mean and standard deviation or median and inter-quartile range, according to their normality profile. Data normality was assessed using the Shapiro-Wilk test. Multiple linear regression and interaction analysis were used to test the ability of the P-index to predict MMSE scores as well as relation of the P-index with other independent clinical and sociodemographic variables. The Receiver Operating Characteristic Curve (ROC curve) was used to determine the weights of the P-index that exhibit the largest area under the curve and ideal cut-off values, in terms of sensibility and specificity, to detect MMSE scores and history of falls. The Bland-Altman analysis was applied to evaluate possible DT-C differences obtained using both time-delta and P-index equations. The within-group and between-group comparisons were assessed using the Friedman and Kruskal-Wallis tests, respectively. The Mann-Whitney U and Wilcoxon tests were also used when indicated. The Spearman’s correlation rank was used to explore possible correlations between the MMSE domains and P-index values. Statistical significance (p) was set at α = 0.05. The Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS, version 25.0) including the PROCESS macro (version 3.5) and GraphPad Prism (version 9.0) were used to analyze data and generate the graphical representations.

Results

All the participants completed the assessment. The sample characterization is shown in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Sociodemographic and clinical characteristics of the studied sample (n = 63).

The Accuracy of Cognitive Replies as a Contributing Factor to Estimate DT Performance (P-Index) in Different iTUG Phases

As shown in Figure 2A, the median accuracy of the cognitive replies differed between the walk 1 and walk 2 iTUG phases. The lower level of accuracy occurred during the walk 2 phase of the iTUG, independently of W2. The P-index curves (Figure 2B) revealed that W2 produced a notable change in the P-index from the walk 1 and turn phases of the iTUG, while W2 effects on walk 2 and Full iTUG phases were less evident (in the walk 2 phase, the P-index value was low regardless of the weight).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. The accuracy of the cognitive replies and DT performance in the iTUG test. (A) Between-group (fallers vs. non-fallers) comparison regarding the accuracy of the cognitive replies at different iTUG test phases. Note the difference in the accuracy of cognitive replies between the walk 1 and walk 2 phases. (B) The novel P-index is also substantially lower in the walk 2 phase. (C) Change in the coefficient of correlation between P-index and MMSE when the weight of the cognitive replies (accuracy; W2) is changed by increasing W2 and decreasing W1 at 0, 0.1, 0.2, 0.3, 0.4, 0.5, 0.6, 0.7, 0.8, 0.9, and 1, respectively. Note the change in the correlation coefficients when the W2 weight is increased. In (A) Box and whiskers plot showing accuracy of the cognitive replies in the different iTUG test phases (walk 1, turn, walk 2, and full iTUG). The central line is the median, the hinges of the plot are the 25th to 75th percentiles and extremes are min to max values. *p < 0.05.

The present findings also revealed no major differences between oldest-old with and without a history of falls using both, the time-delta DT-C (Eq. 1) or the novel P-index (Eq. 2) approaches (Asai et al., 2018). We also applied equations described in other studies (Kelly et al., 2010; Hunter et al., 2018), nonetheless, the results also indicated no significant differences between fallers and non-fallers. Both fallers and non-fallers exhibited greater DT-C during the walk 1 and 2 phases (Supplementary Table 1, p < 0.05). Although there were no between-group differences in the MMSE scores or the accuracy of the cognitive replies, the latter differed when the iTUG phases were compared (Supplementary Table 2). There were more cognitive errors (lower accuracy of the cognitive replies) during the walk 2 phase when compared to the walk 1 phase (Figure 2A and Supplementary Table 2). Additionally, the ROC curve analysis was used to test whether the time-based DT-C and the P-index equations were able to predict falls in our sample. The findings revealed both equations failed in predicting falls among the studied oldest-old (DT-C equation: p ≈ 0.902; P-index: p ≈ 0.485; Supplementary Table 3).

Cognitive Assessment (MMSE) and DT Performance (P-Index)

The exploratory correlation between the P-index values and total scores in the MMSE is shown in Figure 2C. We found the higher the weight attributed to the accuracy of the cognitive replies (W2), the better the correlation between the Full iTUG P-index and the MMSE total score. We also noted some MMSE domain-specific relationships with the P-index values across the iTUG phases. The MMSE temporal orientation scores seemed to better correlate with the walk 1 phase, walk 2 phase, and full iTUG assessment, mainly in the non-fallers (Figure 3A). Nonetheless, when adding weight to the W2 component of the P-index this effect disappeared. Conversely, greater W2 weights led to statistically significant correlations between the P-index and the MMSE in the spatial orientation domain (Figure 3B). Interestingly, only the fallers exhibited P-index values highly correlated with the MMSE spatial orientation domain during the turn phase of the iTUG. The W2 weight was also important when correlating the P-index with the attention domain of the MMSE (Figure 3C). The attention domain was more consistently correlated with P-index values in the walk 1 phase and the full iTUG. P-index values were also correlated with the MMSE memory domain, particularly in the fallers during the turn and full iTUG assessment (Figure 3D). Finally, the language domain of the MMSE was not significantly correlated with any P-index values (Figure 3E).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Correlations between the novel P-index and mini-mental state examination (MMSE) domains. Correlation between the novel P-index and the (A) temporal orientation, (B) spatial orientation, (C) attention, (D) memory, and (E) language domains of the MMSE. *p < 0.05, r,Spearman’s rank correlation coefficient.

The ROC curve analysis revealed the P-index should use weights of 0.6 for W1 (time-delta) and 0.4 for W2 (the accuracy of the cognitive replies) to better predict a total MMSE score ≥26. The P-index cut-off, sensibility, and specificity values are shown in Table 2. We used multiple linear regression, stratifying for fallers and non-fallers, to test whether the MMSE domains could predict the P-index. Briefly, the analysis revealed a statistically significant model for fallers [F(3,27) = 5.28; p = 0.05; R2 = 0.37] in the full iTUG test. The MMSE domains attention (ß = 0.38; t = 2.03; p = 0.05) and spatial orientation (ß = 0.37; t = 2.03; p = 0.05) were predictors of the P-index in the full iTUG. By contrast, temporal orientation (ß = –0.28; t = –1.67; p = 0.11) was unable to predict the P-index. A statistically significant model [F(2,31) = 8.79; p = 0.001; R2 = 0.38] was also found for non-fallers in the full iTUG test. The MMSE domains temporal (ß = 0.56; t = 3.79; p = 0.001) and spatial orientation (ß = 0.33; t = 2.22; p = 0.03) predicted the P-index in the full iTUG test for those without a history of falls (Table 3). Notwithstanding, other P-index weights may be used according to the assessment goals. Interaction-based analyses showed age, schooling, and number of medications in use did not influence the total MMSE score in the studied oldest-old (Table 4). The Bland-Altman analysis indicated the traditional time-based DT-C calculation (W2 = 0) differed from the selected P-index—note the variance in the difference (y) axis. However, we observed that most of the values lie within the 95% limits of agreement, which suggests a good between-method agreement (Supplementary Figure 1).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Receiver Operating Characteristic Curve (ROC curve) to predict a total MMSE score ≥ 26 points.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. MMSE variation explained by the P-index using multiple linear regression.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Interaction analyses to predict MMSE scores (total and domains).

Discussion

This study sought to explore how the accuracy of the cognitive replies in a dual task (DT) influences the task cost (DT-C) by introducing a novel flexible performance index (P-index). This P-index was created by adapting time-delta-based equations commonly used to determine the DT-C. First, this study provides evidence on how the accuracy of cognitive replies changes across the iTUG phases in the oldest-old, regardless of their history of falls in the previous 6 months. Secondly, that weighting the accuracy of cognitive replies in the P-index transferred this effect to the index and afforded a better relation of the novel P-index with neurocognitive scores assessed by the MMSE. The novel P-index enhanced the relationship of the traditional DT-C calculations with cognitive domains related to attention, memory, spatial and temporal orientation.

In terms of the accuracy of the cognitive replies, several factors may explain the difference observed between the walk 1 and walk 2 iTUG phases. The walk 2 phase is preceded by the turn phase (180° turn), which is the most cognitively demanding part of the test (Thigpen et al., 2000; Hollands et al., 2010, 2014). Thus, the accuracy of the cognitive replies in the walk 2 phase is probably influenced by the turn phase, which may explain the difference between the walk 1 and walk 2 phases since they are very similar in terms of biomechanical demand (Courtine and Schieppati, 2003; Al-Yahya et al., 2011; Lowry et al., 2012). This influence is also observed in individuals with Huntington’s disease. The turn phase of the TUG generates a cognitive interference in attention and information processing, which suggests the simultaneous use of motor and cognitive resources increases gait variability during turning movements (Purcell et al., 2020). Similarly, a previous study involving individuals with Parkinson’s disease found an association between processing speed and turning while walking, but no other correlation with cognitive domains (Pal et al., 2016).

To the best of our knowledge, this was the first study to investigate how weighting the accuracy of cognitive replies may impact the DT-C estimation (P-index) in the oldest-old during a mobility task. This effect was not homogeneous across all the test phases. For instance, the greater the weight attributed to the accuracy of the cognitive replies (W2) in the walk 1 phase, the more the P-index increased, suggesting good cognitive performance during this walking phase. Indeed, the walk 1 phase, i.e., the initial 3-m-linear walk, does not typically require significant cortical demand from the participants (Malcolm et al., 2015; Wagshul et al., 2019), which allows them to prioritize the cognitive component of the task and, as a result, may increase the success rate in the cognitive replies. Also, the walk 1 phase is performed soon after the participants are told to do their best in saying the weekdays in reverse order, which probably influences the cognitive focus and task prioritization (Verghese et al., 2007; Kelly et al., 2010). By contrast, the turn phase requires a quick 180° change in walking direction, thus increasing the cognitive-motor demand. The act of turning requires higher levels of motor control than linear walking and involves coordinating visual inputs, spatial memory/direction sense, environmental recognition, and biomechanical adjustments (Herman et al., 2011; Shin and Yoo, 2015; Mellone et al., 2016). Consequently, our findings may indicate that the higher cognitive load during the turn movement may have inhibited the focus on the cognitive replies due to the priority given to the motor task (Li et al., 2012; Porciuncula et al., 2016). Interestingly, weighting the accuracy of the cognitive replies in the walk 2 phase did not impact the P-index values, probably because the respondence rate of the cognitive replies was low. The inhibition of the cognitive component in the DT, which typically occurs during the turn phase of the iTUG, may have persisted long enough to hamper the response rate and accuracy during the walk 2 phase (Haid and Federolf, 2019). Further studies may adapt the P-index equation to other types of cognitive tasks with greater ability to deal with low respondence rates, or with greater sensitivity to detect the cognitive load.

Weighting the accuracy of the cognitive replies in the Full iTUG test resulted in a mild effect on the P-index values. In all likelihood, this occurs because the P-index observed in the walk 1 and turn phases are opposing. As a result, the Full iTUG findings are coherent with the patterns of the accuracy-related curves among the overall iTUG test phases. This suggests calculating the P-index without separating the iTUG test phases may lead to the loss of information regarding task-focus predominance during the test.

The proper cognitive-motor DT is expected to overload cognitive domains while performing a movement (Ehsani et al., 2019; Beurskens, 2020). Here, we also examined how weighting the accuracy of the cognitive replies (W2) in the P-index correlates with the MMSE scores. When W2 is not considered (equal to zero) the correlation between the P-index and MMSE was poor, which suggests the traditional DT-C equations may underestimate the cognitive cost during the DT. The gradual addition of the accuracy of the cognitive replies in the P-index generated dose-response curves indicating the relation of P-index with the MMSE scores. This is an important increment to previous studies highlighting the importance of correcting the performance indexes for cognitive stops and cognitive errors when screening older adults for risk of falls (Tomas-Carus et al., 2019).

The addition of cognitive demand in a DT increases the prioritization levels of cognitive domains such as attention (Maclean et al., 2017). For example, using spatiotemporal gait characteristics, the DT-C displays a positive relationship with a shared attention task (de Bruin and Schmidt, 2010). Our data support the role of attention during the DT, which is seen when weight is added to the W2 component of the P-index. Similar results were observed for the fallers in the turn phase of the iTUG. This indicates individuals with higher scores in the attention domain of the MMSE also performed better in the iTUG during DTs. This may be related to the fact individuals with attention deficits and higher risk of falling tend to look away from the target before completing a task (Chapman and Hollands, 2005). Previous studies have highlighted the direct influence of attention on speed and gait adaptation (Maclean et al., 2017; Bayot et al., 2018; Cedervall et al., 2020). Selective attention is highly associated with multitask performance (Harvey, 2019), which agrees with the present findings.

In addition to attention, our study indicated a relationship between the P-index with the memory and spatial orientation domains of the MMSE. These findings were more evident for individuals with a positive history of falls—i.e., a greater relation between P-index and memory and spatial orientation emerged when the weight of the accuracy of the cognitive replies was increased. Gray et al. (2021) found that individuals with poor memory reduce their walking speed by 11% when performing a DT (series of subtractions in a 10-m walking test), while individuals without evident memory deficits by 5%. Similarly, individuals exhibiting symptoms of cognitive decline also perform worse in DTs involving semantic memory, which is responsible for storing, retaining, and evoking long-term information (Theill et al., 2011). Here, our results also indicated that the memory domain (e.g., recording and evocation) exhibited a strong correlation with the P-index, especially in participants with a history of falls. Remarkably, the attention, memory, and spatial orientation domains of the MMSE only influenced the DT-C when the novel P-index was calculated with enough weight to the cognitive replies (W2). Overall, this study suggests further trials should consider the full range of weights for both the time-delta (W1) and the accuracy of the cognitive replies (W2) to ensure a comprehensive DT-C estimation in the oldest-old. Overall, the P-index may be used in association with neuropsychological assessment to enhance the understanding of how cognitive domains influence performance in cognitive-motor DTs. Translating these findings into a clinical setting, we might consider interventions focusing on different cognitive domains to improve ADLs involving DT performance before and after a fall. For instance, interventions focusing on the spatial and temporal orientation domains may be the best strategy for non-fallers, while the attention and spatial orientation cognitive domains could be more appropriate for fallers.

The interaction analysis suggested the P-index [W1 (0.6)/W2 (0.4)] was not biased toward characteristics such as age, schooling, and medication use. This indicates the P-index could be a good marker to capture the DT-C in walking-based activities. From a clinical perspective, the P-index may help detect cognitive-motor impairments in the oldest-old with different sociodemographic characteristics, thus contributing to improve the assessment of the cognitive-motor functioning—particularly when compared with the traditional DT-C approaches that disregard the accuracy of the cognitive replies. Furthermore, we can speculate that the stability of an individual’s P-index (throughout the full range of W1 and W2 weights) could indicate their ability to deal with simultaneous cognitive-motor demands. Nonetheless, further trials are needed to test how the P-index could contribute to prevent/diagnose problems in cognitive-motor coupling.

Finally, neither the time- or speed-delta DT-C nor the novel P-index equations revealed between-group differences when the history of falls was considered as a dependent factor. Our findings showed the tested DT-C equations (time-based and P-index) were unable to predict falls in our sample. This is not completely unexpected because some of the oldest-old (people aged ≥ 80 years) change their daily life routine and behavior to minimize exposition to situations involving risk of falls. Thus, we can hypothesize that some non-fallers and fallers exhibit similar cognitive-motor capacity, being only differentiated by their taken-risk behavior profile. However, this secondary analysis should be interpreted with caution since the study was not designed/powered to obtain definitive conclusions on this subject. Also, other biomechanical/clinical factors not usually accounted in the time-based iTUG analyses, such as movement quality, may unveil subtle differences between individuals with and without a history of falls, with respective enhancements in the prediction models (Figueiredo et al., 2020). Moreover, in recent years, there has been considerable debate regarding the DT-C assessment and risk of falls (Bootsma-van der Wiel et al., 2003; Beauchet et al., 2008; Uemura et al., 2011; Tomas-Carus et al., 2019). For example, Bootsma-van der Wiel et al. (2003) assessed 380 oldest-old individuals and reported DT involving vocalizations (animals or professions) was not better at predicting falls compared to a ST, which is consistent with our findings (Bootsma-van der Wiel et al., 2003). Additionally, Beauchet et al. (2008) found no differences between fallers and non-fallers using a backward-counting-based DT (Beauchet et al., 2008). Conversely, Uemura and cols suggested a similar DT was able to differentiate fallers and non-fallers during the steady-state walking (Uemura et al., 2011). Finally, Tomas-Carus et al. (2019) suggested a backward-counting-based DT was able to discriminate fallers and non-fallers after correcting the performance score for cognitive stops and cognitive errors (Tomas-Carus et al., 2019). Overall, further trials are required to determine the optimal tasks and equations to predict falls in the oldest-old.

Limitations

This study has some limitations. First, the sample was recruited by convenience, thus the participants may not fully represent the oldest-old population. Moreover, the sample characteristics may have influenced the present findings (88.9% of the participants were female, 71.43% single/widow, 88.9% white, 96.2% non-smokers, and 82.54% non-drinkers). Unfortunately, the unbalanced sample profile introduced a statistical impossibility to examine whether these variables could modify the relationship between P-index and MMSE domains, which is a matter for further investigation. Finally, the current findings might differ using other cognitive tasks during the iTUG. Further studies comparing different tasks using the novel P-index curve values may help detect DT-C in the community-dwelling oldest-old.

Conclusion

A novel P-index to estimate dual task cost (DT-C) was presented, this index uses the weighted components W1 (time-delta) and W2 (accuracy of the cognitive replies). Dose-response curves indicated that weighting the accuracy of cognitive replies (W2) increased the relation of the P-index with the attention, memory, and spatial orientation domains of the MMSE. The novel P-index may assist the mobility assessment by combining cognitive-motor performance during the iTUG test. We identified the turn and walk 2 phases (returning walk) are more cognitively demanding, suggesting the cognitive load at different phases of the test should be considered. The above-mentioned findings may contribute to providing a more accurate assessment of functional mobility in the oldest-old. Importantly, W1 and W2 may be adapted to other mobility and cognitive tasks, respectively—expanding the applicability of the novel P-index.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Pontifical Catholic University of Rio Grande do SUl Research Ethics Committee (research protocol 2.278.707). The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

FB planned the experiments, performed the data collection, and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. GB elaborated the structure of the model and equation, analyzed the data, draw the figures, wrote the first draft of the manuscript, and reviewed the final version of the manuscript. AF performed the data collection and collaborated with the reasoning of the manuscript. DH analyzed the MMSE domains and collaborate in writing the first draft of the manuscript. AS collaborated in the manuscript reasoning and writing the first draft of the manuscript. RM conceived the study idea, elaborated of the structure of the model and equation, supervised the findings, wrote, and reviewed the second draft of the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

The Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nivel Superior—Brazil (CAPES)—Finance Code 001 and the National Council for Scientific and Technological Development (CNPq)—Brazil—provide scholarships to support this investigation.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnhum.2021.720719/full#supplementary-material

Supplementary Figure 1 | Bland-Altman analysis indicated that the novel P-index displays variability in relation to the traditional time-delta index, with good agreement between methods. Bland-Altman plot exhibits 95% limits of agreement between –0.2509 and 0.2614. Note very few dots exceeded the limits of agreement. Bias and standard deviation (SD) of bias were considered low.

Supplementary Table 1 | The dual-task cost (DT-C in %) using the traditional time- or speed-delta equations.

Supplementary Table 2 | Mini-mental state examination (MMSE) scores and cognitive replies during the dual task (DT).

Supplementary Table 3 | Receiver Operating Characteristic Curve (ROC curve) to predict history of falls.

References

Allahverdipour, H., Dianat, I., Mameh, G., and Jafarabadi, M. (2020). Effects of cognitive and physical loads on dynamic and static balance of older adults under single, dual and multi-task conditions. Hum. Factors doi: 10.21203/rs.2.10381/v1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Al-Yahya, E., Dawes, H., Smith, L., Dennis, A., Howells, K., and Cockburn, J. (2011). Cognitive motor interference while walking: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 715–728. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.08.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ambrose, A. F., Paul, G., and Hausdorff, J. M. (2013). Risk factors for falls among older adults: a review of the literature. Maturitas 75, 51–56. doi: 10.1016/j.maturitas.2013.02.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Asai, T., Oshima, K., Fukumoto, Y., Yonezawa, Y., Matsuo, A., and Misu, S. (2018). Association of fall history with the Timed Up and Go test score and the dual task cost: a cross-sectional study among independent community-dwelling older adults. Geriatr. Gerontol. Int. 18, 1189–1193. doi: 10.1111/ggi.13439

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barbosa, J., Prates, B., Gonçalves, C., Aquino, A. R., and Parentoni, A. (2008). Efeito da realização simultânea de tarefas cognitivas e motoras no desempenho funcional de idosos da comunidade Dual task effects on functional performance in community-dwelling elderly. Fisiot. Pesq. São Paulo 15, 374–379. doi: 10.1590/S1809-29502008000400010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bayot, M., Dujardin, K., Tard, C., Defebvre, L., Bonnet, C. T., Allart, E., et al. (2018). The interaction between cognition and motor control: a theoretical framework for dual-task interference effects on posture, gait initiation, gait and turning. Neurophysiol. Clin. 48, 361–375. doi: 10.1016/j.neucli.2018.10.003c

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beauchet, O., Allali, G., Annweiler, C., Berrut, G., Maarouf, N., Herrmann, F. R., et al. (2008). Does change in gait while counting backward predict the occurrence of a first fall in older adults? Gerontology 54, 217–223. doi: 10.1159/000127318

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bertolucci, P. H. F., Brucki, S. M. D., Campacci, S. R., and Juliano, Y. O. (1994). Mini-exame do estado mental em uma população geral: impacto da escolaridade. Arq. Neuropsiquiatr. 52, 1–7.

Google Scholar

Beurskens, R. (2020). Effects of motor versus cognitive task prioritization during dual-task practice on dual-task performance in young adults. Front. Psychol. 11:581225. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.581225

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bherer, L., Kramer, A. F., Peterson, M. S., Colcombe, S., Erickson, K., and Becic, E. (2008). Transfer effects in task-set cost and dual-task cost after dual-task training in older and younger adults: further evidence for cognitive plasticity in attentional control in late adulthood. Exp. Aging Res. 34, 188–219. doi: 10.1080/03610730802070068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bootsma-van der Wiel, A., Gussekloo, J., de Craen, A. J., van Exel, E., Bloem, B. R., and Westendorp, R. G. (2003). Walking and talking as predictors of falls in the general population: the leiden 85-plus study. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 51, 1466–1471. doi: 10.1046/j.1532-5415.2003.51468.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bridenbaugh, S. A., and Kressig, R. W. (2015). Motor cognitive dual tasking early detection of gait impairment, fall risk and cognitive decline. Z. Gerontol. Geriatr. 48, 15–21. doi: 10.1007/s00391-014-0845-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brustio, P., Rabaglietti, E., Liubicich, M., and Formica, S. (2018). Dual-task training in older adults: the effect of additional motor tasks on mobility performance. Arch. Gerontol. Geriatr. 75, 119–124. doi: 10.1016/j.archger.2017.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cedervall, Y., Stenberg, A. M., Åhman, H. B., Giedraitis, V., Tinmark, F., Berglund, L., et al. (2020). Timed up-and-go dual-task testing in the assessment of cognitive function: a mixed methods observational study for development of the UDDGait protocol. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020:1715. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17051715

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chapman, G. J., and Hollands, M. A. (2005). Evidence for a link between changes to gaze behaviour and risk of falling in older adults during adaptive locomotion. Gait Posture 24, 288–294. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2005.10.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Commandeur, D., Klimstra, M. D. D., MacDonald, S., Inouye, K., Cox, M., Chan, D., et al. (2018). Difference scores between single-task and dual-task gait measures are better than clinical measures for detection of fall-risk in community-dwelling older adults. Gait Posture 66, 155–159. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2018.08.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Courtine, G., and Schieppati, M. (2003). Human walking along a curved path. I. Body trajectory, segment orientation and the effect of vision. Eur. J. Neurosci. 18, 177–190. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02736.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Bruin, E. D., and Schmidt, A. (2010). Walking behaviour of healthy elderly: attention should be paid. Behav. Brain. Funct. 6:59. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-6-59

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deandrea, S., Lucenteforte, E., Bravi, F., Foschi, R., La Vecchia, C., Negri, E., et al. (2010). Risk factors for falls in community-dwelling older people. Epidemiology 21, 658–668. doi: 10.1097/EDE.0b013e3181e89905

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ehsani, H., Mohler, M. J., O’connor, K., Zamrini, E., Tirambulo, C., and Toosizadeh, N. (2019). The association between cognition and dual-tasking among older adults: the effect of motor function type and cognition task difficulty. Clin. Interv. Aging. 14, 659–669. doi: 10.2147/CIA.S198697

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fatori, C. D. O., Leite, C., Souza, L., and Patrizzi, L. (2015). Dupla tarefa e mobilidade funcional de idosos ativos. Rev. Bras. Geriatr. Gerontol. 18, 29–37. doi: 10.1590/1809-9823.2015.13180

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Figueiredo, A. I., Balbinot, G., Brauner, F. O., Schiavo, A., Baptista, R. R., Pagnussat, A. S., et al. (2020). SPARC metrics provide mobility smoothness assessment in oldest-old with and without a history of falls: a case control study. Front. Physiol. 11:540. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2020.0054

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., and McHugh, P. R. (1975). “Mini-mental state”: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J. Psychiatr. Res. 12, 189–198.

Google Scholar

Gillain, S., Boutaayamou, M., Schwartz, C., Dardenne, N., Bruyère, O., Brüls, O., et al. (2019). Gait symmetry in the dual task condition as a predictor of future falls among independent older adults: a 2-year longitudinal study. Aging Clin. Exp. Res. 31, 1057–1067. doi: 10.1007/s40520-019-01210-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gray, M., Gills, J. L., Glenn, J. M., Vincenzo, J. L., Walter, C. S., Madero, E. N., et al. (2021). Cognitive decline negatively impacts physical function. Exp. Gerontol. 143:111164. doi: 10.1016/j.exger.2020.111164

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haid, T., and Federolf, P. (2019). The effect of cognitive resource competition due to dual-tasking on the irregularity and control of postural movement components. Entropy 21:70. doi: 10.3390/e21010070

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harvey, P. D. (2019). Domains of cognition and their assessment. Dialogues Clin. Neurosci. 21, 227–237. doi: 10.31887/dcns.2019.21.3/pharvey

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herman, T., Giladi, N., and Hausdorff, J. M. (2011). Properties of the “Timed Up and Go” test: more than meets the eye. Gerontology 57, 203–210. doi: 10.1159/000314963

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herold, F., Hamacher, D., Schega, L., and Müller, N. G. (2018). Thinking while moving or moving while thinking–concepts of motor-cognitive training for cognitive performance enhancement. Front. Aging Neurosci. 10:228. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2018.00228

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hollands, K., Hollands, M. A., Zietz, D., Miles Wing, A., Wright, C., and Van Vliet, P. (2010). Kinematics of turning 180° during the timed up and go in stroke survivors with and without falls history. Neurorehabil. Neural. Repair 24, 358–367.

Google Scholar

Hollands, K. L., Agnihotri, D., and Tyson, S. F. (2014). Effects of dual task on turning ability in stroke survivors and older adults. Gait Posture 40, 564–569. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2014.06.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Howcroft, J., Lemaire, E. D., Kofman, J., and McIlroy, W. E. (2018). Dual-task elderly gait of prospective fallers and non-fallers: a wearable sensor-based analysis. Sensors (Switzerland) 18, 7–12. doi: 10.3390/s18041275

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hughes, T., Gangulli, M., Beer, J., and Chang, C.-C. (2020). Executive function predicts decline in mobility after a fall: the MYHAT study. Exp. Gerontol. 137:110948. doi: 10.1016/j.exger.2020.110948

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hunter, S. W., Divine, A., Frengopoulos, C., and Odasso, M. M. (2018). A framework for secondary cognitive and motor tasks in dual-task gait testing in people with mild cognitive impairment. BMC Geriatr. 18:202. doi: 10.1186/s12877-018-0894-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kafri, M., Hutzler, Y., Korsensky, O., and Laufer, Y. (2017). Functional performance and balance in the oldest-old. J. Geriatr. Phys. Ther. 42, 183–188. doi: 10.1519/JPT.0000000000000133

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kelly, V. E., Janke, A. A., and Shumway-cook, A. (2010). Effects of instructed focus and task difficulty on concurrent walking and cognitive task performance in healthy young adults. Exp. Brain Res. 207, 65–73.

Google Scholar

Kleiner, A., Pacifici, I., Vagnini, A., Camerota, F., Celletti, C., De Pandis, M. F., et al. (2017). Timed Up and Go evaluation with wearable devices: validation in Parkinson’s disease. J. Bodyw. Mov. Ther. 22, 390–395. doi: 10.1016/j.jbmt.2017.07.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kochhann, R., and Lisboa, C. (2010). The mini mental state examination review of cutoff points adjusted for schooling in a large southern Brazilian sample. Dement. Neuropsychol. 4, 35–41. doi: 10.1590/s1980-57642010dn40100006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, K. Z. H., Abbud, G. A., Fraser, S. A., and DeMont, R. G. (2012). Successful adaptation of gait in healthy older adults during dual-task treadmill walking. Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 19, 150–167. doi: 10.1080/13825585.2011.628375

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lowry, K. A., Brach, J., Nebes, R. D., Studenski, S. A., and Vanswearingen, J. M. (2012). Contributions of cognitive function to straight- and curved-path walking in older adults. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 93, 802–807. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2011.12.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maclean, L. M., Brown, L. J. E., Khadra, H., and Astell, A. J. (2017). Observing prioritization effects on cognition and gait: the effect of increased cognitive load on cognitively healthy older adults’ dual-task performance. Gait Posture 53, 139–144. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2017.01.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Malcolm, B. R., Foxe, J. J., Butler, J. S., and De Sanctis, P. (2015). The aging brain shows less flexible reallocation of cognitive resources during dual-task walking: a mobile brain/body imaging (MoBI) study. Neuroimage 117, 230–242. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.05.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mellone, S., Mancini, M., King, L. A., Horak, F. B., and Chiari, L. (2016). The quality of turning in Parkinson’s disease: a compensatory strategy to prevent postural instability? J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 13:39.

Google Scholar

Montero-Odasso, M., Sarquis-Adamson, Y., Song, H. Y., Bray, N. W., Pieruccini-Faria, F., and Speechley, M. (2019). Polypharmacy, gait performance, and falls in community-dwelling older adults. results from the gait and brain study. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 67, 1182–1188. doi: 10.1111/jgs.15774

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pal, G., O’Keefe, J., Robertson-Dick, E., Bernard, B., Anderson, S., and Hall, D. (2016). Global cognitive function and processing speed are associated with gait and balance dysfunction in Parkinson’s disease. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 13:94.

Google Scholar

Patel, M., Pavic, A., and Goodwin, V. A. (2019). Wearable inertial sensors to measure gait and posture characteristic differences in older adult fallers and non-fallers: a scoping review. Gait Posture 76, 110–121. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2019.10.039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patel, P., Lamar, M., and Bhatt, T. (2014). Effect of type of cognitive task and walking speed on cognitive-motor interference during dual-task walking. Neuroscience 260, 140–148. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.12.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pau, M., Corona, F., Pili, R., Casula, C., Guicciardi, M., and Cossu, G. (2018). Quantitative assessment of gait parameters in people with Parkinson’s disease in laboratory and clinical setting: are the measures interchangeable? Neurol. Int. 10, 69–73. doi: 10.4081/ni.2018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Porciuncula, F. S., Rao, A. K., and McIsaac, T. L. (2016). Aging-related decrements during specific phases of the dual-task Timed Up-and-Go test. Aging Clin. Exp. Res. 28, 121–130. doi: 10.1007/s40520-015-0372-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Purcell, N. L., Goldman, J. G., Ouyang, B., Liu, Y., Bernard, B., and O’Keefe, J. A. (2020). The effects of dual-task cognitive interference on gait and turning in Huntington’s disease. PLoS One 15:e0226827. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0226827

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rhodes, S., Jaroslawska, A. J., Doherty, J. M., Belletier, C., Naveh-Benjamin, M., Cowan, N., et al. (2019). Storage and processing in working memory: assessing dual-task performance and task prioritization across the adult lifespan. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 148, 1204–1227. doi: 10.1037/xge0000539

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodrigues, P. F. S., and Pandeirada, J. N. S. (2015). Attention and working memory in elderly: the influence of a distracting environment. Cogn. Process. 16, 97–109. doi: 10.1007/s10339-014-0628-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosso, A. L., Cenciarini, M., Sparto, P. J., Loughlin, P. J., Joseph, M., and Huppert, T. J. (2018). Dynamic postural control. Gait Posture 57, 193–198. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2017.06.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shalev, N., Humphreys, G., and Demeyere, N. (2016). Assessing the temporal aspects of attention and its correlates in aging and chronic stroke patients. Neuropsychologia 92, 59–68. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.08.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sherrington, C., Michaleff, Z. A., Fairhall, N., Paul, S. S., Tiedemann, A., Whitney, J., et al. (2017). Exercise to prevent falls in older adults: an updated systematic review and meta-analysis. Br. J. Sports Med. 51, 1749–1757. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2016-096547

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shin, S.-S., and Yoo, W.-G. (2015). Effects of gait velocity and center of mass acceleration during turning gait in old-old elderly women. J. Phys. Ther. Sci. 27, 1779–1780. doi: 10.1589/jpts.27.1779

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Theill, N., Martin, M., Schumacher, V., Bridenbaugh, S., and Kressig, R. W. (2011). Simultaneously measuring gait and cognitive performance in cognitively healthy and cognitively impaired older adults: the basel motor-cognition dual-task paradigm. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 59, 1012–1018. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2011.03429.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thigpen, M. T., Light, K. E., Creel, G. L., and Flynn, S. M. (2000). Turning difficulty characteristics of adults aged 65 years or older. Phys. Ther. 80, 1174–1187. doi: 10.1093/ptj/80.12.1174

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomas-Carus, P., Biehl-Printes, C., Pereira, C., Vieiga, G., Costa, A., and Collado-Mateo, D. (2019). Dual task performance and history of falls in community-dwelling older adults. Exp. Gerontol. 120, 35–39. doi: 10.1016/j.exger.2019.02.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uemura, K., Yamada, M., Nagai, K., Shinya, M., Ichihashi, N., and Backg, T. R. A. C. T. (2011). Effect of dual-tasking on the center of pressure trajectory at gait initiation in elderly fallers and non-fallers. Aging Clin. Exp. Res. 24, 152–156. doi: 10.1007/bf03325161

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Venema, D. M., Hansen, H., High, R., Goetsch, T., and Siu, K. C. (2019). Minimal detectable change in dual-task cost for older adults with and without cognitive impairment. J. Geriatr. Phys. Ther. 42, E32–E38. doi: 10.1519/JPT.0000000000000194

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verghese, J., Kuslansky, G., Holtzer, R., Katz, M., Xue, X., Buschke, H., et al. (2007). Walking while talking: effect of task prioritization in the elderly. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 88, 50–53. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2006.10.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wagshul, M., Lucas, M., Holtzer, R., Ye, K., and Izzetoglu, M. (2019). Multi-modal neuroimaging of dual-task walking: structural MRI and fNIRS analysis reveals prefrontal grey matter volume moderation of brain activation in older adults. Neuroimage 189, 745–754. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.01.045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Witchel, H. J., Oberndorfer, C., Needham, R., Healy, A., Roggen, D., Eskofier, B. M., et al. (2018). Thigh-derived inertial sensor metrics to assess the sit-to-stand and stand-to-sit transitions in the Timed Up and Go (TUG) task for quantifying mobility impairment in multiple sclerosis. Front. Neurol. 9:684. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2018.00684

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yeo, S. (2017). Changes of gait variability by the attention demanding task in elderly adults sang. J. Korean Phys. Ther. 29, 303–306. doi: 10.18857/jkpt.2017.29.6.303

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yiu, M., Pang, C., Yang, L., Ouyang, H., Man, F., Lam, H., et al. (2018). Dual-task exercise reduces cognitive-motor interference in walking and falls after stroke a randomized controlled study marco. Stroke 49, 2990–2998. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.118.022157

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yogev-Seligmann, G., Hausdorff, J. M., and Giladi, N. (2012). Do we always prioritize balance when walking? Towards an integrated model of task prioritization. Mov. Disord. 27, 765–770. doi: 10.1002/mds.24963

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zaferiou, A. M., Ojeda, L., Cain, S. M., Vitali, R. V., Davidson, S. P., Stirling, L., et al. (2017). Quantifying performance on an outdoor agility drill using foot-mounted inertial measurement units. PLoS One 12:e0188184. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0188184

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zirek, E., Huseyinsinoglu, B. E., Tufekcioglu, Z., Bilgic, B., and Hanagasi, H. (2018). Which cognitive dual-task walking causes most interference on the Timed Up and Go test in Parkinson’s disease: a controlled study. Neurol. Sci. 39, 2151–2157. doi: 10.1007/s10072-019-04125-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: dual task, oldest-old adults, falls, timed up and go test, gait, cognition

Citation: Brauner FO, Balbinot G, Figueiredo AI, Hausen DO, Schiavo A and Mestriner RG (2021) The Performance Index Identifies Changes Across the Dual Task Timed Up and Go Test Phases and Impacts Task-Cost Estimation in the Oldest-Old. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 15:720719. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2021.720719

Received: 04 June 2021; Accepted: 23 August 2021;
Published: 30 September 2021.

Edited by:

Carmen Moret-Tatay, Catholic University of Valencia San Vicente Mártir, Spain

Reviewed by:

Neftali Eduardo Antonio-Villa, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, Mexico
Hilda Maria Rodrigues Moleda Constant, Hospital Moinhos de Vento, Brazil

Copyright © 2021 Brauner, Balbinot, Figueiredo, Hausen, Schiavo and Mestriner. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Régis Gemerasca Mestriner, regis.mestriner@pucrs.br; orcid.org/0000-0001-9837-1691

These authors share first authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.